Skip to main content
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) logoLink to Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
. 2022 Apr 19;33(4):1476–1488. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac150

Metabolic and functional connectivity provide unique and complementary insights into cognition-connectome relationships

Katharina Voigt 1,2,, Emma X Liang 3, Bratislav Misic 4, Phillip G D Ward 5, Gary F Egan 6,7, Sharna D Jamadar 8,9
PMCID: PMC9930619  PMID: 35441214

Abstract

A major challenge in current cognitive neuroscience is how functional brain connectivity gives rise to human cognition. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) describes brain connectivity based on cerebral oxygenation dynamics (hemodynamic connectivity), whereas [18F]-fluorodeoxyglucose functional positron emission tomography (FDG-fPET) describes brain connectivity based on cerebral glucose uptake (metabolic connectivity), each providing a unique characterization of the human brain. How these 2 modalities differ in their contribution to cognition and behavior is unclear. We used simultaneous resting-state FDG-fPET/fMRI to investigate how hemodynamic connectivity and metabolic connectivity relate to cognitive function by applying partial least squares analyses. Results revealed that although for both modalities the frontoparietal anatomical subdivisions related the strongest to cognition, using hemodynamic measures this network expressed executive functioning, episodic memory, and depression, whereas for metabolic measures this network exclusively expressed executive functioning. These findings demonstrate the unique advantages that simultaneous FDG-PET/fMRI has to provide a comprehensive understanding of the neural mechanisms that underpin cognition and highlights the importance of multimodality imaging in cognitive neuroscience research.

Keywords: cognition, functional connectivity, functional positron emission tomography, metabolic connectivity

Introduction

The human connectome is a comprehensive map of neural connections that describes the brain as a complex network of interconnected brain regions (Sporns et al. 2005; Sporns 2013). Noninvasive neuroimaging methods provide us with the opportunity to characterize the functionality of brain connectivity on multiple levels (Raichle 2009). As such, brain connectivity is a multidimensional concept that is defined by its measurement tool. Blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been the dominant tool to characterize functional brain connectivity, based on the temporal coherence of spontaneous, low-frequency large-amplitude changes in blood oxygenation whilst an individual is at rest (Biswal et al. 1995; Raichle 2011). Without appropriate computational quantification of the hemodynamic BOLD response (e.g. see Báez-Yáñez et al. 2020 for a recent biophysical model on that issue), BOLD-fMRI provides a hemodynamic-based surrogate measure of neuronal activity at a high spatial and temporal resolution, but is confounded by non-neuronal components (e.g. heart rate, respiration, and blood volume; Liu 2017; Ward et al. 2020). Additional measures, such as cerebral blood volume and cerebrovascular reactivity are able to be incorporated into experimental protocols to quantify non-neuronal sources of noise within fMRI (Huber et al. 2018; Sleight et al. 2021). Positron emission tomography (PET) scanning using the glucose analogue F18-fluordoxyglucose (FDG) provides the opportunity to characterize metabolic elements of brain connectivity based on cerebral glucose update (Yakushev et al. 2017). In contrast to BOLD-fMRI, FDG-PET is a quantifiable index of neuronal activity capturing cerebral glucose uptake at the synapses. The integration of the 2 modalities in a simultaneous MR-PET system (Judenhofer et al. 2008; Chen et al. 2018) offers the unique opportunity to undertake multidimensional neuroimaging studies to examine the interaction between hemodynamic and metabolic aspects of brain connectivity. How these elements of human brain connectivity (i.e. hemodynamic and metabolic) individually and jointly contribute to human cognition and behavior remains a formidable challenge of contemporary cognitive neuroscience.

A central assumption in cognitive neuroscience is that cognitive processes are emergent properties of neural communication, which is predicted by the coherent and flexible oscillatory activity between neural ensembles (Avena-Koenigsberger et al. 2018; Barack and Krakauer 2021). Two views of how the brain as a neural network relates to cognition have emerged: the domain-specific connectome-cognition view and the global connectome-cognition view. According to the specific view, connectivity is domain-specific and multiple networks arise for distinct cognitive domains. According to the global view, the overall wiring of connectome gives rise to global cognitive functioning. A single set of connectivity patterns predict cognitive functioning across different domains, such as attention, memory, and executive functioning.

Evidence from fMRI research using multivariate analytic approaches to examine brain-behavior relationships has revealed support for both views (e.g. Ziegler et al. 2013; Smith et al. 2015; Zimmermann et al. 2018; Goyal et al. 2020a). In support of the domain-specific connectome-cognition view, Zimmermann et al. (2018) found unique orthogonal sets of resting-state hemodynamic connectivity clusters that were associated with specific cognitive domains. Inter- and intra-hemispheric resting-state hemodynamic connectivity in the frontoparietal, occipital, temporal, and cingulate areas was negatively associated with processing speed, executing functioning, and working memory. Intelligence was related to a separate set of resting-state hemodynamic connectivity in cortico-cortical and cortico-subcortical networks, such as the caudate and putamen. In contrast, in support of the global view, Smith et al. (2015) revealed a single mode of large-scale resting-state hemodynamic connectivity patterns capturing a wide set of behavioral (e.g. intelligence and verbal ability) and demographic variables (e.g. age, sex, income, and drug use). This result has recently been replicated by Goyal et al. (2020b). These studies reveal initial insights into how coherent, low-frequency BOLD-fMRI signaling in spatially distinct brain areas (i.e. hemodynamic connectivity) relates to cognition. However, as the BOLD signal represents a proxy of neural activity that is shaped by non-neuronal contributions to the BOLD signal (Liu 2017; Ward et al. 2020), this considerably restricts our existing understanding of connectome-cognition systems to hemodynamic correlates.

Recent developments in continuous radiotracer delivery and improved PET signal detection of dual-modality magnetic resonance (MR)-PET scanners, has allowed the study of continuous glucose uptake with substantially improved temporal resolution (e.g. 60 s or less; Villien et al. 2014; Rischka et al. 2018; Jamadar et al. 2021). This novel method, termed “functional” FDG-PET (FDG-fPET), provides the opportunity to characterize the metabolic connectome beyond previous covariance measures resulting from static PET (Jamadar et al. 2021) and thus, approaches similar within-subject time-course correlational descriptions as exist for BOLD-fMRI hemodynamic connectivity. Using the fPET approach, we recently found that the metabolic FDG-fPET connectome showed moderate similarity with the BOLD-fMRI hemodynamic connectivity at rest, with the highest similarity between functional and metabolic connectivity obtained primarily with the superior and frontoparietal cortical areas (Jamadar et al. 2021). These initial findings suggest the complementary potential of describing the human connectome via fMRI and FDG-fPET. However, how resting-state metabolic connectivity derived from FDG-fPET relates to cognition and how it differs in their predictive ability from BOLD-fMRI hemodynamic connectivity remains unknown.

The present study aimed to investigate whether (i) a single global or multiple distinct connectivity pattern maps onto cognition, and whether (ii) the connectome-cognition relationship is different for hemodynamic and metabolic connectivity derived from a novel FDG-fPET methodology (Jamadar et al. 2020a, 2021). We acquired FDG-fPET data with high temporal resolution of 16 s to measure glucose metabolic connectivity, and simultaneously acquired BOLD-fMRI data with a temporal resolution of 2.45 s from 26 participants. Participants completed a neuropsychological cognitive test battery, which resulted in 14 cognitive outcome variables indexing cognition across several domains (verbal memory, attention, and executive functioning). We used partial least squares (PLS) to map orthogonal patterns of brain-behavior relationships (McIntosh and Lobaugh 2004; Krishnan et al. 2011; McIntosh and Misic 2013). As evidence for both connectome-cognition views has been reported for fMRI data (e.g. Ziegler et al. 2013; Smith et al. 2015; Zimmermann et al. 2018; Goyal et al. 2020a), we undertook and exploratory analysis to investigate how hemodynamic and metabolic connectomes map onto cognition. However, as hemodynamic and metabolic connectomes have been shown to reveal distinct connectivity patterns (Jamadar et al. 2021), we hypothesized that both would provide a unique, but complementary insight, into the connectome–cognition relationship.

Materials and methods

All methods were reviewed by the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee, following the Australian National Statement of Ethical Conduct in Human Research (2007). Participants provided informed consent to participate in the study. Administration of ionizing radiation was approved by the Monash Health Principal Medical Physicist, following the Australian Radiation Protection and Nuclear Safety Agency Code of Practice (2005). Data from this study are available on OpenNeuro with the accession number ds002898. The Data Descriptor for this study with detailed acquisition and validation analyses is provided in Jamadar et al. (2020b), and results of the comparison between fPET, static PET, and BOLD-fMRI connectomes is presented in Jamadar et al. (2021).

Participants

Twenty-eight participants were recruited from the general community. An initial screening interview assessed that these participants had no history of hypertension or diabetes, had no neurological and psychiatric illness, or were on psychoactive medication affecting cognitive functioning or cerebral blood flow. Participants were also screened for claustrophobia, non-MR compatible implants, clinical or research PET scan in the past 12 months, and women were screened for current or suspected pregnancy. Prior to the scan, participants were directed to consume a high-protein/low-sugar diet for 24 h, fast for 6 h, and drink 2–6 glasses of water. Blood sugar level was measured using an Accu-Check Performa (model NC, Mannheim, Germany); all participants had blood sugar levels < 10 mmol/L with none exceeding 4.73 mmol/L. Two participants were excluded for further analyses, as one participant did not complete the full scan and the infusion pump failed for one participant. The total sample (n = 26, 77% females) were aged between 18 and 23 years (mean age = 19.50 years, SD = 1.36 years), right-handed (Oldfield 1971), English speakers (Supplementary Material, Table S1 for summary demographics). Although the sample consisted of significantly larger proportion of females, there were no significant (P > 0.5) gender-based differences observed in their demographics (Supplementary Material, Fig. S1).

Neuropsychological test battery

Prior to the scan, participants completed a test battery consisting of 6 neuropsychological test or scales assessing a wide range of cognitive functioning: (i) Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (Benedict et al. 1998), (ii) Symbol digit modalities test (Smith 1991), (iii) Stroop Neuropsychological Screening test (Trenerry et al. 1989), (iv) single-letter controlled oral word association test (COWAT; Ruff et al. 1996), (v) Color Trails Task (Reitan 1958), and the (vi) Center of Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale—Revised (Radloff 1977). Full details of the neuropsychological tests are provided in the Supplementary Material.

Overall, the 5 tests produced 14 cognitive outcome variables, which are summarized in Table 1 (Results). Relationships between the cognitive outcome variables were explored via Pearson’s correlations for continuous outcome variables and Spearman correlation for ordinal outcome variables. Relationships were considered significant at a false discovery rate corrected P value of 0.0062 (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995).

Table 1.

Cognitive outcome variables from the cognitive battery.

Cognitive test Cognitive domain Outcome variable Descriptive statistics
Mean SD Range
HVTL-R Verbal episodic memory Total Recall 26.96 3.72 19–33
Delayed Recall 9.73 1.61 7–12
Retention Rate 93.46% 10.93% 73–100%
Recognition Score 11.19 1.02 8–12
SDMT Processing Speed Total Score 56.19 5.72 45.45–68.18
Stroop Executive Function/Inhibition Control Incongruent Score 109.42 5.22 89–112
Incongruent RT 102.50 19.67 55–147
Congruent RT 53.58 10.11 39–89
Frontal Lobe Dysfunction Score 0.26 0.13 0.2–0.8
COWAT Executive Function/Verbal Fluency Total Score 42.50 11.92 21–68
Color Trails Executive Function/Cognitive Flexibility CT1 Score 30.88 8.59 17–50
CT2 Score 56.88 8.77 45–75
Interference Index 0.96 0.54 0.11–2.38
CESD-R Depression Total Score 9.12 8.54 0–31

Abbreviations: HVTL-R, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised; SDMT, The Symbol Digit Modalities Test; COWAT, The Controlled Oral Word Association Test; CESDR-R, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale—Revised; CT, Color Trails; RT, reaction time; SD, standard deviation.

Simultaneous MR-PET data acquisition

Following the completion of the cognitive battery (~30 min), participants underwent preparation for the simultaneous MR-PET scan. They were first cannulated in the vein in each forearm, and a 10-ml baseline blood sample was taken. For all participants, the left cannula was used for FDG infusion, and the right cannula was used for blood sampling.

Participants underwent a 95-min simultaneous MR-PET scan in a Siemens (Erlangen) Biograph 3-Tesla molecular MR scanner. Participants were positioned supine in the scanner bore with their head in a 16-channel radiofrequency head coil and were instructed to lie as still as possible with eyes open and think of nothing in particular. FDG (average dose 233 MBq) was infused over the course of the scan at a rate of 36 mL/h using a BodyGuard 323 MR-compatible infusion pump (Caesarea Medical Electronics, Caesarea, Israel). Infusion onset was locked to the onset of the PET scan.

Plasma radioactivity levels were measured throughout the duration of the scan. At 10-min postinfusion onset, a 10 mL of blood sample was taken from the right forearm using a vacutainer; the time of the 5-mL mark was noted for subsequent decay correction. Subsequent blood samples were taken at 10-min intervals for a total of 10 samples for the duration of the scan. Immediately following blood sampling, the sample was placed in a Heraeus Megafuge 16 centrifuge (ThermoFisher Scientific, Osterode, Germany) and spun at 2,000 rpm for 5 min; 1,000-μL plasma was pipetted, transferred to a counting tube, and placed in a well counter for 4 min. The count start time, total number of counts, and counts per minute were recorded for each sample. The average radioactivity concentration persistently increased over time with the lowest relative rate occurring at the end of the acquisition.

MRI preprocessing

For the structural T1 image, the brain was extracted in Freesurfer, then registered to MNI152 space using Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs). The gray matter, white matter, and brain cortex labels of the structural T1 image were segmented into 82 regions using Freesurfer with Desikan–Killiany Atlas (Diedrichsen et al. 2009).

The 6 blocks of EPI scans for all participants (a total of 1452 volumes) underwent a standard fMRI preprocessing pipeline. Specifically, all scans were brain extracted (FSL BET, Smith 2002), motion corrected (FSL MCFLIRT, Jenkinson et al. 2002), slice timing corrected (FSL, using Fourier-space time-series phase-shifting), and band-pass filtered (0.1 > Hz > 0.01) to remove low-frequency noise (FSL, Jenkinson et al. 2012), and spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM of 8 mm. Across subjects, the average mean framewise translation motion was 0.41 mm, maximum was 1.09 mm.

PET image reconstruction and preprocessing

The 5700-s list-mode PET data for each subject were binned into 356 3D sinogram frames each of 16-s interval. The attenuation for all required data was corrected via the pseudo-CT method (Burgos et al. 2014). Ordinary Poisson-Ordered Subset Expectation Maximization algorithm (3 iterations, 21 subsets) with point spread function correction was used to reconstruct 3D volumes from the sinogram frames. The reconstructed DICOM slices were converted to NIFTI format with size 344 × 344 × 127 (voxel size: 2.09 × 2.09 × 2.03 mm3) for each volume. A 5-mm FWHM Gaussian postfilter was applied to each 3D volume. All 3D volumes were temporally concatenated to form a 4D (344 × 344 × 127 × 356) NIFTI volume. A guided motion correction method using simultaneously acquired MRI was applied to correct the motion during the PET scan. We retained the 225 16-s volumes commencing from the 30-min timepoint, which matched the start of the BOLD-fMRI EPI acquisition, for further analyses.

The 225 PET volumes were motion corrected (FSL MCFLIRT, Jenkinson et al. 2002); the mean PET image was brain extracted and used to mask the 4D data. The fPET data were further processed using a spatiotemporal gradient filter to estimate the short-term change in glucose uptake from the cumulative glucose uptake that was measured (Jamadar et al. 2020a). The filter removed the accumulating effect of the radiotracer and other low-frequency components of the signal to isolate short-term resting-state fluctuations. This approach intrinsically adjusted for the mean signal while avoiding global-signal regression and other approaches that may create spurious anticorrelations in the data (Murphy and Fox 2017). Due to radiotracer dynamics, it was not expected that the fPET sensitivity would be uniform across the 60 min of the resting-state data acquisition. As the radiotracer accumulated in the brain, it was anticipated that the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of the PET image reconstruction would progressively improve. The spatiotemporal filter has been described extensively in our previous work (Jamadar et al. 2020b, 2021).

Functional and metabolic connectivity analyses

For fPET and fMRI, time series were extracted for each of the 82 regions of interest (ROIs) from the segmentation of the T1—weighted image, interpolated using an ANTs rigid registration (Avants et al. 2011). To construct a connectivity matrix, Pearson’s correlation coefficients were estimated between the timeseries from pairs of regions. This produced a per-subject per-modality 26 × 82 × 82 matrix corresponding to the 60 min of resting-state in the experimental protocol. For interpretation of connectivity patterns, the 82 ROIs were anatomically sorted as defined by the Desikan–Killiany Atlas (i.e. frontal, parietal, occipital, subcortical, and temporal; Diedrichsen et al. 2009).

Partial least squares analyses

We used PLS analyses to assess the multidimensional functional relationships between (i) the hemodynamic connectome and cognition, as well as (ii) the metabolic connectome and cognition (McIntosh and Lobaugh 2004; Krishnan et al. 2011; Fig. 1). PLS is an unsupervised multivariate machine learning technique that extracts the common information between 2 datasets (i.e. brain connectivity [X] and cognitive responses [Y]) by finding orthogonal sets of latent variables with maximum covariance, which reflect the linear combinations of the original data. In our case, the brain connectivity is either the hemodynamic connectome [XF] or metabolic connectome [XM]. Prior to the application of PLS, the upper triangle of the hemodynamic connectivity matrix and the metabolic connectivity matrix (i.e. 3,321 connections, respectively) were vectorized and stacked as participant by connection resulting in matrices sized 26 × 3,321, respectively. The cognition matrix was sized at 26 × 14. These subject-specific the hemodynamic connectivity matrix (XF), metabolic connectivity (XM) matrices, and the cognitive response matrix (Y) were subsequently z-scored column-wise.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Overview of partial least squares analyses. Two partial least squares analyses were performed on both brain connectivity data sets (i.e. hemodynamic connectivity and metabolic connectivity) separately. The brain connectivity matrices were first sorted by stacking the upper triangle elements from each participants’ matrices. The rows of the brain connectivity and cognitive matrices correspond to participants and the columns correspond to either the brain connections or cognitive scores. The covariance between the brain connectivity and cognition matrices was computed across participants, resulting in a rectangular connectivity-cognition covariance matrix. This covariance matrix was then subjected to singular value decomposition.

First, the correlation matrix between the brain connectivity (X) and cognition matrix (Y) is computed R = XTY and singular value decomposition (Eckart and Young 1936) is next applied to that connectivity–cognition matrix, resulting in R = USVT. The outcome of the singular value decomposition is a set of mutually orthogonal latent variables, whereby U and V are orthogonal matrices consisting of left and right singular vectors and S is a diagonal matrix of singular values. The number of latent variables is equal to the rank of the covariance matrix R, which is the smaller of its dimensions. Every latent variable is associated with (i) a singular value (diagonal elements of S) indicating the correlation explained by that latent variable, (ii) a vector of singular values U, which represent the brain saliences, and (iii) a vector of singular values V, which represent the behavioral saliences. The behavioral saliences indicate how strong each one of the cognitive variables contributes to the brain-design correlation explained by a particular latent variable. Similarly, the brain saliences express how strong every connection contributes to the brain-design correlation explained by a particular latent variable. The projection of every subject’s original connectivity (in X) onto the multivariate brain salience pattern (in U) results in brain scores Lx = XU. Brain scores measure the similarity of a subject’s individual brain data with the salient brain pattern. Similarly, cognitive scores can be computed by Ly = YV, which represent a projection of every subject’s design variable onto the respective design saliences. Finally, brain loadings (or weights) were computed as the Pearson’s correlation between the brain connectivity matrix and the PLS analysis-derived brain scores. Similarly, cognitive loadings were computed as the Pearson’s correlations between cognitive variables and the PLS analysis-derived cognition scores across the cohort. Loadings can be interpreted as indexing the degree contribution of each variable to the PLS analysis-derived latent variable. Loadings are only interpreted for significant latent variables.

In addition, we ran a separate PLS analysis, during which the fMRI and fPET matrices where concatenated before single value decomposition. This analysis assesses the similarity of fMRI and fPET connectivity in relation to cognition. As our main objective is to assess differences in addition to similarities of both modalities with respect to cognition, these results are reported in the supplementary analysis (Supplementary Material, Fig. S3).

The significance of each latent variable is assessed via permutation tests (5,000 iterations) of the singular values from the singular value decomposition of the brain and cognition matrices and the reliability of each connectivity estimate to the latent variable is assessed via bootstrap resampling (5,000 iterations). The reliability of the loading of each connection onto the brain-cognition relationship in each latent variable is established via bootstrap (5,000 iterations). A connection with a positive bootstrapped loading contributes positively and reliably to the brain-cognition correlation obtained for that latent variable, whereas a connection with a negative high bootstrapped loading contributes negatively and reliably to the brain-cognition relationship. Bootstrapping is also used to construct 95% confidence intervals on the brain-cognition correlations.

Hemodynamic versus metabolic connectivity in relation to cognition

To compare the brain connections that contributed to the hemodynamic connectome-cognition relationship and metabolic connectome-cognition relationship, the scalar product between the brain saliences (U) resulting from each PLS were computed for significant latent variables. Similarly, to compare the cognitive responses that contributed to the metabolic connectome-cognition relationship and functional connectome-relationship, we calculated the dot product between the behavioral saliences (V) that resulted from both PLS analyses. A scalar product of 0 suggests no overlap across modalities and a scalar product of 1 suggests strong overlap across modalities (i.e. fPET and fMRI). Finally, to identify the anatomical location of similar brain loadings across modalities, Pearson’s correlations were performed on the brain loadings matrices of both modalities. This results in a matrix of cosine similarity between the 2 modalities.

Results

We first provide an overview of the cognitive outcome variables of the neuropsychological test battery. Next, we describe the hemodynamic (i.e. fMRI functional connectivity) and metabolic connectivity (i.e. FDG-fPET functional connectivity) across participants. Finally, we show how both connectivity maps relate to cognition and quantify their differences.

Cognitive measures

Participants completed a neuropsychological test battery that described distinct cognitive domains across 14 outcome variables (Table 1). Most cognitive variables correlated significantly within each cognitive test, but not across tests (Fig. 2) suggesting each cognitive test measured distinct cognitive domains. Chronbach’s alpha revealed acceptable to good internal consistency of the HVTL (0.743), Stroop (0.835), and Color Trails (0.597) scores. An exception was that individuals with higher depression scores on the CESD-R were overall slower during congruent trials of the Stroop task (i.e. reading color names; r(24) = 0.54, 95% CI [0.28, 0.83], P < 0.05). Also, performance during the Symbol Digits Modality test, correlated negatively with performance during the second part of the Color Trail test (CT2 score) (r(24) = −0.48, 95% CI [−0.83, −0.28], P < 0.05). For the PLS analyses, the total score from the Stroop congruent trials was removed as there was no variability across participants, as all participants received the maximum score of 112.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Correlation matrix of 14 cognitive outcome variables obtained from the neuropsychological test battery. Significant relationships are indicated with P > 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995). Pearson’s correlation was performed for continuous and spearman correlation was performed for ordinal data. Positive relationships (0 ≤ r ≤ 1) are indicated in blue and negative relationships (0 > r ≤ −1) are indicated in red. Circle size corresponds to the absolute size of the correlation coefficient as indicated by the blue-red colored scale. Abbreviations: HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modality Test; COWAT, Controlled Oral Word Association Test; CESD-R, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale—Revised; CT1, Colour Trails 1; CT2, Colour Trails 2; RT, reaction time.

Hemodynamic and metabolic connectivity

The hemodynamic and metabolic connectomes have been reported previously (Jamadar et al. 2020b, 2021) and are included here for completeness. The hemodynamic connectome (Fig. 3a) showed medium to strong connectivity within most anatomical subdivisions, both within and between hemispheres. The strongest hemodynamic connectivity (r ≥ 0.7) was found bilaterally in the frontal, parietal, and occipital anatomical subdivisions. A number of strong long-range connections included frontoparietal, parieto-occipital, and temporoparietal regional connectivity. These long-range connections were evident both within and between hemispheres but were of smaller magnitude than the short-range and homotopic connections. Subcortical and orbitofrontal regions were the least interconnected regions in the BOLD-fMRI data (r ≥ 0.2).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Hemodynamic and metabolic connectivity at rest. a) The hemodynamic (i.e. fMRI functional connectivity) was thresholded from 0 < r < 0.76. b) The metabolic connectivity (i.e. fPET functional connectivity) was thresholded from 0 < 0 < 0.15. Abbreviations: L, left; R, right.

The metabolic connectome (Fig. 3b) showed the strongest connectivity (r ≥ 0.15) within the frontoparietal areas, which was more apparent within than between hemispheres. Left–right homotopic connectivity was not visually apparent for subcortical, temporal, and occipital cortices.

Partial least squares results

The PLS analyses applied to the fMRI and fPET data sets separately identified one significant latent variable that described the relationship between the hemodynamic connectivity and cognition, and one significant latent variable that described the relationship between the metabolic connectivity and cognition.

Hemodynamic connectivity and cognition relationship

The PLS analyses revealed that one latent variable captures the relationship between hemodynamic connectivity pattern and cognition (67.13% of total covariance; singular value = 39.43, P = 0.003, permutation testing with 5,000 iterations). The distribution of cognitive loadings revealed that each cognitive variable within each test (Table 1) in general loaded uniformly in their direction onto the latent variable (Fig. 4ai). For example, all subscales of the HVLT loaded negatively onto the latent variable, and all subscales of the Stroop loaded positively. Bootstrapped confidence intervals revealed that 3 cognitive variables were expressed the strongest by the latent variable: participant’s depression score (CESD-R score; loading = 0.61, 95% bootstrapped CI [0.15,0.82]), inhibition control speed (i.e. the response time in naming a font color of an incongruent word during the Stroop task; loading = 0.56, 95% bootstrapped CI [0.18,0.57]), and memory retention (HVTL retention score; loading = −0.71, 95% bootstrapped CI [−0.34, −0.72). The hemodynamic connections all loaded strongly positively onto the latent variable (loading > 0.67; Fig. 4aii). The strongest loadings (r ≥ 0.85) were found bilaterally in the frontal and parietal anatomical subdivisions. Thresholding the connectivity matrix at the 99th percentile (Jamadar et al. 2020a), revealed that 41.4% of the strongest connections were part of the frontal cortex (e.g. superiorfrontal, middlefrontal, parstriangularis, parsopercularis, medialorbitofrontal, precentral, and rostralanteriorcingulate), and 24.1% of the total strongest connections were part of the parietal cortex (e.g. supramarginal, posteriorcingulate, precuneus, and isthmuscingulate). The subcortical areas contained (e.g. caudate, hippocampus, insula, and putamen) and the temporal cortex contained 17.2% of the total strongest connections (i.e. superiortemporal, fusiform, and banks), respectively. There were no strong connections in the occipital cortex. Interpreting the cognition loadings together with the brain loadings, the PLS analysis revealed that higher depression, higher inhibitory control speed and lower memory retention are associated with higher hemodynamic connectivity particularly in the frontal and parietal anatomical interhemispheric subdivisions.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Partial least squares analysis results showing the a) cognition-hemodynamic connectivity relationship, and b) cognition-metabolic connectivity relationship. (i) Cognition loadings for significant latent variable. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals from bootstrap resampling (5,000 iterations). (ii) connectivity loadings for significant latent variable. The circular plot shows the strongest connections thresholded at the 99th percentile.

Metabolic connectivity and cognition relationship

The PLS analyses revealed that one latent variable captures the relationship between metabolic connectivity pattern and cognition (30.77% of total covariance; singular value = 24.42, P = 0.04, permutation testing with 5,000 iterations). The distribution of cognitive loadings revealed that each cognitive variable within each test (Table 1) loaded generally uniformly and in their direction onto the latent variable (Fig. 4bi). The direction of the cognitive loadings was similarly expressed by the latent variable describing the hemodynamic connectivity–cognition relationship. Bootstrapped confidence intervals revealed that all outcome variables of the Stroop task, measuring executive functioning/inhibitory control, loaded strongly positively onto the latent variable. Furthermore, the COWAT score loading = −0.58, 95% bootstrapped CI [−0.15, −0.79]), measuring executive functioning/verbal fluency was also expressed strongly negatively by the latent variable. The metabolic connections loaded mostly positively (loading >  0.54) onto the latent variable (Fig. 3bii). However, there were also a few connections that loaded negatively, although very weakly (loading < −0.15; Supplementary Material, Fig. S2). These negative loadings were distributed across the brain. The strongest loadings (r ≥ 0.54) all loaded positively and were found predominantly in the frontal and parietal anatomical subdivisions. Thresholding the connectivity matrix at the 99th percentile, revealed that 50% of the strongest connections were part of the frontal cortex (e.g. frontal pole, superior frontal, middle frontal, lateral orbitofrontal, caudal anterior cingulate, and precentral) and 33% of the total strongest connections were part of the parietal cortex (e.g. postcentral, posterior cingulate, precuneus, inferior parietal, superior parietal, and supramarginal). The occipital cortex contained only 8.3% of the total strongest connections (i.e. lateral occipital) and the subcortical (i.e. thalamus) and temporal cortex (i.e. temporal pole) only 4.2%, respectively. Interpreting the cognition loadings together with the brain loadings, the PLS analysis revealed that higher inhibitory control and lower verbal fluency are associated with predominantly higher metabolic connectivity particularly in the frontal and parietal anatomical subdivisions.

Differences in hemodynamic-cognition and metabolic-cognition relationship

To compare the connections that contributed to the cognition-metabolic connectivity relationship and those that contributed to the cognition–hemodynamic relationship, we computed the scalar dot product between the brain saliences (U) of the significant latent variable from both PLS analyses. A cosine value of 1 means that the saliences are identical and 0 means orthogonality or no correlation. This analysis revealed a cosine similarity of 0.23 (i.e. weak relationship) indicating that the effects of the PLS for the hemodynamic–cognition relationship differed from the effects from the metabolic–cognition relationship. This was confirmed by the similarity matrix of each relationship’s brain loadings, showing overall little overlap across the 2 modalities with the most correlation coefficients ranging between −0.1 and 0.1 (Fig. 5). The highest similarity between the 2 modalities (r > 0.4) was evident for the frontal and parietal cortex for both hemispheres. The loading matrices were anticorrelated (loading < −0.3) for occipital and temporal subdivisions.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Similarity between the metabolic and hemodynamic connectivity loadings. a) Similarity matrix by brain area. b) Histogram of Pearson’s correlation coefficients indicating the frequency of similarity strength.

Discussion

The present study used simultaneous resting-state FDG-PET/fMRI to investigate, for the first time, how spatially distant synchronous brain signals measured via cerebrovascular hemodynamic responses (i.e. fMRI; hemodynamic connectivity) and glucose uptake (i.e. FDG-PET; metabolic connectivity) relate to a range of cognitive functions. Our simultaneous fPET and fMRI acquisition at a high temporal resolution enabled multimodal within-subject analyses of resting-state brain activity without the confound of intra-individual differences (e.g. fatigue, nutrient intake, and blood chemistry) that occur when measuring both modalities not simultaneously. We applied PLS (McIntosh and Lobaugh 2004; Krishnan et al. 2011) to extract latent variables capturing the maximum covariance between hemodynamic and metabolic connectivity matrices with 14 cognitive measures, including episodic memory, processing speed, executive functioning, and depression. Results revealed that one latent variable captured the relationship between hemodynamic connectivity and cognition and one latent variable captured the relationship between metabolic connectivity and cognition. The cognitive battery was indexing orthogonal cognitive domains. This supports the global connectome-cognition view, which states that a global cognitive factor is accounted by a single set of connections (Smith et al. 2015; Goyal et al. 2020b). In contrast, our results do not support the domain-specific connectome-cognition view, which would suggest that distinct sets of connections are required to support cognition (e.g. Ziegler et al. 2013; Zimmermann et al. 2018).

Although cognition was expressed globally by one set of connectivity-cognition latent variable, the specificity of how hemodynamic and metabolic connectivity related to cognition varied. For both modalities the frontoparietal anatomical subdivisions related the strongest to cognition (Fig. 4), however for hemodynamic responses this network expressed executive functioning, episodic memory, and depression, whereas for metabolic responses this network exclusively expressed executive functioning. This is compatible with the argument that metabolic and hemodynamic connectivity provide unique, but complementary insights into cognition (Mier and Mier 2015; Yakushev et al. 2017; Chen et al. 2018; Sala and Perani 2019; Hahn et al. 2020; Jamadar et al. 2021).

A global set of metabolic and hemodynamic connections map onto cognition

Our results support the contention that the overall wiring of a connectivity network has a domain-general role in cognition. Critically, this domain-general characteristic is shared by both the metabolic and hemodynamic processes, indicating that it is a shared characteristic across multiple physiological levels of the human connectome. This finding is in line with classical theoretical proposals that brain networks exhibit a flexible architecture with their functional network assignment to adaptively process changing cognitive demands (Dehaene et al. 1998; Duncan 2001; Miller and Cohen 2001). Flexible, domain-general interactions likely allow different information to become quickly integrated and exchanged, leading to a dominant pattern of co-activation across different cognitive states.

In our results, the frontoparietal anatomical subdivisions emerged as the dominant regions supporting a domain-general role in cognition. The frontoparietal anatomical network was previously coined a multi-demand system that is co-activated when performing a diverse range of cognitive demanding tasks, including selective attention, working memory, task switching, response inhibition, conflict monitoring, learning, or problem solving (Chein and Schneider 2005; Cole  et al. 2014; Assem et al. 2020; reviewed by Marek and Dosenbach 2018). In line with this general systems role to support information integration and exchange that mediates cognitive operations, damage to the frontoparietal network has been reported to be associated with disorganized behavior and decreased fluid intelligence (Hearne et al. 2016). Further, this system has been shown to play domain-general protective role against mental health symptoms such as depression (Schultz et al. 2018).

Metabolic and functional connectivity relate to distinct aspects of cognition

The behavioral variables loaded uniformly on the latent variables for the metabolic connectivity-cognition and hemodynamic connectivity-cognition pattern but differed in their loading strengths. The fPET metabolic and BOLD-fMRI hemodynamic connectivity had the strongest network configuration in frontoparietal cortices. However, this network seems to relate to distinct cognitive functions for both imaging modalities. Specifically, the resting-state hemodynamic connectivity in this network was positively associated with inhibition, depression and negatively with memory retention. The resting-state metabolic connectivity in this network in turn was associated positively with executive functioning and inhibition; and negatively with executive functioning and verbal fluency.

The cognition-connectivity pattern revealed by fMRI is in strong accord with numerous previous fMRI studies revealing the brain mechanisms underlying cognition. For example, the frontoparietal network, particularly involving the anterior cingulate cortex, precuneus or posterior cingulate cortex, has been shown to be a core network involved in cognitive control monitoring and the facilitation of conflict resolution during a task (Botvinick et al. 2004; Shenhav et al. 2013). In addition, this flexible and domain-general hub has also been involved in emotional processing, clinical symptoms such as depression (Schultz et al. 2018), and memory (Wallis et al. 2015). These findings are corroborated in the cognition-connectivity patterns observed in this study. In addition to frontoparietal co-activation, the hemodynamic connectivity loadings were also prevalent in cortico-cortical networks, for example involving the insula or hippocampus. The insula is strongly interconnected with frontal and parietal areas supporting its role as a major multimodal network hub that underpins cognition, memory, and emotional processing (Menon and Uddin 2010; Contreras et al. 2012). The hippocampus supports a vast array of memory functions, such as retaining information across delays (Jeneson et al. 2011; Müller et al. 2018).

In contrast to the hemodynamic connectivity-cognition relationship, the latent variable expressing the metabolic connectivity-cognition relationship was strongly localized in the frontoparietal areas and associated exclusively with executive functioning. Previous studies have reported that resting-state metabolic connectivity is particularly evident in frontoparietal areas (Yakushev et al. 2017; Shokri-Kojori et al. 2019; Hahn et al. 2020). Here, we extend these finding by observing that the co-activation at rest is behaviourally relevant in supporting executive control. We note the existence of a small proportion of negative connections (only 25.22% of connections) that contributed to the cognition-metabolic connectivity relationship. These negative cognition-connectivity associations can reflect either reduced positive associations or anticorrelations (Hearne et al. 2016). There is also the possibility that these scattered negative loadings (Supplementary Material, Fig. S2) might be a preprocessing epiphenomenon (Jamadar et al. 2020b). Future research is needed to investigate whether the small fraction of negative associations in the metabolic connectome are behaviourally meaningful.

The apparent specificity of the cognition-metabolic connectivity relationship, i.e. the exclusive focus on frontoparietal cortices, may be indicative of signal artifacts in either the FDG-fPET or BOLD-fMRI, i.e. reduced signal-to-noise or non-neuronal confounders, respectively. The reduced sensitivity of the FDG-fPET signal must be noted as the processing pipeline, including filters and models, are immature compared to the years of advanced development that has been dedicated to BOLD-fMRI signal processing as reported in the scientific literature. Conversely, this advancement has potentially led to the identification of non-neuronal confounders and spatial artifacts in BOLD-fMRI that are not present in the FDG-fPET signal, such as magnetic field and hemoglobin-based artifacts (Liu 2017; Ward et al. 2020). The disparity in the results from the 2 modalities is augurs well for gaining deeper insights to improve our understanding of cognition-brain connectivity relationships.

Although our sample size exceeds numbers compared to previous fMRI-fPET studies, we acknowledge that our sample is small compared to previous, similar studies (e.g. Rischka et al. 2018). Our analyses include reliability assessments that provide bootstrapped confidence intervals of our effects. Further, we were able to replicate the spatial pattern of connections in the fMRI (Shenhav et al. 2013) and fPET (Yakushev et al. 2017; Shokri-Kojori et al. 2019; Hahn et al. 2020) suggesting that our findings are robust. Future studies might complement our findings with complementary measures to FDG-fPET that provide a more direct measure of neural activity compared to BOLD fMRI, such as functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy (Stanley and Raz 2018). This technique can reliably quantify in vivo concentration of metabolites such as glutamate or GABA levels in cortical and subcortical brain areas and is less sensitive to vascular changes compared to BOLD fMRI.

In conclusion, this study is an important step in revealing that cognition is supported by a domain-general hemodynamic and metabolic processing. Crucially, the metabolic processes appear to be more spatially defined by frontoparietal areas, whereas the hemodynamic processes throughout the frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital areas collectively support cognition. These findings demonstrate the unique advantages that simultaneous FDG-PET/fMRI has to provide a comprehensive understanding of the neural mechanisms that underpin cognition, and highlights the importance of multimodality imaging in cognitive neuroscience research.

Funding

This work was supported by an Australian Research Council (ARC) Linkage Project (LP170100494) that includes financial support from Siemens Healthineers. Jamadar is supported by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council fellowship (APP1174164). Egan, Ward, and Jamadar are supported by the ARC Centre of Excellence for Integrative Brain Function (CE140100007).

Conflict of interest statement: None declared.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Data and code availability

The dataset containing the demographic, fMRI, PET, T1 structural, and gradient field maps is freely available in BIDS format (Gorgolewski et al. 2016) from the OpenNeuro repository (http://openneuro.org) with the accession number ds002898 (Jamadar et al. 2020b). Data and code that generated the results is available on the Open Science Network (DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/DQN5S).

Supplementary Material

CC_RevSupplementaryMaterial_KV_bhac150

Contributor Information

Katharina Voigt, School of Psychological Sciences Turner and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, 18 Innovation Walk, 3800 Clayton VIC, Australia; Monash Biomedical Imaging, Monash University, 770 Blackburn Road, 3800 Clayton VIC, Australia.

Emma X Liang, Monash Biomedical Imaging, Monash University, 770 Blackburn Road, 3800 Clayton VIC, Australia.

Bratislav Misic, McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital, 3801 University Street Montréal, Quebec H3A 2B4, Canada.

Phillip G D Ward, Monash Biomedical Imaging, Monash University, 770 Blackburn Road, 3800 Clayton VIC, Australia.

Gary F Egan, School of Psychological Sciences Turner and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, 18 Innovation Walk, 3800 Clayton VIC, Australia; Monash Biomedical Imaging, Monash University, 770 Blackburn Road, 3800 Clayton VIC, Australia.

Sharna D Jamadar, School of Psychological Sciences Turner and Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, 18 Innovation Walk, 3800 Clayton VIC, Australia; Monash Biomedical Imaging, Monash University, 770 Blackburn Road, 3800 Clayton VIC, Australia.

References

  1. Assem  M, Glasser  MF, Van Essen  DC, Duncan  J. A domain-general cognitive core defined in multimodally parcellated human cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2020:30:4361–4380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Avants  BB, Tustison  NJ, Song  G, Cook  PA, Klein  A, Gee  JC. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage. 2011:54:2033–2044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Avena-Koenigsberger  A, Misic  B, Sporns  O. Communication dynamics in complex brain networks. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018:19:17–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Báez-Yáñez  MG, Siero  JC, Petridou  N. A statistical 3D model of the human cortical vasculature to compute the hemodynamic fingerprint of the BOLD fMRI signal. bioarxiv. 2020. 10.1101/2020.10.05.326512. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barack  DL, Krakauer  JW. Two views on the cognitive brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021:22:359–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benedict  RHB, Schretlen  D, Groninger  L, Brandt  J. Hopkins verbal learning test – revised: normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998:12:43–55. [Google Scholar]
  7. Benjamini  Y, Hochberg  Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol. 1995:57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  8. Biswal  B, Yetkin  FZ, Haughton  VM, Hyde  JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1995:34:537–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Botvinick  MM, Cohen  JD, Carter  CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004:8:539–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burgos  N, Cardoso  MJ, Thielemans  K, Modat  M, Pedemonte  S, Dickson  J, Barnes  A, Ahmed  R, Mahoney  CJ, Schott  JM, et al.  Attenuation correction synthesis for hybrid PET-MR scanners: application to brain studies. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2014:33:2332–2341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chein  JM, Schneider  W. Neuroimaging studies of practice-related change: fMRI and meta-analytic evidence of a domain-general control network for learning. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005:25:607–623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen  Z, Jamadar  SD, Li  S, Sforazzini  F, Baran  J, Ferris  N, Shah  NJ, Egan  GF. From simultaneous to synergistic MR-PET brain imaging: a review of hybrid MR-PET imaging methodologies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018:39:5126–5144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cole  MW, Repovs  G, Anticevic  A. The frontoparietal control system: a central role in mental health. Neurosci Rev J Bringing Neurobiol Neurol Psychiatry. 2014:20:652–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Contreras  M, Billeke  P, Vicencio  S, Madrid  C, Perdomo  G, González  M, Torrealba  F. A role for the insular cortex in long-term memory for context-evoked drug craving in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012:37:2101–2108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dehaene  S, Naccache  L, Le Clec’H  G, Koechlin  E, Mueller  M, Dehaene-Lambertz  G, van de  Moortele  P-F, Le Bihan  D. Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature. 1998:395:597–600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Diedrichsen  J, Balsters  JH, Flavell  J, Cussans  E, Ramnani  N. A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage. 2009:46:39–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Duncan  J. An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001:2:820–829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Eckart C, Young G. The approximation of one matrix by another of lower rank. Psychometrika. 1936:1:211–218. 10.1007/BF02288367. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gorgolewski  KJ, Auer  T, Calhoun  VD, Craddock  RC, Das  S, Duff  EP, Flandin  G, Ghosh  SS, Glatard  T, Halchenko  YO, et al.  The brain imaging data structure, a format for organizing and describing outputs of neuroimaging experiments. Sci Data. 2016:3:160044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Goyal  N, Moraczewski  D, Thomas  AG. Computationally replicating the Smith et al. (2015) positive-negative mode linking functional connectivity and subject measures. In: bioRxiv; 2020a. 2020 April 23.058313
  21. Goyal  N, Moraczewski  D, Thomas  AG. Computationally replicating the Smith et al. (2015) positive-negative mode linking functional connectivity and subject measures. In: bioRxiv. 2020 April 23.058313; 2020b
  22. Hahn  A, Breakspear  M, Rischka  L, Wadsak  W, Godbersen  GM, Pichler  V, Michenthaler  P, Vanicek  T, Hacker  M, Kasper  S, et al.  Reconfiguration of functional brain networks and metabolic cost converge during task performance. elife. 2020:9:e52443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hearne  LJ, Mattingley  JB, Cocchi  L. Functional brain networks related to individual differences in human intelligence at rest. Sci Rep. 2016:6:32328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Huber  L, Tse  D, Wiggins  CJ, Uludağ  K, Kashyap  S, Jangraw  DC, Bandettini  PA, Poser  BA, Ivanov  D. Ultra-high resolution blood volume fMRI and BOLD fMRI in humans at 9.4 T: capabilities and challenges. NeuroImage. 2018:178:769–779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jamadar  SD, Ward  PGD, Close  TG, Fornito  A, Premaratne  M, O’Brien  K, Stäb  D, Chen  Z, Shah  NJ, Egan  GF. Simultaneous BOLD-fMRI and constant infusion FDG-PET data of the resting human brain. Sci Data. 2020a:7:363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Jamadar  SD, Ward  PGD, Thomas  GC, Alex  F, Premaratne  M, O’Brien  K, Staeb  D, Chen  Z, Shah  J, Egan  GF. Monash rsPET-MRI. OpenNeuro. 2020b. 10.18112/openneuro.ds002898.v1.1.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Jamadar  SD, Ward  PGD, Liang  EX, Orchard  ER, Chen  Z, Egan  GF. Metabolic and hemodynamic resting-state connectivity of the human brain: a high-temporal resolution simultaneous BOLD-fMRI and FDG-fPET multimodality study. Cereb Cortex. 2021:31:2855–2867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Jeneson  A, Mauldin  KN, Hopkins  RO, Squire  LR. The role of the hippocampus in retaining relational information across short delays: the importance of memory load. Learn Mem. 2011:18:301–305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jenkinson  M, Bannister  P, Brady  M, Smith  S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002:17:825–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jenkinson  M, Beckmann  CF, Behrens  TEJ, Woolrich  MW, Smith  SM. FSL. NeuroImage 20 Years of fMRI. 2012:62:782–790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Judenhofer  MS, Wehrl  HF, Newport  DF, Catana  C, Siegel  SB, Becker  M, Thielscher  A, Kneilling  M, Lichy  MP, Eichner  M, et al.  Simultaneous PET-MRI: a new approach for functional and morphological imaging. Nat Med. 2008:14:459–465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Krishnan  A, Williams  LJ, McIntosh  AR, Abdi  H. Partial least squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: a tutorial and review. NeuroImage. 2011:56:455–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Liu  TT. Reprint of “noise contributions to the fMRI signal: an overview”. NeuroImage. 2017:154:4–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Marek  S, Dosenbach  NUF. The frontoparietal network: function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2018:20:133–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McIntosh  AR, Lobaugh  NJ. Partial least squares analysis of neuroimaging data: applications and advances. NeuroImage. 2004:23:S250–S263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McIntosh  AR, Misic  B. Multivariate statistical analyses for neuroimaging data. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013:64:499–525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Menon  V, Uddin  LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010:214:655–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mier  W, Mier  D. Advantages in functional imaging of the brain. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015:9:249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Miller  EK, Cohen  JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001:24:167–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Müller  NCJ, Konrad  BN, Kohn  N, Muñoz-López  M, Czisch  M, Fernández  G, Dresler  M. Hippocampal–caudate nucleus interactions support exceptional memory performance. Brain Struct Funct. 2018:223:1379–1389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Murphy  K, Fox  MD. Towards a consensus regarding global signal regression for resting state functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage. 2017:154:169–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Oldfield  RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971:9:97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Radloff  LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977:1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  44. Raichle  ME. A brief history of human brain mapping. Trends Neurosci. 2009:32:118–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Raichle  ME. The restless brain. Brain Connect. 2011:1:3–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ralph M.  Reitan, 1958.  Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. [WWW Document]. 2021. 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271 (last accessed 1 November 2021). [DOI]
  47. Rischka  L, Gryglewski  G, Pfaff  S, Vanicek  T, Hienert  M, Klöbl  M, Hartenbach  M, Haug  A, Wadsak  W, Mitterhauser  M, et al.  Reduced task durations in functional PET imaging with [18F]FDG approaching that of functional MRI. NeuroImage. 2018:181:323–330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Ruff  RM, Light  RH, Parker  SB, Levin  HS. Benton controlled oral word association test: reliability and updated norms. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1996:11:329–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sala  A, Perani  D. Brain molecular connectivity in neurodegenerative diseases: recent advances and new perspectives using positron emission tomography. Front Neurosci. 2019:13:617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schultz  DH, Ito  T, Solomyak  LI, Chen  RH, Mill  RD, Anticevic  A, Cole  MW. Global connectivity of the fronto-parietal cognitive control network is related to depression symptoms in the general population. Netw Neurosci. 2018:17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Shenhav  A, Botvinick  MM, Cohen  JD. The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron. 2013:79:217–240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Shokri-Kojori  E, Tomasi  D, Alipanahi  B, Wiers  CE, Wang  G-J, Volkow  ND. Correspondence between cerebral glucose metabolism and BOLD reveals relative power and cost in human brain. Nat Commun. 2019:10:690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Sleight  E, Stringer  MS, Marshal  I, Wardlaw  JM, Thrippleton  MJ. Cerebrovascular reactivity measurement using magnetic resonance imaging: a systematic review. Front Physiol. 2021:12:643468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Smith  A. Symbol digit modalities test. Los Angeles: CA: Western Psychological Services; 1991 [Google Scholar]
  55. Smith  SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002:17:143–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Smith  SM, Nichols  TE, Vidaurre  D, Winkler  AM, Behrens  TEJ, Glasser  MF, Ugurbil  K, Barch  DM, Van Essen  DC, Miller  KL. A positive-negative mode of population covariation links brain connectivity, demographics and behavior. Nat Neurosci. 2015:18:1565–1567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sporns  O. The human connectome: origins and challenges. NeuroImage, Mapping the Connectome. 2013:80:53–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sporns  O, Tononi  G, Kötter  R. The human connectome: a structural description of the human brain. PLoS Comput Biol. 2005:1:e42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Stanley  JA, Raz  N. Functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy: the "new" MRS for cognitive neuroscience and psychiatry research. Front Psych. 2018:9:76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Trenerry  MR, Crosson  B, DeBoe  J, Leber  WR. Stroop neuropsychological screening test manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1989 [Google Scholar]
  61. Villien  M, Wey  H-Y, Mandeville  JB, Catana  C, Polimeni  JR, Sander  CY, Zürcher  NR, Chonde  DB, Fowler  JS, Rosen  BR, et al.  Dynamic functional imaging of brain glucose utilization using fPET-FDG. NeuroImage. 2014:100:192–199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Wallis  G, Stokes  M, Cousijn  H, Woolrich  M, Nobre  AC. Frontoparietal and Cingulo-opercular networks play dissociable roles in control of working memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015:27:2019–2034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ward  PGD, Orchard  ER, Oldham  S, Arnatkevičiūtė  A, Sforazzini  F, Fornito  A, Storey  E, Egan  GF, Jamadar  SD. Individual differences in haemoglobin concentration influence bold fMRI functional connectivity and its correlation with cognition. NeuroImage. 2020:221:117196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Yakushev  I, Drzezga  A, Habeck  C. Metabolic connectivity: methods and applications. Curr Opin Neurol. 2017:30:677–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Ziegler  G, Dahnke  R, Winkler  AD, Gaser  C. Partial least squares correlation of multivariate cognitive abilities and local brain structure in children and adolescents. NeuroImage. 2013:82:284–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Zimmermann  J, Griffiths  JD, McIntosh  AR. Unique mapping of structural and functional connectivity on cognition. J Neurosci. 2018:38:9658–9667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

CC_RevSupplementaryMaterial_KV_bhac150

Data Availability Statement

The dataset containing the demographic, fMRI, PET, T1 structural, and gradient field maps is freely available in BIDS format (Gorgolewski et al. 2016) from the OpenNeuro repository (http://openneuro.org) with the accession number ds002898 (Jamadar et al. 2020b). Data and code that generated the results is available on the Open Science Network (DOI 10.17605/OSF.IO/DQN5S).


Articles from Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES