Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatrics. 2022 Aug 1;150(2):e2021056082. doi: 10.1542/peds.2021-056082

Gender Identity 5 Years After Social Transition

Kristina R Olson a, Lily Durwood b, Rachel Horton a, Natalie M Gallagher a, Aaron Devor c
PMCID: PMC9936352  NIHMSID: NIHMS1870074  PMID: 35505568

Abstract

Background and Objectives.

Concerns about early childhood social transitions amongst transgender youth include that these youth may later change their gender identification (i.e., retransition), a process that could be distressing. The present study aimed to provide the first estimate of retransitioning and to report the current gender identities of youth an average of 5 years after their initial social transitions.

Methods.

The present study examined the rate of retransition and current gender identities of 317 initially-transgender youth (208 transgender girls, 109 transgender boys; M=8.1 years at start of study) participating in a longitudinal study, the Trans Youth Project. Data were reported by youth and their parents through in-person or online visits or via email or phone correspondence.

Results.

We found that an average of 5 years after their initial social transition, 7.3% of youth had retransitioned at least once. At the end of this period, most youth identified as binary transgender youth (94%), including 1.3% who retransitioned to another identity before returning to their binary transgender identity. 2.5% of youth identified as cisgender and 3.5% as nonbinary. Later cisgender identities were more common amongst youth whose initial social transition occurred before age 6 years; the retransition often occurred before age 10.

Conclusions.

These results suggest that retransitions are infrequent. More commonly, transgender youth who socially transitioned at early ages continued to identify that way. Nonetheless, understanding retransitions is crucial for clinicians and families to help make them as smooth as possible for youth.

Keywords: Transgender children, social transitions, retransitions, desistence, gender identity, detransition

Introduction

Increasing numbers of children are socially transitioning to live in line with their gender identity, rather than the gender assumed by their sex at birth—a process that typically involves changing a child’s pronouns, first name, hairstyle, and clothing. Some concerns about childhood social transitions have been raised1, including that these children may not continue to identify as transgender, rather they might “retransition” (also called a “detransition” or “desistence”), which some suggest could be distressing for the youth13. Research has suggested that ages 10–13 years may be particularly key times for retransition and that identity may be more stable after this period for youth who show early gender nonconformity3.

Other clinicians argue that early social transitions can be beneficial for some gender-diverse youth46. Some clinicians and scholars who support early childhood social transitions encourage families to remain open to later retransitions7,8, which are seen by some as part of a youth’s exploration of their gender9.

Unfortunately, very little data about retransitions exist in the scientific literature. We have been able to find limited data on the number of youth who socially transition in childhood and then go on to retransition afterwards. One paper included 4 youth who socially transitioned; none of them had retransitioned 7 years later10. We know of three (mentions of) early-transitioning youth who retransition8,9. However, these papers include no mention of how many other youth the same clinical team saw who did not retransition, making it impossible to guess at a retransition rate.

In the present paper we aimed to compute an estimate of retransition amongst a cohort of more than 300 early-transitioning children. Here we report the retransition rate an average of 5 years post-initial (binary) social transition, as well as how many of these participants are living as binary transgender youth, nonbinary youth, and cisgender youth at the same timepoint.

Methods

A total of 317 binary socially-transitioned transgender children (Mage = 8.07; SD= 2.36; 208 initially transgender girls, 109 initially transgender boys; see Table 1 for additional demographics) joined this longitudinal study (The Trans Youth Project, TYP) between July 2013 and December 2017. For inclusion in TYP, children had to be 3–12 years of age and had to have made a “complete” binary social transition10 including changing their pronouns to the binary gender pronouns that differed from those used at their births.

Table 1.

Participant demographics (N=317).

Race
 White Non-Hispanic 68%
 White, Hispanic 9%
 Black 2%
 Asian 3%
 Native American <1%
 Multiracial 17%
 did not report <1%
Annual Household Income
 Less than $25,000 4%
 $25,001–$50,000 10%
 $50,001–$75,000 20%
 $75,001–$125,000 31%
 More than $125,000 35%
 did not report 1%
Geographic Location
 Northeast 13%
 Midwest/Upper Plains 21%
 Southeast 15%
 Mountain West 13%
 Pacific Northwest 20%
 Pacific South 16%

As part of the larger longitudinal study, parents and youth were regularly asked about whether they had begun using puberty blockers and/or gender affirming hormones. At most visits they were not asked about whether puberty had begun, though our available data suggestions that because these youth had socially transitioned at such early ages, most participants were followed by an endocrinologist well-before puberty began. The endocrinologists helped families identify the onset of Tanner 2 (the first stage of puberty) and prescribed puberty blockers within a few months of this time. Therefore, the onset of puberty blockers is used as our proxy for the onset of puberty. Of the youth in this sample, 37 (11.7%) had begun puberty blockers before beginning this study.

This study did not assess whether participants met criteria for the DSM-5 diagnosis of Gender Dysphoria in Children. Many parents in this study did not believe that such diagnoses were either ethical or useful and some children did not experience the required distress criterion. Based on data collected at their initial visit, we do know that these participants showed signs of gender identification and gender-typed preferences commonly associated with their gender, not their sex assigned at birth11. Further, parent report using the Gender Identity Questionnaire for Children12, indicated that youth showed significant “cross-sex” identification and preferences (when scored based on sex at birth)12.

Final identity classification for these analyses was based on our most recent interaction with the child and/or their parent before January 1, 2021. Because some families have not participated recently, we also separately report (in Table 2) the results of the n=291 youth with whom the research team had an interaction within the 2 years prior to that deadline. This additional analysis allows us to assess whether those who retransitioned were more likely to have missed their more recent appointments with our team. Importantly, only one of the 26 families with whom we did not meet in the last two years has formally dropped out of the study; the others often did not complete participation during these two years due to personal circumstances at the time we attempted re-recruitment. We anticipate that many in this group will participate again in the future.

Table 2.

Participant information and current identity at last visit before January 1, 2021 Overall, for those with recent visits only, and by initial social transition and gender.

Total Sample Recent Sample (with visits in 2019 or 2020) Sample who initially socially transitioned before age 6 Sample who initially socially transitioned at 6 or later Transgender girls (at recruitment) Transgender boys (at recruitment)
Sample Size 317 291 124 193 208 109
% assigned male at birth 65.6% 65.3% 73.4% 60.6% 100% 0%
Mean age at first transition 6.5 years old 6.4 years old 4.3 years old 7.9 years old 6.2 years old 7.1 years old
Mean age at start of study 8.1 years old 8.0 years old 5.9 years old 9.5 years old 7.7 years old 8.7 years old
Average time since start of study 3.8 years 4.1 years 3.8 years 3.8 years 3.9 years 3.7 years
Average time since first transition 5.4 years 5.7 years 5.4 years 5.4 years 5.5 years 5.3 years
Current Identity
Binary Transgender n=298; 94.0% n=276; 94.8% n=112; n=90.3% n=186; 96.4% n = 194; 93.3% n = 104; 95.4%
Cisgender n=8; 2.5% n=6; 2.1% n=7; 5.6% n=1; 0.5% n = 7; 3.40% n = 1; 0.9%
Nonbinary n=11; 3.5% n=9; 3.1% n=5; 4.0% n=6; 3.1% n = 7; 3.40% n = 4; 3.7%

Based on pronouns at follow-up, participants were classified as binary transgender (pronouns associated with the other binary assigned sex), nonbinary (they/them pronouns or, n=3, a mix of they/them and binary pronouns), or cisgender (pronouns associated with their assigned sex). We confirmed this classification by reviewing other information available to the research team (e.g., child’s self-categorization in an interview or survey, email communications with the parents, etc). Only one classification was debatable; this participant was classified by pronouns (and in this paper) as nonbinary, but could have been classified as binary transgender (and not retransitioned).

This study has been approved by the University of Washington and Princeton University IRBs.

Results

The overall rate of retransition was 7.3%. An average of 5.37 years (SD=1.74 years) after their initial binary social transition, most participants were living as binary transgender youth (94.0%; see Table 2). Included in this group were 4 individuals (1.3% of the total sample) who retransitioned twice (to nonbinary then back to binary transgender). Some youth (3.5%) were currently living as nonbinary, including one who had retransitioned twice (to cisgender then to nonbinary). Finally, 2.5% were using pronouns associated with their sex at birth and could be categorized as cisgender at the time of data collection, including one who first retransitioned to live as nonbinary. Similar percentages were observed when examining the 291 youth who were in touch with the research team in the last two years (see Table 2), when examining only those 280 youth who had not begun puberty blockers at the start of the study (Table 3), or if we examine only the 200 youth who had gone at least 5 years since their initial transition (Table 3).

Table 3.

Participant information and current identity at last visit before January 1, 2021 as a function of stages of medical transition and/or age.

Total Sample Sample of youth who had not begun blockers at start of the study Sample of youth who have begun blockers (and not GAH) at the end of the study Sample of youth who have begun gender-affirming hormones at the end of the study Sample of youth 5+ Years since initial binary social transition Sample of youth who are currently 14+ years old
Sample Size 317 280 92 98 200 70
% assigned male at birth 65.6% 69.6% 57.6% 58.2% 69.0% 52.9%
Mean age at first transition 6.5 years old 6.1 years old 6.6 years old 8.4 years old 6.2 years old 8.9 years old
Mean age at start of study 8.1 years old 7.6 years old 8.3 years old 10.2 years old 8.0 years old 10.8 years old
Average time since start of study 3.8 years 3.9 years 4 years 4.3 years 4.5 years 4.4 years
Average time since first transition 5.4 years 5.5 years 5.8 years 6.1 years 6.4 years 6.3 years
Current Identity
Binary Transgender n=298; 94.0% n = 263; 93.9% n = 88; 95.7% n = 97; 99.0% n=190; 95.0% n = 69; 98.6%
Cisgender n=8; 2.5% n = 8; 2.9% n = 1; 1.1% n = 0 n=4; 2.0% n = 1; 1.4%
Nonbinary n=11; 3.5% n = 9; 3.2% n = 3; 3.3% n = 1, 1.0% n=6; 3.0% n = 0

We observed one potential (post-hoc) age effect. Youth who initially socially transitioned before age 6 (n=124), were more likely to be living as cisgender (5.6%), than youth who transitioned at age 6 or later (n=193; 0.5%), Fisher’s exact test (comparing binary, cisgender, nonbinary; before vs. 6 or later), p =0.02, although low rates of retransition were seen in both groups. In Table 2 we also report the results separately for children assigned male vs. female at birth; this distinction was not significantly associated with later identity, p = 0.47, Fisher’s exact test. Finally, for exploratory purposes, in Table 3 we report outcomes separately for several subsets of our participants, including youth who had started puberty blockers, youth who had used puberty blockers and gender affirming hormones, and youth who are at least 14 years old (the age at which past work3 has suggested retransitions will be less likely).

Discussion

Five years after an initial binary social transition, 7% of youth had retransitioned at least once. Most youth (94%) were living as binary transgender youth at the time of data analysis, including 1.3% who retransitoned initially to cisgender or nonbinary and then retransitioned back to binary trans identities. A small number of youth were living as cisgender youth (2.5%) or nonbinary youth (3.5%). We observed comparable rates when examining all participants who began the study (n=317), those who continue to be in regular contact with the research team (n=291), those who had gone at least 5 years since initial social transition (n=200), and those who started the study before beginning puberty blockers (n=280). We found no differences as a function of participant sex at birth. We observed slightly higher rates of retransition, and particularly later cisgender identity, amongst youth who initially socially transitioned before age 6. However, even in these youth, retransition rates were very low.

Amongst those who had begun puberty blockers and/or gender affirming hormones, only one had retransitioned to live as cisgender (and this youth had begun blockers, but not gender affirming hormones). One likely reason so few retransitions to cisgender occurred amongst those accessing medical transition is that most retransitioning in this cohort happened at early ages. All but one of the 8 cisgender youth had retransitioned by age 9 (the last retransitioned at 11). Some of these youth are still not eligible for blockers because they are still prepubertal; we anticipate that those who identify as cisgender are unlikely to seek blockers or hormones, but that the participants who have not begun puberty and who identify as binary transgender or nonbinary likely will.

Past work has suggested that the ages 10 to 13 are an especially critical time for retransition3. In our sample, many of the youth who retransitioned did so before that time frame, particularly the cisgender youth. In the nonbinary group, however, 6 of 11 retransitioned between age 10 and 13, with the remainder retransitioning before 10. Importantly, our sample differed from the past work upon which this age range was determined in several key ways including that our participants socially transitioned at earlier ages (perhaps pushing retransitions earlier too), had undergone complete social transitions including pronouns and names (not just hairstyle and clothing changes as in most cases in previous studies3), and are living at a different historic time in a different country. Any, or all, of these may turn out to be key differences related to age of retransition.

Our observed low retransition rate is consistent with a study in which 4 youth who had “completely” socially transitioned had not retransitioned 7 years later10. That finding is in the same ballpark as our study’s estimate of approximately 2.5% if we examine the percentage living as cisgender at the end of the study (i.e., those “desisting” from gender diverse outcomes). Together these papers suggest this outcome is relatively rare in this group.

Our observation that few youth who have begun medical intervention have retransitoned to live as cisgender is consistent with findings in the literature. Several studies reporting on outcomes amongst transgender youth receiving blockers and gender affirming hormones have reported relatively low rates of regret or stopping treatment13, which are potential indicators of retransition – though stopping treatment can occur for other reasons as well (e.g., side effects), as can regret (e.g., experiences of transphobia).

Our key finding – that there was a relatively low rate of retransition about five years after initial social transition – may, on the surface, appear contradictory with past clinic-based research on what is sometimes called “persistence and desistence” 3 of childhood gender dysphoria. Several large studies attempted to recontact adolescents and adults who had previously been evaluated for gender dysphoria in childhood1417. Many of those were formally diagnosed with what was, at the time, called Gender Identity Disorder. Those studies reported that a minority of youth later identified in a way that might indicate a transgender identity by today’s definition.

Interpretation of those results, and especially comparison to the present work is difficult for several reasons. First, in past studies, when asked “are you a boy or a girl?” about 90% of the children supplied answers that aligned with their sex at birth18, leading some to question whether or not the majority of those children were the equivalent of transgender children today or not 1921. Second, participants in those studies were children between the 1960’s and the 1990’s, and many features of society have changed since then, including greater rates of acceptance and acknowledgement of transgender identities. Third, the parents of the youth in the present study support their children’s identities, as indicated by their approval of their social transitions, while many of the parents of youth in past studies explicitly discouraged gender nonconformity or “cross-gender” identification15,22. Further, it would have been exceedingly rare for youth in those studies to socially transition, especially completely1,10. Finally, there were substantial drop-out rates in all of the prior studies14,15,17, making the true estimates of persistence or desistence difficult to obtain19,21. Because there are so many possible contributors to differences in rates of “persistence” (in past work) and “retransition” in the current work, we urge caution about overinterpreting differences, or overconfidence about which contributing factors explain the differences.

There are also some reasons why we might have had such a low retransition rate. First, on average, participants had socially transitioned 1.6 years before joining our study. It is possible that some youth initially try socially transitioning and then change their minds quickly. Such youth would be unlikely to be enrolled in this study because their eligibility period would have been quite short and therefore the odds of finding the study and completing it would have been low. This means the children in our study may have been especially unlikely, compared to all children who transition, to retransition because they had already lived – and presumably been fairly content – with that initial transition for more than a year. Second, it is possible that families who failed to participate in the last two years of our study (n=26) were disproportionately those whose children retransitioned and who were therefore hesitant to participate again. If true, this could have reduced our retransition rate. We are skeptical of this possibility for a few reasons. First, four of these participants did retransition and had told us about that outcome, so it does not appear that hesitancy in telling us was rampant in this group. Third, many of these families continue to be in touch with our research team and only missed participation because of ongoing personal issues (e.g., COVID-19, emergency family circumstances, etc.). We anticipate that most of these families will be able to participate as we continue to follow these youth. Finally, from the beginning of the study, the research team has been clear in discussing with the families that we are open to any outcome in their youth.

As with past work, the present work has several key limitations. First, this is a volunteer community sample, meaning there could be biases in the kinds of families who sign up to participate. We know, for example, that unlike many samples of transgender youth, this sample of youth have normative levels of depression and only slight elevations in anxiety23. The parents of the participants in this study are disproportionately higher income and went to college at higher rates than the general population. We do not know whether these potential biases in the sample reflect biases in the cohort of children who socially transitioned in the mid-2010’s in the U.S. and Canada. Therefore, whether the results generalize to youth without these characteristics is unknown.

Another potential limitation is that we used pronouns as the criteria for retransitions. Not everyone who, for example, uses they/them pronouns identifies as nonbinary and someone might identify as transgender even if they are currently using pronouns associated with their sex at birth. However, examination of other data provided by families suggests that our pronoun-based criteria were largely consistent with classification that would have arisen from other types of information provided to the research team (e.g., labels used in an interview). Only one of the youth categorized as “retransitioned” might, by some other criteria, not meet that definition. However, because pronouns were the initial inclusion criterion (that is, to be in the study a child had to be using pronouns not associated with their sex at birth), they were the most consistent route of classification.

A related potential concern with these analyses is that we classified a change from using, for example, binary transgender to nonbinary as a “retransition.” Not everyone would categorize this change as a retransition. Many nonbinary people consider themselves to be transgender24. If we had used a stricter criterion of retransition, more similar to the common use of terms like “detransition” or “desistence”, referring only to youth who are living as cisgender, then our retransition rate would have been lower (2.5%).

One additional limitation in the present work is that the initial sample was disproportionately made up of trans girls. This is counter to recent reports that more peri- and post-pubertal transgender youth seeking clinical services recently are transmasculine2527. Historically, and consistent with our data, samples of prepubertal gender nonconforming youth identified by their parents as such, have included more assigned males at birth15,16,22. Importantly, we did not observe a significant gender effect in terms of rates of retransition, so we do not predict any change in pattern of results if we had a different ratio of participants by sex at birth.

We anticipate continuing to follow this cohort into adolescence and adulthood. This continued follow-up is necessary because it is possible that as more youth move into adolescence and adulthood, their identities could change. As we already saw, some youth will retransition more than once so the present identities should not be interpreted as final.

As more youth are coming out and being supported in their transitions early in development, it is increasingly critical that clinicians understand the experiences of this cohort and not make assumptions about them as a function of older data from youth who lived under different circumstances. Though we can never predict the exact gender trajectory of any child, these data suggest that many youth who identify as transgender early, and are supported through a social transition, will continue to identify as transgender five years after initial social transition. These results also suggest that retransitions to one’s gender assumed at birth (cisgender) might be likely to occur before age 10 amongst those who socially transition at the earliest ages (before age 6), though retransitions are still unlikely in this group. These data suggest that parents and clinicians should be informed that not all youth will continue on the same trajectory over time. Further understanding of how to support youth’s initial and later transitions is needed.

Article Summary.

This article examines the frequency of retransition and current gender identity amongst youth who identified as binary transgender children earlier in childhood.

What’s Known on This Subject.

There has been considerable debate about early childhood social transitions and whether they will lead to high rates of retransition. However, less is known about rates of retransition or identity outcomes following social transitions in childhood.

What This Study Adds.

This study provides an estimate of the frequency of retransitions amongst children who socially transitioned before age 12 and provides an update on their current identities, an average of 5 years after their initial social transitions.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the families for their participation in this research.

Role of Funder/Sponsor (if any): Funders played no role in determining the research questions asked nor have they seen the results prior to review.

Funding/Support:

The collection of the data in this report and some of the authors’ time (KO, RH, and NG) over the course of this project have been supported by: National Institutes of Health grant HD092347, National Science Foundation grants BCS-1523632, SMA-1837857, and SMA-2041463, a grant from the Arcus Foundation (all to KO), and a National Science Foundation GRFP grant to LD. In addition, KO receives financial support from the MacArthur Foundation.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest Disclosures (includes financial disclosures): All authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Clinical Trial Registration (if any): none

References

  • 1.Steensma TD, Cohen-kettenis PT. Gender transitioning before puberty? Arch Sex Behav. 2011;40(4):649–650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.de Vries ALC, Cohen-Kettenis PT. Clinical management of gender dysphoria in children and adolescents: the Dutch approach. J Homosex. 2012;59(3):301–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Steensma TD, Biemond R, de Boer F, Cohen-Kettenis PT. Desisting and persisting gender dysphoria after childhood: A qualitative follow-up study. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;16(4):499–516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ashley F Thinking an ethics of gender exploration: Against delaying transition for transgender and gender creative youth. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2019;24(2):223–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sherer I (2016). Social transition: Supporting our youngest transgender children. Pediatrics. 2016; 137(3):e20154358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Temple Newhook J, Pyne J, Winters K, Feder S, Holmes C, Tosh J, Sinnott MJ, Jamieson A, Pickett S. A critical commentary on follow-up studies and “desistance” theories about transgender and gender-nonconforming children. Intl J Transgenderism. 2018; 19(2): 212–224. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Leibowitz S Social gender transition and the psychological interventions. In: Janssen A, Leibowitz S, eds. Affirmative Mental Health Care for Transgender and Gender Diverse Youth. Springer International Publishing; 2018:31–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Edwards-Leeper L, Spack NP. Psychological evaluation and medical treatment of transgender youth in an interdisciplinary “gender management service” (Gems) in a major pediatric center. J Homosex. 2012;59(3):321–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Menvielle E A comprehensive program for children with gender variant behaviors and gender identity disorders. J Homosex. 2012;59(3):357–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Steensma TD, McGuire JK, Kreukels BP, Beekman AJ, Cohen-Kettenis PT. Factors associated with desistence and persistence of childhood gender dysphoria: a quantitative follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013. Jun 1;52(6):582–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gülgöz S, Glazier J, Enright E et al. Similarity in transgender and cisgender children’s gender development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2019;116(49):24480–24485. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1909367116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Johnson L, Bradley S, Birkenfeld-Adams A et al. A Parent-Report Gender Identity Questionnaire for Children. Arch Sex Behav. 2004;33(2):105–116. doi: 10.1023/b:aseb.0000014325.68094.f3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kuper L, Stewart S, Preston S, Lau M, Lopez X. Body Dissatisfaction and Mental Health Outcomes of Youth on Gender-Affirming Hormone Therapy. Pediatrics. 2020;145(4). doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-3006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Drummond K, Bradley S, Peterson-Badali M, Zucker K. A follow-up study of girls with gender identity disorder. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(1):34–45. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.34 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Green R The Sissy Boy Syndrome. 1987. doi: 10.2307/j.ctt1ww3v4c [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Singh D, Bradley S, Zucker K. A Follow-Up Study of Boys With Gender Identity Disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.632784 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wallien M, Cohen-Kettenis P. Psychosexual Outcome of Gender-Dysphoric Children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008;47(12):1413–1423. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31818956b9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zucker K, Bradley S, Sullivan C, Kuksis M, Birkenfeld-Adams A, Mitchell J. A Gender Identity Interview for Children. J Pers Assess. 1993;61(3):443–456. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6103_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ashley F The clinical irrelevance of “desistance” research for transgender and gender creative youth. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers. 2021. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000504 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Olson K Prepubescent Transgender Children: What We Do and Do Not Know. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2016;55(3):155–156.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.11.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Temple Newhook J, Pyne J, Winters K et al. A critical commentary on follow-up studies and “desistance” theories about transgender and gender-nonconforming children. International Journal of Transgenderism. 2018;19(2):212–224. doi: 10.1080/15532739.2018.1456390 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zucker K, Bradley S. Gender Identity Disorder And Psychosexual Problems In Children And Adolescents. New York: Guilford Press; 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gibson D, Glazier J, Olson K. Evaluation of Anxiety and Depression in a Community Sample of Transgender Youth. JAMA Netw Open. 2021;4(4):e214739. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.4739 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Darwin H Challenging the Cisgender/Transgender Binary: Nonbinary People and the Transgender Label. Gender & Society. 2020;34(3):357–380. doi: 10.1177/0891243220912256 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.15. Aitken M, Steensma T, Blanchard R et al. Evidence for an Altered Sex Ratio in Clinic-Referred Adolescents with Gender Dysphoria. J Sex Med. 2015;12(3):756–763. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.de Graaf N, Carmichael P, Steensma T, Zucker K. Evidence for a Change in the Sex Ratio of Children Referred for Gender Dysphoria: Data From the Gender Identity Development Service in London (2000–2017). J Sex Med. 2018;15(10):1381–1383. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2018.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Meyenburg B, Renter-Schmidt K, Schmidt G. Transidentität in Jugend und Adoleszenz: Zur Veränderung der Sexratio und der Prävalenz in den letzten eineinhalb Jahrzehnten. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie. 2021;49(2):93–100. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917/a000763 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES