Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Patient Educ Couns. 2023 Jan 2;109:107620. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2022.107620

Question Prompt Lists and Caregiver Question Asking in Pediatric Specialty Appointments: A Randomized Controlled Trial

Margaret Waltz a, Haoyang Yan b, R Jean Cadigan a, Courtney Canter c, Lizzy Bain d, Jeannette T Bensen e, Carol Conway f, Chad Haldeman-Englert g, Laura Farnan h, Ann Katherine M Foreman i, Tracey L Grant i, Barbara Leach j, Feng-Chang Lin k, Madeline Mahla i, Julianne M O’Daniel i, Suzanne C O’Neill l, Gerri Smith m, Bradford C Powell i, Jonathan S Berg i, Christine M Rini b,n
PMCID: PMC9931668  NIHMSID: NIHMS1865148  PMID: 36689884

Abstract

Objective:

Question prompt lists (QPLs) have been effective at increasing patient involvement and question asking in medical appointments, which is critical for shared decision making. We investigated whether pre-visit preparation (PVP), including a QPL, would increase question asking among caregivers of pediatric patients with undiagnosed, suspected genetic conditions.

Methods:

Caregivers were randomized to receive the PVP before their appointment (n = 59) or not (control, n = 53). Appointments were audio-recorded. Transcripts were analyzed to determine questions asked.

Results:

Caregivers in the PVP group asked more questions (MeanPVP = 4.36, SDPVP = 4.66 vs. Meancontrol = 2.83, SDcontrol = 3.03, p = 0.045), including QPL questions (MeanPVP = 1.05, SDPVP = 1.39 vs. Meancontrol = 0.36, SDcontrol = 0.81, p = 0.002). Caregivers whose child had insurance other than Medicaid in the PVP group asked more total and QPL questions than their counterparts in the control group (ps = 0.005 and 0.002); there was no intervention effect among caregivers of children with Medicaid or no insurance (ps = 0.775 and 0.166).

Conclusion:

The PVP increased question asking but worked less effectively among traditionally underserved groups. Additional interventions, including provider-focused efforts, may be needed to promote engagement of underserved patients.

Practice Implications:

Patient/family-focused interventions may not be beneficial for all populations. Providers should be aware of potential implicit and explicit biases and encourage question asking to promote patient/family engagement.

Keywords: Genetic conditions, Question prompt lists, patient-provider communication, patient engagement, Pediatrics

1. Introduction

Shared decision-making, the predominant model guiding communication between patients and clinicians, promotes bidirectional communication and patients’ active engagement [1]. It aims to facilitate patient-centered care by fostering a partnership between patients and clinicians, allowing them to make medical decisions that align with the patient’s values, preferences, and goals [2]. These processes are especially important for patients with undiagnosed or suspected genetic conditions because sharing symptom information can help determine appropriate diagnostic testing, including genetic sequencing, and assist with interpreting test findings [3,4]. The quality of communication between providers, patients, and their families is critically important, yet often differs by demographic and social factors. A systematic review of patient and physician communication found that Black patients report worse communication with physicians, share less information with providers, and engage in less shared decision making than white patients [5]. Additionally, patients of lower socioeconomic status (SES) ask fewer questions of providers and are less likely to share information when not prompted compared to patients of higher SES [68].

Question Prompt Lists (QPLs) aim to address this imbalance and support patient involvement [9]. This intervention provides patients and families with a list of possible questions to ask physicians in an effort to promote patient and family involvement and communication during medical appointments [10,11]. QPLs have been shown to increase question asking among patients [1114], including medically underserved patients and patients of color [9, 1520]. They can be tailored for different clinical encounters and are particularly effective when physicians support their use [12,21]. Therefore, when used by patients and families and supported by physicians, QPLs have the potential to aid communication and information sharing between physicians and patients [10,11].

1.1. Overview of the present study

Our study examines the impact of QPLs on caregiver-physician communication in pediatric specialty appointments through the North Carolina Genomic Evaluation by Next-generation Exome Sequencing (NCGENES) 2 study. NCGENES 2 is a randomized controlled trial that applied a factorial design with two independent randomizations, one to test the benefits of a pre-visit preparation (PVP) intervention that included an educational packet and QPL, and the second to evaluate the utility of diagnostic exome sequencing for pediatric patients with undiagnosed conditions. Here we report on the PVP intervention. The PVP was provided to enrolled parents or guardians (hereafter caregivers or participants) of the pediatric patients. Approximately half the participants were randomized to receive the intervention. Our primary goal was to determine whether the intervention could increase question asking (i.e., total questions and QPL questions asked) in initial pediatric specialty appointments.

Our secondary goal was to examine whether the intervention would increase question asking among underrepresented (patients of a race/ethnicity other than non-Hispanic white) or medically underserved (children having Medicaid or no insurance) families; that is, how race/ethnicity and child’s insurance status moderated the effect of the intervention. We hypothesized that caregivers of color or those whose child has either Medicaid or no insurance (MoNI) would ask fewer questions without the intervention and therefore would be more likely to benefit from the PVP. We examined the following potential confounding factors to explore the robustness of effects: number of caregivers at the appointment (e.g., another parent or family member accompanying the participant) [15]; caregivers’ perception of their child’s health (e.g., caregivers may ask more questions when their child is more symptomatic or impaired) [2224]; group-based medical mistrust (e.g., caregivers who mistrust the healthcare system may ask fewer questions) [9,25]; and caregiver perceived patient-centeredness (e.g., caregivers may ask more questions when their provider uses patient-centered communication) [22,23,26].

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were recruited from two NCGENES 2 sites: University of North Carolina (UNC) Health in Chapel Hill and Mission Health in Asheville. Eligible participants included one parent or caregiver from a family with a first-time clinic visit to either a pediatric genetics or neurology clinic at these sites. The patient had to be younger than 16 and have an undiagnosed condition with possible genetic cause. Based on demographic data in the electronic medical record, we oversampled patients of color (patients of a race/ethnicity other than non-Hispanic white) and patients medically underserved (children having MoNI). Potential participants were recruited and consented by phone. Randomization for the PVP study arm occurred during this call via computer-generated algorithm within the web-based study tracking system. Random assignments were concealed electronically until the time of disclosure to caregivers. The tracking system limited access to randomization status by study personnel roles and by caregivers’ status along the study trajectory. Study clinicians remained blind to the caregivers’ PVP intervention status throughout the trial. However, clinicians could become unblinded if the caregiver used the PVP during the clinic visit. UNC’s Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures, and informed consent was obtained from all participants. Additional details about the NCGENES 2 clinical trial protocol have been previously published [27].

2.2. Intervention and Control Group

Pre-Visit Preparation.

Prior to their clinic appointment, caregivers randomized to this intervention were mailed the PVP materials—the QPL and educational booklet. These materials were developed by genetics clinicians with our Community Consult Team, a diverse group of advocates for and caregivers of children with special needs [28]. The booklet covered topics such as information about diagnostic testing and the value of asking questions during the visit. The booklet introduced the QPL (Supplemental Figure 1), which included 11 example questions grouped by themes such as: “Questions about your child’s condition and the future,” “Questions about tests and other evaluations,” and “Questions to ask at the end of the visit.” Caregivers were instructed to mark questions they were interested in asking, write in their own questions, and hand the QPL to the doctor during the appointment.

Control.

Caregivers assigned to the control group (usual care) completed all study procedures except those associated with the PVP materials.

2.3. Procedures

A research team member met caregiver participants upon arrival at their child’s clinic visit. The team member collected a completed intake questionnaire, administered a pre-visit questionnaire on a tablet, and obtained caregivers’ consent to audio-record their appointment. The team member did not discuss the PVP materials with caregivers. Immediately after the visit, the team member administered a post-visit questionnaire to caregivers via tablet.

2.4. Audio-Recording Analysis

Appointment audio-recordings were transcribed verbatim for analysis. Research team members developed a codebook based on the topics covered by the QPL, educational booklet, and an initial reading of a subset of transcripts. Included in the codebook was a code to identify when caregivers asked questions during the appointment as well as a code to denote the number of adults accompanying the caregiver and child to the appointment (e.g., other parent, family); these two codes serve as the basis for the analyses presented in this paper.

Eight team members began the coding process in pairs after receiving training from the lead author (MW). Coders were trained to tag any time a question was asked by caregivers during appointments. Coder pairs first coded a transcript independently before comparing coding and resolving discrepancies. Coding of transcripts continued in pairs until consistency in coding was reached, at which point independent coding proceeded. If coders had any issues with whether text should be coded as a question, they brought it to the team for discussion. Random transcripts were checked by senior qualitative analysts (MW and RJC). If changes needed to be made, the transcript was brought to the team for further training. When all transcripts were coded, the two senior analysts met to review all text coded as a caregiver question. During this process, questions that were not related to the care of the patient (e.g., “Where is the bathroom?”) were removed from the dataset. All remaining questions were sorted into the type of question asked (i.e., QPL question or not).

2.5. Measures

Sociodemographic variables.

The intake questionnaire assessed the caregiver and child’s age; the caregivers’ sex, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, employment, and income; and the child’s insurance status. Missing pediatric data were abstracted from medical records, when available.

Clinic site details.

The clinic was genetics or neurology; study site was UNC or Mission.

Other variables.

The number of caregivers present at the appointment was obtained from the transcripts (1 = participating caregiver only; 2 = participating caregiver and one additional adult). Caregiver-reported perception of child’s health used a 0–100 scale (0 = Worst health you can imagine, 100 = Best health you can imagine) on the pre-visit questionnaire [29]. Group-based medical mistrust, a 12-item measure on the intake questionnaire, assessed perceptions of how participants’ ethnic group are treated in a clinical setting [30]. Responses, which ranged from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 5 (Strongly agree) (reversed scored where appropriate), were summed; higher scores indicated more mistrust (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.92). Caregiver perceived patient-centeredness of the clinic visit was assessed on the post-visit questionnaire with an adapted version of the 21-item Patient Perceptions of Patient Centeredness scale [31]. Responses ranged from 1 (Very strongly disagree) to 7 (Very strongly agree) and were averaged to yield a score. Higher scores indicated stronger perceptions of patient centeredness (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.95).

Primary outcomes.

The number of total questions asked and the number of QPL questions asked, coded as described above.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

The planned enrollment of NCGENES 2 (n = 800) was designed for testing potential utility of first-line exome sequencing early in the diagnostic odyssey of pediatric patients and the efficacy of PVP with factorial design with four study arms [27]. The present analyses focus only on the effects of randomization to PVP or control group, which was revealed to study staff and caregivers prior to revealing randomization to exome sequencing. Randomization resulted in enrolling 59 to the PVP study arm and 53 to the control group, audio-recording patients’ in-person clinic visits. Enrollment was curtailed when COVID-19 shut down in-person appointments.

Descriptive statistics (e.g., means, standard deviation) were used to report continuous variables. Categorical variables were reported with frequencies and percentages. Bivariate analyses were used to characterize between-group comparison (PVP vs. control) on the primary outcomes (i.e., the total number of questions and number of QPL questions), applying a two-sample t-test when an equal variance was assumed or a Welch-Satterthwaite’s test if the equal variance assumption was violated. Levene’s test for homogeneity of variance was used to validate the assumption. Using the same approach, we examined the bivariate effects of the proposed moderators (non-Hispanic white vs. participants of color; MoNI vs. insurance other than Medicaid, which included private and military).

A two-sample t-test or Fisher’s exact test, when appropriate, was used to compare the difference in sociodemographic and clinical variables—age of caregiver and child at visit, caregiver sex, race/ethnicity, education (Bachelor’s degree or greater vs. less education), marital status (married vs. non-married), employment (work for pay, Yes vs. No), annual household income (equal to or over $60K vs. less), child’s insurance status, clinic (genetics vs. neurology), and study site (Mission vs. UNC)—between the PVP and control groups to test the success of randomization in distributing these variables across the groups. Variables were controlled in the regression model if they differed across groups at the p < 0.10 level. We also sought to examine between-group effects of study arm and the effects of proposed moderators on the primary outcomes after controlling for evidence-based potential confounders. To identify these potential confounders, we examined correlations between the primary outcomes and the number of caregivers present, caregivers’ perception of child’s health, group-based medical mistrust, and perceived patient-centeredness. Potential confounders were entered into the multivariate regression models as covariates if they were associated with either primary outcome at the p < 0.10 level. In addition, race/ethnicity or child’s insurance status was entered as a potential confounder if it was not already in the model.

We used four hierarchical linear regression models to test the effect of study arm (PVP vs. control) on the two primary outcomes. Independent variables were entered as follows: the randomly assigned study arm (Step 1); the moderating variable of interest (race/ethnicity or insurance status) (Step 2); the interaction of study arm and the moderating variable (study arm X race/ethnicity or insurance status) (Step 3); and covariates (Step 4). Descriptive statistics and t-tests were used to interpret the moderation effect. The adjusted modification effect was reported as a regression coefficient, concluding statistical significance at the p < 0.05 level based on a Wald-type t-test.

3. Results

3.1. Participant Characteristics

Analysis included 112 participant transcripts and surveys (59 PVP and 53 control; see Supplemental Figure 2). Participants were recruited between August 2018 and March 2020. Table 1 summarizes caregivers’ and children’s characteristics. Nearly all caregivers were women. About a quarter were caregivers of color. Over half the children had MoNI. Over half the caregivers reported income below $60,000 and had less education than a Bachelor’s degree.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of Demographics and Clinical Variables

All PVP (n = 59) Control (n = 53)

M (SD, Range) or Frequency (Percentage*)
Demographics
Age of caregiver at visit 36.1 (7.7, 20.4–63.1) 36.4 (7.4, 20.4–53) 35.8 (8.2, 22.8–63.1)
Sex
 Female 108 (96%) 58 (98%) 50 (94%)
 Male 4 (4%) 1 (2%) 3 (6%)
Race/ethnicity
   White, non-Hispanic 79 (74%) 46 (81%) 33 (66%)
   African American/Black, non-Hispanic 12 (11%) 7 (12%) 5 (10%)
   Hispanic/Latino(a) Only 6 (6%) 2 (4%) 4 (8%)
   Asian 3 (3%) 1 (2%) 2 (4%)
   American Indian, Native American, or Alaska Native 4 (4%) 0 (0%) 4 (8%)
   Other 3 (3%) 1 (2%) 2 (4%)
Child’s insurance status
 No insurance 3 (3%) 2 (3%) 1 (2%)
 Has insurance
  Private 39 (35%) 20 (34%) 19 (36%)
  Medicaid 58 (52%) 29 (49%) 29 (55%)
  Military 8 (7%) 4 (7%) 4 (8%)
  Private and Medicaid+ 3 (3%) 3 (5%) 0 (0%)
  Military and Medicaid+ 1 (1%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%)
Education
 High school or less 30 (29%) 14 (25%) 16 (33%)
 Some post-high school training 36 (35%) 15 (27%) 21 (44%)
 Bachelor’s degree 24 (23%) 16 (29%) 8 (17%)
 Master’s degree or above 14 (13%) 11 (20%) 3 (6%)
Marital status
 Married 66 (65%) 34 (62%) 32 (68%)
 Living with partner 7 (7%) 5 (9%) 2 (4%)
 Divorced 5 (5%) 3 (5%) 2 (4%)
 Separated 3 (3%) 2 (4%) 1 (2%)
 Widowed 1 (1%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%)
 Single, never married 20 (20%) 10 (18%) 10 (21%)
Work for pay
 Yes 63 (61%) 33 (59%) 30 (63%)
 No 41 (39%) 23 (41%) 18 (38%)
Income
 $5,000 – $24,999 33 (33%) 17 (30%) 16 (36%)
 $25,000 – $59,999 26 (26%) 14 (25%) 12 (27%)
 $60,000 – $119,999 24 (24%) 13 (23%) 11 (24%)
 $120,000 or more 18 (18%) 12 (21%) 6 (13%)
Age of child at visit 5.12 (4.04, 0.4–16.0) 4.4 (3.8, 0.4–15.0) 5.9 (4.2, 0.4–16.0)
Clinical Factors
Clinic
 Genetics 100 (89%) 52 (88%) 48 (91%)
 Neurology 12 (11%) 7 (12%) 5 (9%)
Study site
 Mission 34 (30%) 18 (31%) 16 (30%)
 UNC 78 (70%) 41 (69%) 37 (70%)
*

Percentages were calculated after excluding missing data.

+

Counted as Medicaid in analyses

3.2. Overview of Question Asking Behavior

On average, caregivers asked 3.63 questions (SD = 4.03, range 0–21) and 0.72 QPL questions (SD = 1.20, range 0–5). In terms of the number of questions asked, the mean for caregivers in the PVP group (MPVP) was greater than the mean for caregivers in the control group (Mcontrol) (MPVP= 4.36, SDPVP = 4.66 vs. Mcontrol = 2.83, SDcontrol = 3.03, t110 = 2.03, p = 0.045, Cohen’s d = 0.39), including QPL questions (MPVP = 1.05, SDPVP = 1.39 vs. Mcontrol = 0.36, SDcontrol = 0.81, t94.9 = 3.25, p = 0.002, Cohen’s d = 0.61). Non-Hispanic white caregivers tended to ask more questions than participants of color (Mnon-Hispanic white = 4.08, SDnon-Hispanic white = 4.38, vs. Mcolor = 2.36, SDcolor = 2.82, t105 = 1.93, p = 0.056); this difference reached significance for the QPL questions (Mnon-Hispanic white = 0.87, SDnon-Hispanic white = 1.27, vs. Mcolor = 0.32, SDcolor 0.86, t105 =2.12, p = 0.036). Caregivers of children with insurance other than Medicaid asked more questions than those of children with MoNI (Mnon-Medicaid = 4.94, SDnon-Medicaid = 5.05, vs. MMoNI = 2.69, SDMoNI = 2.78, t66.1 = 2.76, p = 0.007), including QPL questions (Mnon-Medicaid = 1.04, SDnon-Medicaid = 1.35, vs. MMoNI = 0.49, SDMoNI = 1.03, t110 = 2.44, p = 0.016).

3.3. Evaluation of potential confounders

Analyses revealed differences across study arms (p < 0.10) in education (48% with a Bachelor’s degree or more in the PVP group vs. 23% in the control, p = 0.009) and age of child at visit (MPVP = 4.45, SDPVP = 3.80 vs. Mcontrol = 5.87, SDcontrol = 4.19, t110 = −1.89, p = 0.062). (See Supplemental Table 1 for statistics of all comparisons). Correlation analyses of the primary outcomes and these potential confounding variables (Supplemental Table 2; descriptive statistics in Supplemental Table 3) revealed that having additional caregivers at the visit was associated with asking more total questions (p = 0.001), but not more QPL questions (p = 0.259). Also, a marginally significant correlation suggested a trend towards having a more positive perception of the child’s health being associated with asking fewer total questions (p = 0.060), but not fewer QPL questions (p = 0.158).

3.4. Hypothesis testing

Group Effects on Total Number of Questions Asked with Race/Ethnicity as a Moderator (Table 2).

Table 2.

Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Total Number of Questions Asked (Race/Ethnicity as the Moderator

Outcome Total Number of Questions Asked

Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model a Moderation Model Adjusted Model b

B (se) P B (se) P B (se) P B (se) P
Step 1
Pre-visit preparation (ref: Control) 1.526 (0.752) 0.045 1.374 (0.786) 0.084 2.160 (0.904) 0.019 1.138 (0.956) 0.237

Step 2
Participant of color (ref: non-Hispanic white) −1.459 (0.893) 0.105 −0.112 (1.184) 0.925 −0.263 (1.179) 0.824

Step 3
Pre-visit preparation X Participant of color −3.048 (1.781) 0.090 −1.607 (1.775) 0.368

Step 4
Medicaid and no insurance (ref: Insurance other than Medicaid) −1.204 (0.922) 0.195
Bachelor’s degree or greater education (ref: less education) 1.688 (0.939) 0.076
Age of child at visit −0.019 (0.100) 0.851
Number of caregivers present 2.374 (0.773) 0.003
Perception of child’s health −0.026 (0.022) 0.242
a

Model included race/ethnicity as the moderating variable of interest.

b

Model controlled child’s insurance status, caregiver educational attainment because educational attainment was higher in the PVP study arm compared to the Control study arm (p = 0.009), age of child at visit because it was lower in the PVP study arm compared to the Control study arm (p = 0.062), and the number of caregivers present and perception of child’s health because they were significantly correlated with the total number of questions asked (p = 0.001 and 0.060, respectively).

Step 1 revealed an effect of study arm—participants in the PVP group asked more total questions. Step 2 revealed no main effect of race/ethnicity, although the effect of study arm became marginally significant once race/ethnicity was in the model. Step 3 revealed a marginally significant interaction between study group and race/ethnicity. Non-Hispanic white caregivers in the PVP group asked more questions than their counterparts in the control group (MPVP = 4.98, SDPVP = 5.03 vs. Mcontrol = 2.82, SDcontrol = 2.92, t77 = 2.21, p = 0.030); for participants of color, there was no effect of study arm on the total number of questions asked (MPVP = 1.82, SDPVP = 1.60 vs. Mcontrol = 2.71, SDcontrol = 3.39, t26 = −0.81, p = 0.426) (Supplemental Figure 3). In Step 4, which controlled for potential confounders, the interaction was not significant; however, more caregivers present was associated with more questions asked and higher education tended to associate with more questions asked.

Group Effects on Number of QPL Questions Asked with Race/Ethnicity as a Moderator (Table 3).

Table 3.

Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Number of QPL Questions Asked (Race/Ethnicity as the Moderator)

Outcome Number of QPL Questions Asked

Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model a Moderation Model Adjusted Model b

B (se) P B (se) P B (se) P B (se) P
Step 1
Pre-visit preparation (ref: Control) 0.692 (0.219) 0.002 0.706 (0.223) 0.002 0.928 (0.257) < 0.001 0.718 (0.282) 0.013

Step 2
Participant of color (ref: non-Hispanic white) −0.418 (0.253) 0.102 −0.039 (0.336) 0.907 −0.039 (0.348) 0.911

Step 3
Pre-visit preparation X Participant of color −0.858 (0.505) 0.092 −0.594 (0.524) 0.260

Step 4
Medicaid and no insurance (ref: Insurance other than Medicaid) −0.097 (0.272) 0.721
Bachelor’s degree or greater education (ref: less education) 0.623 (0.277) 0.027
Age of child at visit 0.006 (0.030) 0.849
Number of caregivers present 0.138 (0.228) 0.546
Perception of child’s health −0.006 (0.007) 0.357
a

Model included race/ethnicity as the moderating variable of interest.

b

Model controlled child’s insurance status, caregiver educational attainment because educational attainment was higher in the PVP study arm compared to the Control study arm (p = 0.009), age of child at visit because it was lower in the PVP study arm compared to the Control study arm (p = 0.062), and the number of caregivers present and perception of child’s health because they were significantly correlated with the total number of questions asked (p = 0.001 and 0.060, respectively).

Step 1 revealed an effect of study arm, such that participants in the PVP group asked more QPL questions. Step 2 revealed no main effect of race/ethnicity; the main effect of study arm remained significant with race/ethnicity in the model. Step 3 revealed a marginally significant interaction. Analyses found non-Hispanic white caregivers in the PVP group asked more QPL questions than those in the control group (MPVP = 1.26, SDPVP = 1.48 vs. Mcontrol = 0.33, SDcontrol = 0.60, t63.0 = 3.84, p < 0.001). For participants of color, there was no effect of group on the number of QPL questions asked (MPVP = 0.36, SDPVP = 0.67 vs. Mcontrol = 0.29, SDcontrol = 0.99, t26 = 0.20, p = 0.840) (Supplemental Figure 4). In Step 4, which controlled for potential confounders, the interaction was not significant; however, being in the PVP group (for non-Hispanic whites) and higher education were associated with more QPL questions asked.

Group Effects on Total Number of Questions Asked with Child’s Insurance Status as a Moderator (Table 4).

Table 4.

Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Total Number of Questions Asked (Child’s Insurance Status as the Moderator)

Outcome Total Number of Questions Asked

Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model a Moderation Model Adjusted Model b

B (se) P B (se) P B (se) P B (se) P
Step 1
Pre-visit preparation (ref: Control) 1.526 (0.752) 0.045 1.588 (0.724) 0.030 4.047 (1.077) < 0.001 3.369 (1.211) 0.007

Step 2
Medicaid and no insurance (ref: Insurance other than Medicaid) −2.288 (0.733) 0.002 −0.070 (1.023) 0.946 0.925 (1.136) 0.418

Step 3
Pre-visit preparation X Medicaid and no insurance −4.247 (1.416) 0.003 −4.302 (1.474) 0.004

Step 4
Participant of color (ref: non-Hispanic white) −1.018 (0.845) 0.231
Bachelor’s degree or greater education (ref: less education) 1.334 (0.912) 0.147
Age of child at visit −0.030 (0.095) 0.751
Number of caregivers present 2.074 (0.750) 0.007
Perception of child’s health −0.031 (0.021) 0.142
a

Model included child’s insurance status as the moderating variable of interest.

b

Model controlled caregiver race/ethnicity, caregiver educational attainment because educational attainment was higher in the PVP study arm compared to the Control study arm (p = 0.009), age of child at visit because it was lower in the PVP study arm compared to the Control study arm (p = 0.062), and the number of caregivers present and perception of child’s health because they were significantly correlated with the total number of questions asked (p = 0.001 and 0.060, respectively).

Here, Step 1 was the same as in Table 2. Step 2 showed a main effect of child’s insurance status and that the effect of study arm remained significant even after controlling for child’s insurance status. Step 3 revealed a significant interaction. Among caregivers of children with insurance other than Medicaid, those in the PVP group asked more questions than those in the control group (MPVP = 6.92, SDPVP = 5.86 vs. Mcontrol = 2.87, SDcontrol = 2.93, t34.1 = 3.01, p = 0.005). For caregivers of children with MoNI, there was no effect of study arm on the number of questions asked (MPVP = 2.60, SDPVP = 2.45 vs. Mcontrol = 2.80, SDcontrol = 3.16, t63 = −0.29, p = 0.775) (Supplemental Figure 5). In Step 4, which controlled for other covariates, the interaction remained significant. In addition, more caregivers present was associated with more questions asked.

Group Effects on Number of QPL Questions Asked with Child’s Insurance Status as a Moderator (Table 5).

Table 5.

Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Number of QPL Questions Asked (Child’s Insurance Status as the Moderator)

Outcome Number of QPL Questions Asked

Unadjusted Model Adjusted Model a Moderation Model Adjusted Model b

B (se) p B (se) P B (se) P B (se) P
Step 1
Pre-visit preparation (ref: Control) 0.692 (0.219) 0.002 0.708 (0.213) 0.001 1.190 (0.324) < 0.001 1.172 (0.365) 0.002

Step 2
Medicaid and no insurance (ref: Insurance other than Medicaid) −0.570 (0.216) 0.009 −0.135 (0.308) 0.663 0.399 (0.342) 0.247

Step 3
Pre-visit preparation X Medicaid and no insurance −0.833 (0.426) 0.053 −0.990 (0.444) 0.028

Step 4
Participant of color (ref: non-Hispanic white) −0.312 (0.255) 0.224
Bachelor’s degree or greater education (ref: less education) 0.548 (0.275) 0.049
Age of child at visit 0.002 (0.029) 0.959
Number of caregivers present 0.072 (0.226) 0.750
Perception of child’s health −0.008 (0.006) 0.218
a

Model included child’s insurance status as the moderating variable of interest.

b

Model controlled caregiver race/ethnicity, caregiver educational attainment because educational attainment was higher in the PVP study arm compared to the Control study arm (p = 0.009), age of child at visit because it was lower in the PVP study arm compared to the Control study arm (p = 0.062), and the number of caregivers present and perception of child’s health because they were significantly correlated with the total number of questions asked (p = 0.001 and 0.060, respectively

Step 1 was the same as that in Table 3. Step 2 revealed a main effect of child’s insurance status, and the effect of study arm remained significant after controlling for child’s insurance status. Step 3 revealed a marginally significant interaction between study arm and child’s insurance status, such that among caregivers of children with insurance other than Medicaid, those in the PVP group asked more QPL questions than in the control (MPVP = 1.63, SDPVP = 1.50 vs. Mcontrol = 0.43, SDcontrol 0.84, t36.6 = 3.37, p = 0.002). For caregivers of children with MoNI, there was no effect of study arm on the number of QPL questions asked (MPVP = 0.66, SDPVP = 1.19 vs. Mcontrol = 0.30, SDcontrol = 0.79, t63 = 1.40, p = 0.166) (Supplemental Figure 6). In Step 4, which controlled for other covariates, the interaction became significant and higher education was associated with more QPL questions asked.

4. Discussion and Conclusion

4.1. Discussion

We examined the effects of a PVP intervention that combined a QPL and educational booklet, hypothesizing that it would increase question asking among caregivers attending a pediatric specialty appointment for their child’s undiagnosed health condition. Like prior research, question asking (including asking questions from the QPL itself) was conceptualized as an indicator of caregivers’ engagement in shared decision making [11]. Compared to the control group, caregivers who received the PVP intervention asked more questions, including more QPL questions, indicating our intervention had the intended effect. However, this effect became more nuanced when considering differences by race/ethnicity and insurance status.

When developing our intervention, we wanted to increase question asking among caregivers from underrepresented (defined as patients of a race/ethnicity other than non-Hispanic white) and medically underserved (defined as children having MoNI) populations, who in prior studies have been shown to ask fewer questions, on average [9,1520]. We found that caregivers of children with MoNI asked fewer questions than those of children covered by insurance other than Medicaid. Additionally, a marginally significant association suggested that caregivers of color may ask fewer questions than non-Hispanic white caregivers. However, the PVP intervention had different effects across groups. Caregivers of children with insurance other than Medicaid asked more questions if they received the PVP compared to their counterparts in the control group. However, in caregivers of children with MoNI, question asking was not statistically different among those who received the PVP versus those who did not. A similar pattern was found for caregivers of color and non-Hispanic white caregivers. Analyses suggest that receiving the PVP increased question asking in non-Hispanic white caregivers but not in caregivers of color. Thus, the positive effects of the PVP were mostly observed in the traditionally served populations.

After controlling for potential confounders in the model, the moderating effect of caregiver race/ethnicity became non-significant for predicting total and QPL questions asked. Having more education (in the model to predict the number of QPL questions), and more caregivers present (in the model to predict total number of questions), were associated with asking more questions, suggesting that the effect of race/ethnicity on intervention efficacy could be partially explained by differences in education and number of caregivers present. These findings should be interpreted in light of the systemic factors contributing to differences in educational attainment in people of color compared to non-Hispanic whites [32,33]. More research is needed on how systemic factors, cultural differences, and power dynamics impact question asking by caregivers of color to develop beneficial interventions. Additional research is also needed to evaluate why caregivers of children with MoNI who received the intervention did not ask more questions than their counterparts in the control group. Potential confounders did not provide evidence to help explain this finding—the interactions between insurance status and intervention group were not diminished when covariates were added to the models for total questions asked or for QPL questions.

Qualitative research may elucidate why our PVP intervention did not affect question asking in caregivers of color and those with children with MoNI. Race/ethnicity and insurance status represent various socioeconomic, social, cultural, and other factors that may have associations with question asking and, more broadly, interactions with healthcare providers [34,35]. We believe it may be worthwhile to investigate whether elements of socioeconomic deprivation may be playing a role. For instance, since having low income is a requirement for qualifying for Medicaid, caregivers of children with MoNI may have more daily stressors and less time to review the PVP materials, so shorter intervention materials may be more effective for these participants. In fact, other studies reveal that providing patients with just 3 “generic” questions helps patients get information in medical appointments and leaves them with similar perceptions of their level of involvement in appointments compared to those who received QPLs [36,37]. Although our PVP materials were measured for easy readability, the 11 QPL questions, in addition to the booklet, may have been too burdensome, resulting in lower efficacy in this population. Moreover, previous research revealing the effectiveness of QPLs has occurred in oncology settings among patients with diagnoses [10,11,21,3840]. In these settings, QPLs can focus on specific questions, like prognosis or recommended treatments, increasing their effectiveness [9,11,41]. Given our setting—initial appointments for potentially genetic conditions—our QPL questions had to be relevant to genetic conditions and testing, but broad enough to cover diverse symptoms and conditions. It may have been too broad to help people who are already less likely to ask questions in medical appointments [68].

4.2. Limitations

Our study’s sample size was smaller than intended because COVID-19 curtailed in-person appointments during the study. Nevertheless, our findings could inform a trial with a larger sample, ideally using mixed methods to understand participants’ perspectives on PVP materials and any effect on question asking. A qualitative component and additional quantitative measures (e.g., of relevant beliefs that may affect question asking) may address a second limitation: race/ethnicity and child’s insurance status are broad categories that obscure effects of a variety of potentially important factors affecting question asking. However, this study’s strengths include a diverse sample, rigorous assessment of question asking using audio-recorded clinical encounters, and ability to control for a number of potential confounds to help inform the design of additional studies. These strengths are important to help understand and address disparities in health care and provide guidance for future in-depth investigations of factors that impact engagement and effective methods for addressing this important issue.

4.3. Conclusion

Shared decision making may improve patient outcomes by, in part, enhancing communication between patients, family members, and providers. Addressing disparities in communication in health care settings is critical. Our PVP intervention worked less effectively among underserved groups, indicating that to attain equitable benefits across populations, a variety of interventions are needed as opposed to a single, one-size-fits-all approach. These findings underscore the need for additional, in-depth research on interventions to improve communication between providers and caregivers and ensure that research benefits are widely accessible to diverse populations.

4.4. Practice Implications

Given unequal power dynamics between patients and clinicians, simply providing patients and caregivers tools to support question asking may not be sufficient to increase engagement. Instead, efforts to encourage providers to support question asking may be more effective in promoting engagement of underserved and underrepresented patients [9,12,21]. Physicians should be trained on implicit and explicit biases that may impact their interactions with various patient populations. Prior studies demonstrate that patients with Medicaid or no insurance report worse clinical experiences, unfair treatment, and providers not listening to or answering their questions, which they attribute to their insurance status [4244]. In addition, physicians may perceive patients of low SES, which is linked to insurance status, as less intelligent and responsible, give them less information, and listen to them less carefully than other patients [6,8]. Thus, improving information sharing and shared decision making in patient-provider interactions likely requires changes by patients and providers.

Supplementary Material

1
2

Highlights:

  • A question prompt list increased question asking among pediatric patient caregivers

  • But the question prompt list worked less effectively among underserved groups

  • Additional interventions are needed to ensure research benefits diverse populations

  • Improving engagement of underserved patients also requires changes by providers

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank those who participated in NCGENES 2 and our Community Consult Team for their ongoing involvement and feedback for the NCGENES 2 study. We thank Dr. Lixin Song for her assistance planning the analytic approach for the transcribed clinical appointments.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) [grant number U01HG006487].

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Ethics Declaration

The Institutional Review Board at the University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill approved all study procedures. Informed consent was obtained from all participants as required by the IRB.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Data Availability Statement

Due to the nature of this research, participants of this study did not agree for their data to be shared publicly, so supporting data is not available.

References

  • 1.Hoving C, Visser A, Mullen PD, van den Borne B. A history of patient education by health professionals in Europe and North America: from authority to shared decision making education. Patient Educ Couns. 2010; 78:275–81. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.President’s Commission for the Study of Ethical Problems in Medicine and Biomedical and Behavioral Research, 1982. Making Health Care Decisions. The Ethical and Legal Implications of Informed Consent in the Patient-Practitioner Relationship. Washington, DC. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Babac A, von Friedrichs V, Litzkendorf S, Zeidler J, Damm K, & Graf von der Schulenburg J. Integrating patient perspectives in medical decision-making: a qualitative interview study examining potentials within the rare disease information exchange process in practice. BMC Medical Informatics and Decision Making. 2019;19:1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pérez-Ramos J, Abt-Sacks A, Perestelo-Pérez L, Rivero-Santana A, Toledo-Chávarri A, González N, & Serrano-Aguilar P, Shared decision making in rare diseases: an overview. Rare Diseases and Orphan Drugs. 2015; 2:39–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Shen MJ, Peterson EB, Costas-Muñiz R, Hernandez MH, Jewell ST, Matsoukas K, Bylund CL. The Effects of Race and Racial Concordance on Patient-Physician Communication: A Systematic Review of the Literature. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities. 2018;5:117–40. doi: 10.1007/s40615-017-0350-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Allen S, Rogers SN, Brown S, Harris RV. What are the underlying reasons behind socioeconomic differences in doctor-patient communication in head and neck oncology review clinics? Health Expect. 2021;24:140–51. doi: 10.1111/hex.13163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Verlinde E, De Laender N, De Maesschalck S, Deveugele M, Willems S. The social gradient in doctor-patient communication. Int J Equity Health. 2012; 12:12. doi: 10.1186/1475-9276-11-12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Siminoff LA, Graham GC, Gordon NH. Cancer communication patterns and the influence of patient characteristics: disparities in information-giving and affective behaviors. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;62:355–60. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2006.06.011. Epub 2006 Jul 24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Eggly S, Hamel LM, Foster TS, Albrecht TL, Chapman R, Harper FWK, Thompson H, Griggs JJ, Gonzalez R, Berry-Bobovski L, Tkatch R, Simon M, Shields A, Gadgeel S, Loutfi R, Ali H, Wollner I, Penner LA. Randomized trial of a question prompt list to increase patient active participation during interactions with black patients and their oncologists. Patient Educ Couns. 2017;100:818–26. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2016.12.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Brandes K, Linn AJ, Butow PN, van Weert JC. The characteristics and effectiveness of Question Prompt List interventions in oncology: a systematic review of the literature. Psychooncology. 2015;24:245–52. doi: 10.1002/pon.3637. Epub 2014 Jul 31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sansoni JE, Grootemaat P, Duncan C. Question Prompt Lists in health consultations: A review. Patient Educ Couns. 2015;3. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2015.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lemmon ME, Huffstetler HE, Donohue P, Katz M, Barks MC, Schindler E, Brandon D, Boss RD, Ubel PA. Neurodevelopmental Risk: A Tool to Enhance Conversations With Families of Infants. J Child Neurol. 2019;34:653–9. doi: 10.1177/0883073819844927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sleath B, Carpenter DM, Davis SA, Watson CH, Lee C, Loughlin CE, Garcia N, Reuland DS, Tudor G. Improving youth question-asking and provider education during pediatric asthma visits. Patient Educ Couns. 2018;101:1051–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2018.01.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ahmed R, McCaffery KJ, Silove N, Butow P, Clarke S, Kohn M, Aslani P. The evaluation of a question prompt list for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in pediatric care: A pilot study. Res Social Adm Pharm. 2017;13:172–86. doi 10.1016/j.sapharm.2016.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Barton E, Moore TF, Hamel L, Penner L, Albrecht T, Eggly S. The influence of a question prompt list on patient-oncologist information exchange in an African-American population. Patient Educ Couns. 2020;103:505–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2019.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Aboumatar HJ, Carson KA, Beach MC, Roter DL, Cooper LA. The impact of health literacy on desire for participation in healthcare, medical visit communication, and patient reported outcomes among patients with hypertension. J Gen Intern Med. 2013;28:1469–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Eggly S, Harper FWK, Penner LA, Gleason MJ, Foster T, Albrecht TL. Variation in question asking during cancer clinical interactions: a potential source of disparities in access to information. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;82:63–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gordon HS, Street RL, Sharf BF, Souchek J. Racial differences in doctors’ information-giving and patients’ participation. Cancer. 2006;107:1313–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Katz MG, Jacobson TA, Veledar E, Kripalani S. Patient literacy and question-asking behavior during the medical encounter: a mixed-methods analysis. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22:782–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Willems S, De Maesschalck S, Deveugele M, Derese A, De Maeseneer J. Socio-economic status of the patient and doctor-patient communication: does it make a difference? Patient Educ Couns. 2005;56:139–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Clayton JM, Butow PN, Tattersall MH, Devine RJ, Simpson JM, Aggarwal G, Clark KJ, Currow DC, Elliott LM, Lacey J, Lee PG, Noel MA. Randomized controlled trial of a prompt list to help advanced cancer patients and their caregivers to ask questions about prognosis and end-of-life care. J Clin Oncol. 2007;25:715–23. doi: 10.1200/JCQ.2006.06.7827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Street RL Jr, (2003). Communication in medical encounters: An ecological perspective. In The Routledge handbook of health communication (pp. 77–104). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Street RL Jr, Gordon HS, Ward MM, Krupat E, Kravitz RL. Patient participation in medical consultations: why some patients are more involved than others. Med Care. 2005;43:960–9. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000178172.40344.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Del Piccolo L, Pietrolongo E, Radice D, Tortorella C, Confalonieri P, Pugliatti M, Lugaresi A, Giordano A, Heesen C, Solari A; AutoMS Project. Patient expression of emotions and neurologist responses in first multiple sclerosis consultations. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0127734. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Thompson AG. The meaning of patient involvement and participation in health care consultations: a taxonomy. Soc Sci Med. 2007;64:1297–310. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ha JF, Longnecker N. Doctor-patient communication: a review. Ochsner J. 2010;10:38–43. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Staley BS, Milko LV, Waltz M, Griesemer I, Mollison L, Grant TL, Farnan L, Roche M, Navas A, Lightfoot A, Foreman AKM, O’Daniel JM, O’Neill SC, Lin FC, Roman TS, Brandt A, Powell BC, Rini C, Berg JS, Bensen JT. Evaluating the clinical utility of early exome sequencing in diverse pediatric outpatient populations in the North Carolina Clinical Genomic Evaluation of Next-generation Exome Sequencing (NCGENES) 2 study: a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2021;22:395. doi: 10.1186/s13063-021-05341-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Griesemer I, Staley BS, Lightfoot AF, Bain L, Byrd D, Conway C, Grant TL, Leach B, Milko L, Mollison L, Porter N, Reid S, Smith G, Waltz M, Berg JS, Rini C, O’Daniel JM. Engaging community stakeholders in research on best practices for clinical genomic sequencing. Per Med. 2020;17:435–44. doi: 10.2217/pme-2020-0074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wille N, Badia X, Bonsel G, Burström K, Cavrini G, Devlin N, Egmar AC, Greiner W, Gusi N, Herdman M, Jelsma J, Kind P, Scalone L, Ravens-Sieberer U. Development of the EQ-5D-Y: a child-friendly version of the EQ-5D. Qual Life Res. 2010;19:875–86. doi: 10.1007/s11136-010-9648-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Thompson HS, Valdimarsdottir HB, Winkel G, Jandorf L, Redd W. The Group-Based Medical Mistrust Scale: psychometric properties and association with breast cancer screening. Prev Med. 2004;38:209–18. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.09.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Little P, Everitt H, Williamson I, Warner G, Moore M, Gould C, Ferrier K, Payne S. Preferences of patients for patient centred approach to consultation in primary care: observational study. BMJ. 2001;322:468–72. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7284.468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Darling-Hammond L (1998). Unequal opportunity: Race and education. The Brookings Review, 16:28–32. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ladson-Billings G (2021). Critical race theory in education: A scholar’s journey. Teachers College Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Williams DR, Lawrence JA, Davis BA. Racism and Health: Evidence and Needed Research. Annu Rev Public Health. 2019;40:105–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-040218-043750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Bailey ZD, Krieger N, Agénor M, Graves J, Linos N, Bassett MT. Structural racism and health inequities in the USA: evidence and interventions. Lancet. 2017;3:1453–63. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)30569-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Roe AK, Eppler SL, Shapiro LM, Satteson ES, Yao J, Kamal RN. Engaging Patients to Ask More Questions: What’s the Best Way? A Pragmatic Randomized Controlled Trial. J Hand Surg Am. 2021;46:818.e1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jhsa.2021.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mariano DJ, Liu A, Eppler SL, Gardner MJ, Hu S, Safran M, Chou L, Amanatullah DF, Kamal RN. Does a Question Prompt List Improve Perceived Involvement in Care in Orthopaedic Surgery Compared with the AskShareKnow Questions? A Pragmatic Randomized Controlled Trial. Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2021;479:225–32. doi: 10.1097/CORR.0000000000001582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Miller N, Rogers SN. A review of question prompt lists used in the oncology setting with comparison to the Patient Concerns Inventory. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl). 2018;27. doi: 10.1111/ecc.12489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Henselmans I, de Haes HC, Smets EM. Enhancing patient participation in oncology consultations: a best evidence synthesis of patient-targeted interventions. Psychooncology. 2013;22:961–77. doi: 10.1002/pon.3099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dimoska A, Butow PN, Lynch J, Hovey E, Agar M, Beale P, Tattersall MH. Implementing patient question-prompt lists into routine cancer care. Patient Educ Couns. 2012;86:252–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.04.020. Epub 2011 Jul 7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Jayasekera J, Vadaparampil ST, Eggly S, Street RL Jr, Foster Moore T, Isaacs C, Han HS, Augusto B, Garcia J, Lopez K, O’Neill SC. Question Prompt List to Support Patient-Provider Communication in the Use of the 21-Gene Recurrence Test: Feasibility, Acceptability, and Outcomes. JCO Oncol Pract. 2020;16:e1085–97. doi: 10.1200/JOP.19.00661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Arpey NC, Gaglioti AH, Rosenbaum ME. How Socioeconomic Status Affects Patient Perceptions of Health Care: A Qualitative Study. J Prim Care Community Health. 2017;8:169–75. doi: 10.1177/2150131917697439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Gray M, Jones KG, Wright BJ. Patients With Health-Related Social Needs More Likely to Report Poor Clinic Experiences. J Patient Exp. 2021; 8: 23743735211008307. doi: 10.1177/23743735211008307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gonzalez D, Kenney GM, O’Brien C, McDaniel M, Karpman M, Publicly Insured and Uninsured Patients Are More Likely Than Other Patients to Be Treated Unfairly in Health Care Settings Because of Their Coverage Type. Urban Institute Report. 2022. https://www.urban.org/research/publication/publicly-insured-and-uninsured-patients-are-more-likely-other-patients-be. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1
2

Data Availability Statement

Due to the nature of this research, participants of this study did not agree for their data to be shared publicly, so supporting data is not available.

RESOURCES