Abstract
Shortly after touching an object, humans can tactually gauge the frictional resistance of a surface. The knowledge of surface friction is paramount to tactile perception and the motor control of grasp. While potent correlations between friction and participants’ perceptual response have been found, the causal link between the friction of the surface, its evolution and its perceptual experience has yet to be demonstrated. Here, we leverage new experimental apparatus able to modify friction in real time, to show that participants can perceive sudden changes in friction when they are pressing on a surface. Surprisingly, only a reduction of the friction coefficient leads to a robust perception. High-speed imaging data indicate that the sensation is caused by a release of a latent elastic strain over a 20 ms timeframe after the activation of the friction-reduction device. This rapid change of frictional properties during initial contact is interpreted as a normal displacement of the surface, which paves the way for haptic surfaces that can produce illusions of interacting with mechanical buttons.
Keywords: friction, tactile perception, skin mechanics, surface haptics
1. Introduction
Whenever our fingers physically interact with objects, tools or materials, the interaction causes our skin to deform. The deformation stimulates mechanoreceptors embedded in the tissues that signal to the brain material properties such as compliance, texture and how far the object is from sliding away. Particularly, our sense of touch can discriminate between the slipperiness of a wet soap or the grip provided by a rough surface at the first instant of touch [1,2]. The tactile sensations reinforce the internal model of our surroundings and are fundamental to properly tune our motor commands [3]. Transient events such as the crackle of a piece of bread or the subtle click of a button help us understand the dynamic behaviour of the environment [4].
However, the update of this mental model does not always reflect perfectly the physical interaction between the skin and the environment. For every interaction, the brain has to infer what is the nature of the surface being touched from an incomplete picture.
Wang and Hayward showed that unusual stimulations of the skin could fool the brain in perceiving an indentation when only local shear was applied [5,6]. Similarly, our perception of tactile speed is influenced by the texture of a surface [7]. Other illusions involve tactile and proprioception, where the compliance of an object can bias the sensation of moving the fingertip [8]. In this study, we explain the illusion that occurs when we rapidly remove the friction of the fingertip pushing on a rigid surface. Removing friction creates the illusion that the surface has moved downward (i.e. similar to the click of a keyboard). We postulate that during a simple press, the brain must decipher whether the afferent flux originates from the deformation induced by self-motion or induced from the object deformation. The distinction between the deformation of the skin coming from intrinsic or extrinsic causes is also imperfect and therefore creates the illusion of pressing a button.
We previously demonstrated that during the initial compression of the skin on a rigid surface, the friction constrains the lateral expansion of the skin [2]. In parallel, we also showed that rapid friction changes during compression lead to a clear feeling of pressing a button [9,10]. Could an intrinsic relaxation of the skin cause this sensation of key click?
When an operator is pressing on a button, elastic strain is stored in the fingertip, then suddenly released by the mechanical detent [11–13]. The interaction with buttons creates similar patterns of deformation to what happens during the rapid change of friction. Therefore, for the nervous system, the most likely explanation for this lateral relaxation of the skin would be the interaction with a button that accelerates downward.
This illusion is particularly useful for surface-haptic devices that use ultrasonic friction modulation to provide artificial tactile sensations. Ultrasonic friction modulation relies on transverse waves of the order of micrometres and outside the perceptual window of touch. However, the vibrations create a near-field acoustic levitation that reduces friction. When friction is modulated with the users’ movement, these devices can produce the illusion of shape and texture on a two-dimensional plate [14–17]. The sensation of touching a button can be triggered, not by physically displacing a button but by dynamically changing the static friction of the skin on a surface [9,10,18]. Remarkably, this behaviour arises even in the absence of sliding or lateral forces, on a static finger.
In this paper, we show that the sensation of touching a virtual button arises when the ultrasonic waves release the elastic energy stored during pressing (see figure 1a,b). When the ultrasonic vibration is turned on, the skin slightly levitates (figure 1c) causing a decrease in real area of contact and a deformation of the skin (figure 1d). The deformation of the skin following the stress release spreads radially outward (see figure 1e).
2. Material and methods
2.1. Apparatus
The experimental apparatus comprised two sub-modules: an optical path to image the contact and the skin topography, and a plate excited with ultrasonic bending waves to modulate the friction coefficient. The friction modulation section of the apparatus is illustrated in figure 2a and the optical arrangement to measure the contact is shown in figure 2b. The influence of the vibration on the friction of a sliding finger is shown figure 2c.
2.1.1. Contact optical imaging
The custom optical system has two distinct illuminations of two different colours. The first illumination is a 450 nm blue light (Thorlabs M455L3) directed to the fingertip with a low incidence angle. The grazing light highlights the topography of the skin and provides robust information for tracking the features of the skin, including before the contact occurs. The second illumination is a 660 nm red light (Thorlabs M660L4) and is shone orthogonal to the surface of the glass. The light is emitted through a beam splitter which ensures coaxiality with the return path. This illumination uses frustrated reflection of the glass–air interface to highlight the micro-junctions where the asperities of the skin are in intimate contact with the plate [19]. In the absence of the finger, 4% of the incident light is reflected toward the sensor, but if part of the skin is in intimate contact with the glass plate, the reflection is frustrated, leading to a dark area on the image. A dichroic filter is used to spatially separate the two illumination sources so the two images are acquired by a single high-speed camera (Miro M110).
2.1.2. Friction modulation device
To modulate friction in real time, we used an ultrasonic glass plate. The transverse vibrations levitate the skin away from the surface, reducing the number of micro-junctions and, therefore, reducing friction. The device used in the experiment measured 50 × 67 × 5 mm3 and vibrated in a 3 × 0 mode at a resonant frequency of 28.85 kHz. A piezoelectric sensor glued to the centre of the plate was used to measure the plate deformation in real time. The sensor, calibrated with an interferometer (IDS3010, Attocube, Munchen, Germany), gave a linear response within the entire amplitude range. The plate was mounted on an aluminium frame fixed on a 6-axis sensor (Nano43, ATI Industrial Automation, Norwalk, CT, USA). We optimized the force sensor placement to avoid large torques and cross-talk.
The piezoelectric actuator glued to the glass plate was driven at resonance by a ±200 V carrier signal, whose amplitude was modulated under computer control.
2.2. Participants
Twelve right-handed subjects aged from 22 to 35 years (3 females and 9 males) participated in the study. They were naive as to the purpose of the experiments and had no previous experience with haptic devices. None of them reported having any skin conditions or perceptual deficits. They gave their informed consent. The study was approved by the Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud Mediteranée ethics committee (2019-14-11-003). Finger images of four participants were acquired and processed to extract contact brightness and finger skin deformation.
2.3. Protocol
We used a randomized two-alternative forced choice (2-AFC) protocol where participants had to discriminate which one of the two stimuli moved similarly to a mechanical detent found in computer keyboards.
The reference stimulus had a medium friction level, maintained throughout the entire press and the comparison stimulus was a step change of friction whose average was the medium friction level. This medium friction level was obtained for a vibration amplitude of ±1.5 μm from base to peak. The six comparison signals were constructed by choosing between falling friction (where the amplitude was going from low to high) and rising friction followed by choosing one of the three levels of friction changes (low, medium, high) that correspond to an amplitude change ΔA of 1, 2 and 3 μm, respectively. As an example, for the falling friction condition, the three amplitude changes were constructed as follows: from 3 to 10−3 μm, from 2.5 to 0.5 μm and from 2 to 1 μm. The change of friction was triggered by measuring in real time the force amplitude, and if the force reached the threshold of 0.7 N, the controller would change the ultrasonic amplitude. The controller has a latency of 0.5 ms, which translates into a negligible jitter of the force threshold. During a pilot experiment, we measured the dynamic coefficients of friction when the vibration amplitude was modulated from 10−3 to 3 μm. When participants were steadily sliding across the plate, we found that the coefficient of friction varied from μ = 0.75 ± 0.14 down to μ = 0.12 ± 0.07.
Each stimulus was repeated 10 times for a total of 120 trials. The experiment lasted approximately 60 min. Participants could take a 5 min break every 15 min. To mitigate the learning effects, the participants were first shown the reference stimulus and a typical falling friction condition with a 3 μm amplitude variation. Participants wore headphones playing pink noise. A tone indicated the start of each trial and a double tone that they needed to answer.
To avoid any lateral movement during pressing, the finger was secured to a vertical linear guide. The palm rested on a support, which stabilized the participant’s hand, ensuring repeatable interaction.
2.4. Image analysis
The contact image and deformation image were recorded synchronously. We registered the images by computing the homogeneous transform from a known calibration object. The illumination drift was corrected using images without any finger. We recorded images at 1000 Hz 5 ms before and 40 ms after the friction change was triggered.
To capture the deformation of the skin, we tracked 800 features on the topography image with a key image at the instant of the trigger. These features were tracked inside the ellipse of contact using Lucas and Kanade optic flow algorithm. We computed a metric to extract the average displacement of the skin by taking the spatial average of the norm-2 of the displacement field u(x, y):
2.1 |
where S is the contact area and x, y are the spatial coordinates.
2.5. Statistical analysis
The influences of friction change in direction or amplitude ΔA on contact area and skin displacement were assessed with analyses of variance (ANOVA). One-sample t-tests were used for post hoc comparisons when ANOVA showed a significant effect of one or multiple independent variables. The significance threshold used was 0.05.
3. Results
3.1. Perceptual experiment
The probability of detecting the comparison from the reference of the psychophysics experiment for all cases is shown in figure 3. The vibration amplitude and the direction of the frictional change had a significant effect on the perception of the keyclick (ANOVA, F5,66 = 13.4, p = 5 × 10−9 and F1,70 = 12.65, p = 0.0007, respectively). The responses follow a psychometric curve, with an unambiguous detection for rapidly decreasing friction with the highest amplitude variation of 3 μm. Participants responded at chance level for every rising friction condition, where the friction was changing from low to high, consistent with previous studies [9,10,20].
We fitted the results with a logit function and found that the 75% detection threshold was reached for ΔA = 2.5 μm. For reference, when actuating the plate at 2.5 μm, the sliding friction is reduced by .
The psychophysical results clearly show that only a significant decrease in friction is perceptible, and the perception is unreliable when friction increases. We postulated in a previous paper that this specificity to the friction reduction was the effect of the sudden release of frictional stress at the interface [2]. The following section explores the contact mechanics associated with this perception of click.
3.2. Contact evolution
The contact images are used to extract a measure of the real contact area. This real contact area is the sum of all the junctions between the asperities of the skin and glass plate. The real area of contact is known to be linearly correlated to the frictional strength of the contact, therefore providing a measurement of the friction at the interface without resorting to force measurement during gross slippage [19,21]. A larger contact area has more junctions to adhere to the surface and therefore experiences more friction, assuming a constant shear strength of the interface.
For every trial, the increase in normal force causes an increase in the area of contact. The normal force typically reaches 2–3 N at the end of the press. We set the trigger when the force reached a 0.7 N threshold so that the friction modulation occurred at the early stage of the establishment of the contact. During reference trials, the friction was unchanged, and the contact brightness steadily increased by . The increase of brightness can be explained by an increase of apparent contact area and normal force when the participant pressed their finger.
Significant differences in the evolution of the contact area during falling and rising friction conditions with respect to reference trials are found (ANOVA, F2,477 = 164.23, p < 0.001). During the transition from low to high friction, the area of contact has notable dynamics. Around 5 ms after the change of friction is triggered, the contact area rises sharply (figure 4b,c left), marking a lower levitation height and the formation of new junctions between the skin and the glass plate (, , t119 = 8.11, p < 0.001).
During falling friction trials, where the friction changed from high to low, the contact area rapidly decreases within 5 ms after the transition (, , t119 = −7.41, p < 0.001) (see figure 4b,c right). This decrease demonstrates that the ultrasonic actuation reduces the number of junctions between the skin and the glass plate. The decrease of contact is positively correlated with friction reduction (Spearman's coefficient = 0.123, p < 0.0001). Twenty milliseconds after this rapid reduction of contact, the contact rises again under the increase of normal force from the participant.
When inspecting individual trials, we also observed that the majority of falling friction trials shows that the losses of contact are concentrated in specific clusters that grow slowly, and the front of which propagates laterally. This effect might be due to the complex interaction between ultrasonic levitation and soft skin. Pockets of over-pressured air are nucleating and diffusing throughout the area of contact before finally reaching equilibrium. By contrast, during rising friction cases, contact increases over the entire finger surface.
3.3. Contact and skin displacement
In addition to the real contact area, we measured the displacement field of the skin. We observed the movement of patches of skin during the normal compression of the fingertip pulp. During reference trials, the ultrasonic vibrations are maintained at 1.5 μm causing an incomplete contact between the finger and the plate. The observed increase in contact area after 0.7 N is only due to the increase in finger pressure. The skin of the fingertip exhibits a lateral displacement during compression (M = 15.6 μm, s.d. = 17.1 μm), in line with previously reported findings [2].
In the condition where friction increases, the movement of the skin only occurs before the trigger, since almost no friction is present at the initial moment of contact (figure 5a). After the rapid increase of friction, the growth of the contact area increases the friction and reduces the lateral displacement of the skin to almost zero in the 20 ms following the trigger (M = 7.4 μm, s.d. = 8.5 μm, t119 = 9.51, p < 0.001) (see figure 4d–f left and figure 5d). This sudden cease of lateral displacement can be explained by the high friction state that prevents any lateral movement of the skin.
In the falling friction condition, the contact initially develops with high friction. Consequently, only a slight lateral deformation of the skin is observed before the trigger. After the rapid reduction of friction, several detachment fronts nucleate in the area of contact, which causes a non-uniform movement of the skin (figure 5c). We measured a lateral displacement of the skin after the change of friction of 43.3 ± 54.1 μm (t119 = 8.77, p < 0.001) (see figure 4d–f right). The size of the change of lateral displacement followed a normal distribution, verified by a chi-square test rejecting the null hypothesis (χ2(1, N = 120) = 16.87, p < 0.001). The amplitude of the lateral movement significantly increases between friction conditions and for the reference stimulus (ANOVA, F2,477 = 49.63, p < 0.001).
The skin movement 20 ms after the trigger has an average displacement of 43.3 ± 54 μm. However, the displacement field is not uniform. The outer regions show a larger movement of the skin than the inner region, showing an expanding pattern similar to what is observed in [2] (see figure 5f).
Overall, the change of contact area decreases with increasing change of amplitude (ANOVA, F6,553 = 87.37, p < 0.001) (see figure 5b). Conversely, the global displacement shows a positive correlation with the amplitude of the change of ultrasonic vibrations (ANOVA, F6,473 = 21.39, p < 0.001) (see figure 5e).
3.4. Skin deformation causes perception
While the deformation of the skin is of the order of a few hundred micrometres, the relaxation of the skin after a reduction of friction is quick enough to generate a strain rate of roughly 5% per second compatible with the known detection threshold of rapidly adapting afferents [22].
Using the data from the experiment, we can establish a causal connection between skin deformation and the perception of mechanical stimuli. The connection emerges when selecting only trials where the friction was decreased (falling friction). While the change of contact area pre- and post-trigger was not significantly different when the participant detected the stimuli or not (figure 6a), the amount of deformation of the skin had an unambiguous explanatory power over the probability of detection by the participant (figure 6b, Spearman's coefficient = 0.123, p < 0.01).
The trials where the stimulus was not detected, but the amplitude was 3 μm, correspond to near-zero changes in the contact area and negligible deformation (see figure 6c). Conversely, the trials where the stimuli were correctly detected are associated with larger contact area changes and skin deformation.
3.5. Model predictions
To better understand how the skin deforms after a sudden change of friction, we modelled the biomechanics of the fingertip under frictional contact. We used a finite-difference time-domain scheme to capture the deformation of the skin, viscoelasticity and local friction behaviour at the interface. The model is identical to the one used in [2]. We simulated the interaction between the finger skin and the plate surface under seven friction changes from −3 to 3 μm with a normal force of 4 N applied to the bone element (see figure 7a).
The rising friction case has a similar mechanical behaviour to the reference, which sees no change in friction. Because of the low friction, the elements start to expand laterally before the mechanical detent, when the friction is small. Then when we increase friction, the lateral component of the interfacial pressure increases and locks the elements in place. The simulated median displacements are shown in figure 7b. The curves flatten in all conditions, but despite a saturation, the median of the average displacement metric is eight times higher in the maximal falling friction condition compared to the rising friction condition.
Simulation results are shown in figure 7c where individual displacements of skin patches appear in red, and the stresses at the interface appear in white. In the falling friction condition, the lateral component of the interfacial pressure decreases significantly after the friction change, which happens at 0 ms. This release of mechanical stress results in significant lateral deformation of the skin.
4. Discussion
The skin of our fingertip is highly deformable and, therefore, can conform to a wide range of shapes and surfaces [23,24]. During the first instant of contact, friction plays a critical role in the way the skin will deform [1,2]. If friction is low, the skin will expand laterally and a negligible amount of tangential stress will be stored. Conversely, when friction is high, the skin grips to the surface when it makes contact, blocking its tangential expansion and storing elastic energy.
Surprisingly, this work shows that a sudden release of elastic energy by rapidly reducing friction can be perceived. Effectively this rapid change of friction is analogous to changing the boundary condition at the contact from fixed to a roller. A similar change of boundary condition arises when operating real buttons [13], which might explain why we perceive these changes of friction as a button click. The change in friction produces a notable lateral expansion of the skin, which takes less than 20 ms to fully develop. The time response is consistent with known values of the relaxation of the skin [25] suggesting that this duration is limited by the viscoelastic behaviour. The brief deformation of the skin can be sufficiently large to produce conscious sensations.
The ultrasonic vibration excites the skin in a range of frequencies and deformations that do not stimulate the mechanoreceptors in the skin. Yet participants reported perceiving the event, suggesting that the mechanical deformation of the skin, due to the release of friction, is sufficient to trigger a sensation. Participants reported that they felt they pressed a mechanical button, although no significant normal movement of the plate was recorded. Our hypothesis behind this association is that the detent that we feel when interacting with a real button produces a similar release of elastic energy to what is found when interacting with a mechanical button. Because the release of energy is congruent to the user’s action, this sensation of mechanical detent after a reduction of friction is perceived as the activation of a mechanical button.
The salience of the illusory button directly connects to the size of the friction change. The larger the friction change, the larger the skin deformation and the easier it is to detect the stimulus. Since the stored energy during compression is limited by the mechanics of the skin, there is an upper bound on the strength of the stimuli. In the present study, we focused our attention on purely normal exploratory motions, where the participant could only press on the plate since their finger was guided by a vertical linear guide. Therefore, we recorded only an insignificant amount of lateral force. However, we could increase the strength of the stimuli by storing additional elastic energy in the gross lateral deformation of the fingertip during compression, using surface-haptic devices that provide active forcing, such as in [26,27]. A lateral deformation at the initial instant of skin deformation could lead to potent sensations and salient click.
The observed skin deformation is of the order of a few tens of micrometres and yet it leads to a robust perception. We postulate that such a small change is perceivable because the deformation is quick enough to stimulate the rapidly adapting afferents. We showed that the expansion of the skin is a fundamental factor of our estimate of friction during the moments following the initial contact [2]. However, the discrimination of friction on initial contact is challenging, and a proper estimate of friction is made when there is a lateral motion of the skin on the surface [28].
5. Conclusion
In this study, we showed that micrometre-scale ultrasonic vibrations, which are not perceivable by touch, can create a distinct tactile stimulus resembling a button click when the friction suddenly decreases. The stimulus is created by an indirect stimulation of the skin: a near-field levitation effect reduces the friction and allows the skin to expand. To observe this skin expansion, we developed a custom optical apparatus that can simultaneously monitor the change in the real contact area—connected to the frictional strength of the contact—and the deformation of the skin. The observed skin expansion when pressing on the surface is at the root of the perception since larger deformation leads to an improved probability of detection by participants.
The perception of friction is central to the appreciation of materials and the motor control of grasp. Taken together, the observations presented in this paper allow us to postulate that the tactile perception of friction when actively interacting with our environment is initially caused by the lateral expansion of the skin.
Ethics
Twelve participants participated in the study and gave their informed consent prior to the procedure. The study was conducted with the approval of Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud Mediteranée Ethics Committee (2019-14-11-003).
Data accessibility
Data from this study are available at the open-access 4TU.ResearchData Repository (https://doi.org/10.4121/21220352.v1). Source code of the finger skin model data has been deposited in GitLab (https://gitlab.tudelft.nl/lwillemet/finger-model).
Authors' contributions
J.M.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, resources, writing—original draft; L.W.: formal analysis, investigation, methodology, software, validation, visualization, writing—review and editing; M.W.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, investigation, project administration, resources, supervision, validation, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
This work was supported by generous funding from Stellantis, and the French Research Agency ANR (PHASE 16-CE10-0003 & IOTA 16-CE33-0002). M.W. acknowledges the support of the 4TU Soft Robotics programme.
References
- 1.Johansson RS, Flanagan JR. 2009. Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object manipulation tasks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 345-359. ( 10.1038/nrn2621) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Willemet L, Kanzari K, Monnoyer J, Birznieks I, Wiertlewski M. 2021. Initial contact shapes the perception of friction. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2109109118. ( 10.1073/pnas.2109109118) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Flanagan JR, Wing AM. 1997. The role of internal models in motion planning and control: evidence from grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads. J. Neurosci. 17, 1519-1528. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-04-01519.1997) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Terekhov AV, Hayward V. 2015. The brain uses extrasomatic information to estimate limb displacement. Proc. R. Soc. B 282, 20151661. ( 10.1098/rspb.2015.1661) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hayward V, MacLean KE. 2007. Do it yourself haptics: part I. IEEE Robot. Autom. Mag. 14, 88-104. ( 10.1109/M-RA.2007.907921) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wang Q, Hayward V. 2008. Tactile synthesis and perceptual inverse problems seen from the viewpoint of contact mechanics. ACM Trans. Appl. Percept. 5, 7. ( 10.1145/1279920.1279921) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Delhaye BP, O’Donnell MK, Lieber JD, McLellan KR, Bensmaia SJ. 2019. Feeling fooled: texture contaminates the neural code for tactile speed. PLoS Biol. 17, e3000431. ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000431) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Moscatelli A, Bianchi M, Serio A, Terekhov A, Hayward V, Ernst MO, Bicchi A. 2016. The change in fingertip contact area as a novel proprioceptive cue. Curr. Biol. 26, 1159-1163. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.052) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Monnoyer J, Diaz E, Bourdin C, Wiertlewski M. 2016. Ultrasonic friction modulation while pressing induces a tactile feedback. In Eurohaptics 2016 (eds Bello F, Kajimoto H, Visell Y). Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol. 9774, pp. 171-179. Cham, Switzerland: Springer. ( 10.1007/978-3-319-42321-0_16) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Monnoyer J, Diaz E, Bourdin C, Wiertlewski M. 2017. Optimal skin impedance promotes perception of ultrasonic switches. In 2017 IEEE World Haptics Conf., Munich, Germany, 6–9 June 2017, pp. 130–135. ( 10.1109/WHC.2017.7989889) [DOI] [PubMed]
- 11.Hoggan E, Brewster SA, Johnston J. 2008. Investigating the effectiveness of tactile feedback for mobile touchscreens. In Proc. SIGCHI Conf. on Human Factors in Computing Systems, pp. 1573–1582. New York, NY: ACM. ( 10.1145/1357054.1357300) [DOI]
- 12.Kim S, Lee G. 2013. Haptic feedback design for a virtual button along force-displacement curves. In Proc. 26th Annual ACM Symp. on User Interface Software and Technology, pp. 91–96. New York, NY: ACM. ( 10.1145/2501988.2502041) [DOI]
- 13.Weir DW, Peshkin M, Colgate JE, Buttolo P, Rankin J, Johnston M, 2004. The haptic profile: capturing the feel of switches. In Proc. 12th Int. Symp. on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, Chicago, IL, USA, 27–28 March 2004, pp. 186–193. ( 10.1109/HAPTIC.2004.1287195) [DOI]
- 14.Biet M, Giraud F, Lemaire-Semail B. 2007. Squeeze film effect for the design of an ultrasonic tactile plate. IEEE Trans. Ultrason. Ferroelectr. Freq. Control 54, 2678-2688. ( 10.1109/TUFFC.2007.596) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Watanabe T, Fukui S. 1995. A method for controlling tactile sensation of surface roughness using ultrasonic vibration. In Proc. 1995 IEEE Int. Conf. on Robotics and Automation, Nagoya, Japan, 21–27 May 1995, pp. 1134–1139. ( 10.1109/ROBOT.1995.525433) [DOI]
- 16.Wiertlewski M, Friesen RF, Colgate JE. 2016. Partial squeeze film levitation modulates fingertip friction. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 9210-9215. ( 10.1073/pnas.1603908113) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Winfield L, Glassmire J, Colgate JE, Peshkin M. 2007. T-pad: tactile pattern display through variable friction reduction. In 2nd Joint EuroHaptics Conf. and Symp. on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, Tsukuba, Japan, 22–24 March 2007, pp. 421–426. ( 10.1109/WHC.2007.105) [DOI]
- 18.Tashiro K, Shiokawa Y, Aono T, Maeno T. 2009. Realization of button click feeling by use of ultrasonic vibration and force feedback. In 3rd Joint EuroHaptics Conf. and Symp. on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, Salt Lake City, UT, USA, 18–20 March 2009, pp. 1–6. ( 10.1109/WHC.2009.4810877) [DOI]
- 19.Huloux N, Willemet L, Wiertlewski M. 2021. How to measure the area of real contact of skin on glass. IEEE Trans. Haptics 14, 235-241. ( 10.1109/TOH.2021.3073747) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Saleem MK, Yilmaz C, Basdogan C. 2018. Psychophysical evaluation of change in friction on an ultrasonically-actuated touchscreen. IEEE Trans. Haptics 11, 599-610. ( 10.1109/TOH.2018.2830790) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pasumarty SM, Johnson SA, Watson SA, Adams MJ. 2011. Friction of the human finger pad: influence of moisture, occlusion and velocity. Tribol. Lett. 44, 117-137. ( 10.1007/s11249-011-9828-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Edin BB. 2004. Quantitative analyses of dynamic strain sensitivity in human skin mechanoreceptors. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 3233-3243. ( 10.1152/jn.00628.2004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dzidek B, Bochereau S, Johnson SA, Hayward V, Adams MJ. 2017. Why pens have rubbery grips. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 10 864-10 869. ( 10.1073/pnas.1706233114) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Warman PH, Ennos AR. 2009. Fingerprints are unlikely to increase the friction of primate fingerpads. J. Exp. Biol. 212, 2016-2022. ( 10.1242/jeb.028977) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wang Q, Hayward V. 2007. In vivo biomechanics of the fingerpad skin under local tangential traction. J. Biomech. 40, 851-860. ( 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2006.03.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gueorguiev D, Kaci A, Amberg M, Giraud F, Lemaire-Semail B. 2018. Travelling ultrasonic wave enhances keyclick sensation. In Eurohaptics 2018 (eds Prattichizzo D, Shinoda H, Tan H, Ruffaldi E, Frisoli A). Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol. 10894, pp. 302-312. Cham, Switzerland: Springer. ( 10.1007/978-3-319-93399-3_27) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Xu H, Klatzky RL, Peshkin MA, Colgate JE. 2019. Localized rendering of button click sensation via active lateral force feedback. In 2019 IEEE World Haptics Conf., Tokyo, Japan, 9–12 July 2019, pp. 509–514. ( 10.1109/WHC.2019.8816158) [DOI]
- 28.Khamis H, Afzal N, Sanchez J, Vickery R, Wiertlewski M, Redmond SJ, Birznieks I. 2021. Friction sensing mechanisms for perception and motor control: passive touch without sliding may not provide perceivable frictional information. J. Neurophysiol. 125, 809-823. ( 10.1152/jn.00504.2020) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data from this study are available at the open-access 4TU.ResearchData Repository (https://doi.org/10.4121/21220352.v1). Source code of the finger skin model data has been deposited in GitLab (https://gitlab.tudelft.nl/lwillemet/finger-model).