Skip to main content
Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2023 Feb 16;24(3):1075–1099. doi: 10.1007/s10902-023-00633-7

Cross-Cultural Psychometric Analysis of the Mature Happiness Scale-Revised: Mature Happiness, Psychological Inflexibility, and the PERMA Model

David F Carreno 1, Nikolett Eisenbeck 2,, James Greville 3, Paul T P Wong 4
PMCID: PMC9932412  PMID: 36820217

Abstract

The present study aimed to evaluate the psychometric properties of the Mature Happiness Scale, a measure focused on inner harmony. Mature happiness is achieved when a person can live in balance between both positive and negative aspects of their life. A total sample of 2,130 participants from five countries (Canada: n = 390, United States: n = 223, United Kingdom: n = 512, Spain: n = 724, and Hungary: n = 281) responded to an online survey including the original Mature Happiness Scale, the PERMA-Profiler, and the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses yielded a one-factor solution with seven positive items (non-reversed). We called this new version of the questionnaire the Mature Happiness Scale-Revised (MHS-R). Measurement invariance was found across countries, age groups, gender, and mental disorder diagnosis. Internal consistency and test–retest reliability were high. Older people, males, and people without a mental disorder diagnosis scored higher in mature happiness than younger ones, females, and those with a mental health disorder diagnosis, respectively. Mature happiness showed strong positive associations with various subscales of the PERMA-Profiler, specifically with positive emotions and meaning in life. In addition, mature happiness was strongly correlated with less negative affect and inner conflict and lower psychological inflexibility, whereas it was moderately correlated with lower loneliness. This validity evidence supports the cross-cultural use of the MHS-R in the aforementioned countries to reliably measure happiness among adults. With its holistic approach, the MHS-R may be a unique complement to other well-being measures, particularly to better predict mental health problems.

Keywords: Mature Happiness Scale, Mature happiness, Inner harmony, PERMA, Psychological inflexibility, Validation

Introduction

The construct of well-being has evolved from different theoretical and philosophical approaches (e.g., Diener, 1984; Keyes, 2002; Ryff, 1989, 2014; Seligman, 2011). Among the challenges for traditional conceptualizations of well-being is the inclusion of negative emotions. The relationship between the so-called positive and negative emotions is much more complex than simply being opposite ends of a spectrum (Diener & Emmons, 1984; Watson et al., 1988; Zevon & Tellegen, 1982). Given their complex relationship, positive affect and negative affect have been considered two separate constructs to be assessed in traditional measures of well-being (Bradburn, 1969; Butler & Kern, 2016; Diener et al., 1985; Watson et al., 1988). However, most of these well-being approaches such as the PERMA model (acronym established by Seligman, 2011, to refer to a well-being model based on Positive emotions, Engagement, Relationships, Meaning, and Achievement) do not incorporate negative emotions into the conceptualization of well-being. One of the main critiques of the first wave of positive psychology (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) has been this neglection of negative emotions, framing them as if they simply represent the absence of positive states (e.g., Held, 2004; Wong & Roy, 2017).

Mature Happiness: A Holistic Approach to Well-Being Based on Inner Harmony

An alternative approach to well-being has been formalized in existential positive psychology (PP2.0, Wong, 2009), also called the second wave of positive psychology (Ivtzan et al., 2015; Lomas & Ivtzan, 2016). PP2.0 may be distinguished from the first wave of positive psychology (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) because it highlights the importance of negative emotions and stressful events in the understanding of well-being. As noted by Lomas et al. (2021), from the second wave of positive psychology perspective, there is a difference between positive and negative valence (whether something is experienced as pleasant or unpleasant) and positive and negative outcome (whether something facilitates or hinders well-being). PP2.0 emphasizes that, while adverse experiences and suffering may intuitively be seen as undesirable parts of life, they are not only inevitable but can indeed serve as promoters of personal growth and resilience (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2006; Ivtzan et al., 2015; Kashdan & Biswas-Diener, 2014). Negative emotions have of course played an adaptive, protective role in human evolution (see Nesse, 2019). In this vein, several studies have shown the mental health benefits of accepting painful emotions and thoughts (e.g., Levin et al., 2012; Stockton et al., 2019), meanwhile rigid patterns of avoiding unpleasantness or difficulty have been linked to psychological problems (e.g., Chawla & Ostafin, 2007; Spinhoven et al., 2014). These findings suggest that the relationship one has with suffering should be integrated into the conceptualization of well-being and happiness.

In one of the recent efforts to do so, Wong and Bowers (2018) proposed an integrative concept of well-being called mature happiness. This construct refers to a positive state of mind characterized by inner harmony, calmness, acceptance, contentment, and satisfaction with life (Wong & Bowers, 2018). Mature happiness is understood as the positive mental state resulting from living in balance with the various aspects of one’s life, including both positive and negative. Rooted in the principles of Confucianism, Dao/Taoism, and Buddhism, mature happiness addresses the degree to which a person lives in harmony with their strengths and weaknesses, with other people, and with the world in general (Haybron, 2013; Wong, 2014). It is called “mature” because this type of well-being requires a significant level of personal maturity, particularly to live in attunement with one’s vulnerabilities (accepting painful emotions, personal burdens, and the fragility of human existence).

This concept of happiness thus goes beyond subjective/hedonic happiness and eudaimonic happiness, by including a third type of happiness, called chaironic happiness (Wong, 2011). Chaironic happiness (from Greek: “chairo” meaning “blessing” or “joy”) is related to harmony, the adoption of a positive mature attitude towards life, being mindful of the present moment, and being attuned to a self-transcendental existence (Wong, 2011; for similar considerations see Aydin & Khan, 2021). The positive state of mature happiness differs from the positive emotions included in the traditional approaches to subjective well-being such as joy, contentment, excitement, or enthusiasm (Diener et al., 1985; Watson et al., 1988) as it represents the overall positive state resulting from being in attunement with both positive and negative aspects of one’s life. In contrast, the positive emotions included in subjective well-being are related to fluctuating happiness, which normally is associated with higher arousal and less stability, and may not be that dependent on how one relates to their negative side. As a result, mature happiness is considered durable happiness because of its higher stability, low-arousal feelings, and relationship with self-transcendence (Wong et al., 2021). This difference between fluctuating happiness and durable happiness has been extensively reviewed by Dambrun and Ricard (2011). Mature happiness fits into the category of the so-called authentic-durable happiness. Apart from the previously mentioned aspects, authentic-durable happiness is characterized by selflessness and self-transcendence, while hedonic-fluctuating happiness is related to self-centeredness and self-enhancement (Dambrun et al., 2012).

Although the research on harmony in psychology has been quite scattered and disconnected, the conceptualization of well-being based on harmony has gained considerable support in recent years (for a review, see Lomas, 2021). For example, Delle Fave et al. (2016) reported that inner harmony and relational connectedness are the most-mentioned aspects that laypeople associate with happiness. Definitions of inner harmony included terms such as inner peace, emotional stability, balance, acceptance of life, serenity, and contentment. Satisfaction (with life and oneself), positive emotions (joy, cheerfulness, vitality, enthusiasm, and elation), and health were also mentioned as facets of happiness but to a lesser degree.

Mature happiness can be considered one of the experiential outcomes of living a meaningful life (Wong, 2020). This construct is related to the stoic concept of apatheia, which describes a state of peace of mind and equanimity in the face of passions (a term in the stoic literature that refers to disturbing emotions such as distress, fear, lust, and excessive delight). Apatheia has been considered to be a principal constituent of the eudaimonic life (Pigliucci, 2020). Despite the connection between eudaimonic well-being and mature happiness, an important distinction should be made; while eudaimonic happiness is focused on the pursuit of virtue, meaning, and flourishing; mature happiness aims to achieve equanimity and peace of mind, being mindful of and attuned to one’s nature as a human being, and finding a balance between the dialectics of life (Wong, 2011). From this theoretical perspective, mature happiness may represent a relevant facet of well-being to be taken into account, particularly in circumstances in which people are amid adversity (i.e. achievement and pursuit of excellence is difficult), such as during the challenges of the global COVID-19 pandemic. An appropriate measure of mature happiness may therefore be an invaluable tool for research on suffering vs. flourishing, particularly during this unique period in our collective history.

The Mature Happiness Scale and Other Related Measures

Thus far there is only one scale aimed to measure mature happiness, called the Mature Happiness Scale (MHS, Wong & Bowers, 2018). This instrument has been used in recent studies (e.g., Carreno et al., 2021; Wong et al., 2021). For instance, in a study during the initial phase of the COVID-19 pandemic, Carreno et al. (2021) observed that mature happiness appeared as a related but separate factor from PERMA (latent correlation of 0.78). In this study, a revised version of the MHS, which contains exactly the same items as the scale presented in the current paper, predicted psychological distress beyond and above the PERMA-Profiler. Mature happiness was a better negative predictor of stress, anxiety, and general distress, while the PERMA-Profiler showed to be a better negative predictor of depression. Additionally, mature happiness acted as a moderator between the perceived noxious effects of the pandemic and all markers of distress (stress, anxiety, depression, and general distress). At the same time, the PERMA-Profiler moderated the relationship between stress and depression. The revised version of the MHS used by Carreno et al. (2021) was the result of an initial factorial analysis, however, the authors did not provide any detailed information about this and other steps of the scale validation process. Most of the MHS psychometric properties are yet to be analyzed and published. This was the main purpose of the present study.

In the literature, we have identified at least other three existing measures of harmony. One is the Peace of Mind Scale (Lee et al., 2013) which assesses the affective well-being valued in Chinese culture. This measure includes feelings such as peace and stability (e.g., “I have peace and harmony in my mind”). The second related measure is the Harmony in Life Scale (Kjell et al., 2016) which assesses the subjective perception of overall harmony in life. According to the conceptualization of this scale (Kjell et al., 2016), harmony is characterized by selflessness, relatedness, mindfulness, psychological flexibility, sustainability, and secondary control, that is, it represents how an individual aligns or adapts to their environment (Kjell, 2011). This construct is different from the construct of satisfaction with life, which is characterized by self-centeredness, self-enhancement, fluctuation, and primary control; it addresses an individual’s ability to change their environment according to their wishes and needs. Thus, the Harmony in Life Scale measures cognitive evaluations of one’s relationship with life as a whole (e.g., “Most aspects of my life are in balance”, “I fit in well with my surroundings”). The third measure similar to the MHS is the Subjective Authentic-Durable Happiness Scale (Dambrun et al., 2012), which was developed to compare fluctuating happiness and authentic-durable happiness.

In contrast with the MHS however, these three scales focus on either feelings of peace or harmony with life (in terms of fitting into the personal context). Furthermore, these scales do not explicitly address negative aspects of living. The MHS meanwhile combines both feelings of peace and harmony with life (e.g., “I feel comfortable in my own skin”, “I am able to live in harmony with people that matter to me”), including the acceptance of negative aspects of living (e.g., “I have learned to accept life as it is”; “I have learned to let go of all my cares and burdens”). Similar to the MHS, the recently developed Equanimity Scale (Juneau et al., 2020) measures equanimity (even-minded state of mind and hedonic independence), however, this instrument does not represent a measure of happiness since it does not include other positive features such as satisfaction with life and contentment. For the aforementioned reasons, the MHS may collect important features in the evaluation of happiness and inner harmony beyond other measures in the literature.

Psychological Inflexibility and Mature Happiness

On the other side of the spectrum, the opposite of mature happiness can be understood as a mental state in which the person feels broken, experiencing an inner battle with their emotions and their life as a whole. This negative state is often linked to feelings of being disconnected from oneself, others, and the world. Some of the proposed items in the original MHS are aimed to measure this state (e.g., “I am troubled by inner conflict”, “My conscience bothers me”). In this state of mind, the person usually attempts to avoid their painful emotions and thoughts at all costs because they have acquired strong aversive functions. Thus, it is very unlikely that the person would accept these private events and would be willing to be in contact with them. This approach strongly resembles the construct of psychological inflexibility: the rigid avoidance of negative emotions and thoughts that is incompatible with actions based on personal values (Bond et al., 2011). Not surprisingly, several studies have demonstrated the links between psychological inflexibility and mental health problems (Bond et al., 2011; Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010; Levin et al., 2014).

Instead, the opposite of psychological inflexibility, termed psychological flexibility, is defined as “the ability to contact the present moment more fully as a conscious human being, and to either change or persist when doing so serves valued ends” (Hayes et al., 2006). This construct is related to the ability to accept unpleasant emotions and thoughts and act in accordance with personal values, and it has been related to positive mental health outcomes (Kashdan & Rottenberg, 2010).

Of note, the construct of psychological flexibility, because of its behavioral background, is defined in terms of the ability to respond to one’s emotions and thoughts. Therefore, this construct is based on cognitive-behavioral processes (Hayes et al., 2006; Hofmann & Hayes, 2019). However, the conceptualization and the evaluation of psychological flexibility do not include the experiential/emotional state (e.g., peace of mind, inner harmony, satisfaction with life) resulting from developing such processes. In contrast, the concept of mature happiness is based on this positive state, capturing the emotional experience of acceptance and living in harmony with most parts of oneself and the context in which one lives. Despite these theoretical connections between the two constructs, no previous study has empirically explored their relationship, which constitutes the second aim of this study.

The Present Study

This study aimed to investigate for the first time the psychometric properties of the MHS (factor structure, measurement invariance, internal consistency, and validity evidence based on the relationship with other measures). In line with positive cross-cultural psychology (Lomas, 2015) and the third wave of positive psychology (Lomas et al., 2021) highlighting the inclusion of multiculturality in positive psychology research, a cross-cultural analysis was performed including participants from five different countries (Canada, United States, United Kingdom, Spain, and Hungary). For the evaluation of concurrent and discriminant validity, we analyzed the relationship of the MHS with the PERMA-Profiler (Butler & Kern, 2016) and the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011), the latter being a measure of psychological inflexibility. The hypotheses tested to evaluate concurrent and discriminant validity were:

Hypothesis 1

Mature happiness would be higher in older people as it requires emotional maturity, which normally increases across the lifespan (Lawton et al., 1992; Márquez-González et al., 2008).

Hypothesis 2

Although mature happiness and PERMA represent different constructs (Carreno et al., 2021), MHS scores would be strongly associated (r above 0.50) with well-being scores of the PERMA-Profiler (positive emotions, engagement, relationships, meaning, and accomplishment) as both instruments assess subjective and eudaimonic aspects of well-being.

Hypothesis 3

Mature happiness would present negative moderate relationships (r from − 0.30 to − 0.49) with mental health problems as measured by the PERMA subscale of negative affect and the previous diagnosis of a mental disorder.

Hypothesis 4

Mature happiness would show a negative strong association (r above − 0.50) with psychological inflexibility since the latter refers to rigid avoidance of negative emotions and thoughts while mature happiness is about accepting and integrating most aspects of oneself.

Method

Participants and Procedure

A sample of 2130 people from five different countries participated in the present study (Canada: n = 390, United States: n = 223, United Kingdom: n = 512, Spain: n = 724, and Hungary: n = 281). This sample was selected from a larger research project (Carreno et al., 2021; Eisenbeck et al., 2022; link to the project: https://www.researchgate.net/project/Psychological-coping-with-the-COVID-19-pandemic). The entry criterion was to be older than 18 years old. The average age of the participants was 38.88 years (SD = 13.76), ranging between 18 and 84. The majority of the participants were female (77.4%) and most had no diagnosed mental health condition (83.3%). Detailed sociodemographic characteristics of the sample in each country are provided in Table 1.

Table 1.

Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample

Canada United States United Kingdom Spain Hungary Total
n 390 223 512 724 281 2130
Age
M (SD) 36.92 (13.23) 43.70 (15.64) 42.37 (15.26) 36.55 (11.83) 37.40 (12.40) 38.88
Range 18–84 18–77 18–76 18–73 18–71 18–84
Gender (female %) 52.8 81.6 88.1 77.1 89.3 77.4
Mental health diagnosis (no %) 84.6 73.1 74.2 84.4 92.9 88.3
Religiousness (yes %) 55.9 70.4 42.8 41.7 64.1 50.5
Marital status (%)
Single/divorced/widowed 32.6 33.2 36.6 34.9 35.2 34.7
In a relationship 12.3 12.1 13.9 20.0 14.9 15.6
Married/cohabiting with partner 55.1 54.7 47.9 45.0 49.8 49.2
Highest level of education (%)
Primary/secondary 15.1 13.9 28.9 14.9 22.4 19.2
Higher education 74.9 71.3 48.8 72.7 70.1 66.9
Still a student 10.0 14.8 22.3 12.4 7.5 13.9

As a part of a larger project, in this study participants completed various questionnaires about psychological distress and different coping strategies before participating in this part of the project (Eisenbeck et al., 2022). The questionnaires of this study were the last ones of the package (order: PERMA, AAQ-II, and finally, MHS).

The participating investigators agreed on the theoretical framework and the objectives of the study. All translations were made based on the recommended best practices (Beaton et al., 2000), including various translators and back-translations. Data collection methods were identical in all participating countries (for the importance of establishing as much equivalence as possible in cross-cultural research, see for instance Matsumoto & Yoo, 2006). The study was approved by institutional ethics boards for all participating countries.

Participants were recruited through email invitations and social media announcements using snowball sampling. Participation consisted of responding to an online survey that took approximately 15 min. It was anonymous, voluntary, and without any monetary compensation except for part of the Canadian sample that we recruited via MTurk. Participants reported their socio-demographic characteristics (age, gender, mental health diagnosis, religiousness, marital status, economic status, and highest level of education) and completed the MHS, the PERMA-Profiler, and the AAQ-II. Respondents were given the option to leave their email addresses to partake in the second part of the study to evaluate the test–retest reliability of the questionnaire. This measurement took place 3 to 4 weeks after the first one.

Measures

Mature Happiness Scale

The MHS measures different aspects of mature happiness such as inner harmony, calmness, acceptance, contentment, and satisfaction with life (Wong & Bowers, 2018). Participants rate items on a Likert scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (all of the time). The original scale contains 12 items with three reversed items that measure inner conflict. However, after the analyses reported in this paper, the final scale, which we called the Mature Happiness Scale-Revised (MHS-R), was composed of seven items without reversed statements (see Appendix). In this analysis, two of the original three reversed items (“I am troubled by inner conflict”, “My conscience bothers me”) appeared as a separate factor from mature happiness (termed “inner conflict”, see Results). Total scores of the MHS-R oscillate between 7 and 35, with higher scores indicating higher levels of mature happiness. Correlations between the two items of inner conflict ranged between 0.433 and 0.497 in all countries, p > 0.001.

PERMA-Profiler

The PERMA-Profiler (Butler & Kern, 2016) measures the five domains of the PERMA model of flourishing: Positive emotion, Engagement, Relationships, Meaning, and Accomplishment. It contains three items for each domain, with the total score representing a global measure of well-being. Additionally, we included the subscales of physical health (three items), negative emotion (three items), and loneliness (one item) proposed by the authors (Butler & Kern, 2016). To be consistent with the rest of the questionnaires, in this study, participants rated on a Likert scale from 0 to 6 instead of 0–10 (as in the original) and questionnaire means were calculated (the sum of all items of each subscale) (see Dawes, 2008). In the participating five countries, Cronbach’s alphas for well-being, physical health, and negative emotion ranged between 0.73 and 0.94.

Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II

The Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011) consists of seven items that measure psychological inflexibility. Respondents rate each statement on a 7-point scale from 1 (never true) to 7 (always true). Higher scores indicate higher psychological inflexibility, that is, a higher tendency to experiential avoidance that is incompatible with personal values. This questionnaire has shown satisfactory psychometric properties in English (Bond et al., 2011), Spanish (Ruiz et al., 2013), and Hungarian (Eisenbeck & Szabó-Bartha, 2018). Alphas in this study ranged between 0.91 and 0.93.

Data Analytic Strategy

Data were analyzed using Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 2016) and SPSS (Version 25). Due to a technical issue, basic demographic data (age, gender) of 40 participants were not recorded and these cases were discarded. Data were excluded in the case of participants (n = 11) who failed to complete at least 50% of any of the measures of the study or showed straightlining (e.g., employed the same to all items of a questionnaire even though the items aim to measure in opposite directions, n = 12). Missing data were then less than 0.01%, missing completely at random, χ2 (43) = 56.43, p = 0.082, and were not replaced. The final sample consisted of 2130 participants. Distribution of the variables was assessed with evaluation of kurtosis, and skewness, and with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk tests.

Following the best practices, to evaluate the factor structure of the MHS, we employed exploratory factor analysis (EFA) using principal component analysis with oblimin rotation on the first randomly selected half of the Canadian sample (n = 201). Subsequently, confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) were used on the second random half of the Canadian sample (n = 189) and on the rest of the samples to assess the proposed factor structure of the scale (see for instance Clark & Watson, 2019; Fenn et al., 2020; Rosellini & Brown, 2021). To be sensitive to the possible influence of culture to the measures and conclusions (e.g., Chen, 2008), the samples of the five participating countries were treated separately regardless of the language of the country. People can give slightly different meanings to different items based on their culture, which eventually can affect their view of that item and the underlying factor structure of the measure. All analyses were performed using the robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimator because violations of univariate and multivariate normality were observed. In all cases, sample sizes were adequate for factor analyses: ratios of the number of cases to the number of estimated parameters were above the established threshold of 20 (see Jackson, 2003).

Due to the χ2 test’s sensitivity to sample size (e.g., Hu & Bentler, 1998), model fits were assessed using the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), the Root-Mean-Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and the Standardized Root-Mean-Square Residual (SRMR). Values of CFI and TLI are considered acceptable above 0.90 and excellent above 0.95 (Hu & Bentler, 1998), while SRMR values below 0.80 indicate a good fit (Schreiber et al., 2006). An RMSEA value lower than 0.08 shows a reasonable fit, between 0.08 and 0.10 a mediocre fit, and above 0.10 is unacceptable (Browne & Cudeck, 1993).

Configural, measurement, and scalar invariance were evaluated across countries (Canada, United States, United Kingdom, Spain, and Hungary), age groups (cut-off points for three approximately equal groups were: 18–30, 30–45, and 45 +), gender (male/female), and mental disorder diagnosis (yes/no) in the entire sample. Successively restrictive models were compared with the Satorra–Bentler scaled χ2 difference tests using scaling correction factors and with CFI, RMSEA, and SRMR difference scores, the latter serving as indices for final decisions. As the sample size was large, changes of ≤  − 0.010 for CFI, ≤ 0.015 for RMSEA, and ≤ 0.030 / ≤ 0.010 for SRMR (depending on the level of testing) showed invariance (Chen, 2007).

Descriptive statistics (scale/questionnaire means depending on the measure, medians, standard deviations), Cronbach’s alphas, and Spearman’s correlation coefficients were calculated for continuous variables. Test–retest reliability was assessed with intraclass correlation coefficient with two-way mixed model and absolute agreement in samples from the United Kingdom, Spain, and Hungary. MHS-R levels as a function of dichotomous and categorical demographic variables were tested using Mann–Whitney U tests and Kruskal–Wallis H tests, respectively (as the data did not fulfill the criteria to use their parametric counterparts). Dunn’s pairwise tests with the Bonferroni correction were carried out for the post-hoc tests in the case of the Kruskal–Wallis tests. Effect sizes r were calculated for the Mann–Whitney tests (r = Z/√N). According to Cohen’s (1988) classification of effect sizes, r from 0.10 to 0.29 means a small effect, from 0.30 to 0.49 a moderate effect, and above 0.50 a large or strong effect. In the case of the Kruskal–Wallis tests, eta squared was calculated, η2 = (H − k + 1)/(n − k). The benchmarks that define small, medium, and large effect sizes are η2 = 0.01, η2 = 0.06, and η2 = 0.14, respectively (Cohen, 1988). To evaluate the effects of MHS-R on other variables, a series of hierarchical linear regression analyses were employed, in each case, controlling for the effect of the country in the first step.

Results

Mature Happiness Scale-Revised

We conducted EFA on the first random half of the Canadian sample (n = 201). Data were suited for factor analysis, as the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was 0.90. Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant, χ2 = 1086.35, df = 66, p < 0.001.

A one-factor solution explained 46.91% of the variance. According to parallel analysis (Hayton et al., 2004), the eigenvalue of the second possible factor (1.27) remained below the established threshold (1.37) calculated for this specific case, thus it was not retained. The three negatively-worded items showed the lowest factor loadings, warranting their removal (factor loadings of 0.07, − 0.11, and 0.42). The remaining factor loadings were as follows: 0.69, 0.50, 56, 68, 0.81, 0.89, 0.80, 0.70, 0.68.

This nine-item solution then was assessed with CFA on the second random half of the Canadian sample (n = 189). The fit was not satisfactory, χ2 = 84.54, df = 27, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.912, TLI = 0.883, RMSEA = 0.106 [90% CI 0.081, 0.132], SRMR = 0.052. Modification indices, residual variances, and regression weights were assessed, and two items were removed to make the model more parsimonious (“I have learned to be content in every situation.” and “I am able to give thanks at all times.”). Fit of the final, seven-item solution was excellent, χ2 = 13.00, df = 14, p = 0.526, CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.00 [90% CI 0.00, 0.066], SRMR = 0.024. The same one-factor structure was deemed to be an acceptable fit in all samples: United States, χ2 = 21.67, df = 14, p = 0.086, CFI = 0.987, TLI = 0.981, RMSEA = 0.050 [90% CI 0.00, 0.088], SRMR = 0.030, United Kingdom, χ2 = 58.85, df = 14, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.956, TLI = 0.934, RMSEA = 0.079 [90% CI 0.059, 0.101], SRMR = 0.039, Spain χ2 = 58.60, df = 14, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.972, TLI = 0.957, RMSEA = 0.066 [90% CI 0.049, 0.084], SRMR = 0.028, and Hungary, χ2 = 37.29, df = 14, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.962, TLI = . 944, RMSEA = 0.077 [90% CI 0.048, 0.107], SRMR = 0.035.

In all countries, item-total correlations ranged between 0.56 and 0.83 and regression weights ranged between 0.502 and 0.880 (see Table 2). The questionnaire also showed good internal consistency as Cronbach’s alphas in Canada, United States, United Kingdom, Spain, and Hungary were 0.87, 0.88, 0.85, 0.87, and 0.86, respectively. Moreover, kurtosis and skewness were in the acceptable range in all samples (see Table 3), although formal normality testing (Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Shapiro–Wilk tests) showed non-normal distribution in each country, p > 0.001. intraclass correlation coefficients were 0.83, 0.78 and 0.83 in the UK (n = 25), in Spain (n = 41), and in Hungary (n = 29), respectively, indicating good test–retest reliability of the measure. Detailed descriptive statistics of the MHS-R in all samples can be observed in Table 3.

Table 2.

CFA standardized regression weights and standardized residual variances per country

Canada (2nd random half, n = 189) United States (n = 223) United Kingdom (n = 512) Spain (n = 724) Hungary (n = 281)
e r. v e r. v e r. v e r. v e r. v
1. I am able to maintain inner peace .793 .327 .880 .225 .812 .341 .758 .425 .773 .402
2. I feel comfortable in my own skin .686 .529 .758 .426 .763 .418 .794 .370 .739 .453
3. I have learned to accept life as it is .749 .439 .732 .465 .613 .625 .735 .460 .730 .467
4. I am at peace with myself .861 .259 .856 .266 .879 .227 .829 .312 .843 .289
5. I have learned to remain calm, whatever comes .675 .544 .649 .579 .566 .679 .703 .506 .622 .613
6. I am able to live in harmony with people that matter to me .635 .596 .515 .734 .508 .742 .590 .652 .502 .748
7. I have learned to let go of all my cares and burdens .620 .616 .697 .515 .561 .685 .532 .717 .626 .608

e. standardized estimate, r. v. standardized residual variance

Table 3.

Descriptive statistics of the MHS-R in all countries

Canada (n = 390) United States (n = 223) United Kingdom (n = 512) Spain (n = 724) Hungary (n = 281) Total sample (N = 2130)
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Age
18–34 23.79 (5.37) 22.68 (5.77) 21.55 (5.32) 24.43 (5.10) 22.98 (4.99) 23.44 (5.33)
35–44 24.46 (5.65) 25.08 (4.31) 22.98 (4.79) 24.26 (4.84) 23.43 (5.30) 24.02 (5.06)
45–84 26.12 (4.70) 26.04 (5.09) 23.31 (5.91) 25.09 (5.49) 25.43 (4.85) 24.77 (5.53)
Gender
Female 24.30 (5.62) 24.56 (5.37) 22.59 (5.61) 24.24 (5.13) 23.61 (5.16) 23.74 (5.41)
Male 24.78 (5.07) 25.83 (5.03) 23.28 (5.30) 25.62 (5.07) 25.33 (4.58) 25.00 (5.10)
Mental disorder diagnosis
Not diagnosed 25.09 (5.16) 25.82 (5.09) 23.76 (5.14) 25.11 (4.82) 24.23 (4.78) 24.75 (5.01)
Diagnosed 21.43 (5.49) 22.02 (4.98) 19.55 (5.61) 20.37 (5.63) 18.05 (6.05) 20.39 (5.60)
Religiousness
Religious 25.39 (5.48) 25.64 (5.18) 23.68 (5.39) 24.45 (5.25) 23.92 (5.10) 24.57 (5.33)
Not religious 23.43 (5.02) 22.79 (5.17) 21.91 (5.60) 24.63 (5.08) 23.55 (5.20) 23.46 (5.34)
Marital status
Single/divorced/widowed 23.23 (5.23) 24.72 (5.44) 22.10 (5.92) 24.38 (5.26) 23.04 (5.43) 23.46 (5.54)
In a relationship 24.65 (5.53) 23.33 (5.86) 21.92 (5.84) 24.65 (4.95) 24.88 (5.13) 23.99 (5.42)
Married/cohabiting with partner 25.27 (5.29) 25.16 (5.11) 23.47 (5.09) 24.65 (5.15) 23.99 (4.85) 24.47 (5.16)
Highest level of education
Primary/secondary 22.83 (5.93) 26.06 (5.05) 22.23 (6.28) 23.32 (6.46) 23.29 (6.04) 23.06 (6.21)
Higher education 24.86 (5.28) 24.86 (5.22) 23.31 (5.32) 25.00 (4.71) 24.19 (4.73) 24.55 (5.03)
Still a student 24.62 (4.76) 23.27 (5.85) 21.85 (4.99) 23.46 (5.48) 21.52 (5.31) 22.84 (5.30)
α .87 .88 .85 .87 .86 .87
Skewness (SE)  − 0.26 ( .12)  − 016 ( .16)  − 0.42 ( .11)  − 0.46 ( .10)  − 0.72 ( .15)  − 0.42 ( .53)
Kurtosis (SE)  − .41 ( .25)  − .28 ( .32)  − .15 ( .22)  − .07 ( .18) .44 ( .18)  − .06 ( .11)

Measurement Invariance

Configural, metric, and scalar invariance was tested among different countries, age groups, gender, and mental health diagnosis status. In all multi-group analyses, configural and metric invariance were held (see Table 4). In the case of age and mental disorder diagnosis, scalar invariance was also obtained. Regarding the country and gender, only scalar invariance was not supported, which means that item intercepts were not fully equal across groups. These results indicate that we can at least assume a quantitatively invariant measurement model of latent constructs across all tested groups, which supports the reliability of the measure.

Table 4.

Invariance testing of MH-R (N = 2130)

Model Comparison χ2 df Δχ2, scaled Δdf p for Δχ2 RMSEA ΔRMSEA CFI ΔCFI SRMR ΔSRMR
Country
1. Configural 211.20*** 70 .069 [CI .058, .080] .969 .032
2. Metric 1 269.31*** 94 64.56 24  < .001 .066 [CI .057, .075] .003 .962 .007 .063 .029
3. Scalar 2 390.35*** 118 131.24 24  < .001 .074 [CI .066, .082] .008 .941 .021 .076 .013
Age
4. Configural 183.54*** 42 .069 [CI .059, .079] .968 .030
5. Metric 4 196.74*** 54 8.56 12 .740 .061 [CI .052, .070] .008 .968 .000 .037 .007
6. Scalar 5 232.83*** 66 34.42 12  < .001 .060 [CI .051, .068] .001 .962 .006 .042 .005
Gender
7. Configural 153.59*** 28 .065 [CI .055, .075] .971 .028
8. Metric 7 167.00*** 34 10.01 6 .124 .061 [CI .052, .070] .004 .969 .002 .038 .010
9. Scalar 8 230.33*** 40 72.84 6  < .001 .067 [CI .059, .075] .006 .956 .014 .050 .012
Mental disorder diagnosis
10. Configural 158.66*** 28 .066 [CI .056, .076] .967 .029
11. Metric 10 175.48*** 34 12.80 6 .046 .063 [CI .054, .072] .003 .965 .002 .038 .009
12. Scalar 11 198.98*** 40 22.06 6 .001 .061 [CI .053, .070] .002 .960 .005 .045 .007

Δ refers to change in the respective statistic. *p < .050; **p < .010; ***p < .001. Two-tailed

Country: Canada (n = 390); United States (n = 223); United Kingdom (n = 512); Spain (n = 724); Hungary (n = 281)

Gender: female (n = 1648); male (n = 482)

Age: 18–34 (n = 728); 35–44 (n = 731); 45–84 (n = 671)

Mental disorder diagnosis: no (n = 1774); yes (n = 356)

MHS-R and Socio-demographic Data

In the entire sample, older participants tended to show higher MHS-R scores than younger ones (Hypothesis 1), rS = 0.138, p < 0.001. Similarly, males (Mdn = 25), compared to females (Mdn = 24) showed higher scores on the measure, U = 350,619.00, Z =  − 3.93, p < 0.001, r = 0.08.

In line with Hypothesis 3, participants diagnosed with mental disorder (Mdn = 21) reported lower scores in the MHS-R compared to those without such diagnosis (Mdn = 25), U = 177,333.50, Z =  − 13.10, p < 0.001, r = 0.28. Religious participants (Mdn = 25) had higher score on this questionnaire than non-religious ones (Mdn = 24), U = 1,051,885.00, Z =  − 4.91, p < 0.001, r = 0.11.

Marital status, H(2) = 15.53, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.006, education level, H(2) = 39.80, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.018, and economic status, H(2) = 42.10, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.019, were related to scores obtained on MHS-R. Post-hoc Dunn’s pairwise tests with Bonferroni corrections showed that married respondents (Mdn = 25) indicated higher levels of mature happiness than their single counterparts (Mdn = 24), Z = 3.93, p < 0.001, r = 0.08. While participants in non-cohabiting relationships did not differ significantly from the other two groups, p > 0.05. Respondents with average economic status (Mdn = 25) showed higher scores on mature happiness than participants with higher than average (Mdn = 24) and lower than average levels (Mdn = 23) (Below average-average: Z = 4.34, p < 0.001, r = 0.09, below average-above average: Z = 6.49, p < 0.001, r = 0.14, average-above average: Z = 3.81, p < 0.001, r = 0.08. Other comparisons were not significant, p > 0.05. The same trends can be observed in each country separately (see Table 3).

Correlates and Predictive Power of the MHS-R

Table 5 shows the average data of the study questionnaires of the study in all participating countries. As can be seen in Table 6, MHS-R scores positively correlated with all positive subscales of the PERMA-Profiler, namely with physical health and well-being. In line with Hypothesis 2, strong correlations were found between MHS-R scores and positive emotion (rS = 0.71), meaning (rS = 0.61) and general well-being (rS = 0.69). We also observed a strong negative correlation with the negative emotion subscale (Hypothesis 3, rS =  − 0.53) and a moderate correlation with the loneliness subscale (rS =  − 0.36) of the PERMA-Profiler. It also showed a moderate inverse association with inner conflict (rS =  − 0.49) and a strong negative association with the AAQ-II (Hypothesis 4, rS =  − 0.59). All correlations were significant at the p < 0.001 level (see Table 6). Inner conflict was also strongly positively associated with both AAQ-II, rS = 0.65, p > 0.001, and negative emotion, rS = 0.50, p > 0.001.

Table 5.

Descriptive statistics across variables

Canada United States United Kingdom Spain Hungary Total
PERMA-Profiler
Positive emotion M (SD) 11.39 (3.67) 11.79 (3.55) 10.52 (4.01) 11.92 (3.62) 11.98 (3.27) 11.48 (3.72)
Engagement M (SD) 12.01 (3.36) 12.56 (3.45) 11.62 (4.03) 12.89 (3.42) 12.91 (2.89) 12.39 (3.54)
Relationship M (SD) 12.49 (3.79) 13.17 (3.75) 12.21 (4.36) 13.62 (3.63) 13.76 (3.97) 13.04 (3.95)
Meaning M (SD) 12.47 (3.92) 13.84 (3.37) 11.74 (4.49) 13.17 (4.15) 13.78 (3.62) 12.85 (4.12)
Accomplishment M (SD) 11.83 (3.16) 12.58 (3.36) 10.99 (3.86) 11.36 (3.74) 13.15 (2.90) 11.72 (3.60)
Wellbeing M (SD) 64.28 (16.24) 68.22 (15.44) 60.89 (18.92) 67.18 (17.07) 69.74 (13.98) 65.58 (17.12)
Negative emotion M (SD) 7.90 (3.80) 7.57 (3.90) 9.04 (3.98) 7.18 (3.71) 8.05 (3.54) 7.91 (3.85)
Health M (SD) 12.00 (3.60) 12.21 (3.80) 10.83 (4.22) 12.42 (3.6) 12.82 (3.48) 11.99 (3.84)
Loneliness M (SD) 2.68 (1.85) 2.50 (1.91) 2.70 (2.03) 2.08 (1.83) 2.35 (2.00) 2.42 (1.93)
Psychological inflexibility M (SD) 21.64 (9.67) 18.68 (9.00) 23.12 (11.03) 19.85 (9.70) 20.55 (10.20) 20.93 (10.13)
Inner conflict M (SD) 4.93 (1.98) 4.37 (1.86) 5.20 (2.15) 4.72 (1.96) 4.67 (1.93) 4.83 (2.01)
Mature happiness M (SD) 24.53 (5.37) 24.79 (5.32) 22.67 (5.57) 24.56 (5.14) 23.79 (5.13) 24.02 (5.36)

M represents questionnaire means

Table 6.

Correlations between mature happiness (MHS-R) and other measures of the study using rho

Measure Canada (n = 390) United States (n = 223) United Kingdom (n = 512) Spain (n = 724) Hungary (n = 281) Total sample (N = 2130)
PERMA-Profiler
Positive emotion .74*** .69*** .70*** .69*** .73*** .71***
Engagement .48*** .49*** .55*** .48*** .29*** .48***
Relationship .56*** .41*** .47*** .50*** .23*** .46***
Meaning .64*** .59*** .63*** .59*** .55*** .61***
Accomplishment .55*** .56*** .56*** .56*** .54*** .55***
Well-being .72*** .67*** .71*** .68*** .63*** .69***
Negative emotion  − .54***  − .61*** .56***  − .42***  − .56***  − .53***
Health .48*** .38*** .40*** .39*** .28*** .41***
Loneliness  − .47***  − .36***  − .37***  − .25***  − .42***  − .36***
Psychological inflexibility  − .54***  − .61***  − .66***  − .54***  − .63***  − .59***
Inner conflict  − .44***  − .48***  − .54***  − .43***  − .57***  − .49***

*p < .050; **p < .010; ***p < .001. Two-tailed

After controlling for the effect of country, MHS-R scores explained 50% of the variance of well-being of PERMA, β = 0.70, p < 0.001. In separate analyses, it explained 19.1% of the variance of physical health, β = 0.44, p < 0.001; and 36.4% of the variance of AAQ-II, β = − 0.60, p < 0.001.

Discussion

This study aimed to evaluate the psychometric properties of the MHS (Wong & Bowers, 2018), a recently proposed measure of mature happiness. For that purpose, we employed the original MHS, the PERMA-Profiler, and the AAQ-II in a cross-cultural sample including participants from Canada, the United States, the United Kingdom, Spain, and Hungary.

Factor Structure and Internal Consistency

In terms of factor structure, the final model, providing an excellent fit to the data in all participating countries, was the one-factor solution with seven items. To obtain this fit, reversed items from the original measure were removed, suggesting that the items measuring inner conflict may be separate from the construct of mature happiness. Similarly, the only item referring to gratitude showed to be not part of the MHS-R, which means that gratitude and mature happiness may be different, albeit quite possibly related concepts. We called the resulting scale the Mature Happiness Scale-Revised (MHS-R). Configural and metric measurement invariance of this scale were observed across countries, age groups, gender, and mental disorder diagnosis, supporting the notion that the psychological meanings measured by the MHS-R were equivalent across all tested groups. Cronbach’s alphas were high in all five countries (ranging from 0.85 to 0.88), as was the test–retest reliability after a period of 3 to 4 weeks (intraclass correlation coefficients of 0.75 and 0.83).

Overall, these findings show a solid structure and reliability of the MHS-R in all five cultures, in three different languages. As this is the first study evaluating the factor structure and internal consistency of the MHS, we recommend the use of our revised version in future studies. These results also provide evidence of a priori incremental validity of the MHS-R in comparison with the Peace of Mind Scale (Lee et al., 2013) and the Harmony in Life Scale (Kjell & Diener, 2021) since it has shown measurement invariance across more groups than the latter scales (across age groups, clinical/non-clinical sample, and five countries). These findings support the meaningfulness of the comparisons of the MHS-R scores between these populations.

Validity Evidence Based on Relations with Other Variables

To evaluate the validity of the MHS-R based on relations with other variables, we formulated four hypotheses. Hypothesis 1 predicted that mature happiness would increase across the lifespan. This hypothesis was supported since older people showed higher scores in mature happiness than younger ones, albeit the effect size was small. This may be because mature happiness requires high levels of emotional maturity and regulation, which typically increases with age (e.g., Lawton et al., 1992; Márquez-González et al., 2008). The findings are consistent with previous studies showing that well-being increases across the lifespan in Western countries (e.g., Schönfeld et al., 2017), similarly to meaning in life (Carreno et al., 2020; Schnell, 2009; Steger et al., 2009) At the same time, levels of mental illness typically decrease (e.g., Westerhof & Keyes, 2010). In addition, males scored higher in mature happiness than females. In the literature, there seems to be a controversy about the possible gender-based differences in well-being, thus this finding requires further studies (e.g., Batz-Barbarich et al., 2018; Schönfeld et al., 2017).

In line with Hypothesis 2, mature happiness was strongly associated with the PERMA-Profiler scores in physical health and well-being, including the subscales of positive emotions, engagement, relationships, meaning, and accomplishment. Mature happiness explained up to half of the variance of the PERMA well-being. These results provide evidence of the concurrent validity of the MHS-R as a measure of well-being. The strongest correlations were observed with positive emotions and meaning in life, demonstrating that mature happiness most probably includes aspects of both subjective and eudaimonic well-being (Carreno et al., 2021).

Despite these positive associations, there is empirical evidence showing that mature happiness represents a different construct from the PERMA-Profiler (Carreno et al., 2021). Mature happiness is a positive state based on inner harmony and peace of mind as a result of living in balance between the positive and negative aspects of one’s life. Other traditional measures of well-being such as the PERMA-Profiler (Butler & Kern, 2016), the Oxford Happiness Questionnaire (Hills & Argyle, 2002), or the Satisfaction with Life Scale (Diener et al., 1985) do not include these harmony-based features as they exclusively focus on subjective and eudaimonic well-being. Indeed, recent findings have shown that the PERMA-Profiler and Diener’s (1984) model of subjective well-being do not represent different factors but the same well-being factor (Goodman et al., 2018). This is not the case of mature happiness (see Carreno et al., 2021), which supports the idea that mature happiness may represent a third type of happiness called chaironic happiness (Wong, 2011).

As predicted by Hypothesis 3, mature happiness was negatively related to negative affect and inner conflict. In the case of negative affect, this association was stronger than predicted. We observed that people diagnosed with a mental health disorder scored significantly lower in mature happiness than people without such diagnosis (although with a small effect). In general, these findings show the discriminant validity of the MHS-R for the prediction of mental health problems. Based on the inclusion of negative aspects of living into the conceptualization of mature happiness (Wong, 2020; Wong & Bowers, 2018), the MHS-R seems to be a measure of well-being with more predictive power of distress than other well-being measures. For instance, Carreno et al. (2021) observed that the MHS-R showed a better predictive power of psychological distress (particularly of stress and anxiety) during the COVID-19 pandemic than the PERMA-Profiler. Together with the measurement invariance between clinical and non-clinical samples, the evidence collected in this paper encourages the reliable use of the MHS-R in studies and interventions including people with mental health problems.

Moreover, mature happiness was negatively associated with loneliness, a factor linked to mental illness (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010; Mushtaq et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2018). The conceptualization of mature happiness highlights the relational facet of well-being (Wong, 2020; Wong & Bowers, 2018), being explicitly represented in one of the MHS-R items (“I am able to live in harmony with people that matter to me”). These results manifest the importance of social relatedness in the achievement of inner harmony and happiness in general, as previously suggested (e.g., Delle Fave et al., 2016; Kjell et al., 2016; Seligman, 2011).

Finally, we observed a strong negative relationship between mature happiness and psychological inflexibility, confirming our Hypothesis 4. Participants with higher scores on mature happiness reported a lower tendency to avoid negative emotions and thoughts. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first empirical study showing a connection between inner harmony and psychological flexibility, which thus far had been only theorized (Kjell et al., 2016; Wong, 2020). This finding supports the idea that mature happiness may be considered the experiential component of psychological flexibility. Putting it differently, to achieve mature happiness, most probably it is necessary to be psychologically flexible. The results also suggest that psychological inflexibility, understood as the rigid avoidance of negative emotions and thoughts (Bond et al., 2011), may represent an opposite state to mature happiness, the latter being characterized by acceptance (Wong & Bowers, 2018).

Limitations and Conclusion

The main limitations of this study include a convenience sampling method, meaning that the distribution of the sample based on gender, age, educational level and other demographic data is not representative of the general population. For instance, our sample contains a high percentage of women, married individuals, and people with higher levels of education. As it is a cross-sectional study based on self-reported data, causal relationships cannot be confirmed, and it is possible that the findings were affected by the common method bias. The order of the questionnaires might have also been a source of bias.

Moreover, there was a missing criterion measure of harmony (e.g., the Peace of Mind Scale, Lee et al., 2013, or the Harmony in Life Scale, Kjell et al., 2016) to test convergent validity of the MHS-R as a measure of inner harmony. This happened because the original data collection belongs to a larger research project and an additional measure would have made the survey excessively long. Future studies could analyze this validity aspect of the MHS-R. Similarly, in order to understand more in depth the role of mature happiness in well-being, future studies are needed that compare this measure with other well-established measures of happiness or subjective well-being (e.g., Diener et al., 1985; Hills & Argyle, 2002). All these limitations are, however, at least partially mitigated by the multinational nature of the study and the fact that we mainly had participants from the community sample and not only undergraduate students.

In summary, this paper provides several indices of validity evidence of the MHS-R to measure mature happiness across five different countries in three languages. Mature happiness represents an empirically supported conceptualization of harmony-based well-being. The MHS-R, as a validated measure, can be a noteworthy contribution that opens the door for future empirical research on mature happiness across different populations and cultures.

Appendix: Mature Happiness Scale-Revised in English, Spanish (S), and Hungarian (H).

Considering your life overall, please characterize your life by rating the following statements using the scale (from 1 to 5) below.

(S) Considerando su vida en general, por favor, caracterice su vida valorando las siguientes afirmaciones según la siguiente escala (1–5).

(H) Általánosságban szemlélve, kérjük jellemezze az életét a megadott állítások segítségével egy 1-től 5-ig terjedő skálán.

1 2 3 4 5

Not at all

(S) Nada

(H) Egyátalán nem

All of the time

(S) Todo el tiempo

(H) Mindig

1. I am able to maintain inner peace

(S) Soy capaz de mantener paz interior

(H) Képes vagyok megőrizni a belső békémet

12345

2. I feel comfortable in my own skin

(S) Me siento cómodo/a en mi propia piel

(H) Jól érzem magam a bőrömben

12345

3. I have learned to accept life as it is

(S) He aprendido a aceptar la vida tal cual es

(H) Megtanultam elfogadni az életet úgy ahogy van

12345

4. I am at peace with myself

(S) Estoy en paz conmigo mismo/a

(H) Békében vagyok önmagammal

12345

5. I have learned to remain calm, whatever comes

(S) He aprendido a mantener la calma, venga lo que venga

(H) Megtanultam, hogy nyugodt maradjak, bármi is történik

12345

6. I am able to live in harmony with people that matter to me

(S) Soy capaz de vivir en armonía con la gente que me importa

(H) Képes vagyok harmóniában élni azokkal, akik fontosak számomra

12345

7. I have learned to let go of all my cares and burdens

(S) He aprendido a dejar de lado todas mis preocupaciones y cargas

(H) Megtanultam elengedni minden gondomat és terhemet

12345

Authors' contributions

DC has contributed to this article by coordinating data collection, elaborating the theoretical framework, and writing the manuscript. NE has contributed to this article through the coordination of data collection, data analysis, and writing the manuscript. JG has contributed to this article through data collection and a critical revision of the manuscript. PW has contributed to this article through data collection and the revision of the manuscript.

Funding

Funding for open access publishing: Universidad de Sevilla/CBUA. The research presented in this article has received no funding from any institution or any other source.

Availability of data and material

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Code availability

Code is available upon request from the corresponding author.

Declarations

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Ethics approval

The study was approved by the Faculty Ethics Committee of University of South Wales, the Institutional Review Board of Rutgers University, the Bioethics Committee of the University of Almería, and the Ethics Committee of the Eötvös Loránd University, Faculty of Education and Psychology.

Consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

David F. Carreno, Email: carrenodf@ual.es

Nikolett Eisenbeck, Email: neisenbeck@us.es.

James Greville, Email: james.greville@southwales.ac.uk.

Paul T. P. Wong, Email: drpaulwong@gmail.com

References

  1. Aydin N, Khan H. G-donic happiness: An alternative to hedonic and eudemonic happiness for sustainable consumption. Sustainability. 2021;13(11):6096. doi: 10.3390/su13116096. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Batz-Barbarich C, Tay L, Kuykendall L, Cheung HK. A meta-analysis of gender differences in subjective well-being: Estimating effect sizes and associations with gender inequality. Psychological Science. 2018;29(9):1491–1503. doi: 10.1177/0956797618774796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beaton DE, Bombardier C, Guillemin F, Ferraz MB. Guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. Spine. 2000;25(24):3186–3191. doi: 10.1097/00007632-200012150-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bond FW, Hayes SC, Baer RA, Carpenter KM, Guenole N, Orcutt HK, Waltz T, Zettle RD. Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II: A revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy. 2011;42(4):676–688. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago, IL: Aldine. https://www.norc.org/PDFs/publications/BradburnN_Struc_Psych_Well_Being.pdf.
  6. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sage Focus Editions. 1993;154:136–136. [Google Scholar]
  7. Butler J, Kern ML. The PERMA-Profiler: A brief multidimensional measure of flourishing. International Journal of Wellbeing. 2016;6(3):1–48. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v6i3.526. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (2006). Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research & practice. - PsycNET. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2006-05098-000
  9. Carreno DF, Eisenbeck N, Cangas AJ, García-Montes JM, Del Vas LG, María AT. Spanish adaptation of the Personal Meaning Profile-Brief: Meaning in life, psychological well-being, and distress. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology. 2020;20(2):151–162. doi: 10.1016/J.IJCHP.2020.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carreno DF, Eisenbeck N, Pérez-Escobar JA, García-Montes JM. Inner harmony as an essential facet of well-being: A multinational study during the COVID-19 pandemic. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;12:648280. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.648280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chawla N, Ostafin B. Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2007;63(9):871–890. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 2007;14(3):464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chen, F. F. (2008). What happens if we compare chopsticks with forks? The impact of making inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1005–1018. 10.1037/a0013193. [DOI] [PubMed]
  14. Clark LA, Watson D. Constructing validity: New developments in creating objective measuring instruments. Psychological Assessment. 2019;31(12):1412–1427. doi: 10.1037/pas0000626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Taylor & Francis; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  16. Dambrun M, Ricard M. Self-centeredness and selflessness: A theory of self-based psychological functioning and its consequences for happiness. Review of General Psychology. 2011;15(2):138–157. doi: 10.1037/a0023059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dambrun M, Ricard M, Després G, Drelon E, Gibelin E, Gibelin M, Loubeyre M, Py D, Delpy A, Garibbo C, Bray E, Lac G, Michaux O. Measuring happiness: From fluctuating happiness to authentic–durable happiness. Frontiers in Psychology. 2012;3:16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dawes J. Do data characteristics change according to the number of scale points used? An experiment using 5-point, 7-point and 10-point scales. International Journal of Market Research. 2008;50(1):61–104. doi: 10.1177/147078530805000106. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Delle Fave A, Brdar I, Wissing MP, Araujo U, Castro Solano A, Freire T, Hernández-Pozo MR, Jose P, Martos T, Nafstad HE, Nakamura J, Singh K, Soosai-Nathan L. Lay definitions of happiness across nations: The primacy of inner harmony and relational connectedness. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7(JAN):30. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Diener E. Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin. 1984;95:542–575. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Diener E, Emmons RA. The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1984;47(5):1105–1117. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsem RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1985;49(1):71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eisenbeck N, Carreno DF, Wong PTP, Hicks JA, María R-RG, Puga JL, Greville J, Testoni I, Biancalani G, López ACC, Villareal S, Enea V, Schulz-Quach C, Jansen J, Sanchez-Ruiz M-J, Yıldırım M, Arslan G, Cruz JFA, Sofia RM, Ferreira MJ, Ashraf F, Wąsowicz G, Shalaby SM, Amer RA, Yousfi H, Chukwuorji JC, Guerra VM, Singh S, Heintzelman S, Hutapea B, Béjaoui B, Dash A, Schlosser KK, Anniko MK, Rossa M, Wongcharee H, Avsec A, Kocjan GZ, Kavčič T, Leontiev DA, Taranenko O, Rasskazova E, Maher E, García-Montes JM. An international study on psychological coping during COVID-19: Towards a meaning-centered coping style. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology. 2022;22(1):100256. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2021.100256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Eisenbeck N, Szabó-Bartha A. Validation of the Hungarian version of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II (AAQ-II) Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science. 2018;9:80–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.07.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fenn J, Tan C-S, George S. Development, validation and translation of psychological tests. Bjpsych Advances. 2020;26(5):306–315. doi: 10.1192/bja.2020.33. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Goodman FR, Disabato DJ, Kashdan TB, Kauffman SB. Measuring well-being: A comparison of subjective well-being and PERMA. Journal of Positive Psychology. 2018;13(4):321–332. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2017.1388434. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2010;40(2):218–227. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Haybron DM. Happiness: A very short introduction. Oxford University Press; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hayes SC, Luoma JB, Bond FW, Masuda A, Lillis J. Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2006;44(1):1–25. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hayton JC, Allen DG, Scarpello V. Factor retention decisions in exploratory factor analysis: A tutorial on parallel analysis. Organizational Research Methods. 2004;7(2):191–205. doi: 10.1177/1094428104263675. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Held BS. The negative side of positive psychology. Journal of Humanistic Psychology. 2004;44(1):9–46. doi: 10.1177/0022167803259645. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hills P, Argyle M. The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: A compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences. 2002;33(7):1073–1082. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00213-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hofmann SG, Hayes SC. The future of intervention science: Process-based therapy. Clinical Psychological Science. 2019;7(1):37–50. doi: 10.1177/2167702618772296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hu L-T, Bentler PM. Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods. 1998;3(4):424. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.424. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ivtzan I, Lomas T, Hefferon K, Worth P. Second wave positive psychology: Embracing the dark side of life. Routledge; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  36. Jackson DL. Revisiting sample size and number of parameter estimates: Some support for the N:Q hypothesis. Structural Equation Modeling. 2003;10:128–141. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM1001_6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Juneau C, Pellerin N, Trives E, Ricard M, Shankland R, Dambrun M. Reliability and validity of an equanimity questionnaire: the two-factor equanimity scale (EQUA-S) PeerJ. 2020;8:e9405. doi: 10.7717/peerj.9405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kashdan, T. B., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2014). The upside of your dark side: Why being your whole self-not just your “good” self-drives success and fulfillment. Retrieved from, https://www.penguinrandomhouse.com/books/314151/the-upside-of-your-dark-side-by-todd-kashdan-robert-biswas-diener/
  39. Kashdan TB, Rottenberg J. Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(4):865–878. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Keyes CLM. The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2002;43(2):207–222. doi: 10.2307/3090197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kjell ONE. Sustainable well-being: A potential synergy between sustainability and well-being research. Review of General Psychology. 2011;15(3):255–266. doi: 10.1037/a0024603. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kjell ONE, Daukantaitė D, Hefferon K, Sikström S. The Harmony in Life Scale complements the Satisfaction with Life Scale: Expanding the conceptualization of the cognitive component of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research. 2016;126(2):893–919. doi: 10.1007/s11205-015-0903-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kjell ONE, Diener E. Abbreviated three-item versions of the satisfaction with life scale and the harmony in life scale yield as strong psychometric properties as the original scales. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2021;103(2):183–194. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2020.1737093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lawton MP, Kleban MH, Rajagopal D, Dean J. Dimensions of affective experience in three age groups. Psychology and Aging. 1992;7(2):171–184. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.7.2.171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lee YC, Lin YC, Huang CL, Fredrickson BL. The construct and measurement of peace of mind. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2013;14(2):571–590. doi: 10.1007/s10902-012-9343-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Levin ME, Hildebrandt MJ, Lillis J, Hayes SC. The impact of treatment components suggested by the psychological flexibility model: A meta-analysis of laboratory-based component studies. Behavior Therapy. 2012;43(4):741–756. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2012.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Levin ME, MacLane C, Daflos S, Seeley J, Hayes SC, Biglan A, Pistorello J. Examining psychological inflexibility as a transdiagnostic process across psychological disorders. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science. 2014;3(3):155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lomas T. Positive cross-cultural psychology: Exploring similarity and difference in constructions and experiences of wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing. 2015;5(4):60–77. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v5i4.3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lomas T. Life balance and harmony. International Journal of Wellbeing. 2021;11(1):50–68. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v11i1.1477. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lomas T, Ivtzan I. Second wave positive psychology: Exploring the positive–negative dialectics of wellbeing. Journal of Happiness Studies. 2016;17(4):1753–1768. doi: 10.1007/s10902-015-9668-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lomas T, Waters L, Williams P, Oades LG, Kern ML. Third wave positive psychology: Broadening towards complexity. Journal of Positive Psychology. 2021;16(5):660–674. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2020.1805501. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Márquez-González M, De Trocóniz MIF, Cerrato IM, Baltar AL. Emotional experience and regulation across the adult lifespan: comparative analysis in three age groups. Psicothema. 2008;20(4):616–622. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Matsumoto D, Yoo SH. Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2006;1(3):234–250. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mushtaq R, Shoib S, Shah T, Mushtaq S. Relationship between loneliness, psychiatric disorders and physical health ? A review on the psychological aspects of loneliness. Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research. 2014;8:WE01–WE04. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2014/10077.4828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (2016). Mplus. The Comprehensive Modelling Program for Applied Researchers: User's Guide (pp. 5). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
  56. Nesse RM. Good reasons for bad feelings: Insights from the frontier of evolutionary psychiatry. Dutton; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  57. Pigliucci, M. (2020). Stoicism. In Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
  58. Rosellini AJ, Brown TA. Developing and validating clinical questionnaires. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2021;17(1):55–81. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-115343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ruiz FJ, Langer Herrera ÁI, Luciano C, Cangas AJ, Beltrán I. Measuring experiential avoidance and psychological inflexibility: The Spanish version of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II. Psicothema. 2013;25(1):123–129. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2011.239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ryff CD. Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1989;57(6):1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ryff CD. Psychological well-being revisited: Advances in the science and practice of Eudaimonia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 2014;83(1):10–28. doi: 10.1159/000353263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schnell T. The Sources of Meaning and Meaning in Life Questionnaire (SoMe): Relations to demographics and well-being. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2009;4(6):483–499. doi: 10.1080/17439760903271074. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Schönfeld P, Brailovskaia J, Margraf J. Positive and negative mental health across the lifespan: A cross-cultural comparison. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology. 2017;17(3):197–206. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2017.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Schreiber JB, Nora A, Stage FK, Barlow EA, King J. Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: A review. The Journal of Educational Research. 2006;99(6):323–338. doi: 10.3200/JOER.99.6.323-338. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Seligman MEP. Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being. Free Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  66. Seligman MEP, Csikszentmihalyi M. Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist. 2000;55(1):5–14. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Spinhoven P, Drost J, de Rooij M, van Hemert AM, Penninx BW. A longitudinal study of experiential avoidance in emotional disorders. Behavior Therapy. 2014;45(6):840–850. doi: 10.1016/J.BETH.2014.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Steger MF, Oishi S, Kashdan TB. Meaning in life across the life span: Levels and correlates of meaning in life from emerging adulthood to older adulthood. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2009;4(1):43–52. doi: 10.1080/17439760802303127. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Stockton D, Kellett S, Berrios R, Sirois F, Wilkinson N, Miles G. Identifying the underlying mechanisms of change during Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT): A systematic review of contemporary mediation studies. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2019;47(3):332–362. doi: 10.1017/S1352465818000553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wang J, Mann F, Lloyd-Evans B, Ma R, Johnson S. Associations between loneliness and perceived social support and outcomes of mental health problems: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry. 2018;18(1):156. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1736-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54(6):1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Westerhof GJ, Keyes CLM. Mental illness and mental health: The two continua model across the lifespan. Journal of Adult Development. 2010;17(2):110–119. doi: 10.1007/s10804-009-9082-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wong, P. T. P., & Bowers, V. (2018). Mature happiness and global wellbeing in dificult times. In Scientific concepts behind happiness, kindness, and empathy incontemporary society (pp. 112–134). IGI Global.
  74. Wong PTP. Existential positive psychology. In: Lopez S, editor. Encyclopedia of positive psychology. Wiley Blackwell; 2009. pp. 361–368. [Google Scholar]
  75. Wong PTP. Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model of the good life. Canadian Psychology/psychologie Canadienne. 2011;52(2):69–81. doi: 10.1037/a0022511. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wong PTP. From attunement to a meaning-centred good life: Book Review of Daniel Haybron’s Happiness: A very short introduction. International Journal of Wellbeing. 2014;4(2):100–105. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v4i2.5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wong PTP. Made for resilience and happiness: Effective coping with COVID-19. INPM Press; 2020. [Google Scholar]
  78. Wong PTP, Arslan G, Bowers VL, Peacock EJ, Kjell ONE, Ivtzan I, Lomas T. Self-transcendence as a buffer against COVID-19 suffering: The development and validation of the Self-transcendence Measure-B. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;12:648549. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.648549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wong PTP, Roy S. Critique of positive psychology and positive interventions. In: Brown NJL, Lomas T, Eiroa-Orosa FJ, editors. The Routledge international handbook of critical positive psychology. Routledge; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  80. Zevon, M. A., & Tellegen, A. (1982). The structure of mood change: An idiographic/nomothetic analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(1), 111–122. 10.1037/0022-3514.43.1.111

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Code is available upon request from the corresponding author.


Articles from Journal of Happiness Studies are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES