Skip to main content
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research : JSLHR logoLink to Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research : JSLHR
. 2022 Jul 19;65(8):3004–3055. doi: 10.1044/2022_JSLHR-21-00543

Caregiver-Implemented Communication Interventions for Children Identified as Having Language Impairment 0 Through 48 Months of Age: A Scoping Review

Lizbeth H Finestack a,, Marianne Elmquist b, Kirstin Kuchler a, Andrea Boh Ford a, Betul Cakir-Dilek a, Amy Riegelman a, Sarah Jane Brown a, Scott Marsalis a
PMCID: PMC9911096  PMID: 35858263

Abstract

Purpose:

Caregiver-implemented interventions are frequently used to support the early communication of young children with language impairment. Although there are numerous studies and meta-analyses supporting their use, there is a need to better understand the intervention approaches and identify potential gaps in the research base. With that premise, we conducted a scoping review to synthesize existing data with an end goal of informing future research directions.

Method:

We identified relevant studies by comprehensively searching four databases. After deduplication, we screened 5,703 studies. We required included studies (N = 59) to evaluate caregiver-implemented communication interventions and include at least one caregiver communication outcome measure. We extracted information related to the (a) study, child, and caregiver characteristics; (b) intervention components (e.g., strategies taught, delivery method and format, and dosage); and (c) caregiver and child outcome measures (e.g., type, quality, and level of evidence).

Results:

We synthesized results by age group of the child participants. There were no studies with children in the prenatal through 11-month-old age range identified in our review that yielded a caregiver language outcome measure with promising or compelling evidence. For the 12- through 23-month group, there were seven studies, which included eight communication intervention groups; for the 24- through 35-month group, there were 21 studies, which included 26 intervention groups; and for the 36- through 48-month group, there were 21 studies, which included 23 intervention groups. Across studies and age groups, there was considerable variability in the reporting of study characteristics, intervention approaches, and outcome measures.

Conclusion:

Our scoping review highlights important research gaps and inconsistencies in study reporting that should be addressed in future investigations.

Supplemental Material:

https://doi.org/10.23641/asha.20289195


Communication development is critical for later academic, social, and vocational success (Hoff, 2013; Morreale et al., 2017; Roulstone et al., 2011). However, communication impairments impact between 6 and 8 million people in the United States alone (National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders [NIDCD], 2016). To reduce this impact and promote positive outcomes for young children who demonstrate communication delays and disorders, the receipt of early intervention aimed at facilitating and strengthening communication skills is critical (e.g., Justice et al., 2009; Rescorla, 2009). Toward that end, intervention approaches that teach caregivers 1 to implement strategies that target and enhance their everyday communication interactions with their child experiencing communication delays and disabilities are empirically supported (e.g., Heidlage et al., 2020; Roberts & Kaiser, 2011).

Rationale and Evidence Base for Caregiver-Implemented Communication Interventions

Caregiver-implemented communication interventions can support a wide range of skills central to a child's development. Communication reflects the process of sharing information and may be characterized as language-based (i.e., linguistic; e.g., use of conventional signs, words, and syntax) or social (i.e., nonlinguistic; e.g., use of gestures, vocalizations, eye gaze, and appropriate turn taking in both receptive and expressive domains). Thus, focus on caregiver-implemented communication interventions is important for several reasons. First, this approach is consistent with theoretical and empirical knowledge that communication is learned within the context of social interactions and is transactional, with important roles for the child and the caregiver, such as contingent responses from the caregiver (e.g., Dunst et al., 1989; Ford et al., 2020; Sameroff, 2009). Second, embedding intervention within children's routine interactions with their caregivers is logical, effective, and efficient given that these interactions frequently occur and children are often motivated to regularly engage with their caregivers (e.g., Heidlage et al., 2020; Schreibman et al., 2015). Third, these interventions can be further used in ways that align with the child's and caregiver's cultural, ethnic, social, and developmental assets, a necessary component of broad scale implementation within an increasingly diverse society (National Association for the Education of Young Children [NAEYC], 2020).

Meta-analyses are useful to evaluate overall effects of caregiver-implemented interventions across studies. Results of several meta-analyses and reviews provide rich support for teaching caregivers strategies to promote early development broadly (e.g., Jeong et al., 2021) as well as specific strategies to enhance the language skills of young children with delays and disabilities (e.g., Heidlage et al., 2020; Law et al., 2004; Roberts & Kaiser, 2011; Roberts et al., 2019). These meta-analyses demonstrate that caregiver-implemented interventions positively impact early child social communication and language skills, particularly expressive language, of children who are at risk or who have communication impairment (Barton & Fettig, 2013; Heidlage et al., 2020; Roberts & Kaiser, 2011; Roberts et al., 2019). Additionally, results indicate that communication outcomes do not differ when the intervention is implemented by trained parents or clinicians (Fuller & Kaiser, 2020; Law et al., 2004), and there is some indication that for children on the autism spectrum, interventions that include both parents and clinicians may be most beneficial (Hampton & Kaiser, 2016).

Need for More Information About Caregiver-Implemented Communication Interventions

Across meta-analyses and reviews focused on caregiver-implemented communication interventions, there is great variability in populations represented in the studies included (e.g., age and disability), the specific interventions evaluated, and the caregiver and child outcome measures. For example, the Roberts and Kaiser (2011) meta-analysis included 18 experimental studies published between 1988 and 2010 with parent-implemented interventions targeting child communication (excluding dialogic reading) for children between 18 and 60 months of age with any type of language impairment. Studies had to include at least one type of child language measure. The Barton and Fettig (2013) review included 24 studies with experimental designs published between 1972 and 2012 in nine selected peer-reviewed journals. Their inclusion criteria specified a focus on a parent-implemented intervention for children with disabilities 8 years of age or younger, with child outcomes that could include language, play, or a reduction in challenging behaviors. Hampton and Kaiser (2016) included 26 studies published between 1980 and 2014 with children on the autism spectrum under the age of 8 years. Studies were required to include spoken language outcome measures. Roberts et al. (2019) included 76 studies published between 1988 and 2019 with children under the age 6 years for whom parents served as the primary interventionist with parent and/or child outcome measures. Studies were excluded if the comparison group received treatment. The Heidlage et al. (2020) meta-analysis included 25 randomized control trials (RCTs) of parent-implemented interventions published between 1992 and 2015 for children 0–8 years of age. Child language outcomes were based on parent reports, standardized direct assessments, and observations.

Because a typical goal of a meta-analysis is to address a specific clinical question for a particular population and necessitate rigorous study designs, they can be narrow in scope (Gurevitch et al., 2018). In contrast, the goal of a scoping review is to better understand the nature and extent of a research base to help motivate a more rigorous review, such as a systematic review (Grant & Booth, 2009). Systematic reviews typically aim to determine what is known in a particular area and provide recommendations for future research (Grant & Booth, 2009). Given that there is a strong clinical research base supporting the use of caregiver-implemented communication interventions for young children with communication delays and disabilities (i.e., meta-analyses described above), it is now important to identify any gaps in this base. Addressing these gaps is critical to ensure that the research base is robust and applicable to as many populations as possible. A scoping review is thus a logical next step for the field to summarize critical components of caregiver-implemented communication interventions, such as populations represented, features of the intervention (e.g., intervention provider, language, location, dosage, strategies, and context), and outcomes targeted.

Purpose and Research Questions

The purpose of the current scoping review was to better understand the extent, range, and nature of research focused on caregiver-implemented communication interventions. We aimed to identify research gaps in the existing literature to motivate further, empirically driven research on caregiver-implemented communication interventions to expand the evidence base (Arksey & O'Malley, 2005). At the time that we preregistered our scoping review (May 2019), there were no other known studies that shared our review objectives. Since that time, the Bridging the Word Gap Research Network (BWGRN; Carta et al., 2014) has published a series of reviews (see the works of Greenwood et al., 2020; Haring Biel et al., 2020; Larson et al., 2020; Mahoney et al., 2020; Walker et al., 2020) with some overlap in scope with this study.

There are, however, several distinct differences between the studies. First, the BWGRN literature base included studies with children birth to 5 years of age, and preschool- and kindergarten-aged; whereas, we limited our review to only include studies of children 0 through 48 months of age who have not yet received formal schooling. We did this with recognition that caregiver-implemented communication intervention approaches are likely to differ considerably for infants, toddlers, and preschoolers compared with older children who are entering more formal education prekindergarten and kindergarten programs and likely to have different communication and language goals. Second, related to this focus, the BWGRN literature base included studies for which caregivers and/or classroom teachers were taught to implement interventions; we limited our studies to only those for which primary caregivers were taught the interventions. None of the BWGRN reviews have focused exclusively on early language interventions implemented by primary caregivers of children with language impairment. As such, although the breadth of our scoping review was wide, we limited our study questions in the current review to

  1. What populations have been represented in research focused on caregiver-implemented communication interventions for children with language impairment in terms of child and caregiver characteristics?

  2. How have intervention components of caregiver-implemented communication interventions been represented in the research? The components of interest included the (a) person delivering the intervention, (b) language used to deliver the intervention, (c) location, (d) intervention dosage, (e) delivery format (e.g., group and one-on-one) and method (e.g., in-person and telehealth), (f) instructional methods, (g) strategies taught to caregivers, and (h) targeted contexts (e.g., play routines).

  3. What caregiver and child outcome measures have been used to evaluate caregiver-implemented communication interventions for children with communication impairment?

Method

Protocol and Registration

Our method is primarily based on the scoping review framework laid out by Arksey and O'Malley (2005), incorporating some recommendations of Levac et al. (2010), and reported in accordance with PRISMA-ScR (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews; Tricco et al., 2018). Supplemental Material S1 includes the PRISMA-ScR Checklist. Our protocol was drafted using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses Protocols (PRISMA-P) and registered prospectively with the Open Science Framework (OSF) on May 23, 2019. The protocol (https://osf.io/sbx6k) and affiliated project are available without embargo and discoverable within OSF Registries. In preparation for this review, we searched for existing registered protocols on similar topics in the Campbell Collaboration, Cochrane Collaboration, and Prospero registries.

Eligibility Criteria

We included studies in peer-reviewed journal articles (published or in press), which evaluated interventions targeting caregivers with the aim of improving child communication development. Caregiver was defined as parents or other family primary caregivers, as well as foster parents, but not professional service providers such as therapists, teachers, or day care providers. To be included in the current review, the study population must have included caregivers of children identified as having a language or communication impairment 0 through 48 months of age. This cutoff age was selected to help focus the review on caregiver-implemented communication interventions delivered prior to formal school opportunities (e.g., kindergarten) and which target communication skills relevant to a more restricted age range. Because the immediate goal of caregiver-implemented interventions is to modify caregiver behaviors, to be included in the review, the study must have included at least one caregiver communication outcome measure. We did not restrict by date or location of the study; however, results needed to be reported in English. Although, as part of a larger project, we included case studies, single-case designed studies, group quasi-experimental, or experimental designed studies, the current review was limited to group studies to allow for more consistent data synthesis and reliable interpretation. We also limited the present review to studies that included at least one group receiving a caregiver-implemented communication intervention who were identified as children with language impairment (e.g., developmental language disorder) or a condition closely associated with language or communication impairment (e.g., autism 2 and Down syndrome). Our inclusion/exclusion criteria are detailed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Study inclusion and exclusion criteria related to the publication, child, and caregiver.

Criteria Inclusion criteria description Exclusion criteria description
Study a
 Type Peer reviewed; published or in press Gray literature
 Language Conducted in any language; reported in English Studies not reported in English
 Design Group experimental and/or quasi-experimental designs SCED, case studies, reviews, meta-analyses
Child
 Age Children with a mean age or estimated mean < 48 months for which the age was reported/obtainable for all children Children aged 48 months and older or unable to determine mean age or age range
 Language or communication status Children identified as having a language or communication impairment (e.g., DLD) or condition closely associated with language or communication impairment (autism, DS) Typically developing children or children identified at risk for language of communication impairment (e.g., DHH, sibling of a child with autism)
Caregiver
 Target group designation Intervention target group was individuals who were primary caregivers (e.g., parent, foster parent, and relatives) of child participants, with no restriction on age
If a parent education component was included in addition to a nonprimary caregiver group, the study was included
Intervention target group was nonprimary caregiver (e.g., teachers, childcare provider, service providers, and researchers)
No caregiver training component
 Communicative behavior as focus of intervention and measurement Study targeted and included measurement of at least one caregiver verbal or nonverbal communicative behavior (e.g., words per minute, responsiveness, and diversity of words)
Data came from self-report, behavior coding, SALT, LENA, etc.
No measure of caregiver communicative behavior
Exclusion criteria added after beginning to read abstracts:
  • Drugs as intervention

  • Crying as sole reason for intervention

Note.  SCED = single-case experimental design; DLD = developmental language disorder; DS = Down syndrome; DHH = Deaf and hard of hearing; SALT = Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts; LENA = Language ENvironment Analysis.

a

We had no restriction on publication date or study setting.

Information Sources

We used our inclusion criteria to develop customized search strategies in four databases selected for their coverage of relevant literature. Medline (Ovid), APA PsycInfo (Ovid), and Linguistics & Language Behavior Abstracts (ProQuest) offer complementary disciplinary scope, while including Scopus helped to ensure inclusion of studies, which might otherwise be missed. The searches were designed and conducted by social science librarians with assistance from health science librarian colleagues. The full, reproducible strategy for each database is available in Supplemental Material S2. We conducted the initial searches in May 2019 and then updated the searches in August 2020 and November 2021. The searches in the four databases produced 9,716 results. After we removed duplicates, 5,703 studies remained.

Selection of Sources of Evidence

We imported returned studies into Covidence (Covidence, Veritas Health Innovation, Melbourne, Australia; covidence.org), a software tool for facilitating synthesizing reviews and automatically deduplicating records. Prior to the title and abstract screening and full-text screening, the screeners were calibrated by screening the same 15 publications independently and then discussing disagreements. During calibration, we refined our inclusion/exclusion criteria to increase reliability. When this step was complete, two research team members independently screened the rest of the publication titles and abstracts using the inclusion/exclusion criteria (see Table 1), with disagreements resolved by consensus. We then retrieved the full text for publications that passed this screening level. Next, two research team members independently screened full texts, with disagreements again resolved by consensus. Figure 1 summarizes the flow of information through the retrieval and screening stages.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flowchart.

Data Extracting/Charting Process

We developed surveys in Qualtrics (Qualtrics; qualtrics.com), a web-based survey development tool, to extract our data. We tested several survey versions, refining the survey after each test, to ensure that we could reliably extract the necessary data. After several trials, we determined that it was most efficient and reliable to have two researchers extract from each publication in tandem. This approach allowed the researchers to discuss any discrepancies immediately and avoid differences in data extraction that may have cascading effects. For example, if one researcher failed to identify a relevant group, they would also not report on the group characteristics. For each study, thus, two members of our team opened separate Qualtrics surveys on their own computer. Each independently completed each page of the survey. As they finished each page, they compared responses before proceeding to the next page. A total of five researchers extracted the data. Data extracted included information regarding participant demographics, intervention characteristics and type of outcomes measured. Table 2 lists the data extracted from each publication.

Table 2.

Data extracted from studies by domain.

Extraction domain Data extracted
Study characteristics
  • Year of publication

  • Country in which study took place

  • Study design: (a) RCT, (b) non-RCT, or (c) single group

Caregiver characteristics
  • Relationship to the child

  • Age

  • Gender

  • Race

  • Special characteristics (e.g., low income and identified disability)

Child characteristics
  • Age

  • Gender

  • Race

  • Special characteristics (e.g., diagnosed as being on the autism spectrum)

Intervention characteristics
  • Person delivering the intervention

  • Language used to deliver the intervention

  • Location (e.g., home, clinic, and university setting)

  • Dosage (i.e., number of sessions, length of session, and duration of intervention)

  • Delivery method (e.g., in-person and telehealth)

  • Delivery format (e.g., group and one-on-one)

  • Instructional methods (e.g., information and live feedback)

  • Strategies taught (e.g., models, expansions, and following the child's lead) and information provided to caregivers

  • Specific context (e.g., play routines and shared book reading)

Caregiver communication outcomes
  • Outcome name

  • Measurement context (e.g., caregiver–child interaction)

  • Communication domain:

    • Nonverbal: Communicative behaviors that do not involve speech (e.g., gestures)

    • Spoken language input: Communicative behavior involving verbal language (e.g., MLU and adult word count)

    • Global: Captured the overall language environment (e.g., HOME)

    • Interactional: Communication behaviors that are contingent on child communicative acts (e.g., conversational turns and responsivity)

  • Maintenance and/or generalization outcome, if applicable

Child communication outcomes
  • Outcome name

  • Measurement context (e.g., caregiver–child interaction)

  • Communication domain: (a) expressive language, (b) receptive language, (c) social/pragmatic language, or (d) global language (e.g., mix of expressive and expressive language)

  • Maintenance and/or generalization outcome, if applicable

Note.  RCT = randomized control trial; MLU = mean length of utterance.

We also assessed the quality of each communicative outcome and assigned an evidence-level rating. Using ratings adapted from a scoping review conducted by Guiberson and Ferris (2019) and our consensus method, we rated each outcome as compelling, promising, or lacking. We based our ratings on study design, statistical significance, and effect sizes. Statistical significance and effect sizes were determined in relation to the study's comparison group(s), which in some cases was a no communication intervention control group and in other cases was another communication treatment group. An outcome was rated as compelling if the study demonstrated experimental control (e.g., quasi-experimental design or RCT), had a standard effect size (Cohen's d) > 0.25, and/or there was a significant difference between groups. An outcome was coded as promising, if there were moderate to large effects, but no experimental control. Outcomes were coded as lacking, if there was no intervention effect on the outcome (i.e., effect size < 0.25), or analyses did not include or enable the calculation of effect sizes. When a study included a communication treatment control group, we only rated the evidence level for the treatment group identified by the study authors as being the primary intervention group. We included broad indications of outcome measure quality to ensure that each of the studies included in our scoping review was minimally associated with at least one positive caregiver outcome. As we aimed to better understand the composition caregiver-implemented communication interventions, we excluded studies with only outcomes associated with lacking evidence (no experimental control, no significant effect, and small effect size). If a study included follow-up data, communication outcomes were also evaluated to determine if effects maintained, decreased, or increased. This was based on how each study analyzed their results (e.g., some studies evaluated pretest/baseline to follow-up, whereas others evaluated posttest to follow-up). Our full data extraction manual is available on our OSF project page.

Finally, we characterized each intervention using the system developed by Sandbank et al. (2020). Three researchers independently characterized each of the intervention groups as Behavioral (i.e., grounded in operant theories of learning or comprising predominantly behavioral techniques), developmental (i.e., grounded in constructivist theories of learning or primarily child-led), Naturalistic Developmental Behavioral Intervention (NDBI; i.e., combination of adult-led behavioral teaching approaches and child-led activities embedded in natural contexts and follow a natural developmental sequence), or sensory-based intervention (i.e., incorporated exposure to sensory stimuli, including auditory, tactile, or visual). Following Sandbank et al.'s system, we characterized any intervention, which the authors identified as TEACCH (Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children) as such. If a study did not include adequate information to characterize the intervention, we noted this. Upon independently characterizing the interventions, the researchers discussed discrepancies to come to a final consensus.

Results

After duplicates were removed, a total of 5,703 citations were identified from the searches of the electronic databases. On the basis of the title and the abstract, 4,838 were excluded, with 865 full-text articles to be retrieved and assessed for eligibility. Of these, 805 were excluded for the following reasons: 228 did not include a caregiver language or communication outcome measure, 169 did not include children 0 through 48 months of age, 119 did not include a study group comprising children with an identified language or communication impairment or condition closely associated with such, 66 did not include a caregiver language or communication intervention, 155 were not group studies, 45 did not include a language or communication intervention, 11 were not reported in English, and eight were ongoing studies with no study results. We identified an additional five studies as duplicates. The remaining 59 studies were considered eligible for the purposes of this review (see Figure 1). Supplemental Material S9 includes the complete reference list for all studies included in the review.

Overall Study Characteristics

The 59 studies identified were published between 1987 and 2021, with the majority (n = 42) published after 2010. The identified studies were most commonly reported to be conducted in the United States (n = 32), Canada (n = 10), the United Kingdom (n = 9), Australia (n = 2), and India (n = 2). Other locations included Italy, Northern Ireland, Saudi Arabia, and Turkey (ns = 1). The studies included 30 RCTs, 11 non-RCTs, and 18 single-group studies. There were nine studies that had more than one intervention group meeting our criteria, yielding a total of 72 unique groups that included children identified as having communication impairment who were receiving some type of caregiver-implemented communication intervention. Of the nine studies with more than one identified treatment group with children with communication impairment, the treatment groups for three studies differed based on participant characteristics (e.g., different etiologies), the groups for one study differed based on treatment intensity (i.e., dosage), and the groups for the remaining five studies differed based on the treatment approach delivered.

Specific Study Question Results

For each of our study questions, we synthesized studies according to four groups based on child age due to supposed differences in intervention approaches to match the child's stage of language development as follows: 0 through 11 months (n = 1), 12 through 23 months (n = 10), 24 through 35 months (n = 23), and 36 through 48 months (n = 26). Grouping by age also helped to reduce variability in intervention types as well as caregiver and child language outcome measures to yield more specific syntheses. We determined the groupings based on the mean child age of each caregiver-implemented communication intervention group. For studies in which the mean age was not reported, we used the midpoint between the minimum and maximum ages reported. There was one study (i.e., Tannock et al., 1992) with intervention groups representing two different age groups and thus are reported in both age groups. Next, we created subgroups within each age group based on child diagnostic characteristics. The broad subgroups included developmental disorder (e.g., Down syndrome, unspecified developmental disorder), autism (i.e., inclusion of children diagnosed with autism), and language delay/disorder (i.e., specified significant weaknesses in language development).

For our final synthesis, we only included studies for which there was at least one adult communication-based outcome measure rated as providing compelling or promising evidence (see Table 3). In our synthesis, there were four communication intervention groups that served as treatment control groups and for which no evidence ratings were completed (i.e., 12 through 23 months: Fey et al., 2013; and Rogers et al., 2019; 36 through 48 months: Ingersoll et al., 2016). Table 4 contains the characteristics of studies included in the synthesis. Table 5 contains key details of child and caregiver characteristics. Tables 6 and 7 include information regarding the intervention components. Tables 3 and 8 contain the number of caregiver and child outcome measures associated with each study that were rated as compelling, promising, or lacking, categorized by communication domain, respectively. For a summary of the study, child, caregiver characteristics, intervention components, and outcomes measures for studies with no compelling or promising adult outcome measures, see Supplemental Materials S3S9.

Table 3.

Number of caregiver outcomes for each study by evidence level and outcome domain.

Study Targeted treatment (TT) and treatment comparison (TC) or other comparison (OC) study groups Evidence level and outcome domain
Compelling
Promising
Lacking
NV SLI GL IN NV SLI GL IN NV SLI GL IN
12 through 23 months
 Developmental disorder
  Fey et al., 2013 a. Milieu Communication Teaching - High Intensity (TT) 1 2
b. Milieu Communication Teaching - Low Intensity (TC*)
Follow-up (6 months) 1/1 2/2
Follow-up (15 months) 1/1 2/2
  Mahoney & Powell, 1988 a. Transactional Intervention Program (TT) 2 5
  Yoder & Warren, 2002 a. Responsivity Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching (TT) 2
b. Control Group (OC)
 Autism
  Brian et al., 2016 a. Social ABCs (TT) 2
Follow-up (3 months) 2/2
  Carter et al., 2011 a. Hanen More Than Words (TT) 1
b. Business as Usual (OC)
Follow-up (4 months) 1/1
  Stahmer et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT for Toddlers (TT) 1 10
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
Follow-up (3 months) 3/3
 Language delay/disorder
  Rajesh & Venkatesh, 2019 a. Low-Intensity Parent Training Program (TT) 3 4 1
Follow-up (2.5 months) 1/3
2/3 +
3/4
1/4 +
1/1
24 through 35 months
 Developmental disorder
  Cycyk et al., 2020 a. Language and Play Every Day en Español Intervention (TT) 1 3 1 2
  Fey et al., 2006 a. Responsivity Education/PMT – Down syndrome (TT) 1
b. Responsivity Education/PMT – Other Etiology (TC) 1
c. Responsivity Education/PMT – Entire sample (TC*)
  Iven et al., 1989 a. Pragmatic Interaction Strategies Intervention (TT) 2 2 2 2
  Pennington et al., 2009 a. It Takes Two to Talk (TT) 2 1 4 1
Follow-up (4 months) 2/2 1/1 4/4 1/1
   Romski et al., 2010 a. PI Communication Intervention – Augmented Communication (TT) 1 1 1
b. PI Communication Intervention – Spoken communication (TT)*
c. Augmented Communication Output (TT) 1 1 1
  Yang, 2016 a. Musical Bonds Program (TT) 2
 Autism
  Barrett et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Intervention for Social Motivation (TT) 1
b. Waitlist Control (OC)
  Liu & Schertz, 2021 a. Joint Attention Mediated Learning (TT) 2 1 1
b. Business as Usual (OC)
   Rogers et al., 2019 a. Enhanced Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model (TT) 1
b. Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model (TC*)
  Rollins et al., 2019 a. Pathways (TT) 1
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
 Language delay/disorder
  Baxendale et al., 2003 a. Hanen Parent Program (TT) 1 1
b. Conventional Clinic (TC)
Follow-up (6 months) 1/1 1/1
  Ciccone et al., 2012 a. Parent-Focused Early Intervention Program (TT) 1
  Falkus et al., 2016 a. Parent–child Interaction Therapy (TT) 1 1
  Gibbard & Smith, 2016 a. Enhanced Parent-Based Intervention (TT) 1
  Girolametto et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents (TT) 4 1 1
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  McDade & McCartan, 1998 a. Hanen Program (TT) 2 2
b. Control Group (OC)
  Pearce et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents (TT) 1 3 2 1 1 1
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2012 a. PI Enhanced Milieu Teaching (TT) 3 3
b. LI – business as usual (OC)
c. TD – business as usual (OC)
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2015 a. Enhanced Milieu Teaching (TT) 3 3
b. Business as Usual (OC)
  Suttora et al., 2021 a. Oltre il Libro – Dialogic Book Reading (TT) 1 2 2 1 1 1
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program (TT) 1 1 3 1
b. Wait List Control (OC)
Follow-up (4 months) - 1/1 1/1 1/1
36 through 48 months
 Developmental disorder
  Girolametto et al., 1998 a. Hanen Program (TT) 3 2
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Girolametto, 1988 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program (TT) 2 2 2
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Kaiser & Roberts, 2013 a. Parent + Therapist Enhanced Milieu Teaching (TT) 2 2 4
b. Therapist Only Enhanced Milieu Teaching (OC)
Follow-up (6 months) 2/2 2/2 4/4
Follow-up (12 months) 2/2 2/2 4/4
  Kot & Law, 1995 a. Parent Linguistic Input (TT) 1 3 1
b. Group Only – No parent training (OC)
  Weistuch et al., 1991 a. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 1 (TT) 1 2
b. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 2 (TT) 1 2
c. Control Group (OC)
 Autism
  Aldred et al., 2004 a. Social Communication Intervention (TT) 2 1
b. Routine Care (OC)
  Alquraini et al., 2018 a. Responsive Teaching (TT) 1
b. Standard Community Services (OC)
  Braiden et al., 2012 a. Forward Steps Early Intervention Program with TEACCH (TT) 4
  Casenhiser et al., 2013 a. Milton & Ethel Harris Research Initiative (TT) 1 1
b. Business as Usual/Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Green et al., 2010 a. Preschool Autism Communication Trial (TT) 2
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
  Hampton et al., 2020 a. Joint Attention, Structured Play, Engagement and Regulation plus Enhanced Milieu Teaching plus Speech-Generating Device (TT) 1 3 1
b. Business as Usual (OC)
Follow-up (4 months) 1/1 1/3
2/3
1/1 +
  Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013 a. Project ImPACT (TT) 1 3 1
   Ingersoll et al., 2016 a. Therapist-Assisted ImPACT Group (TT) 1
b. Self-Directed ImPACT Group (TC*)
Follow-up (3 months) 1/1
  Matthews et al., 2018 a. JumpStart (TT) 1
b. Waitlist Control (OC)
Follow-up (3 months) 1/1
  McGarry et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Training (TT) 1
  Minjarez et al., 2011 a. Pivotal Response Training (TT) 1
  Noyan Erbas et al., 2020 a. Hanen More Than Words (TT) 1 1 1
Follow-up (1 months) 0/1 0/1 1/1
  Sengupta et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT (TT) 5
  Venker et al., 2012 a. Hanen More Than Words (TT) 2 2
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Wainer et al., 2021 a. Stepped-care Online Reciprocal Imitation Training (TT) 2
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
 Language delay/disorder
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program (TT) 1 1 2 2
b. Wait List Control (OC)
Follow-up (4 months) - 1/1 1/1 1/1

Note. Only studies where the language intervention group had promising or compelling evidence are included in the table. NV = nonverbal; SLI = spoken language input; GL = global; IN = interactional; PI = parent implemented; PMT = prelinguistic milieu teaching; TEACHH = Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children; ImPACT = Improving Parents as Communication Teachers.

*

Denotes language intervention groups without evidence ratings included in syntheses. For studies with a follow-up, the denominator represents the total number of outcomes assessed at follow-up for a given outcome category.

+

Indicates a significant positive change at follow-up.

Indicates significant negative change at follow-up.

Table 4.

Characteristics of studies with compelling or promising evidence.

Study Country or territory Study design Number of study groups Caregiver–child language intervention name
12 through 23 months
 Developmental disorder
   Fey et al., 2013 USA RCT 2 a. Milieu Communication Teaching - High Intensity
b. Milieu Communication Teaching - Low Intensity
  Mahoney & Powell, 1988 USA SG 1 a. Transactional Intervention Program
  Yoder & Warren, 2002 USA RCT 2 a. Responsivity Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching
 Autism
  Brian et al., 2016 Canada SG 1 a. Social ABCs
  Carter et al., 2011 USA RCT 2 a. Hanen More Than Words
  Stahmer et al., 2020 USA Non-RCT 2 a. Project ImPACT for Toddlers
 Language delay/disorder
  Rajesh & Venkatesh, 2019 India SG 1 a. Low-Intensity Parent Training Program
24 through 35 months
 Developmental disorder
  Cycyk et al., 2020 USA SG 1 a. Language and Play Every Day en Español Intervention
   Fey et al., 2006 USA RCT 6 a. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching - Down syndrome
b. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching - Other Etiology
  Iven et al., 1989 USA SG 1 a. Pragmatic Interaction Strategies Intervention
  Pennington et al., 2009 Australia
UK
SG 1 a. It Takes Two to Talk
  Romski et al., 2010 USA RCT 3 a. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention - Augmented Communication Input
b. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention - Spoken communication
c. Augmented Communication Output
  Yang, 2016 USA SG 1 a. Musical Bonds Program
 Autism
  Barrett et al., 2020 USA RCT 2 a. Pivotal Response Intervention for Social Motivation
  Liu & Schertz, 2021 USA RCT 2 a. Joint Attention Mediated Learning
   Rogers et al., 2019 USA RCT 2 a. Enhanced Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model
b. Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model
  Rollins et al., 2019 USA RCT 2 a. Pathways
 Language delay/disorder
  Baxendale & Hesketh, 2003 UK Non-RCT 2 a. Hanen Parent Program
b. Conventional Clinic
  Ciccone et al., 2012 Australia SG 1 a. Parent-Focused Early Intervention Program
  Falkus et al., 2016 UK SG 1 a. Parent–child Interaction Therapy
  Gibbard & Smith, 2016 UK SG 1 a. Enhanced Parent-Based Intervention
  Girolametto et al., 1996 Canada RCT 2 a. Hanen Program for Parents
  McDade & McCartan, 1998 UK Non-RCT 2 a. Hanen Program
  Pearce et al., 1996 Canada RCT 2 a. Hanen Program for Parents
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2012 USA RCT 3 a. Parent-implemented Enhanced Milieu Teaching
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2015 USA RCT 2 a. Enhanced Milieu Teaching
  Suttora et al., 2021 Italy Non-RCT 2 a. Oltre il Libro – Dialogic Book Reading
   Tannock et al., 1992 Canada RCT 4 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program
36 through 48 months
 Developmental disorder
  Girolametto et al., 1998 Canada RCT 2 a. Hanen Program
  Girolametto, 1988 Canada RCT 2 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program
  Kaiser & Roberts, 2013 USA RCT 2 a. Parent + Therapist Enhanced Milieu Teaching
  Kot & Law, 1995 UK RCT 2 a. Parent Linguistic Input
  Weistuch et al., 1991 USA Non-RCT 3 a. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 1
b. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 2
 Autism
  Aldred et al., 2004 UK RCT 2 a. Social Communication Intervention
  Alquraini et al., 2018 Saudi Arabia RCT 2 a. Responsive Teaching
  Braiden et al., 2012 Northern Ireland SG 1 a. Forward Steps Early Intervention Program with TEACCH Approach
  Casenhiser et al., 2013 Canada RCT 2 a. Milton & Ethel Harris Research Initiative
  Green et al., 2010 UK RCT 2 a. Preschool Autism Communication Trial
  Hampton et al., 2020 USA RCT 2 a. Joint Attention, Structured Play, Engagement and Regulation plus Enhanced Milieu Teaching plus Speech-Generating Device
  Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013 USA SG 1 a. Project ImPACT
   Ingersoll et al., 2016 USA RCT 2 a. Therapist-Assisted ImPACT Group
b. Self-Directed ImPACT Group
  Matthews et al., 2018 USA Non-RCT 2 a. JumpStart
  McGarry et al., 2020 USA SG 1 a. Pivotal Response Training
  Minjarez et al., 2011 USA SG 1 a. Pivotal Response Training
  Noyan Erbas et al., 2020 Turkey SG 1 a. Hanen More Than Words
  Sengupta et al., 2020 India SG 1 a. Project ImPACT
  Venker et al., 2012 USA RCT 2 a. Hanen More Than Words
  Wainer et al., 2021 USA RCT 2 a. Stepped-care Online Reciprocal Imitation Training
 Language delay/disorder
   Tannock et al., 1992 Canada RCT 4 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program

Note.  RCT = randomized control trial; SG = single group; TEACCH = Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children; ImPACT = Improving Parents as Communication Teachers.

Table 5.

Caregiver and child characteristics.

Study Caregiver–child language intervention name Group sample size Mean child age at enrollment (months) Child characteristics Child gender Caregiver relationship Mean caregiver age (years) Family race
12 through 23 months
 Developmental disorder
   Fey et al., 2013 a. Milieu Communication Teaching - High Intensity 33 21.83 DD;
DS
M = 61%
F = 39%
Parent
b. Milieu Communication Teaching - Low Intensity 31 23 DD;
DS
M = 65%
F = 35%
Parent
  Mahoney & Powell, 1988 a. Transactional Intervention Program 41 17.6 DS;
cerebral palsy;
spina bifida;
hydrocephalus;
congenital infectious diseases;
undiagnosed etiologies
Mother;
father
Mother: 30.2
Father: 32.3
White = 100%
  Yoder & Warren, 2002 a. Responsivity Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching 19.5 21 DD;
DS;
cerebral palsy;
encephalitis
M = 56%
F = 44%
Mother;
father;
foster parent;
grandparent
Black = 48%
White = 48%
Other = 4%
 Autism
  Brian et al., 2016 a. Social ABCs 20 22.05 Autism;
at risk for autism
M = 70%
F = 30%
Parent Asian/Pacific Islander = 10%
White = 75%
East Indian = 15%
  Carter et al., 2011 a. Hanen More Than Words 32 21.11 Autism;
at risk for autism
M = 82%
F = 18%
Parent
  Stahmer et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT (Improving Parents as Communication Teachers) for Toddlers 12 23.08 DD;
autism
M = 92%
F = 8%
Parent Asian/Pacific Islander = 25%
White = 33%
Latino = 50%
Multiracial = 8%
Other = 33%
 Language delay/disorder
  Rajesh & Venkatesh, 2019 a. Low-Intensity Parent Training Program 10 18.2 Language delay M = 70%
F = 30%
Mother;
father
Average (SD)
Min-max
24.81 (11.04)
10–41
20.98 (2.05)
17.6–23.08
M = 71%
F = 29%
31.25 (1.48)
30.2–32.3
Asian/Pacific Islander = 18%
Black = 48%
East Indian = 15%
Latino = 50%
White = 64%
Multiracial = 8%
24 through 35 months
 Developmental disorder
  Cycyk et al., 2020 a. Language and Play Every Day en Español Intervention 8 35.1 DD;
language delay (confirmed or at risk)
M = 75%
F = 25%
Mother;
father;
grandparent
45.9 Latino = 100%
  Fey et al., 2006 a. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching - Down syndrome 13 25.31 DS Parent Black or Asian/Pacific Islander = 16%
b. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching - Other Etiology 12 27.17 DD Parent Black or Asian/Pacific Islander = 16%
  Iven et al., 1989 a. Pragmatic Interaction Strategies Intervention 7 27 DD:
DS;
cerebral palsy;
DHH;
language delay
Parent
  Pennington et al., 2009 a. It Takes Two to Talk 11 26 Cerebral palsy;
myopathy
M = 73%
F = 27%
Mother
   Romski et al., 2010 a. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention - Augmented Communication Input 21 29 DS;
cerebral palsy;
language delay
M = 68%
F = 32%
Mother;
father
37.33 Black = 29%
Asian/Pacific Islander = 10%
White = 62%
b. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention - Spoken communication 21 29 DS;
cerebral palsy;
language delay
M = 68%
F = 32%
Mother;
father
37.33 Black = 29%
Asian/Pacific Islander = 10%
White = 62%
c. Augmented Communication Output 20 30 DS;
cerebral palsy;
language impairment (at risk)
M = 72%
F = 32%
Mother;
father
37.33 Black = 30%
Asian/Pacific Islander = 10%
White = 60%
  Yang, 2016 a. Musical Bonds Program 26 27.96 DD;
autism;
DHH;
DLD;
motor delay
M = 77%
F = 23%
Mother 34.38 Black = 23%
White = 58%
Latino = 8%
Other = 12%
 Autism
  Barrett et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Intervention for Social Motivation 12 35.75 Autism M = 92%
F = 8%
Parent Asian/Pacific Islander = 8%
White = 75%
Latino = 17%
  Liu & Schertz, 2021 a. Joint Attention Mediated Learning 30 24.77 Autism M = 87%
F = 13%
Parent 30.6 Black = 17%
White = 70%
Latino = 3%
Multiracial = 10%
   Rogers et al., 2019 a. Enhanced Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model 21 25.2 Autism M = 71%
F = 29%
Parent Black = 14%
Asian/Pacific Islander = 5%
White = 38%
Other = 19%
Not Reported = 33%
b. Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model 24 25.2 Autism M = 67%
F = 33%
Parent Asian/Pacific Islander = 4%
White = 50%
Other = 13%
Not Reported = 33%
  Rollins et al., 2019 a. Pathways 32 28.8 Autism M = 69%
F = 31%
Parent Black = 6%
Asian/Pacific Islander = 22%
White = 22%
Latino = 50%
 Language delay/disorder
  Baxendale et al., 2003 a. Hanen Parent Program 19 31.5 Language delay M = 74%
F = 26%
Parent White = 100%
b. Conventional Clinic 18 34.3 Language delay M = 89%
F = 11%
Parent White = 100%
  Ciccone et al., 2012 a. Parent-Focused Early Intervention Program 18 33.4 Language delay M = 67%
F = 33%
Mother;
father
  Falkus et al., 2016 a. Parent–child Interaction Therapy 18 29.6 Language delay M = 83%
F = 17%
Parent
  Gibbard & Smith, 2016 a. Enhanced Parent-Based Intervention 18 30 Language delay Parent;
caregiver
  Girolametto et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents 12 28.7 Language delay Mother 35
  McDade & McCartan, 1998 a. Hanen Program 11 24 DLD Mother
  Pearce et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents 8 30.3 DLD M = 63%
F = 38%
Mother 32
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2012 a. Parent-implemented Enhanced Milieu Teaching 16 31 DD; DLD M = 88%
F = 13%
Mother; father 32.69
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2015 a. Enhanced Milieu Teaching 45 30.3 DD;
language delay
M = 82%
F = 18%
Caregiver
  Suttora et al., 2021 a. Oltre il Libro – Dialogic Book Reading 24 30.86 Language delay (confirmed or at risk) M = 58%
F = 42%
Mother;
father
Mother: 40.04
father: 42.00
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program 8 24.8 language delay (confirmed or at risk) M = 50%
F = 50%
Mother 36.9
Average (SD)
Min-max
18.19 (8.70)
7–45
29.04 (3.29)
24–35.75
M = 74%
F = 26%
36.79 (4.36)
30.6–45.9
Asian/Pacific Islander = 10%
Black = 21%
Black or Asian/Pacific Islander = 16%
Latino = 36%
White = 63%
Multiracial = 10%
Other = 15%
Not reported = 33%
36 through 48 months
 Developmental disorder
  Girolametto et al., 1998 a. Hanen Program 6 39.2 DS M = 83%
F = 17%
Mother 32
  Girolametto, 1988 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program 9 39 DD; DS M = 56%
F = 44%
Mother 34.7
  Kaiser & Roberts, 2013 a. Parent + Therapist Enhanced Milieu Teaching 39 40.05 DD;
autism;
DS
M = 74%
F = 26%
Mother;
father
36
  Kot & Law, 1995 a. Parent Linguistic Input 7 39 DD;
DLD
Parent
  Weistuch et al., 1991 a. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 1 16 42 DD;
DS
Mother
b. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 2 10 42 DD;
DS
Mother
 Autism
  Aldred et al., 2004 a. Social Communication Intervention 14 48 Autism M = 93%
F = 7%
Parent Black = 7%
White = 93%
  Alquraini et al., 2018 a. Responsive Teaching 13 42 Autism M = 85%
F = 16%
Mother 38.5
  Braiden et al., 2012 a. Forward Steps Early Intervention Program with TEACCH Approach 18 38 Autism M = 94%
F = 6%
Parent
  Casenhiser et al., 2013 a. Milton & Ethel Harris Research Initiative 25 42.52 Autism Parent
  Green et al., 2010 a. Preschool Autism Communication Trial 77 45 Autism M = 92%
F = 8%
Parent 33 White = 60%
Other = 40%
  Hampton et al., 2020 a. Joint Attention, Structured Play, Engagement and Regulation plus Enhanced Milieu Teaching plus Speech-Generating Device 34 43 Autism M = 76%
F = 24%
Mother;
father;
grandparent
White = 64%
Not reported = 36%
  Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013 a. Project ImPACT 24 44.9 Autism M = 88%
F = 13%
Parent
   Ingersoll et al., 2016 a. Therapist-Assisted ImPACT Group 14 41.57 Autism M = 79%
F = 21%
Mother
b. Self-Directed ImPACT Group 13 46.08 Autism M = 61%
F = 39%
Mother;
father
  Matthews et al., 2018 a. JumpStart 18 40.83 Autism (diagnosed or at risk) Mother;
father
34.61
  McGarry et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Training 11 36.82 Autism M = 73%
F = 27%
Mother; father Asian/Pacific Islander = 18%
White = 27%
Latino = 36%
Multiracial = 10%
Other = 10%
  Minjarez et al., 2011 a. Pivotal Response Training 17 47 Autism Parent Asian/Pacific Islander = 28%
White = 33%
Latino = 11%
Other = 28%
  Noyan Erbas et al., 2020 a. Hanen More Than Words 16 36.14 Autism Parent 33
  Sengupta et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT (Improving Parents as Communication Teachers) 57 43.2 Autism M = 84%
F = 16%
Mother 34.18
  Venker et al., 2012 a. Hanen More Than Words 7 41.14 Autism Mother;
father
  Wainer et al., 2021 a. Stepped-care Online Reciprocal Imitation Training 10 40.1 Autism M = 80%
F = 20%
Mother Asian/Pacific Islander = 10%
Black = 40%
Latino = 40%
Multiracial = 10%
 Language delay/disorder
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program 8 40.3 Language delay (confirmed or at risk) M = 53%
F = 47%
Mother 33.6
Average (SD)
Min-max
20.13 (17.21)
6–77
41.65 (3.09)
36.14–48
M = 78%
F = 22%
34.40 (1.93)
32.00–38.50
Asian/Pacific Islander = 19%
Black = 24%
Latino = 29%
White = 56%
Multiracial = 10%
Other = 26%
Not reported = 36%

Note. Only studies where the language intervention group had promising or compelling evidence are included in the table. Em dashes indicate data not reported. DD = developmental delay; DS = Down syndrome; DLD = developmental language disorder; F = female; M = male; TEACCH = Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children; ImPACT = Improving Parents as Communication Teachers.

Table 6.

Summary of intervention trainer, language, location, and dosage.

Study Caregiver–child language intervention name Intervention trainer Language of intervention Intervention location Intervention dosage
Number of sessions Session length Duration of intervention Frequency of sessions
12 through 23 months
 Developmental disorder
   Fey et al., 2013 a. Milieu Communication Teaching - High Intensity Trained paraprofessionals English Home 9 1 hr 3 months Weekly
b. Milieu Communication Teaching - Low Intensity Trained paraprofessionals English Home 9 1 hr 3 months Weekly
  Mahoney & Powell, 1988 a. Transactional Intervention Program Teacher consultant English Home 11.5 months Weekly
  Yoder & Warren, 2002 a. Responsivity Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching Researcher English Unspecified location 12
 Autism
  Brian et al., 2016 a. Social ABCs Parent educator English Home 12 1–1.5 hr 8.7 months 3/week, then taper
  Carter et al., 2011 a. Hanen More Than Words Speech-language pathologist English Home;
unspecified location
3 home; 8 group 3.5 months
  Stahmer et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT for Toddlers Trained early intervention providers English Home;
early childhood education setting
2.5 hr/week 12 weeks
 Language delay/disorder
  Rajesh & Venkatesh, 2019 a. Low-Intensity Parent Training Program English;
Tamil
Home 3 1 hr 1 week 3/week
Average (SD)
Min–max
Sessions:
9.33 (3.39)
3–12
Minutes:
81.0 (39.12)
60–150
Weeks:
19.4 (16.86)
1–50
24 through 35 months
 Developmental disorder
  Cycyk et al., 2020 a. Language and Play Every Day en Español Intervention Speech-language pathologist;
Bilingual graduate students
English;
Spanish
Home;
Community location
Group: 6;
individual: 2
1–2 hr 3 months
  Fey et al., 2006 a. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching - Down syndrome Speech-language pathologist English Home 8 1 hr 6 months
b. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching - Other Etiology Speech-language pathologist English Home 8 1 hr 6 months
  Iven et al., 1989 a. Pragmatic Interaction Strategies Intervention English 4 1 hr 4 weeks Weekly
  Pennington et al., 2009 a. It Takes Two to Talk Speech-language pathologist English Home;
unspecified location
Home: 3;
group: 7–8
Group: 2.5 hr 13 weeks
   Romski et al., 2010 a. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention - Augmented Communication Input Trained interventionist English Home;
university setting
Home: 6;
university: 18
30 min 12 weeks Home: 2/week;
University: 3/week
b. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention - Spoken communication Trained interventionist English Home;
university setting
Home: 6;
university: 18
30 min 12 weeks Home: 2/week;
University: 3/week
c. Augmented Communication Output Trained interventionist English Home;
university setting
24 30 min 12 weeks Home: 2/week;
University: 3/week
  Yang, 2016 a. Musical Bonds Program Music therapist English Home 6 40 min 6 weeks Weekly
 Autism
  Barrett et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Intervention for Social Motivation Graduate student clinician English 8 hr/week 6 months
  Liu & Schertz, 2021 a. Joint Attention Mediated Learning Intervention coordinators English Home 32 1 hr 8 months Weekly
   Rogers et al., 2019 a. Enhanced Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model Psychologist;
speech-language pathologist; behavior analysts family therapist
English Home; clinic 24 1.5 hr 12 weeks 2/week
b. Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model Psychologist;
speech-language pathologist; behavior analysts family therapist
English Clinic 12 1.5 hr 12 weeks Weekly
  Rollins et al., 2019 a. Pathways Clinician English;
Spanish
Home 11 1.5 hr Weekly
  Language delay/disorder
  Baxendale et al., 2003 a. Hanen Parent Program Speech-language pathologist English Group based: Not reported;
Home
Group: 8; home: 3 Group: 2.25 hr 11 weeks Group: Weekly
b. Conventional Clinic Speech-language pathologist English Clinic;
nursery
8–12 45 min 8–12 weeks Weekly
  Ciccone et al., 2012 a. Parent-Focused Early Intervention Program Speech-language pathologist;
trained students
English School classroom 6 1.25 hr 6 weeks Weekly
  Falkus et al., 2016 a. Parent–child Interaction Therapy Therapist English Clinic 4 1 hr 10 weeks
  Gibbard & Smith, 2016 a. Enhanced Parent-Based Intervention Speech-language pathologist;
teacher
English Home;
Early childhood education setting
20 1.5 hr 20 weeks Weekly
  Girolametto et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents speech-language pathologist;
parent associate
English Home;
unspecified location
8 group; 3 individual Group: 2.5 hr 11 weeks
  McDade & McCartan, 1998 a. Hanen Program English Home;
unspecified location
Home: 3;
other: 9
12 weeks
  Pearce et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents Speech-language pathologist (Hanen certified) English Group: 7;
individual: 3
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2012 a. Parent-implemented Enhanced Milieu Teaching Speech-language pathologist English Home;
clinic
28 1 hr 3 months 2/week
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2015 a. Enhanced Milieu Teaching Trained interventionist English Clinic;
home
Clinic: 4;
home: 24
1 hr 3 months 2/week
  Suttora et al., 2021 a. Oltre il Libro – Dialogic Book Reading Psychologist Italian 6 2 hr 2 months
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program Parent educator;
speech-language pathologist
English Home;
unspecified
Group: 9;
individual: 3
Group: 2.5 hr 12 weeks Weekly
Average (SD)
Min-max
Sessions:
14.40 (8.50)
4–32
Minutes:
78.90 (37.92)
30–150
Weeks:
13.46 (6.62)
4–32
36 through 48 months
  Developmental disorder
  Girolametto et al., 1998 a. Hanen Program Speech-language pathologist (Hanen certified) English Clinic;
home
Group: 9;
home: 4
Group: 2.5 hr 13 weeks Weekly
   Girolametto, 1988 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program Speech-language pathologist;
parent assistant
Program center; home Group: 8; individual: 3 Group: 3 hr 11 weeks
  Kaiser & Roberts, 2013 a. Parent + Therapist Enhanced Milieu Teaching Trained interventionist English Clinic;
home
Workshop: 1;
clinic: 24;
home: 12
Workshop: 2–3 hr;
Home: 20 min
3 months Clinic: 2/Week;
Home: Weekly
  Kot & Law, 1995 a. Parent Linguistic Input Speech-language pathologist;
child development worker
English 7 7 weeks Weekly
  Weistuch et al., 1991 a. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 1 Speech-language pathologist English Early childhood education setting 40 2 hr Weekly
b. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 2 Speech-language pathologist English Early childhood education setting 40 2 hr Weekly
  Autism
  Aldred et al., 2004 a. Social Communication Intervention Therapist English 9 + workshops 12 months Monthly then bi-monthly
  Alquraini et al., 2018 a. Responsive Teaching Trained interventionist Arabic Home;
center
16 1 hr 4 months Weekly
  Braiden et al., 2012 a. Forward Steps Early Intervention Program with TEACCH Approach Trained TEACCH facilitators English Home;
unspecified location
Home: 8;
other: 6
Home; 1–2 hr;
Other: 2–3 hr
10 weeks Weekly
  Casenhiser et al., 2013 a. Milton & Ethel Harris Research Initiative Speech-language pathologist;
occupational therapist
English 52 2 hr 12 months Weekly
  Green et al., 2010 a. Preschool Autism Communication Trial Speech-language pathologist English Home;
clinic
18 2 hr 1 year 6 mo: 2/week; 6 mo: monthly
  Hampton et al., 2020 a. Joint Attention, Structured Play, Engagement and Regulation plus Enhanced Milieu Teaching plus Speech-Generating Device Master-level clinician English Home;
clinic
40 40–90 min 4 months 3/Week
  Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013 a. Project ImPACT Teacher English Central location in school district Group: 6;
individual: 6
Group: 2 hr;
Individual: 45 min
12 weeks Weekly
   Ingersoll et al., 2016 a. Therapist-Assisted ImPACT Group Trained interventionist English Home Online: 12;
video conferencing: 24
Online: 1.25 hr
Video: 30 min
6 months 3/Week
b. Self-Directed ImPACT Group NA English Home 12 1.25 hr 6 months Weekly
  Matthews et al., 2018 a. JumpStart BCBA clinician;
trained interventionist
English Clinic 8 2.5 hr 4 weeks 2/Week
  McGarry et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Training English Online 6 15–30 min 9.5 weeks Weekly
  Minjarez et al., 2011 a. Pivotal Response Training Psychologist specializing in PRT English Clinic Group: 10;
individual: 1
Group: 1.5 hr;
individual: 50 min
11 weeks Weekly
  Noyan Erbas et al., 2020 a. Hanen More Than Words Researcher; Hanen therapist Turkish University setting; home Group: 8;
video: 3
  Sengupta et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT Speech-language pathologist;
occupational therapist;
psychologist; special educators
English;
Hindi
Child development center Group: 6;
individual: 6
Group: 2 hr;
individual: 1 hr
6 weeks Weekly
  Venker et al., 2012 a. Hanen More Than Words Speech-language pathologist (Hanen certified) English University setting Group: 5;
individual: 2;
small group: 14
Parent Group: 2 hr;
individual: 45 min;
Small group: 1 hr
  Wainer et al., 2021 a. Stepped-care Online Reciprocal Imitation Training Researcher English Home Self-paced: 5;
telecoaching: 5
10 weeks Weekly
 Language delay/disorder
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program Parent educator speech-language pathologist English Unspecified group location home Group: 9; individual: 3 Group: 2.5 hr; individual: Not reported 12 weeks Weekly
Average (SD)
Min-max
Sessions: 19.48 (13.65)
6–52
Minutes: 90.5 (45.83)
20–180
Weeks: 18.82 (14.38)
6–52

Note. Only studies where the language intervention group had promising or compelling evidence are included in the table. Em dashes indicate data not reported. TEACCH = Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children; ImPACT = Improving Parents as Communication Teachers.

Table 7.

Summary of delivery format, key intervention strategies taught and information provided to caregivers, and intervention contexts.

Study Caregiver–child language intervention name Intervention characteristics Delivery format Key intervention strategies taught (S)/ information provided (I) to caregivers Intervention context
12 through 23 months
 Developmental disorder
   Fey et al., 2013 a. Milieu Communication Teaching - High Intensity NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) waiting for child to act or communicate

  • (S) follow child's lead

  • (S) imitate child play acts and gestures, vocalizations, and eye contact

  • (S) comment on child's acts and objects of attention

  • (S) put the child's nonverbal communication acts into words (linguistic mapping)

  • (S) add semantically to the child's topic

  • (S) copy and add structure and meaning to the child's use of words (recasts)

Familiar activities & routines
b. Milieu Communication Teaching - Low Intensity NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) waiting for child to act or communicate

  • (S) follow child's lead

  • (S) imitate child play acts and gestures, vocalizations, and eye contact

  • (S) comment on child's acts and objects of attention

  • (S) put the child's nonverbal communication acts into words (linguistic mapping)

  • (S) add semantically to the child's topic

  • (S) copy and add structure and meaning to the child's use of words (recasts)

Familiar activities & routines
  Mahoney & Powell, 1988 a. Transactional Intervention Program Developmental One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) turn-taking strategies: waiting for child to act, imitating child's behavior, following child's lead, and occasionally elaborating on child's activity

  • (S) interactive match strategies: adjusting behavioral style so that it is similar to child's tempo or pace

  • (S) engaging in activities that are within child's current range of development

  • (S) following and responding to child's current interests

  • (S) incorporating strategies into daily routines

  Yoder & Warren, 2002 a. Responsivity Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching NDBI Mixed: In-person
  • (S) optimal responding to children's communication acts

Play routines
 Autism
  Brian et al., 2016 a. Social ABCs NDBI One-on-one: In-person;
text/emails;
phone calls
  • (S) ABC's of learning

  • (S) enhancing communication

  • (S) sharing positive emotion

  • (S) motivation and arousal

  • (S) play and the social ABC's

  • (S) daily care-giving activities

  • (S) managing behavioral challenges

  • (S) parent self-care

Familiar activities & routines; play routines
  Carter et al., 2011 a. Hanen More Than Words Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) responding to child's communicative attempts

  • (S) following child's lead

  • (S) building and participating in joint action routines in play

  • (S) enhancing interaction during caregiving routines

  • (S) using books and play as contexts for communication elicitation and reward

  • (S) using visual supports to help children understand expectations

  • (S) supporting peer interactions

  • (S) scaffolding peer play dates

Familiar activities & routines
  Stahmer et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT for Toddlers NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) limit distractions

  • (S) adjust animation

  • (S) focus on child

  • (S) comment

  • (S) imitate child

  • (S) model and adjust communication and play

  • (S) create opportunities

  • (S) help child anticipate interruptions

  • (S) use communication temptations

  • (S) wait

  • (S) respond and expand

  • (S) expands communication and play

  • (S) help child succeed

  • (S) provide related and clear prompts and adjust

  • (S) provide natural and immediate rewards

Play routines
 Language disorder/delay
  Rajesh & Venkatesh, 2019 a. Low-Intensity Parent Training Program Developmental One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) speech-language stimulation strategies in the contexts of free play and daily routines

  • speech and language milestones

  • (I) play development

Familiar activities & routines; play routines
24 through 35 months
 Developmental disorder
  Cycyk et al., 2020 a. Language and Play Every Day en Español Intervention Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) select activities that occur frequently and interest the child

  • (S) give full attention to the activity and not rush through it

  • (S) maintain the activity or routine for at least 5 min

  • (S) limit distractions to communication

  • (S) focusing on the child's interest whenever possible or acceptable

  • (S) fully participate in the activity

  • (S) put preferred item where the child can see it but cannot reach it

  • (S) create opportunity for child to request help

  • (S) offer choice of two or more objects or activities

  • (S) give small portions of preferred items

  • (S) do something silly in the routine

  • (S) add something new or unexpected to a routine

  • (S) balance turn-taking

  • (S) limit yes/no or test questions

  • (S) responding immediately to child verbal and nonverbal communication bids

  • (S) expand on child's communication attempts

Familiar activities & routines; play routines;
  Fey et al., 2006 a. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching - Down syndrome NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) recognize real or possible child communicative attempts and respond to them meaningfully

  • (S) how to provide recasts and linguistic mappings of child's communication acts

  • (S) awareness of child's developing nonintentional and intentional communication behaviors

  • (S) wait for child to produce interpretable behaviors

  • (S) attend to child's focus of attention by following the child's lead

  • (S) provide appropriate verbal and nonverbal consequences to child's acts

Familiar activities & routines
b. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching - Other Etiology NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) recognize real or possible child communicative attempts and respond to them meaningfully

  • (S) how to provide recasts and linguistic mappings of child's communication acts

  • (S) awareness of child's developing nonintentional and intentional communication behaviors

  • (S) wait for child to produce interpretable behaviors

  • (S) attend to child's focus of attention by following the child's lead

  • (S) provide appropriate verbal and nonverbal consequences to child's acts

Familiar activities & routines
  Iven et al., 1989 a. Pragmatic Interaction Strategies Intervention NDBI Group: In-person
  • (S) reference - direct child's attention to an object or event through gestures or verbalizations

  • (S) model - label items or events in the environment not contingent upon a request for information

  • (S) imitation - repeat exactly or partially child's verbal and/or nonverbal behaviors

  • (S) description - provide child with more information about items or events, including adjectives, prepositions and adverbs

  • (S) parallel talk - describe actions of self and child as the actions are occurring

  • (S) expansion - repeat child utterance while adding new words to make it more grammatically complete

Play routines
  Pennington et al., 2009 a. It Takes Two to Talk Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) strategies to facilitate child language development

  • (S) strategies to maximize child communication opportunities

Familiar activities & routines;
play routines
   Romski et al., 2010 a. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention - Augmented Communication Input NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) model augmented and spoken word use of the individualized target vocabulary via an SGD

  • (S) avoid direct communication demands of the child

  • (S) provide the child with natural reinforcers

  • (S) shorten utterance length

  • (S) give child choice between two toys or books or snacks

  • (S) pause and slow interactions so that child has a chance to communicate

Mealtimes;
play routines;
shared book reading
b. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention - Spoken communication NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) visually, verbally, and physically prompt child to produce augmented words for an individualized target set of vocabulary

  • (S) hand-over-hand prompting

  • (S) shorten utterance length

  • (S) give child choice between two toys or books or snacks

  • (S) pause and slow interactions so that child has a chance to communicate

Mealtimes;
play routines;
shared book reading
c. Augmented Communication Output NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) provide verbal and/or hand-over-hand prompts so that child produces communication using speech generating device.

  • (S) use of naturalistic routines

  • (S) use of environmental arrangement

  • (S) provide choices

  • (S) pause to “encourage” communication

Mealtimes;
play routines;
shared book reading
  Yang, 2016 a. Musical Bonds Program Sensory-based One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) facilitate parent–child interactions and synchrony during musical play

  • (S) expand musical play repertoire

  • (S) responsiveness to child: use of affect, behavioral and developmental matching, reciprocity, shared control, and contingency

Music time
 Autism
  Barrett et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Intervention for Social Motivation NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) use social activities as reinforcement

  • (S) noncontingent exposure

  • (S) high affect bids verbal prompts

  • (S) child attention

  • (S) child choice

  • (S) playful opportunity

  • (S) natural reinforcement

  • (S) contingency of reinforcement

  • (S) reinforcing attempts

Social activities
  Liu & Schertz, 2021 a. Joint Attention Mediated Learning NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S/I) mediated learning principles

  • (S) how to address child's social challenges

  • (S) how to distinguish social versus nonsocial acts

Familiar activities & routines;
play routines
   Rogers et al., 2019 a. Enhanced Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) increasing child's attention and motivation

  • (S) using sensory social routines

  • (S) promoting dyadic engagement and joint activity routines

  • (S) enhancing nonverbal communication

  • (S) building imitation skills

  • (S) facilitating joint attention

  • (S) promoting speech development

  • (S) using antecedent behavior-consequence relationships (“ABC's of learning”)

  • (S) employing prompting, shaping, and fading techniques

  • (S) conducting functional assessment of behavior to develop new interventions

Familiar activities & routines;
mealtimes;
play routines;
shared book reading
b. Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) increasing child's attention and motivation

  • (S) using sensory social routines

  • (S) promoting dyadic engagement and joint activity routines

  • (S) enhancing nonverbal communication

  • (S) building imitation skills

  • (S) facilitating joint attention

  • (S) promoting speech development

  • (S) using antecedent behavior-consequence relationships (“ABC's of learning”)

  • (S) employing prompting, shaping, and fading techniques

  • (S) conducting functional assessment of behavior to develop new interventions

Familiar activities & routines;
mealtimes;
Play routines;
shared book reading
  Rollins et al., 2019 a. Pathways NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) following child's lead, limiting of distractions, use of wait time

  • (S) face-to-face dyadic interactions and social sensory routines

  • (S) reinforcement of activities, with behaviors as reinforced immediately and consistently

  • (S) facilitation of social eye contact without aversive prompts

  • (S) use of animation

  • (S) contingent imitation of the child vocalizations, words, and gestures

  • (S) use of imitation, modeling, and expansion of language

Play routines
 Language delay/disorder
  Baxendale et al., 2003 a. Hanen Parent Program Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) strategies focused on enhancing parents' language and interaction style

Play routines
b. Conventional Clinic NDBI In-person
  • (S) modeling

  • (S) imitation

  • (S) expansion

  • (S) focusing attention through play

Play routines
  Ciccone et al., 2012 a. Parent-Focused Early Intervention Program NDBI Mixed: In-person
  • (S) following the child's lead

  • (S) encouraging participation

  • (S) language modelling strategies

  • (S) focused stimulation

  • (S) using music and using books

  Falkus et al., 2016 a. Parent–child Interaction Therapy Developmental One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) letting child choose toy

  • (S) following what child wants to do with toy

  • (S) sitting where child can easily see you

  • (S) waiting for child to start the talking with words/sounds/gestures

  • (S) giving child enough time to talk

  • (S) showing child you are listening by repeating or answering

  • (S) commenting on what child is doing

  • (S) not asking child questions

  • (S) praising child

  • (S) talking slowly so child understands

Familiar activities & routines;
play routines
  Gibbard & Smith, 2016 a. Enhanced Parent-Based Intervention Cannot be Determined Mixed: In-person
  • (S) strategies to facilitate language development through interaction, daily routines, naturally occurring situations and parenting activities

  • (I) increased awareness of role and importance in child's language development

Familiar activities & routines
  Girolametto et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) incorporate 10 target words into existing daily routines

  • (S) repeat target word at least five times during each interaction

  • (S) join in the child's play

  • (S) model targets using labels, short comments, or expansions

  • (S) set up new routines to model target words in many different contexts using different exemplars

  • (S) select additional lexical targets once the child had used the target word spontaneously at least three times in three different situations within one week

  • (S) model two-word combinations

Familiar activities & routines
  McDade & McCartan, 1998 a. Hanen Program Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) interaction-promoting strategies: taking one turn and waiting expectantly for child to respond, signaling child nonverbally to take a turn

  • (S) language modeling strategies: responsive labels, comments, expansions, extensions, parallel talk

  • (S) child-oriented strategies: follow child's lead, interpret child's cues, respond to child's focus of attention, and reduce directiveness

Familiar activities & routines
  Pearce et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) use target words in labels, comments, or expansions during naturalistic routines

  • (S) model target words in many different contexts

  • (S) use different exemplars

  • (S) express different intentions (e.g., request, comment)

Familiar activities & routines;
Play routines;
shared book reading
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2012 a. Parent-implemented Enhanced Milieu Teaching NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) use of modeling and expanding communication

  • (S) time delay strategies

  • (S) prompting strategies

Familiar activities & routines;
mealtimes;
play routines;
shared book reading
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2015 a. Enhanced Milieu Teaching NDBI Mixed: In-person
  • (S) Enhanced Milieu Teaching strategies

  • (S) promote language acquisition in everyday interactions

Familiar activities & routines
  Suttora et al., 2021 a. Oltre il Libro – Dialogic Book Reading NDBI Group: In-person
  • (S) turn-taking skills

  • (S) use of extra-verbal cues as intonation, rhythm, and gestures

  • (S) adjust speech to child's linguistic skills

  • (S) use simple sentences and redundant lexicon

  • (S) use open-ended, wh-questions

  • (S) focused stimulation on target words that are already understood but not produced by child

  • (S) expand child's verbal productions

shared book reading
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) interaction-promoting strategies: taking one turn and waiting expectantly for child to respond, signaling the child nonverbally to take a turn, avoid adult dominance of the interaction

  • (S) language-modeling strategies: responsive labels and comments, expansions, extensions, parallel talk

  • (S) child-oriented strategies: follow child's lead, interpret child's cues, respond to child's focus of attention, and reduce directiveness

36 through 48 months
 Developmental disorder
  Girolametto et al., 1998 a. Hanen Program Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) incorporating target words into existing daily routines

  • (S) using target words in response to the child's interest or activity

  • (S) join in child's play and model words using labels, short comments, or expansions

  • (S) use routines to model target words in many different contexts

  • (S) strategies to select target words

  • (S) model two-word combinations

Familiar activities & routines
  Girolametto, 1988 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) observe

  • (S) follow child's lead

  • (S) use play, games, art, books, and music

  • (S) respond contingently to child's communicative attempts

  • (S) promote turn taking

  • (S) use conversational repair signals

  • (S) provide clear linguistic models

  • (I) increase awareness of the form, content, and use of child's communication system

Play routines
  Kaiser & Roberts, 2013 a. Parent + Therapist Enhanced Milieu Teaching NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) arrange environment to set stage for adult–child interactions

  • (S) model specific language targets appropriate to child's skill level

  • (S) expand child communication forms by adding words to child utterances

  • (S) respond to child's requests with prompts for elaborated language

  • (S) functional reinforcement of child's production of prompted target forms

Familiar activities & routines;
mealtimes;
play routines;
shared book reading
  Kot & Law, 1995 a. Parent Linguistic Input Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) increase frequency and saliency of linguistic input with relevance to child's focus of attention (word-mapping opportunities)

  Weistuch et al., 1991 a. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 1 Developmental Group: In-person
  • (S) how to help child form word categories

  • (S) use words across a range of communicative intents

  • (S) build from words to content categories that govern word combinations

b. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 2 Developmental Group: In-person
  • (S) how to help child form word categories

  • (S) use words across a range of communicative intents

  • (S) build from words to content categories that govern word combinations

 Autism
  Aldred et al., 2004 a. Social Communication Intervention Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) shared attention

  • (S) sensitivity

  • (S) modelling

  • (S) consolidation

  • (S) elaboration

  Alquraini et al., 2018 a. Responsive Teaching NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) contingency

  • (S) affect

  • (S) match

Play routines
  Braiden et al., 2012 a. Forward Steps Early Intervention Program with Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH) Approach TEACCH One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) visual cues

  • (I) understanding child's autism

  Casenhiser et al., 2013 a. Milton & Ethel Harris Research Initiative Developmental In-person
  • (S) co-regulation

  • (S) expression of enjoyment of child

  • (S) sensory-motor supports

  • (S) joining

  • (S) use of affect

  • (S) support of reciprocity

  • (S) support of independent thinking

  Green et al., 2010 a. Preschool Autism Communication Trial Developmental One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) increase sensitivity and responsiveness to child communication

  • (S) reduce mistimed parental responses

  • (S) use action routines

  • (S) use familiar repetitive language

  • (S) pause

  Hampton et al., 2020 a. Joint Attention, Structured Play, Engagement and Regulation plus Enhanced Milieu Teaching plus Speech-Generating Device NDBI One-on-one: In-person
  • (S) matched turns

  • (S) target-level language

  • (S) expansions

  • (S) time delays

  • (S) milieu prompting

  • (S) engagement and play strategies

  • (S) using joint attention and gestures

Familiar activities & routines;
mealtimes;
play routines;
shared book reading
  Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013 a. Project ImPACT NDBI Mixed: In-person
  • (S) use a blend of developmental and naturalistic behavioral intervention strategies

  • (S) promote child's social engagement

  • (S) promote language

  • (S) use imitation

  • (S) use strategies in play and daily routines

Familiar activities & routines
   Ingersoll et al., 2016 a. Therapist-Assisted ImPACT Group NDBI One-on-one: Self-paced tutorials
  • (S) targets social communication using a naturalistic, developmental-behavioral intervention

b. Self-Directed ImPACT Group NDBI One-on-one: Self-paced tutorials
  • (S) targets social communication using a naturalistic, developmental-behavioral intervention

  Matthews et al., 2018 a. JumpStart Behavioral Mixed: In-person
  • (S/I) applied behavior analysis

  • (S/I) pivotal response treatment

  • (S/I) disruptive behaviors

  • (S/I) toilet training

  • What is autism?

  • accessing services

  • (I) Individualized Education Plans

  McGarry et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Training NDBI One-on-one: Self-paced tutorials
  • (S) set up a learning opportunity PRT strategies: child choice/shared control, clear learning opportunities, interspersing maintenance and acquisition tasks

  • (S) respond to child's language PRT strategies: natural reinforcers, reinforcing attempts, immediate/contingent reinforcement

  • (S) choose a treatment plan identifying a developmentally appropriate target skill

  • (S) use model, open-ended, and time-delay prompts

  • (S) using social reinforcers Incorporating child interests within social activities

Familiar activities & routines;
play routines
  Minjarez et al., 2011 a. Pivotal Response Training NDBI Mixed: In-person
  • (S/I) Pivotal Response Training

Play routines
  Noyan Erbas et al., 2020 a. Hanen More Than Words Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) follow child's lead

  • (S) strategies to keep interactions going

  • (S) observe, wait, and listen to child

  • (S) how to help child learn daily routines

  • (S) how to use strategies to teach toy play

  • (S) how to help child with peer interactions

  • (S) how to use books for interaction

  • (S) how to transfer newly learned interaction skills to practical applications with child in daily environment

Familiar activities & routines;
play routines
  Sengupta et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT NDBI Mixed: In-person; texts & emails
  • (S) follow child's lead

  • (S) face to face

  • (S) imitate child

  • (S) join in child's play and make interactive

  • (S) use animation

  • (S) model and expand child's language

  • (S) use communicative temptations to create opportunities for communication and increase social

  • engagement

  • (S) prompts and reinforcement to increase complexity of child's response

  • (S) taking turns during play

  • (I) stages of play

Familiar activities & routines
  Venker et al., 2012 a. Hanen More Than Words Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) be verbally responsive by talking about child's play (follow-in commenting)

  • (S) respond to child's communication acts using linguistic mapping and expansions

  • (S) use of strategies to engage child in play: establishing play routines and supporting object-focused engagement

  • (S) prompt communication: arrange environment with favored items in sight but out of reach, provide choices, give pieces of toys bit by bit, and insert pauses during familiar verbal routines

  • (S) strategies to establish interactive “people games” that provide child with predictable social turn-taking routines

  • (S) decrease redirects of child's attention away from a toy of interest

Play routines
  Wainer et al., 2021 a. Stepped-care Online Reciprocal Imitation Training NDBI One-on-one: Self-paced tutorial; telehealth/video conferencing coaching
  • (S) select activities and antecedent controls

  • (S) schedule practice time

  • (S) ensure a support system

  • (S) imitate child

  • (S) describing play using linguistic mapping, simple and descriptive language at or slightly above child's linguistic level

  • (S) use modeling, prompting, and reinforcement to teach a target skill, and pace the interaction

Play routines
 Language delay/disorder
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program Developmental Mixed: In-person
  • (S) interaction-promoting strategies: taking one turn and waiting expectantly for child to respond, signaling the child nonverbally to take a turn, avoid adult dominance of the interaction

  • (S) language-modeling strategies: responsive labels and comments, expansions, extensions, parallel talk

  • (S) child-oriented strategies: follow child's lead, interpret child's cues, respond to child's focus of attention, and reduce directiveness

Note. Only studies where the language intervention group had promising or compelling evidence are included in the table. Em dashes indicate data not reported. NDBI = naturalistic developmental behavioral intervention; TEACCH = Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication Handicapped Children; ImPACT = Improving Parents as Communication Teachers.

Table 8.

Number of child outcomes for each study by evidence level and outcome domain.

Study Targeted treatment (TT) and treatment comparison (TC) or other comparison (OC) study groups Evidence level and outcome domain
Compelling
Promising
Lacking
EX RE S/P GL EX RE S/P GL EX RE S/P GL
12 through 23 months
 Developmental disorder
   Fey et al., 2013 a. Milieu Communication Teaching – High Intensity (TT) 6 1
b. Milieu Communication Teaching – Low Intensity (TC*)
Follow-up (6 months) 6/6 1/1
Follow-up (15 months) 6/6 1/1
  Yoder & Warren, 2002 a. Responsivity Education and Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching (TT) 7
b. Control Group (OC)
 Autism
  Brian et al., 2016 a. Social ABCs (TT) 4 1 1 1
Follow-up (3 months) 4/4 1/1 1/1 1/1
  Carter et al., 2011 a. Hanen More Than Words (TT) 3 1
b. Business as Usual (OC)
Follow-up (4 months) 3/3 1/1
  Stahmer et al. 2020 a. Project ImPACT for Toddlers (TT) 3 1 2 2 1 1
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
Follow-up (3 months) 3/3 1/1 2/2 2/2 1/1 + 1/1
24 through 35 months
 Developmental disorder
  Cycyk et al., 2020 a. Language and Play Every Day en Español Intervention (TT) 3 1 1
  Fey et al., 2006 a. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching – Down syndrome (TT) 1 4 1
b. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching – Other Etiology (TC*) 3 3
c. Responsivity Education/Prelinguistic Milieu Teaching – Entire sample (TC)
  Pennington et al., 2009 a. It Takes Two to Talk (TT) 4 1 1
Follow-up (4 months) 4/4 1/1 1/1
   Romski et al., 2010 a. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention – Augmented Communication Input (TT) 4 2 4
b. Parent-Implemented Communication Intervention – Spoken communication (TT)*
c. Augmented Communication Output (TT) 4 2 4
  Yang, 2016 a. Musical Bonds Program (TT) 1
 Autism
  Barrett et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Intervention for Social Motivation (TT) 2 1 1
b. Waitlist Control (OC)
   Rogers et al., 2019 a. Enhanced Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model (TT) 3
b. Parent-Implemented Early Start Denver Model (TC*)
 Language delay/disorder
  Baxendale et al., 2003 a. Hanen Parent Program (TT) 1 1
b. Conventional Clinic (TC)
Follow-up (6 months) 1/1 1/1
  Ciccone et al., 2012 a. Parent-Focused Early Intervention Program (TT) 1 1 1 1
  Falkus et al., 2016 a. Parent–child Interaction Therapy (TT) 1
  Gibbard & Smith, 2016 a. Enhanced Parent-Based Intervention (TT) 1
  Girolametto et al., 1996 a. Hanen Program for Parents (TT) 4 8
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  McDade & McCartan, 1998 a. Hanen Program (TT) 1 1 1 1
b. Control Group (OC)
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2012 a. Parent-implemented Enhanced Milieu Teaching (TT) 4 1 1
b. LI – business as usual (OC)
c. TD – business as usual (OC)
  Roberts & Kaiser, 2015 a. Enhanced Milieu Teaching (TT) 1 2 3
b. Business as Usual (OC)
  Suttora et al., 2021 a. Oltre il Libro – Dialogic Book Reading (TT) 3
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program (TT) 1 2 1 6
b. Wait List Control (OC)
Follow-up (4 months) 1/1 2/2 1/1 + 5/6
1/6 +
36 through 48 months
 Developmental disorder
  Girolametto et al., 1998 a. Hanen Program (TT) 2 2
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Girolametto, 1998 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program (TT) 5 2
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Kaiser & Roberts, 2013 a. Parent + Therapist Enhanced Milieu Teaching (TT) 3 4 7 2
b. Therapist Only Enhanced Milieu Teaching (OC)
Follow-up (6 months) 3/3 2/4
2/4 +
6/7
1/7 +
2/2
Follow-up (12 months) 3/3 2/4
2/4 +
6/7
1/7 +
2/2
   Kot & Law, 1995 a. Parent Linguistic Input (TT) 1 4 1
b. Group Only – No parent training (OC)
   Weistuch et al., 1991 a. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 1 (TT) 1
b. Language Interaction Intervention Program – Site 2 (TT) 1
c. Control Group (OC)
 Autism
  Aldred et al., 2004 a. Social Communication Intervention (TT) 2 1
b. Routine Care (OC)
  Alquraini et al., 2018 a. Responsive Teaching (TT) 1 1
b. Standard Community Services (OC)
  Braiden et al., 2012 a. Forward Steps Early Intervention Program with TEACCH (TT) 2 2
  Casenhiser et al., 2013 a. Milton & Ethel Harris Research Initiative (TT) 1 1
b. Business as Usual/Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Green et al., 2010 a. Preschool Autism Communication Trial (TT) 2 1 3 1 1 1 1
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
  Hampton et al., 2020 a. Joint Attention, Structured Play, Engagement and Regulation plus Enhanced Milieu Teaching plus Speech-Generating Device (TT) 1 3 1
b. Business as Usual (OC)
Follow-up (4 months) 0/1 2/3
1/3 +
1/1
  Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013 a. Project ImPACT (TT) 2 5 2 1
   Ingersoll et al., 2016 a. Therapist-Assisted ImPACT Group (TT) 1 2 1
b. Self-Directed ImPACT Group (TC)
Follow-up (3 months) 1/1 2/2 1/1
  Matthews et al., 2018 a. JumpStart (TT) 1
b. Waitlist Control (OC)
Follow-up (3 months) 1/1
  McGarry et al., 2020 a. Pivotal Response Training (TT) 1 1
  Minjarez et al., 2011 a. Pivotal Response Training (TT) 1
  Noyan Erbas et al., 2020 a. Hanen More Than Words (TT) 2 1 2 1
Follow-up (4 weeks) 2/2 1/1 2/2 1/1 +
  Sengupta et al., 2020 a. Project ImPACT (Improving Parents as Communication Teachers; TT) 1 1 4
  Venker et al., 2012 a. Hanen More Than Words (TT) 1 1 1
b. Delayed Treatment (OC)
  Wainer et al., 2021 a. Stepped-care Online Reciprocal Imitation Training (TT) 2
b. Treatment as Usual (OC)
 Language delay/disorder
   Tannock et al., 1992 a. Hanen Early Language Parent Program (TT) 1 8 1
b. Wait List Control (OC)
Follow-up (4 months) 7/8
1/8 +
1/1 +

Note. Only studies where the language intervention group had promising or compelling evidence are included in the table. EX = expressive; RE = receptive; S/P = social/pragmatic; GL = global (receptive & expressive); LI = language impairment; TD = typical development; ImPACT = Improving Parents as Communication Teachers.

*

Denotes language intervention groups without evidence ratings included in syntheses.

+

Indicates a significant positive change in outcome between posttest and follow-up analyses.

Zero Through 11 Months

There were no studies with children in the 0- through 11-month-old age range identified in our review that yielded an adult language outcome measure with promising or compelling evidence. The only study identified with children under 12 months of age (Barrera et al., 1987) lacked any compelling or promising adult communication outcomes measures to support a caregiver-implemented communication intervention. The Barrera study was a non-RCT evaluating a developmental Home Responsiveness Intervention for children with Down syndrome (see Supplemental Materials S3S7 for further information on the Barrera et al. [1987] study).

Twelve Through 23 Months

Study, Child, and Caregiver Characteristics

There were 10 studies that yielded 12 caregiver-implemented communication intervention groups for children 12 through 23 months identified as having communication impairment. Of the 10 studies, three did not report outcomes with compelling or promising evidence, yielding seven studies for our synthesis comprising eight intervention groups. The three excluded studies included children with language delay or disorder; two were RCTs and one was a non-RCT; and two evaluated developmental interventions and two evaluated NDBIs. Of the seven studies included, one included two intervention groups, yielding a total of eight intervention groups. The included studies were published from 1988 through 2020 and were conducted in three different countries or territories, including the United States (n = 5), Canada (n = 1), and India (n = 1). Three studies were RCTs, two were a non-RCTs, and two were single-group studies. See Table 4.

The number of children in the intervention groups ranged from 10 to 41 with an average of 24.81 (SD = 11.04) children per group. The average age of the children in the intervention group was 20.98 months (SD = 2.05; min–max: 17.6–23.08). Four groups comprised children with developmental disorder, three comprised children on the autism spectrum, and one comprised children language delay or disorder. For seven groups, child gender was reported with males and females comprising 71% and 29% of the groups, respectively.

Caregivers typically included an unspecified parent (n = 5). For two study groups, it was specified that the caregivers included both mothers and fathers, and for one group, the caregivers included foster parents and grandparents in addition to mothers and fathers. Caregiver age was reported for one group (mother: 30.2 years; father: 32.3 years). Four studies reported the race of their participant groups (n = 4 groups reporting), with, on average, 64% (SD = 29.63) of participants within these groups being identified as White (n = 4 groups reporting), 48% as Black (n = 1 group reporting), 15% as East Asian (n = 1 group reporting), 50% as Latino (n = 1 group reporting), and 12% (SD = 8.10) as Asian or Pacific Islander (n = 7 groups reporting). See Table 5.

Intervention Components

Three of the interventions were identified as being variations of milieu teaching, all of which were in the developmental delay subgroup. The other interventions delivered had unique names. The person who trained caregivers to implement the intervention varied across studies including the following: trained paraprofessionals (n = 2 groups), speech-language pathologists (n = 1 group), trained early intervention providers (n = 1 group), teacher consultants (n = 1 group), researchers (n = 1 group), and parent educators (n = 1 group). For all groups, the intervention was delivered in English (n = 8 groups). For the one study (n = 1 group), in the language delay/disorder subgroup, the intervention language was also delivered in Tamil. For six groups, the intervention was delivered in family homes. For two of these groups, intervention was also delivered in an early childhood education setting (n = 1 group) and an unspecified location (n = 1 group). For one group, no location was specified. See Table 6.

The number of intervention sessions ranged from three to 12 with an average of 9.33 sessions (SD = 3.39), with two groups unreported. Session lengths ranged from 1 to 2.5 hr with an average length of 81.0 min (SD = 39.12), with three groups unreported. The most common session length was 1 hr (n = 4 groups). The duration of the intervention ranged from 1 to 50 weeks with an average duration of 19.4 weeks (SD = 16.86). Duration was not reported for one group. Frequency of sessions was reported for five groups, with three groups receiving weekly sessions and two receiving sessions 3 times per week. See Table 6.

The most common delivery format included one-on-one, in-person (n = 6 groups). For one of these groups, the intervention was also delivered via phone calls and texts/e-mails. For two groups, the format was a mix of one-on-one and group, in-person. We characterized five of the interventions as NDBIs and three as developmental. The delivery formats and intervention characterizations were mixed across the diagnostic subgroups. All but one of the intervention groups reported teaching strategies to caregivers. One study shared both strategies and information. The depth of details regarding specific intervention strategies taught to caregivers varied across studies, ranging from broad “speech-language stimulation strategies” to specific strategies such as “following child's lead” and “comment on child's acts and objects of attention.” Information shared included speech and language milestones, and information on play development. No studies only shared information with caregivers. The contexts in which caregivers were taught to use the strategies were specified for seven of the eight groups. These contexts included familiar activities and routines (n = 5 groups) and play routines (n = 5 groups). See Table 7.

Caregiver and Child Outcome Measures

Caregiver outcomes. Across the seven studies and eight communication intervention groups, there were a total of 34 caregiver outcomes (note that for one study group evidence levels were not coded; see Table 3). There were 4.25 caregiver outcomes per group (min–max: 1–11). There were no outcome measures of nonverbal communicative behaviors (e.g., use of gestures or sign language) within this age group.

Of the three studies in the developmental disorder subgroup, none included a measure of spoken language input, whereas two included global communicative measures, yielding a total of three outcomes (compelling = 1; promising = 2). All three studies included measures based on caregiver child interactions, yielding a total of nine outcomes (compelling = 4; promising = 5). Only one study within this group included follow-up assessments, which provided evidence that caregiver communicative outcomes maintained 6 and 12 months postintervention.

Within the autism subgroup, none of the three studies included spoken language input outcomes, while two included global communicative measures, yielding a total of three outcomes (compelling = 1; promising = 2). Measures based on caregiver child interactions were assessed in two studies, yielding a total of 11 outcomes (compelling = 10; promising = 1). All three studies in the autism subgroup included follow-up assessments, with results suggesting that caregiver communicative outcomes maintained postintervention.

For the one study within this age range that was represented in the language delay/disorder subgroup, seven of the outcomes were spoken language input outcomes (promising = 3; lacking = 4) and one outcome was based on caregiver–child interactions, which provided lacking evidence. This study also included a follow-up assessment with results suggesting that caregiver communicative outcomes were maintained postintervention.

Child outcomes. Of the seven studies that had caregiver outcomes with compelling or promising evidence, five also included child outcome measures (see Table 8). There were seven communication intervention groups that yielded a total 34 outcomes with a group average of 4.86 (min–max: 3–10).

There were two studies represented in the developmental disorder subgroup, both studies included measures of expressive language, yielding a total of 13 outcomes all with lacking evidence. One study included one measure of receptive language, also providing lacking evidence. Neither study included social/pragmatic or global measures of language. One of these studies included follow-up assessments with results indicating that child communication outcomes were maintained postintervention.

All three of the studies in the autism subgroup included expressive language measures, yielding a total of 12 outcomes (compelling = 3; promising = 4; lacking = 5). Two studies included receptive language outcomes, yielding a total of three outcomes (compelling = 1; promising = 1; lacking = 1). Two studies included social/pragmatic outcomes, yielding a total of four outcomes (compelling = 3; lacking = 1). Global measures of language were only assessed in one study, yielding a total of two outcomes, both with compelling evidence. All three studies included a follow-up time point with results suggesting that child communicative outcomes were maintained postintervention.

Twenty-Four Through 35 Months

Study, Child, and Caregiver Characteristics

There were 23 studies that yielded 28 intervention groups for children 24 through 35 months identified as having communication impairment. Of the 23 studies, two did not report outcomes with compelling or promising evidence, yielding 21 studies for our synthesis comprising 26 caregiver communication intervention groups. The two excluded studies included children with language delay or disorder; one was an RCT and one was a non-RCT; and one evaluated a developmental interventions and one evaluated an NDBI. Of the 21 studies included, three included two intervention groups and one included three intervention groups. These studies were published from 1989 through 2021 and were conducted in five different countries or territories, including the United States (n = 11), the United Kingdom (n = 5), Canada (n = 3), Australia (n = 2), and Italy (n = 1). Most studies were RCTs (n = 11), followed by single-group studies (n = 7) and non-RCTs (n = 3). Study characteristics varied within the diagnostic subgroups, with the exception that all four of the studies within the autism subgroup were RCTs conducted in the United States. See Table 4.

The number of children in the intervention groups ranged from seven to 45 with an average of 18.19 (SD = 8.70) children per group. The average age of the children in the intervention group was 29.04 months (SD = 3.29; min–max: 24–35.75). Twelve of the intervention groups comprised children with language delay or disorder, nine comprised children with developmental disorder, and five comprised children on the autism spectrum. For 20 groups, child gender was reported with males and females comprising 74% and 26% of the groups, respectively. See Table 5.

Caregivers typically included an unspecified parent or caregiver (n = 13). For seven study groups, it was specified that the caregivers included both mothers and fathers, one of which also included grandparents. Six study groups only included mothers, whereas there were no study groups with only fathers. For 11 groups, caregiver age was reported with a mean of 36.79 years (SD = 4.36; min–max: 30.6–45.9). Nine studies reported the race of their participant groups (n = 14 groups reporting), with, on average, 63% (SD = 23.32) of participants within these groups being identified as White (n = 11 groups reporting), 36% (SD = 40.39) as Latino (n = 5 groups reporting), 21% (SD = 9.15) as Black (n = 7 groups reporting), 16% (SD = 0) as Black or Asian or Pacific Islander (n = 2 groups reporting), 10% (SD = 5.90) as Asian or Pacific Islander (n = 7 groups reporting), and 10% as multiracial (n = 1 group reporting). See Table 5.

Intervention Components

Common interventions delivered included variations of the Hanen Program (n = 6 groups), with five groups in the language delay/disorder subgroup and one group in the developmental disorder subgroup receiving such, and milieu teaching (n = 4 groups), with two groups in the developmental delay subgroup and two groups in the language delay/disorder subgroup receiving such. Most frequently, the person who trained caregivers to implement the intervention was a speech-language pathologist (n = 14 groups). Other trainers included unspecified trainers/educators (n = 7 groups), psychologists (n = 3 groups), students (n = 3 groups), trained parents (n = 2 groups), behavior analysts family therapists (n = 2 groups), and teachers (n = 1 group). For the majority of the groups, the intervention was delivered in English only (n = 23 groups). For two studies (n = 2 groups), the intervention was delivered in both English and Spanish, and for one study (n = 1 group), the intervention language was Italian. For 18 groups, the intervention was delivered in family homes. For four of these groups, intervention was delivered in a second location that was not specified. Other locations included clinics (n = 6 groups), university setting (n = 3 groups), early childhood setting (n = 3 groups), and an unspecified community location (n = 1 group). For four groups, the location was not reported. See Table 6.

The number of intervention sessions ranged from four to 32 with an average of 14.40 sessions (SD = 8.50). Number of sessions was not reported for one group. Session length ranged from 30 min to 2.5 hr with an average length of 78.90 min (SD = 37.92). The most common session length was 1 hr (n = 7 groups). Session length was not reported for two groups. The duration of the intervention ranged from 4 weeks to 8 months with an average duration of 13.46 weeks (SD = 6.62). The most common intervention durations were 3 months (n = 9 groups) and 6 months (n = 3 groups). Duration was not reported for two groups. Frequency of sessions was reported for 19 groups, with nine groups receiving weekly sessions and 10 receiving sessions 2–3 times per week. See Table 6.

We characterized 16 of the interventions as NDBIs, eight as developmental, and one as sensory-based. All of the interventions in the autism subgroup were NDBIs, whereas the other subgroups included a mix of intervention types. For one group, there was not enough information available to reliably characterize the intervention. The most common delivery formats included one-on-one, in-person (n = 13 groups), and mix of one-on-one and group, in-person (n = 10 groups). Two groups used an only group, in-person format, and for one group, it was only noted that sessions were in-person (i.e., individual vs. group was not reported). For all of the autism groups, interventions were delivered one-on-one, in-person. The depth of details regarding specific intervention strategies taught to caregivers varied across studies, ranging from broad “strategies to facilitate child language development” to specific strategies such as “following child's lead” and “use of imitation, modeling, and expansion of language.” Only one study, which was in the language delay/disorder subgroup indicated that a key component of the intervention was to share information with caregivers: “increased awareness of role and importance in child's language development.” No studies only shared information with caregivers. The contexts that caregivers were taught in which to use the strategies were specified for all but two groups. These contexts included play routines (n = 15 groups), familiar activities and routines (n = 14 groups), shared book reading (n = 8 groups), mealtimes (n = 6 groups), music time (n = 1 group), and social activities (n = 1 group). See Table 7.

Caregiver and Child Outcome Measures

Caregiver outcomes. Across the 21 studies and 26 communication intervention groups, there were a total of 91 caregiver outcomes (note that for three study groups, evidence levels were not coded; see Table 3). There was an average of 3.37 caregiver outcomes per group (min–max: 1–9). There were no outcome measures of nonverbal communicative behaviors (e.g., use of gestures or sign language) within this age group.

Within the developmental disorder subgroup, three studies included measures of spoken language input, yielding a total of 14 outcomes (promising = 6; lacking = 8). Two of these studies included global communicative outcomes, yielding a total of four outcomes (promising = 1; lacking = 3). All six studies within the developmental disorder subgroup included measures based on caregiver–child interactions, yielding a total of 15 outcomes (compelling = 3; promising = 6; lacking = 6). One of these studies included a follow-up assessment, with results suggesting that caregiver communicative outcomes were maintained postintervention.

None of the four studies in the autism subgroup included spoken language input outcomes. Of these four studies, two included global communicative measures, yielding a total of two outcomes (promising = 1; lacking = 1). All four studies included measures based on caregiver–child interactions, yielding a total of five outcomes (compelling = 3; promising = 1; lacking = 1).

Of the 11 studies in the language delay/disorder subgroup, five included measures of spoken language input, yielding a total of 18 outcomes (compelling = 7; promising = 6; lacking = 5). Six of these studies included global communicative outcomes, yielding a total of 13 outcomes (compelling = 10; promising = 2; lacking = 1). Eight of these included measures based on caregiver–child interactions yielding a total of 20 outcomes (compelling = 13; promising = 5; lacking = 2). There were two studies with follow-up assessments within the language delay/disorder subgroup, both indicating that for the caregiver communicative outcomes assessed, results maintained postintervention.

Child outcomes. Of the 21 studies that had caregiver outcomes with compelling or promising evidence, 17 also included child outcome measures (see Table 8). There were 23 communication intervention groups that yielded 100 child communication-focused outcomes with a group average of 4.35 measures (min–max: 1–12).

All five of the studies in the developmental disorder subgroup included expressive language outcomes, yielding a total of 42 outcomes (compelling = 9; promising = 19; lacking =14). Only one study included one receptive language measure that provided promising evidence. One study included one social/pragmatic outcome that yielded promising evidence. None of the five studies included any global measures of language. One of these studies included follow-up assessments, with results suggesting that child communicative outcomes maintained postintervention.

Of the two studies in the autism group, one included three measures of expressive language (compelling = 2; lacking = 1). Both studies included global measures of language, yielding a total of four outcomes (compelling = 1; lacking = 3).

Each of the 10 studies in the language delay/disorder subgroup included measures of expressive language, yielding a total of 38 outcomes (compelling = 12; promising = 13; lacking =13). Five studies included measures of receptive language, yielding a total of six outcomes (compelling = 3; promising = 1; lacking = 2). One study included one social/pragmatic outcome that provided promising evidence. Four studies included a global measure of language, yielding a total of four outcomes (compelling = 1; promising = 2; lacking = 1). Of studies within the language delay/disorder subgroup, two studies included follow-up assessments, with results suggesting that child communicative outcomes were maintained postintervention.

Thirty-Six Through 48 Months

Study, Child, and Caregiver Characteristics

There were 26 studies that yielded 28 intervention groups for children 36 through 48 months identified as having communication impairment. Of the 26 studies, five did not report outcomes with compelling or promising evidence, yielding 21 studies for our synthesis comprising 23 caregiver communication intervention groups. A total of 19 studies included one such group and two included two groups. The excluded studies comprised four studies in the autism subgroup and one in the language delay/disorder subgroup, all of which were conducted in the United States with the exception of one study in the autism subgroup conducted in the United Kingdom. The studies were published from 1988 through 2021, 15 of which were published in 2010 or later. The studies were conducted in nine different countries or territories, including the United States (n = 10), Canada (n = 4), the United Kingdom (n = 3), India (n = 1), Northern Ireland (n = 1), Saudi Arabia (n = 1), and Turkey (n = 1). Most studies were RCTs (n = 13), followed by single-group studies (n = 6), and non-RCT (n = 2). See Table 5.

The number of children in the intervention groups ranged from six to 77 with an average of 20.13 (SD = 17.21) children per group. The average age of the children in the intervention group was 41.65 months (SD = 3.09; min–max: 36.14–48). Six of the intervention groups comprised children with developmental disorder, 16 comprised children on the autism spectrum, and one comprised children with language delay or disorder. For 15 of the 21 groups, child gender was reported with males and females comprising 78% and 22% of the groups, respectively. See Table 6.

Caregivers typically included an unspecified parent (n = 8) or both mothers and fathers (n = 6; plus grandparents for one group). Another nine study groups only included mothers, whereas there were no study groups with only fathers. For nine groups, caregiver age was reported with a mean of 34.40 years (SD = 1.93; min–max: 32–38.5). Six studies reported the race of their participant groups, with, on average, 55% (SD = 26.54) of participants within these groups being identified as White (n = 5 groups reporting), 29% (SD = 15.72) as Latino, 24% (SD = 23.33) as Black (n = 2 groups reporting), and 19% (SD = 9.02) as Asian or Pacific Islander (n = 3 groups reporting). See Table 6.

Intervention Components

Common interventions across studies included variations of the Hanen Program (n = 5 groups), Project ImPACT (Improving Parents as Communication Teachers; n = 4 groups), and Pivotal Response Training (n = 2 groups). Most frequently, the person who trained caregivers to implement the intervention was a speech-language pathologist (n = 10 groups), including for four of the five groups in the developmental delay subgroup. Other trainers included unspecified trainers/educators (n = 8 groups), parent educators (n = 2 groups), teachers (n = 1 group), psychologists (n = 2 group), occupational therapists (n = 2 groups), researchers (n = 2 groups), BCBA clinicians (n = 1 group), child development workers (n = 1 group), and special educators (n = 1 group). For 19 of the 23 groups, the intervention was delivered in only English. Other delivery languages included English with Hindi, Arabic, and Turkish (n = 1 group each). For one group, the language was not specified. For 12 groups, the intervention was delivered at least partially in homes; for six groups, the intervention was delivered in a clinic. Other locations included schools (n = 6 groups), university setting (n = 2 groups), unspecified community location (n = 2 groups), and online (n = 1 group). For three groups, the location was not specified. See Table 7.

The number of intervention sessions ranged from six to 52 with an average of 19.48 sessions (SD = 13.65). Session length ranged from 20 min to 3 hr, with an average length of 1.50 hr (SD = 0.76). The most common session lengths were 1 (n = 4 groups) and 2 hr (n = 5 groups). Session length was not reported for four groups. The intervention duration ranged from 1.5 months to 1 year with an average of 17.71 weeks (SD = 13.05). The most common intervention duration was 3 months (n = 3 groups). Duration was not reported for four groups. Frequency of sessions was reported for all but three groups, with 14 groups receiving weekly sessions, three receiving sessions 2–3 times per week, and three having mixed schedules. See Table 7.

We characterized 10 of the interventions as NDBIs, 11 as developmental, one as behavioral, and one as TEACHH. Five of the six interventions in the developmental delay subgroup were developmental. The most common intervention format was a mix of one-on-one and group, in-person (11 groups). Other formats included one-on-one, in-person (n = 5 groups); group, in-person (n = 2 groups); one-on-one self-paced tutorials (n = 4 groups). For one group in addition to one-on-one self-paced tutorials, telehealth video coaching was used. For one group, it was not specified if the in-person format was group or one-on-one. The specific intervention strategies taught and information provided to caregivers varied across studies. For some studies, strategies were broadly described using descriptors such as “Pivotal Response Training.” All study interventions included teaching caregiver specific strategies, such as “arrange environment with favored items in sight but out of reach, provide choices, give pieces of toys bit by bit, and insert pauses during familiar verbal routines” and “how to use books for interaction.” Five studies specified that they provided caregivers with information. Examples of information provided to caregivers include “understanding child's autism” and “Individualized Education Plans.” The contexts caregivers were taught in which to use the strategies included play routines (n = 9 groups), familiar activities and routines (n = 7 groups), shared book reading (n = 2 groups), and mealtimes (n = 2 groups). For 11 groups, the contexts were not specified. See Table 3.

Caregiver and Child Outcome Measures

Caregiver outcomes. For the 36- through 48-month group, across the 21 studies and 23 communication intervention groups with compelling or promising evidence, there were a total of 76 caregiver outcomes (see Table 3). There was an average of 3.3 caregiver outcomes per group (min–max: 1–8). There were no outcome measures of nonverbal communicative behaviors within this age group.

All five of the studies in the developmental delay subgroup included measures of spoken language input, yielding a total of 17 outcomes (compelling = 2; promising = 8; lacking = 7). One study included two outcomes based on global communicative measures, both yielding compelling evidence. Four of these studies included outcomes based on caregiver–child interactions, yielding a total of 11 outcomes (compelling = 4; promising = 2; lacking = 5). Only one study within this group included follow-up assessments; results suggested that caregiver communicative outcomes maintained postintervention.

Of the 15 studies included in the autism subgroup, only one included a spoken language input outcome, which yielded promising evidence. Twelve studies included measures of global communicative outcomes, yielding a total of 20 outcomes (compelling = 7; promising 11; lacking = 2). Nine studies included measures based on caregiver–child interactions, yielding a total of 19 outcomes (compelling = 6; promising = 12; lacking = 1). There were four studies that included follow-up assessments within the autism subgroup. Results suggest just over half of the caregiver communicative outcomes were maintained during the follow-up periods.

There was only one study within this age range that included children with a language delay/disorder. This study included one spoken language outcome and one outcome based on global communicative behaviors, both providing compelling evidence and four outcomes based on caregiver–child interactions (promising = 2; lacking = 2). Of the results assessed at follow-up, caregiver communicative outcomes were maintained postintervention.

Child outcomes. Of the 21 studies that had caregiver outcomes with compelling or promising evidence, they all also included child outcome measures (see Table 8). There were 23 language intervention groups that yielded 106 child communication-focused outcomes with a group average of 4.61 measures (min–max: 1–16).

All five studies in the developmental disorder subgroup included measures of expressive language, yielding a total of 32 outcomes (compelling = 3; promising = 14; lacking = 15). One study included two measures of receptive language both providing lacking evidence. One study included one social/pragmatic measure of language that provided lacking evidence. None of the studies included global measures of language. There was one study within this group that included follow-up assessments, with results suggesting that child communicative outcomes were maintained postintervention.

Twelve of the 15 studies in the autism subgroup included measures of expressive language, yielding a total of 22 outcomes (compelling = 4; promising = 11; lacking = 7). Six studies included measures of receptive language yielding a total of eight outcomes (promising = 5; lacking = 3). Eight studies included social/pragmatic outcomes, yielding a total of 21 outcomes (compelling = 4; promising = 15; lacking =2). Seven studies included global measures of language yielding a total of 10 outcomes (compelling = 2; promising = 5; lacking = 3). Four studies within this group included follow-up assessments with results suggesting that most child communicative outcomes were maintained postintervention.

There was one study included in the language delay/disorder subgroup, which included nine measures of expressive language (compelling = 1; promising = 2; lacking = 6) and one receptive language outcome that yielded promising evidence. This study also included follow-up assessments, with results suggesting that child communicative outcomes were maintained postintervention.

Discussion

The primary aim of this scoping review was to better understand the nature of studies that have been conducted to evaluate caregiver-implemented communication interventions for young children with language impairment. By examining the existing research base, we aimed to identify research gaps to guide future intervention studies. Here, we summarize major findings regarding each of our study questions and relate these findings to calls for future research directions.

Summary of Evidence

Study Question 1: What Populations Have Been Represented in Research Focused on Caregiver-Implemented Communication Interventions for Children With Language Impairment in Terms of Child and Caregiver Characteristics?

We identified only one caregiver-implemented communication intervention study that involved children less than 12 months of age (Barrera et al., 1987), and this study was excluded from our synthesis because it did not yield any language outcomes with either compelling or promising evidence. Thus, there is a substantial need for more caregiver-implemented communication studies for these populations. There are several disorders typically identified prenatally or very early in development that are commonly associated with communication impairment (e.g., Down syndrome and cerebral palsy), which likely warrant very early intervention. More studies are needed to better understand how to target these populations, who may also be experiencing complicated medical issues, and to determine if interventions for children this young lead to short-term and long-term gains in communication development.

For both the 24- through 35- and 36- through 48-month groups, the majority of studies included RCTs (52% and 62%, respectively), demonstrating high-experimental control. For all groups, the majority of studies were conducted since 2020 (67%). We advocate for the continued and increased use of RCTs to allow for critical evaluation of caregiver-implemented communication interventions. However, it is important to note that the study sample sizes have been relatively small with averages of 24.81, 18.19, and 20.13 children per treatment groups for the 12- through 23-, 24- through 35-, and 36- through 48-month groups, respectively. For each of these three age groups, the most common country that the studies were conducted in was the United States (71%, 52%, and 48%, respectively). Thus, there is a need to further evaluate caregiver-implemented interventions with larger samples and in additional countries throughout the world as cultural differences may impact treatment outcomes.

In the 12- through 23-month group, majority of intervention studies fell into the developmental disorder and autism subgroups (43% in each), with only one study (14%) in the language delay/disorder subgroup. In contrast, in the 24- through 35-month group, 29% of the studies fell into the developmental disorder subgroup, 52% in the language delay/disorder subgroup, and only 19% in the autism subgroup; while in the 36- through 48-month group, 24% of intervention groups fell into the developmental disorder subgroup, 71% in the autism subgroup, and only 5% in the language delay/disorder subgroup. In the 24 through 35-month group, each of the four studies in the autism subgroup were published since 2019. Thus, future studies should continue to focus on younger children on the autism spectrum as assessments are developed to identify very young children with ASD. The synthesis also highlights a lack of studies focused on 3-year-olds with language delay/disorder as only one study, which was published in 1992, was identified in this subgroup. Thus, there is a need for future evaluations of caregiver-implemented interventions for this population.

Many of the intervention groups within the developmental delay/disorder subgroup comprised children with mixed diagnoses. For example, one study in the 12- through 23-month group comprised children with Down syndrome, cerebral palsy, spina bifida, hydrocephalus, or congenital infectious diseases. Similarly, in the 24- through 35-month group, one study comprised children with developmental disorder, Down syndrome, cerebral palsy, Deaf and hard of hearing, or language delay. Future research studies should be designed in a manner that will allow for evaluation of the intervention based on diagnosis to move our field closer to being able to better tailor treatments. To do this, we need to consider conducting more studies with study groups based on diagnosis or conducting very large-scale studies with heterogeneous samples, so that we can learn how to predict which individuals within a certain diagnostic category are most likely to benefit from a specified intervention (Bzdok et al., 2021; Lenze et al., 2020).

Additionally, in all of the groups, the majority of child participants were males, ranging from 71% to 78%, respectively. While this rate reflects the higher prevalence of boys than girls for many conditions (e.g., autism [Maenner et al., 2020] and developmental language disorder [Tomblin et al., 1997]), the lack of larger samples of girls limits the study generalizability. Similarly, examination of the caregiver characteristics revealed that for both the 24- through 35- and 36- through 48-month groups, there were intervention groups with only mothers and no groups with only fathers. Several study groups included both mothers and fathers, but often the breakdown of the sample across caregiver roles was unspecified. Future studies need to better report the composition of their targeted caregivers. Additionally, future studies should further consider the impact of caregiver characteristics on intervention outcomes (e.g., Shalev et al., 2020) and evaluate how to best serve families with diverse caregiver characteristics. Our extraction process included documenting caregiver characteristics beyond age and race; however, only two studies, neither of which were associated with promising or compelling evidence, noted unique caregiver characteristics. The caregiver characteristics of these two studies included head of single-parent home with medical or environmental risks (Armstrong, 1998) and low income (Towson & Gallagher, 2014). Thus, there is a great need for future studies to consider how caregiver characteristics relate to intervention outcomes for children with communication disorders.

Study Question 2: How Have Intervention Components of Caregiver-Implemented Communication Interventions Been Represented in the Research?

For the 12- through 23-month group, there was variability in interventionists, including trained paraprofessionals, speech-language pathologists, trained early intervention providers, teacher consultants, researchers, and parent educators, while for the 24- through 35-month and 36- through 48-month group, speech-language pathologist was the most common interventionist (54% and 48% of all groups, respectively). Across all age groups, examination of other intervention components indicates that the interventions were most commonly conducted in English in caregiver homes and comprised a mix of one-on-one and group sessions completed in-person. Additional investigations are necessary to determine if these specific components result in optimal efficiency and outcomes across different populations.

For the 12- through 23-month group, the average dosage across all study groups was 9.33 sessions, with sessions lasting an average of 1.35 hr. The average intervention duration was 19 weeks with an average of one to three sessions weekly sessions. For the 24- through 35-month group, the average dosage was 14.40 sessions that were 1.30 hr in length. The average duration was 13.5 weeks. The most common frequency of sessions was once a week followed by 2–3 times per week. For the 36- through 48-month group, the average dosages were higher than for the other groups with an average of 19.5 sessions, 1.5 hr in length over a 5-month period. For all groups, some level of dosage was reported, but it was common for studies to report on only a few of the dosage variables. Thus, more complete reporting of dosage is needed to facilitate the evaluation of optimal dosages for children and their caregivers (Warren et al., 2007). Additionally, of the studies included in our review, only one study compared intervention dosage, highlighting the need for research focused on dosage optimization.

For all but one study group, we were able to use the information provided to broadly characterize the intervention delivered. In the 12- through 23-month group, we characterized 63% as NDBIs and 37% as developmental. Similarly, in the 24- through 35-month group, we characterized 62% as NDBIs, 31% as developmental, and 1% as Sensory-based. In the 36- through 48-month group, the distribution of NDBI and developmental interventions was more even with 48% and 43% being characterized as such, respectively. The 36- through 48-month group also included one behavior intervention group and one TEACHH group. Examination of these characterizations by diagnostic subgroup revealed that the majority of the interventions for the developmental disorder and autism groups were NDBIs (63% and 70%, respectively), while the most common intervention type for the language delay/disorder group was developmental (54%) followed by NDBI (38%). Further evaluations of the intervention types are needed to determine if one intervention may be better suited for children of a certain age or diagnosis.

There was considerable variability in reporting on the level detail provided regarding the specific strategies and information provided to caregivers. There appears to be similarities in these strategies and information; however, this information was difficult to synthesize due to heterogeneity of report detail and variability in terminology used. Examples of similarities noted by reviewing Table 7 include following the child's lead (n = 4 of 8 groups for 12- through 23-month group), modeling (n = 10 of 26 groups for 24- through 35-month group), and expanding child's utterances (n = 6 of 23 groups for 36- through 48-month group). The development of a taxonomy of terms relevant to caregiver-implemented communication interventions using an approach like the Denman et al. (2021) Delphi study, which established consensus terminology to describe child language interventions used by speech-language pathologists in Australia may promote more consistency in terminology use across settings and allow for a more precise synthesis. Additionally, to facilitate more complete reporting, we urge for more studies to use the 12-item Template for Intervention Description and Replication (TIDieR) developed by Hoffmann et al. (2014). Examples of items included on the TIDieR checklist include the intervention materials, the intervention procedures, who provided the intervention, where the intervention took place, and intervention dosage. A brief review of Table 6 reveals inconsistency in reporting of the intervention location as well as each dosage element.

Study Question 3: What Caregiver and Child Outcome Measures Have Been Used to Evaluate Caregiver-Implemented Communication Interventions for Children With Language Impairment?

Across all three represented child age groups, measures based on caregiver–child interactions most commonly served as outcome measures, followed by measures of caregiver spoken language and global measures of language input and environment, respectively. Across the 24- through 35- and 36- through 48-month groups, there were three studies in each that included measures of each of these three types. Across all child age groups, there were no studies that included measures of caregiver nonverbal communication and sign language. This finding is surprising given that the use of gestures and sign language are often recommended for young children with communication impairment (e.g., McGregor, 2008). The heterogeneity of outcome measure types and inconsistency of use of the types may call for the need for a gold standard of outcome measure types (as well as specific measures) to be used in caregiver-implemented communication studies. For example, a gold standard could be set that all caregiver-implemented communication studies include at least one measure of caregiver language input, one measure of language contingent on child language output (e.g., measure derived from a caregiver-child interaction), and one global measure of the home language environment.

Examination of child language outcome measures in the 12- through 23-month group revealed that more than half (71%) of the studies that included caregiver outcome measures with compelling or promising evidence also included child outcome measures. All studies included measures of child expressive language, resulting in 25 (71%) expressive language outcomes across studies compared with four (11%) receptive language outcomes, four (11%) social/pragmatic measures, and two (6%) global measures. For the 24- through 35-month group, a larger proportion (80%) of studies included both caregiver and child language outcome measures. However, a pattern similar to the 12- through 23-month group emerged with the majority of outcomes based on child expressive language (83%), and few outcomes based on global language (8%), receptive (7%), or social/pragmatic measures (2%). In the 36- through 48-month group, all studies included adult and child communicative measures; again, the majority of child outcomes were expressive (58%); however, within this age group, there was an increase in social/pragmatic (20%) followed by receptive (10%) and global language measures (9%). Overall, studies across age ranges targeted more language measures (i.e., expressive, receptive, or global) compared with communication (i.e., social/pragmatic); future research should make sure to target both child and language communication outcomes. Consistent with our recognition for a gold standard specifying outcome domains to be reflected in caregiver outcome measure for caregiver-implemented communication studies, we advocate for developing a gold standard for child communication domains to be included in intervention studies.

Across all included studies, 13 (22%) included follow-up assessments, these ranged from 4 weeks to 15 months postintervention. Overall, results provide encouraging evidence that changes in adult and child communicative outcomes maintained postintervention. However, these results should be interpreted with caution as there was variability in how follow-up analyses were conducted, with some studies examining change between posttest and follow-up, others comparing follow-up with pretest. Future research is needed to determine the “minimum” changes needed in caregiver communicative outcomes that result in positive changes to child language development.

Study Limitations

There are a few study limitations that should be taken into consideration when interpreting the current scoping review. Due to our inclusion and exclusion criteria, our review may have been too limited in scope. Limitations in resources prompted us to only include studies on caregiver-implemented communication interventions that were available in peer-reviewed journals published in English. Inclusion of studies reported in non–peer-reviewed journals (e.g., dissertations) and in other languages would have provided a more complete description of the current research base. Due to the potential lag time between journal articles being published and then indexed in the databases (e.g., Scopus) used in this study, additional studies that meet our inclusion criteria may not have been identified (e.g., Peredo et al., 2022, was indexed after we conducted our last search update). Additionally, to focus our scoping review, we required studies to report on at least one caregiver-based outcome measure. This helped to ensure that the intervention was caregiver focused and aimed to change caregiver behavior. Including studies with caregiver-implemented communication interventions, but only child outcome measures, would likely have yielded a greater number of studies in our review. Moreover, this study reflects only a portion of our entire scoping review process. In this review, we provide an extensive overview of studies examining caregiver-implemented communication interventions for children with communication disorders to better understand the populations that have been included in previous studies as well as the intervention components. While we describe the context and modality interventionists used to instruct caregivers, there remains a need to further examine the specific strategies and approaches used for caregiver instruction. Additional papers will be forthcoming that describe reported caregiver instructional approaches, as well as those that focus on caregiver interventions for children at risk for communication impairment, that include single-case designed studies, or that provide greater detail on specific outcome measures.

Conclusions

Although many studies have examined caregiver-implemented communication intervention for children with communication impairment, particularly for children greater than 12 months of age, this scoping review highlights important research gaps and inconsistencies in study reporting that should be addressed. With this kind of attention, the evidence base can move beyond examining whether caregiver-implemented communication interventions are effective and specify the conditions under which and for whom intervention approaches are most optimal.

Author Contributions

Lizbeth H. Finestack: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. Marianne Elmquist: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft. Kirstin Kuchler: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology. Andrea Boh Ford: Data curation, Investigation, Writing – original draft. Betul Cakir-Dilek: Data curation, Investigation. Amy Riegelman: Data curation, Methodology. Sarah Jane Brown: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology. Scott Marsalis: Data curation, Methodology.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Material S1. PRISMA-ScR checklist.
Supplemental Material S2. Search strategies.
Supplemental Material S3. Characteristics of studies without compelling or promising evidence.
Supplemental Material S4. Participant characteristics for groups in studies without compelling or promising evidence.
Supplemental Material S5. Summary of intervention trainer, language, location, and dosage for groups in studies without compelling or promising evidence.
Supplemental Material S6. Summary of instruction format, key intervention strategies taught and information provided to caregivers, and intervention contexts for groups in studies without compelling or promising evidence.
Supplemental Material S7. Number of caregiver outcomes rated as providing lacking evidence according to measure domain for studies with a group without promising or compelling evidence.
Supplemental Material S8. Number of child outcomes for each study with caregiver outcomes rated as providing lacking evidence according to measure domain for studies with a group without promising or compelling evidence.
Supplemental Material S9. Complete references for studies included in scoping review.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a research sprint conducted by the University of Minnesota Libraries. Additionally, this publication was supported in part by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development under Award Numbers T32HD007489 and U54 HD090256 and the University of Wisconsin–Madison. Special thanks to University of Minnesota Librarians: Katherine Chew and Richmond Kinney as well as lead undergraduate research assistants: Lola DeFever and Sakina Salemohamed. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Funding Statement

This study was supported by a research sprint conducted by the University of Minnesota Libraries. Additionally, this publication was supported in part by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development under Award Numbers T32HD007489 and U54 HD090256 and the University of Wisconsin–Madison.

Footnotes

1

We use the term caregiver here broadly to be inclusive of all adults who may be implementing interventions, including parents, grandparents, and foster parents. Later, we will define caregiver specific to the aims of the scoping review.

2

Throughout the article, we use the terminology autism and individual on the autism spectrum based on the recommendations of Bottema-Beutel et al. (2021) and Botha et al., (2021), respectively.

References

*Studies that did not include caregiver language outcome measures with compelling or promising evidence and were not included in the primary review.

**Studies that included caregiver language outcome measures with compelling or promising evidence and included in the primary review.

  1. ** Aldred, C. , Green, J. , & Adams, C. (2004). A new social communication intervention for children with autism: Pilot randomized controlled treatment study suggesting effectiveness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(8), 1420–1430. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00338.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. ** Alquraini, T. , Al-Odaib, A. , Al-Dhalaan, H. , Merza, H. , & Mahoney, G. (2018). Feasibility of responsive teaching with mothers and young children with autism in Saudi Arabia. Journal of Early Intervention, 40(4), 304–316. https://doi.org/10.1177/1053815118789176 [Google Scholar]
  3. Arksey, H. , & O'Malley, L. (2005). Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 8(1), 19–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/1364557032000119616 [Google Scholar]
  4. * Armstrong, M. B. (1998). Storybooks to literacy: A collaborative shared reading project. Infant–Toddler Intervention, 8(4), 365–375. [Google Scholar]
  5. * Barrera, M. E. , Watson, L. J. , & Adelstein, A. (1987). Development of Down's syndrome infants with and without heart defects and changes in their caretaking environment. Child: Care, Health and Development, 13(2), 87–100. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.1987.tb00526.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. ** Barrett, A. C. , Vernon, T. W. , McGarry, E. S. , Holden, A. N. , Bradshaw, J. , Ko, J. A. , Horowitz, E. J. , & German, T. C. (2020). Social responsiveness and language use associated with an enhanced PRT approach for young children with ASD: Results from a pilot RCT of the PRISM model. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 71, 101497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2019.101497 [Google Scholar]
  7. Barton, E. E. , & Fettig, A. (2013). Parent-implemented interventions for young children with disabilities. Journal of Early Intervention, 35(2), 194–219. https://doi.org/10.1177/1053815113504625 [Google Scholar]
  8. ** Baxendale, J. , & Hesketh, A. (2003). Comparison of the effectiveness of the Hanen Parent Programme and traditional clinic therapy. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(4), 397–415. https://doi.org/10.1080/1368282031000121651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Botha, M. , Hanlon, J. , & Williams, G. L. (2021). Does language matter? Identity-first versus person-first language use in autism research: A response to Vivanti. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04858-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bottema-Beutel, K. , Kapp, S. K. , Lester, J. N. , Sasson, N. J. , & Hand, B. N. (2021). Avoiding ableist language: Suggestions for autism researchers. Autism in Adulthood, 3(1), 18–29. http://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2020.0014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. ** Braiden, H. J. , McDaniel, B. , McCrudden, E. , Janes, M. , & Crozier, B. (2012). A practice-based evaluation of Barnardo's Forward Steps Early Intervention Programme for children diagnosed with autism. Child Care in Practice, 18(3), 227–242. https://doi.org/10.1080/13575279.2012.683773 [Google Scholar]
  12. ** Brian, J. A. , Smith, I. M. , Zwaigenbaum, L. , Roberts, W. , Bryson, S. E. , & Baranek, B. (2016). The social ABCs caregiver-mediated intervention for toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: Feasibility, acceptability, and evidence of promise from a multisite study. Autism Research, 9(8), 899–912. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bzdok, D. , Varoquaux, G. , & Steyerberg, E. W. (2021). Prediction, not association, paves the road to precision medicine. JAMA Psychiatry, 78(2), 127–128. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carta, J. C. , Greenwood, C. R. , & Walker, D. (2014). Bridging the word gap national research network. Child and Maternal Health; Human Resources Services Administration (HRSA) (HRSA: UA6MC27762). https://bwg.ku.edu/ [Google Scholar]
  15. ** Carter, A. S. , Messinger, D. S. , Stone, W. L. , Celimli, S. , Nahmias, A. S. , & Yoder, P. (2011). A randomized controlled trial of Hanen's 'More Than Words' in toddlers with early autism symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(7), 741–752. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02395.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. ** Casenhiser, D. M. , Shanker, S. G. , & Stieben, J. (2013). Learning through interaction in children with autism: Preliminary data from asocial-communication-based intervention. Autism, 17(2), 220–241. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361311422052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. ** Ciccone, N. , Hennessey, N. , & Stokes, S. F. (2012). Community-based early intervention for language delay: A preliminary investigation. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 47(4), 467–470. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00149.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. ** Cycyk, L. M. , Moore, H. W. , De Anda, S. , Huerta, L. , Méndez, S. , Patton, C. , & Bourret, C. (2020). Adaptation of a caregiver-implemented naturalistic communication intervention for Spanish-speaking families of Mexican immigrant descent: A promising start. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(3), 1260–1282. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Denman, D. , Kim, J. H. , Munro, N. , Speyer, R. , & Cordier, R. (2021). Consensus on terminology for describing child language interventions: A Delphi study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 64(9), 3504–3519. https://doi.org/10.1044/2021_JSLHR-20-00656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dunst, C. J. , Lowe, L. W. , & Bartholomew, P. C. (1989). Contingent social responsiveness, family ecology, and infant communicative competence. National Student Speech Language Hearing Association Journal, 17, 39–49. https://pubs.asha.org/doi/pdf/10.1044/nsshla_17_39 [Google Scholar]
  21. ** Falkus, G. , Tilley, C. , Thomas, C. , Hockey, H. , Kennedy, A. , Arnold, T. , Thorburn, B. , Jones, K. , Patel, B. , Pimenta, C. , Shah, R. , Tweedie, F. , O'Brien, F. , Leahy, R. , & Pring, T. (2016). Assessing the effectiveness of parent–child interaction therapy with language delayed children: A clinical investigation. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 32(1), 7–17. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265659015574918 [Google Scholar]
  22. ** Fey, M. E. , Warren, S. F. , Brady, N. , Finestack, L. H. , Bredin-Oja, S. L. , Fairchild, M. , Sokol, S. , & Yoder, P. J. (2006). Early effects of responsivity education/prelinguistic milieu teaching for children with developmental delays and their parents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(3), 526–547. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/039) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. ** Fey, M. E. , Yoder, P. J. , Warren, S. F. , & Bredin-Oja, S. J. (2013). Is more better? Milieu communication teaching in toddlers with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(2), 679–693. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0061) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ford, A. L. , Elmquist, M. , Merbler, A. M. , Kriese, A. , Will, K. K. , & McConnell, S. R. (2020). Toward an ecobehavioral model of early language development. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 246–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.11.004 [Google Scholar]
  25. Fuller, E. A. , & Kaiser, A. P. (2020). The effects of early intervention on social communication outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(5), 1683–1700. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-03927-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. ** Gibbard, D. , & Smith, C. (2016). A transagency approach to enabling access to parent-based intervention for language delay in areas of social disadvantage: A service evaluation. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 32(1), 19–33. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265659014567785 [Google Scholar]
  27. ** Girolametto, L. E. (1988). Improving the social-conversational skills of developmentally delayed children: An intervention study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53(2), 156–167. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5302.156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. ** Girolametto, L. , Pearce, P. S. , & Weitzman, E. (1996). Interactive focused stimulation for toddlers with expressive vocabulary delays. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(6), 1274–1283. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3906.1274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. ** Girolametto, L. , Weitzman, E. , & Clements-Baartman, J. (1998). Vocabulary intervention for children with Down syndrome: Parent training using focused stimulation. Infant-Toddler Intervention, 8(2), 109–125. [Google Scholar]
  30. Grant, M. J. , & Booth, A. (2009). A typology of reviews: An analysis of 14 review types and associated methodologies. Health Information & Libraries Journal, 26(2), 91–108. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-1842.2009.00848.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. ** Green, J. , Charman, T. , McConachie, H. , Aldred, C. , Slonims, V. , Howlin, P. , Le Couteur, A. , Leadbitter, K. , Hudry, K. , Byford, S. , Barrett, B. , Temple, K. , Macdonald, W. , Pickles, A., & PACT Consortium. (2010). Parent-mediated communication-focused treatment in children with autism (PACT): A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 375(9732), 2152–2160. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60587-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Greenwood, C. R. , Schnitz, A. G. , Carta, J. J. , Wallisch, A. , & Irvin, D. W. (2020). A systematic review of language intervention research with low-income families: A word gap prevention perspective. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 230–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ECRESQ.2019.04.001 [Google Scholar]
  33. Guiberson, M. , & Ferris, K. P. (2019). Early language interventions for young dual language learners: A scoping review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28(3), 945–963. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_AJSLP-IDLL-18-0251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gurevitch, J. , Koricheva, J. , Nakagawa, S. , & Stewart, G. (2018). Meta-analysis and the science of research synthesis. Nature, 555(7695), 175–182. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hampton, L. H. , & Kaiser, A. P. (2016). Intervention effects on spoken-language outcomes for children with autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 60(5), 444–463. https://doi.org/10.1111/jir.12283 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. ** Hampton, L. H. , Kaiser, A. P. , & Fuller, E. A. (2020). Multi-component communication intervention for children with autism: A randomized controlled trial. Autism, 24(8), 2104–2116. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320934558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Haring Biel, C. , Buzhardt, J. , Brown, J. A. , Romano, M. K. , Lorio, C. M. , Windsor, K. S. , Kaczmarek, L. A. , Gwin, R. , Sandall, S. S. , & Goldstein, H. (2020). Language interventions taught to caregivers in homes and classrooms: A review of intervention and implementation fidelity. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50(Part 1), 140–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  38. Heidlage, J. K. , Cunningham, J. E. , Kaiser, A. P. , Trivette, C. M. , Barton, E. E. , Frey, J. R. , & Roberts, M. Y. (2020). The effects of parent-implemented language interventions on child linguistic outcomes: A meta-analysis. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 6–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  39. Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 4–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hoffmann, T. C. , Glasziou, P. P. , Boutron, I. , Milne, R. , Perera, R. , Moher, D. , Altman, D. G. , Barbour, V. , Macdonald, H. , Johnston, M. , Lamb, S. E. , Dixon-Woods, M. , McCulloch, P. , Wyatt, J. C. , Chan, A. W. , & Michie, S. (2014). Better reporting of interventions: Template for intervention description and replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide. BMJ (Clinical research ed.), 348, g1687. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.g1687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. ** Ingersoll, B. , Wainer, A. L. , Berger, N. I. , Pickard, K. E. , & Bonter, N. (2016). Comparison of a self-directed and therapist-assisted telehealth parent-mediated intervention for children with ASD: A pilot RCT. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(7), 2275–2284. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2755-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. ** Ingersoll, B. R. , & Wainer, A. L. (2013). Pilot study of a school-based parent training program for preschoolers with ASD. Autism, 17(4), 434–448. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361311427155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. ** Iven, C. J. , Albritton, E. G. , Eaton, B. B. , & Montague, J. C. (1989). A pilot study on the effect of training parents of language-delayed children in pragmatic interaction strategies. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69(1), 295–303. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1989.69.1.295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Jeong, J. , Franchett, E. E. , Ramos de Oliveira, C. V. , Rehmani, K. , & Yousafzai, A. K. (2021). Parenting interventions to promote early child development in the first three years of life: A global systematic review and meta-analysis. PLOS Medicine, 18(5), e1003602. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1003602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Justice, L. M. , Bowles, R. P. , Pence Turnbull, K. L. , & Skibbe, L. E. (2009). School readiness among children with varying histories of language difficulties. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 460–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014324 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. ** Kaiser, A. P. , & Roberts, M. Y. (2013). Parent-implemented enhanced milieu teaching with preschool children who have intellectual disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(1), 295–309. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0231) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. ** Kot, A. , & Law, J. (1995). Intervention with preschool children with specific language impairments: A comparison of two different approaches to treatment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 11(2), 144–162. https://doi.org/10.1177/026565909501100202 [Google Scholar]
  48. Larson, A. L. , Cycyk, L. M. , Carta, J. J. , Hammer, C. S. , Baralt, M. , Uchikoshi, Y. , An, Z. G. , & Wood, C. (2020). A systematic review of language-focused interventions for young children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 157–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.06.001 [Google Scholar]
  49. Law, J. , Garrett, Z. , & Nye, C. (2004). The efficacy of treatment for children with developmental speech and language delay/disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(4), 924–943. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/069) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lenze, E. J. , Rodebaugh, T. L. , & Nicol, G. E. (2020). A framework for advancing precision medicine in clinical trials for mental disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(7), 663–664. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Levac, D. , Colquhoun, H. , & O'Brien, K. K. (2010). Scoping studies: Advancing the methodology. Implementation Science, 5(1), Article No. 69. https://doi.org/10.1186/1748-5908-5-69 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. ** Liu, X. , & Schertz, H. H. (2021). Parents outcomes of parent-mediated intervention for toddlers with autism. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/0271121421101911 [Google Scholar]
  53. Maenner, M. J. , Shaw, K. A. , Baio, J. , Washington, A. , Patrick, M. , DiRienzo, M. , Christensen, D. L. , Wiggins, L. D. , Pettygrove, S. , Andrews, J. G. , Lopez, M. , Hudson, A. , Baroud, T. , Schwenk, Y. , White, T. , Rosenberg, C. R. , Lee, L.-C. , Harrington, R. A. , Huston, M. , … Dietz, P. M. (2020). Correction and republication: Prevalence and characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder among children aged 8 years — Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States, 2012. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 67(45), 1279. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm6745a7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mahoney, A. D. , McConnell, S. R. , Larson, A. L. , Becklenberg, A. , & Stapel-Wax, J. L. (2020). Where do we go from here? Examining pediatric and population-level interventions to improve child outcomes. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 205–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.01.009 [Google Scholar]
  55. ** Mahoney, G. , & Powell, A. (1988). Modifying parent-child interaction: Enhancing the development of handicapped children. Early Intervention for Infants With Handicaps and Their Families, 22(1), 82–96. https://doi.org/10.1177/002246698802200110 [Google Scholar]
  56. ** Matthews, N. L. , Orr, B. C. , Harris, B. , McIntosh, R. , Openden, D. , & Smith, C. J. (2018). Parent and child outcomes of JumpStartTM, an education and training program for parents of children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 56, 21–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2018.08.009 [Google Scholar]
  57. ** McDade, A. , & McCartan, P. (1998). ‘Partnership with parents’: A pilot project. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 33(S1), 556–561. https://doi.org/10.3109/13682829809179485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. ** McGarry, E. , Vernon, T. , & Baktha, A. (2020). Brief report: A pilot online pivotal response treatment training program for parents of toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(9), 3424–3431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04100-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. McGregor, K. K. (2008). Gesture supports children's word learning. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10(3), 112–117. https://doi.org/10.1080/17549500801905622 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. ** Minjarez, M. B. , Williams, S. E. , Mercier, E. M. , & Hardan, A. Y. (2011). Pivotal response group treatment program for parents of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(1), 92–101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1027-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Moher, D. , Liberati, A. , Tetzlaff, J. , Altman, D. G. , & The PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLOS Medicine, 6(7), e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed1000097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Morreale, S. P. , Valenzano, J. M. , & Bauer, J. A. (2017). Why communication education is important: A third study on the centrality of the discipline's content and pedagogy. Communication Education, 66(4), 402–422. https://doi.org/10.1080/03634523.2016.1265136 [Google Scholar]
  63. National Association for the Education of Young Children. (2020, April). Developmentally appropriate practice: A position statement of the national association for the education of young children. https://www.naeyc.org/resources/position-statements/dap/contents
  64. National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. (2016, July 11). Statistics on voice, speech, and language. https://www.nidcd.nih.gov/health/statistics/statistics-voice-speech-and-language
  65. ** Noyan Erbas, A. , Ozcebe, E. , & Cak Esen, T. (2020). Investigation of the effect of Hanen's “More Than Words” on parental self-efficacy, emotional states, perceived social support, and on communication skills of children with ASD. Logopedics, Phoniatrics, Vocology, 46(1), 17–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/14015439.2020.1717601 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. ** Pearce, P. S. , Girolametto, L. , & Weitzman, E. (1996). The effects of focused stimulation intervention on mothers of late-talking toddlers. Infant-Toddler Intervention, 6(3), 213–227. [Google Scholar]
  67. ** Pennington, L. , Thomson, K. , James, P. , Martin, L. , & McNally, R. (2009). Effects of It Takes Two to Talk—The Hanen Program for Parents of Preschool Children With Cerebral Palsy: Findings from an exploratory study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(5), 1121–1138. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0187) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Peredo, T. N. , Mancilla-Martinez, J. , Durkin, K. , & Kaiser, A. P. (2022). Teaching Spanish-speaking caregivers to implement EMT en Español: A small randomized trial. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 58, 208–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. ** Rajesh, V. , & Venkatesh, L. (2019). Preliminary evaluation of a low-intensity parent training program on speech-language stimulation for children with language delay. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 122, 99–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2019.03.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Rescorla, L. (2009). Age 17 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers: Support for a dimensional perspective on language delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52(1), 16–30. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0171) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Roberts, M. Y. , Curtis, P. R. , Sone, B. J. , & Hampton, L. H. (2019). Association of parent training with child language development: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association Pediatrics, 173(7), 671–680. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.1197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Roberts, M. Y. , & Kaiser, A. P. (2011). The effectiveness of parent-implemented language interventions: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20(3), 180–199. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0055) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. ** Roberts, M. Y. , & Kaiser, A. P. (2012). Assessing the effects of a parent-implemented language intervention for children with language impairments using empirical benchmarks: A pilot study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(6), 1655–1670. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0236) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. ** Roberts, M. Y. , & Kaiser, A. P. (2015). Early intervention for toddlers with language delays: A randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 135(4), 686–693. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-2134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. ** Rogers, S. J. , Estes, A. , Vismara, L. , Munson, J. , Zierhut, C. , Greenson, J. , Dawson, G. , Rocha, M. , Sugar, C. , Senturk, D. , Whelan, F. , & Talbott, M. (2019). Enhancing low-intensity coaching in parent implemented early start Denver model intervention for early autism: A randomized comparison treatment trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(2), 632–646. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3740-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. ** Rollins, P. R. , John, S. , Jones, A. , & De Froy, A. (2019). Pathways Early ASD Intervention as a moderator of parenting stress on parenting behaviors: A randomized control trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(10), 4280–4293. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04144-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. ** Romski, M. , Sevcik, R. A. , Adamson, L. B. , Cheslock, M. , Smith, A. , Barker, R. M. , & Bakeman, R. (2010). Randomized comparison of augmented and nonaugmented language interventions for toddlers with developmental delays and their parents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 350–364. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0156) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Roulstone, S. , Law, J. , Rush, R. , Clegg, J. , & Peters, T. J. (2011). Investigating the role of language in children's early educational outcomes (Research report DFE-RR134) . Department for Education. https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOrderingDownload/DFE-RR134.pdf [Google Scholar]
  79. Sameroff, A. (2009). The transactional model. In Sameroff A. (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp. 3–21). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/11877-001 [Google Scholar]
  80. Sandbank, M. , Bottema-Beutel, K. , Crowley, S. , Cassidy, M. , Dunham, K. , Feldman, J. I. , Crank, J. , Albarran, S. A. , Raj, S. , Mahbub, P. , & Woynaroski, T. G. (2020). Project AIM: Autism intervention meta-analysis for studies of young children. Psychological Bulletin, 146(1), 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000215 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Schreibman, L. , Dawson, G. , Stahmer, A. C. , Landa, R. , Rogers, S. J. , McGee, G. G. , Kasari, C. , Ingersoll, B. , Kaiser, A. P. , Bruinsma, Y. , McNerney, E. , Wetherby, A. , & Halladay, A. (2015). Naturalistic developmental behavioral interventions: Empirically validated treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(8), 2411–2428. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2407-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. ** Sengupta, K. , Mahadik, S. , & Kapoor, G. (2020). Glocalizing project ImPACT: Feasibility, acceptability and preliminary outcomes of a parent-mediated social communication intervention for autism adapted to the Indian context. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 76, Article 101585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2020.101585 [Google Scholar]
  83. Shalev, R. A. , Lavine, C. , & Di Martino, A. (2020). A systematic review of the role of parent characteristics in parent-mediated interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 32, 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-018-9641-x [Google Scholar]
  84. ** Stahmer, A. C. , Rieth, S. R. , Dickson, K. S. , Feder, J. , Burgeson, M. , Searcy, K. , & Brookman-Frazee, L. (2020). Project ImPACT for Toddlers: Pilot outcomes of a community adaptation of an intervention for autism risk. Autism, 24(3), 617–632. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319878080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. ** Suttora, C. , Zuccarini, M. , Aceti, A. , Corvaglia, L. , Guarini, A. , & Sansavini, A. (2021). The Effects of a parent-implemented language intervention on late-talkers' expressive skills: The mediational role of parental speech contingency and dialogic reading abilities. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 3645. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.723366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. ** Tannock, R. , Girolametto, L. , & Siegel, L. S. (1992). Language intervention with children who have developmental delays: Effects of an interactive approach. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 97(2), 145–160. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Tomblin, J. B. , Records, N. L. , Buckwalter, P. , Zhang, X. , Smith, E. , & O'Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245–1260. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. * Towson, J. A. , & Gallagher, P. A. (2014). Training Head Start parents in dialogic reading to improve outcomes for children. International Journal of Child Health and Human Development, 7(3), 287–296. [Google Scholar]
  89. Tricco, A. C. , Lillie, E. , Zarin, W. , O'Brien, K. K. , Colquhoun, H. , Levac, D. , Moher, D. , Peters, M. D. J. , Horsley, T. , Weeks, L. , Hempel, S. , Akl, E. A. , Chang, C. , McGowan, J. , Stewart, L. , Hartling, L. , Aldcroft, A. , Wilson, M. G. , Garritty, C. , … Straus, S. E. (2018). PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and explanation. Annals of Internal Medicine, 169(7), 467–473. https://doi.org/10.7326/M18-0850 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. * Venker, C. E. , McDuffie, A. , Weismer, S. E. , & Abbeduto, L. (2012). Increasing verbal responsiveness in parents of children with autism: A pilot study. Autism, 16(6), 568–585. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361311413396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Wainer, A. L. , Arnold, Z. E. , Leonczyk, C. , & Valluripalli Soorya, L. (2021). Examining a stepped-care telehealth program for parents of young children with autism: A proof-of-concept trial. Molecular Autism, 12(1), Article 32. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00443-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Walker, D. , Sepulveda, S. J. , Hoff, E. , Rowe, M. L. , Schwartz, I. S. , Dale, P. S. , Peterson, C. A. , Diamond, K. , Goldin-Meadow, S. , Levine, S. C. , Wasik, B. H. , Horm, D. M. , & Bigelow, K. M. (2020). Language intervention research in early childhood care and education: A systematic survey of the literature. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 68–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.02.010 [Google Scholar]
  93. Warren, S. F. , Fey, M. E. , & Yoder, P. J. (2007). Differential treatment intensity research: A missing link to creating optimally effective communication interventions. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 13(1), 70–77. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. * Weistuch, L. , Lewis, M. , & Sullivan, M. W. (1991). Use of a language interaction intervention in the preschools. Journal of Early Intervention, 15(3), 278–287. https://doi.org/10.1177/105381519101500307 [Google Scholar]
  95. ** Yang, Y.-H. (2016). Parents and young children with disabilities: The effects of a home-based music therapy program on parent–child interactions. Journal of Music Therapy, 53(1), 27–54. https://doi.org/10.1093/jmt/thv018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. ** Yoder, P. J. , & Warren, S. F. (2002). Effects of prelinguistic milieu teaching and parent responsivity education on dyads involving children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6), 1158–1174. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/094) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material S1. PRISMA-ScR checklist.
Supplemental Material S2. Search strategies.
Supplemental Material S3. Characteristics of studies without compelling or promising evidence.
Supplemental Material S4. Participant characteristics for groups in studies without compelling or promising evidence.
Supplemental Material S5. Summary of intervention trainer, language, location, and dosage for groups in studies without compelling or promising evidence.
Supplemental Material S6. Summary of instruction format, key intervention strategies taught and information provided to caregivers, and intervention contexts for groups in studies without compelling or promising evidence.
Supplemental Material S7. Number of caregiver outcomes rated as providing lacking evidence according to measure domain for studies with a group without promising or compelling evidence.
Supplemental Material S8. Number of child outcomes for each study with caregiver outcomes rated as providing lacking evidence according to measure domain for studies with a group without promising or compelling evidence.
Supplemental Material S9. Complete references for studies included in scoping review.

Articles from Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research : JSLHR are provided here courtesy of American Speech-Language-Hearing Association

RESOURCES