Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2023 Mar 8;290(1994):20222108. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2022.2108

The effectiveness of pseudomagic traits in promoting premating isolation

Thomas G Aubier 1,2,, Reinhard Bürger 3, Maria R Servedio 1
PMCID: PMC9993058  PMID: 36883275

Abstract

Upon the secondary contact of populations, speciation with gene flow is greatly facilitated when the same pleiotropic loci are both subject to divergent ecological selection and induce non-random mating, leading to loci with this fortuitous combination of functions being referred to as ‘magic trait’ loci. We use a population genetics model to examine whether ‘pseudomagic trait’ complexes, composed of physically linked loci fulfilling these two functions, are as efficient in promoting premating isolation as magic traits. We specifically measure the evolution of choosiness, which controls the strength of assortative mating. We show that, surprisingly, pseudomagic trait complexes, and to a lesser extent also physically unlinked loci, can lead to the evolution of considerably stronger assortative mating preferences than do magic traits, provided polymorphism at the involved loci is maintained. This is because assortative mating preferences are generally favoured when there is a risk of producing maladapted recombinants, as occurs with non-magic trait complexes but not with magic traits (since pleiotropy precludes recombination). Contrary to current belief, magic traits may not be the most effective genetic architecture for promoting strong premating isolation. Therefore, distinguishing between magic traits and pseudomagic trait complexes is important when inferring their role in premating isolation. This calls for further fine-scale genomic research on speciation genes.

Keywords: speciation, mate choice, recombination, secondary contact, third-order linkage disequilibrium, mathematical model

1. Introduction

Assortative mating—the tendency of individuals of similar phenotype to mate more often than expected by chance—has been reported in animals [1,2] and plants [3,4], and plays a key role in generating premating reproductive isolation during ecological speciation [5]. When assortative mating is driven by mate choice, the mating rule and nature of the loci underlying assortative mating have important implications for the speciation process [6]. For instance, speciation is less likely to occur if assortative mating is associated with a ‘preference/trait rule’ (i.e. preferences for specific traits used as mating signals) compared to a ‘matching rule’ (i.e. preference for matching mates), partly because speciation under a preference/trait rule requires linkage disequilibrium (i.e. statistical associations between alleles) between more loci [7]. This well-established theoretical result demonstrates that linkage disequilibrium between the loci underlying premating isolation can have a significant impact on speciation with gene flow.

In concert with assortative mating, divergent selection is a key component of speciation with gene flow, as divergent selection drives the incipient species apart [5]. Notably, many models of speciation have shown that across generations, selection generates linkage disequilibrium between ecological loci under divergent selection and loci coding for mating signals [710]. All loci subject to divergent selection thus have the potential to strengthen the premating isolation caused by mate choice.

Given that recombination consistently degrades allelic associations, tight physical linkage between the loci underlying premating isolation facilitates the maintenance of strong linkage disequilibrium [7]. In particular, a strong link between divergent ecological selection and assortative mating is guaranteed when the mating signal is itself under divergent ecological selection. Such pleiotropic traits that are both subject to divergent ecological selection and used as the basis of assortative mating (so-called ‘magic traits’ [11]) can facilitate speciation for the simple reason that there is no recombination degrading the link between divergent selection and assortative mating. In other words, pleiotropy of the underlying genes is much more effective than linkage disequilibrium in transmitting the force of divergent ecological selection to the genes causing premating isolation, thereby favouring divergence between incipient species [8,11,12]

While many theoretical models of speciation assume magic traits [8,1317], it can be very difficult to demonstrate that candidate traits are pleiotropic [6,18]. In several strong candidate examples of magic traits, it is not yet possible to distinguish between pleiotropy and tight linkage. For instance, the co-localization of quantitative trait loci for traits under divergent selection and traits involved in assortative mating found in Acyrthosiphon pisum pea aphids [19] or in Mimulus [20] is actually consistent with both possibilities.

Several authors have speculated that ‘non-magic’ trait complexes, i.e. loci that are coding separately for ecological traits and mating signals, could conceivably mimic the role of magic traits in the speciation process when these loci are tightly physically linked [18,21,22]. Strong linkage disequilibrium between a pair of loci—one ecological locus subject to divergent selection and one mating signal locus affecting reproductive isolation—could substitute for the pleiotropic characteristic of true magic traits, assuming that this linkage disequilibrium is strong from the outset of its involvement in speciation (e.g. upon the secondary contact of divergent populations) and assuming that the physical linkage between loci is tight enough [18]. Nonetheless, it is only recently that this intuitive idea has been investigated formally with a population genetics model, with one ecological locus under divergent selection and one locus that acts as a mating signal [23]. Upon secondary contact, such non-magic trait complexes are actually very effective in promoting divergence, and surprisingly this is also the case if the loci are loosely physically linked, or even if they are physically unlinked (i.e. located on different chromosomes).

Given that non-magic trait complexes mimic magic traits in terms of the ecological divergence they allow, complexes of physically linked loci coding for separate ecological traits and mating signals have been called ‘pseudomagic trait’ complexes [23]. Although the adjective ‘pseudomagic’ was originally coined to describe gene complexes where tight linkage was indistinguishable from pleiotropy [23], we use it here to describe any physically linked gene complex. Therefore, ‘non-magic’ trait complexes include pseudomagic trait complexes and freely recombining ecological and mating signal loci. Nevertheless, the question of whether these pseudomagic trait complexes promote speciation in the same way as magic traits cannot be answered on the basis of their effect on divergence alone. The establishment of premating isolation additionally relies on the strength of mate choice (hereafter called ‘choosiness’), which can also be genetically encoded and can, therefore, be subject to evolution [6]. Linkage disequilibrium plays an important role in the evolution of choosiness (or any ‘one-allele mechanism’ [7]). Yet, magic traits and pseudomagic trait complexes vary notably in terms of the linkage disequilibrium that can be built.

In the case of a magic trait, the evolution of choosiness relies on the linkage disequilibrium between the choosiness locus and the locus encoding the magic trait [8,1315]. By contrast, in the case of a pseudomagic trait complex, there are three types of genetic associations which can affect the evolution of choosiness: linkage disequilibria of the choosiness locus with the ecological locus, with the mating signal locus, and with the combination of these two loci (so-called ‘three-way linkage disequilibrium’ or ‘third-order linkage disequilibrium’). The fact that the sources of indirect selection are so different makes it unlikely that the same level of choosiness will evolve with a magic trait and with a pseudomagic trait complex. The evolution of choosiness is thus expected to depend on the genetic architecture underlying ecological and mating signal traits, i.e. on whether choosiness evolves alongside a magic trait or a pseudomagic trait complex.

Using a two-island, three-locus population genetics model, we explore the degree to which pleiotropy (a magic trait) versus varying degrees of physical linkage between an ecological locus and a mating signal locus (pseudomagic trait complexes, and non-magic traits complexes with physically unlinked loci) can affect the evolution of choosiness upon secondary contact. Our analyses highlight that, although magic traits favour trait divergence and the maintenance of polymorphism, pseudomagic trait complexes, and to a lesser extent physically unlinked loci, can promote stronger choosiness (and premating isolation) than do magic traits. This is because assortative mating preferences are favoured when there is a risk of producing maladapted recombinants, as occurs with non-magic trait complexes but not with magic traits.

2. Methods

(a) . The model

We use a population genetics model to consider a secondary contact scenario in which assortative mating evolves based on three haploid diallelic loci. We implement an ecological locus E, subject to divergent viability selection in both sexes, and a mating signal locus T also expressed in both sexes, on which females base their mate choice using a matching rule (where females prefer to mate with males that have the same mating signal allele as their own; see [6] for a review of the theoretical and empirical literature on phenotype matching). In addition, we implement a choosiness locus C expressed in females. Importantly, choosiness is ecologically neutral and not subject to direct selection. Nevertheless, it undergoes indirect selection via linkage disequilibrium with the other loci (or via linkage disequilibrium with the pleiotropic locus when we consider a magic trait). Importantly, analyses of variants of the model presented here show that our qualitative results are generalizable to diploids and to the evolution of costly choosiness (see electronic supplementary material, text).

We assume that two allopatric populations have diverged and that secondary contact occurs between these populations. Each generation first undergoes symmetric migration with rate m. Next, divergent selection occurs. The ecological trait encoded by locus E is locally adapted, such that each ecological allele, E1 and E2, is favoured by viability selection (with selection coefficient s) in the population in which it was common in allopatry. Mating follows, during which, females may express different propensities to mate with males. Choosy females prefer to mate with males that match their own mating signal trait encoded by locus T. For instance, a T1 female is more likely to mate with a T1 male than a T2 male upon encounter. Such mate choice generates positive frequency-dependent sexual selection acting on the T locus, because males carrying the locally rare allele at the T locus are unpopular and thus have low mating success [14]. Locus C determines the strength of female preference (hereafter, choosiness), so that C1 (resp. C2) females are 1 + α1 (resp. 1 + α2) times more likely to mate with a preferred male than with a non-preferred male, if they were to encounter one of each. The expected genotype frequencies in the next generation depend on the probabilities of mating between different genotypes, with the new generation being formed by assuming Mendelian inheritance at all loci. In particular, we assume that gene order is ETC (we relax this assumption in analyses shown in the electronic supplementary material), and that recombination occurs at a rate rET between loci E and T, and rTC between loci T and C. We consider that there is no crossover interference, so that the recombination rate between the E and C loci is determined by the other recombination rates. We assume that loci E and T are initially at maximum linkage disequilibrium in each subpopulation, so that genotypes E1T2 and E2T1 are initially absent. We can thus model a magic trait by setting rET = 0, a pseudomagic trait complex by assuming 0 < rET < 0.5, and physically unlinked E and T loci by considering rET = 0.5.

All recursion equations are detailed in electronic supplementary material, text.

(b) . Migration–selection equilibrium and the maintenance of polymorphism

We initiate the populations assuming that the choosiness allele C1 is fixed in the two populations; this corresponds to the two-island, two-locus pseudomagic model analysed in a previous study [23]. Following this previous study, we assume that the two allopatric populations have diverged such that all individuals in population 1 have the E1 and T1 alleles and that most individuals in population 2 have the E2 and T2 alleles (at a frequency equal to 0.99; other individuals in population 2 have the E1 and T1 alleles), thereby avoiding possible artefacts of starting with symmetric initial conditions.

To assess the migration–selection equilibrium, we run numerical iterations until the change in each genotype frequency is less than 10−9 per generation. We can thus numerically assess the allelic frequencies and the linkage disequilibria between loci (i.e. statistical associations of alleles at different loci) at migration–selection equilibrium. As a check, the assumption that there is no variation in choosiness allows us to reproduce results of the previous study mentioned earlier [23], including the finding, important for the current study, that divergence at the ecological and mating signal loci, E and T, peaks at intermediate choosiness values (figure 1). Indeed, very high choosiness causes rare, very choosy females to mate with rare males in proportion to their frequency, resulting in the loss of positive frequency-dependent sexual selection and thus the reduction or loss of mating signal divergence [16]. Additionally, polymorphism at the T locus can be maintained, in particular for a low recombination rate between the E and T loci (low rET) and for an intermediate choosiness value (intermediate α1; remember that we assume here that allele C2 is absent) (figure 1; as in [23]). Importantly, under random mating (α1 = 0), we observe that polymorphism at the T locus can be maintained only for rET = 0; with random mating and rET > 0, the T locus is neutral and polymorphism can thus not be maintained at it [23].

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Population divergence depending on genetic architecture and choosiness. We represent the equilibria reached in the two populations after secondary contact for a range of values of choosiness α1 (assuming that allele C2 is absent) and recombination rates rET, with initial maximum linkage disequilibrium between the E and T loci. We do not represent cases where polymorphism is lost at the T locus (for very low or very high choosiness, as shown where lines end, where frequencies actually fall to zero). (a) The equilibrium frequencies e2,k of allele E2 in each population k. (b) The equilibrium frequencies t2,k of allele T2 in each population k. Double-headed arrows are placed at the choosiness value maximizing divergence at the E and T loci in (a) and (b), respectively. Single-headed arrows correspond to ESS choosiness values that are favoured for rTC = 0.01 (see figure 2). In (b), the double-headed arrows overlap. Note that the divergence-maximizing choosiness (double-headed arrows) and the ESS choosiness (single-headed arrows) do not have the same value, unless rET = 0; this means that additional evolutionary forces, specific to the case where rET > 0 and which are the focus of our study, come into play. See ref. [23] for explanations on the divergence pattern. Here, m = 0.01, s = 0.05.

We next determine the lowest choosiness value, αpoly, that allows the maintenance of polymorphism at both the E and T loci. Using numerical methods detailed in the Mathematica notebook [24], we determine αpoly with a precision of 10−3. Notably, if αpoly > 0, higher choosiness cannot evolve from random mating (α1 = 0), assuming that migration–selection equilibrium without polymorphism at the T locus is reached rapidly following initial conditions (i.e. before mutation at the C locus occurs). Indeed, choosiness is neutral if there is no polymorphism at the T locus.

(c) . Invasion of a choosier allele and evolutionarily stable strategy choosiness

Our central issue, which is to study the evolution of choosiness which controls the strength of assortative mating, is tackled by assuming ancestral choosiness that allows the maintenance of polymorphism at the E and T loci and by numerically assessing the invasion conditions of choosiness alleles introduced in low frequency in a population at migration–selection equilibrium. We can thus determine the choosiness value, αESS, that constitutes an evolutionary stable strategy (ESS; i.e. the ESS choosiness that is always favoured) in cases where choosiness evolves alongside a magic trait, versus a pseudomagic trait complex, versus a non-magic trait complex with physically unlinked loci.

More precisely, we start the population at the migration–selection equilibrium at the E and T loci given the ancestral choosiness, which is encoded by allele C1. We initially assume that allele C1 codes for the choosiness value α1 = αpoly, the lowest value that allows the maintenance of polymorphism. The allele C2, coding for a higher choosiness value so that α2 = α1 + Δα, is then introduced in linkage equilibrium with the other loci, in the same frequency (=0.01) in both populations. After 200 generations, if the allele C2 has increased in frequency in the two populations, we assume that it is able to invade and to completely replace the allele C1. We verified that invasion of the mutant allele at the C locus eventually leads to fixation, by checking that the mutant allele cannot be invaded by the allele coding for lower choosiness. If allele C2 is able to invade, we consider in another simulation that the ancestral allele C1 codes for the choosiness value previously encoded by allele C2, and we repeat the process by assessing whether an even choosier allele can invade. The evolutionarily stable choosiness αESS corresponds to the choosiness value encoded by C1 that is robust to invasion by a choosier allele C2. We checked that this allele coding for αESS is also robust to invasion by an allele coding for lower choosiness, so that α2 = α1 − Δα.

Δα corresponds to the mutation effect size. To assess the ESS choosiness, we consider a small mutation effect size Δα = 0.01, but in other analyses where we track the full invasion of a mutation, we consider larger mutation effect sizes.

(d) . Contribution of linkage disequilibria to frequency change at the choosiness locus

The change in frequency at the C locus in population k, Δc2,k, due to viability selection, mating and the production of zygotes can be decomposed into different components,

Δc2,k=DTC,k×fT,k(t2,k,c2,k,α1,α2)+DEC,k×fE,k(e2,k,s)+DETC,k×fET,k(e2,k,t2,k,c2,k,α1,α2,s)+gk(e2,k,t2,k,c2,k,DET,k,DTC,k,DEC,k,DETC,k,α1,α2,s), 2.1

where s is the selection coefficient during viability selection, α1 and α2 are the choosiness values and e2,k, t2,k and c2,k are the allelic frequencies after migration at the E, T and C loci, respectively. Furthermore, DTC,k, DEC,k and DET,k are the linkage disequilibria after migration between loci T and C, E and C, and E and T, respectively. Finally, DETC,k is the three-way linkage disequilibrium after migration between loci E, T and C.

The component DTC,k×fT,k(t2,k,c2,k,α1,α2) corresponds to the first-order contribution of DTC,k to the change in frequency at the C locus, with fT,k(t2,k,c2,k,α1,α2) being the first-order approximation of the Barton–Turelli selection coefficient a^T,k (this component can be isolated in this way because there is no direct selection on locus C, which could also contain terms to the first order in DTC,k [25]). In other words, this term reflects how sexual selection at the locus T contributes to the evolution at the locus C via linkage disequilibrium. Likewise, the component DEC,k×fE,k(e2,k,s) reflects how viability selection at the locus E contributes to the evolution at the locus C via linkage disequilibrium. The component DETC,k×fET,k(e2,k,t2,k,c2,k,α1,α2,s) reflects how epistatic selection at the E and T loci contributes to the evolution at the locus C via three-way linkage disequilibrium. Finally, the term described by function g corresponds to the sum of all other higher-order contributions of DEC,k, DTC,k and DETC,k, e.g. the terms associated with DEC,k×DTC,k, DEC,k×DETC,k, etc. This last term is generally negligible relative to the first three terms (electronic supplementary material, figures S1 and S2).

We can show analytically that the functions fT,k and fET,k are positive or negative depending on whether the local allelic frequencies at the E and T loci are greater or less than 1/2, and that the function fE,k is negative for k = 1 and positive for k = 2 (see Mathematica notebook). Therefore, in our simulations where allelic divergence occurs (figure 1), the signs of the linkage disequilibria DTC,k, DEC,k and DETC,k determine the direction of indirect sexual selection, indirect viability selection and indirect epistatic selection, respectively (electronic supplementary material, figures S3 and S4); in particular, in population 2 where allelic frequencies at the E and T loci are greater than 1/2, positive (resp. negative) linkage disequilibria lead to positive (resp. negative) indirect selection on choosiness.

3. Results

(a) . Magic traits, pseudomagic trait complexes and the evolution of choosiness

With a magic trait (rET = 0), divergence at the pleiotropic locus, which is both under divergent ecological selection and used as a mating signal, and thus fulfils the functions of both E and T loci, is maximized for an intermediate choosiness value (black double-headed arrows in figure 1; as explained previously). In addition, the ESS choosiness promoted with a magic trait corresponds to the choosiness value that maximizes divergence (black single-headed arrows in figure 1). This ESS choosiness is promoted by indirect viability and sexual selection, through the build-up of linkage disequilibrium between the choosiness locus and the pleiotropic locus [16] (see also electronic supplementary material, text). Because it is set by selection (in this case indirect), it is an example of partial reproductive isolation evolving as an adaptive optimum [26].

We now show that intermediate choosiness is favoured not only with a magic trait (for rET = 0 in figure 2), but also with any non-magic trait complex with separate ecological and mating signal loci (for rET > 0 in figure 2). We get qualitatively the same result if we consider the gene order CET instead of ETC (electronic supplementary material, figure S5). Polymorphism at the mating signal locus T is maintained under random mating only with a magic trait (as shown for low choosiness in figure 1; see also electronic supplementary material, figure S6). With a non-magic trait complex, polymorphism at the T locus is lost under random mating, and the ESS choosiness can evolve only if at least a small level of choosiness is already present before secondary contact occurred (figure 1 and electronic supplementary material, figure S6). If we condition on such initial maintenance of polymorphism, contrary to intuition, pseudomagic trait complexes characterized by physically linked loci (for 0 < rET < 0.5), and also to a lesser extent physically unlinked loci (for rET = 0.5), have the potential to favour the evolution of higher choosiness than do magic traits (figures 1 and 2). This effect is particularly pronounced under strong viability selection relative to migration, and when the choosiness modifier is linked to the other loci (rTC < 0.5). Note that this effect can occur even if the choosiness modifier is unlinked (for rTC = 0.5; this is not visible in figure 2, but see electronic supplementary material, figure S7).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Choosiness at the evolutionary equilibrium depending on the level of gene flow and genetic architecture. We represent the evolutionary stable choosiness, αESS, depending on the selection coefficient (s), the migration rate (m) and the recombination rates (rET and rTC). The case of rET = 0 can be interpreted as a magic trait (remember that we assume that loci E and T are at maximum linkage disequilibrium, initially). For a given combination of parameters (s, m), an intermediate rET leads to the highest ESS choosiness; if rET is too small, three-way linkage disequilibrium and its effect on the evolution of high choosiness (as detailed in the main text) are too weak, and if rET is too high, recombination breaks linkage disequilibrium that causes indirect selection on choosiness (as detailed in electronic supplementary material, figures S1 and S2). For rTC = 0.5, changes in the recombination rate rET lead to slight changes in the ESS choosiness that are not visible here; in particular, for high ratio s/m, a non-magic trait complex (rET > 0) can lead to a higher choosiness at evolutionary equilibrium than can be found with a magic trait (rET = 0) (see electronic supplementary material, figure S7). The higher choosiness allowed by non-magic trait complexes results in stronger reproductive isolation, as measured by a lower effective migration rate (see electronic supplementary material, figure S8).

In our model, the higher choosiness that evolves alongside non-magic trait complexes ultimately leads to stronger reproductive isolation than is seen with a magic trait, even though divergence at the T locus is lower (figure 1). We can show this by measuring the effective migration rate, which is inversely proportional to reproductive isolation (electronic supplementary material, figure S8; note, however, that the measurement of reproductive isolation is still a matter of debate [27]).

Below, we will first describe the mechanism by which a non-magic trait complex, and in particular a pseudomagic trait complex, can promote the evolution of higher choosiness than does a magic trait. We will then briefly explain why this mechanism is particularly efficient in promoting the evolution of choosiness when the choosiness modifier is tightly linked to the other loci (i.e. for a low rTC). Finally, we will outline the implication of a pseudomagic trait complex for the invasion of large-effect choosiness mutations.

(b) . Mechanism by which a non-magic trait complex promotes the evolution of high choosiness

Unlike with a magic trait, the ESS choosiness that is favoured with a non-magic trait complex does not maximize divergence at either the E or T locus (for rET > 0 in figure 1), which means that additional evolutionary forces come into play. In the case of a non-magic trait complex, higher choosiness is favoured because it prevents the deleterious consequences of recombination between the E and T loci, namely a mismatch between being locally adapted and having the locally preferred male phenotype. Such a higher choosiness is not favoured in the case of a magic trait because recombination between the E and T loci is impossible by definition. As we now describe in detail, we can isolate this evolutionary force by dissecting the sources of indirect selection affecting the evolution of choosiness.

In the case of a non-magic trait complex, indirect selection favouring the evolution of high choosiness relies on the build-up of an association among the choosiness, signal and ecological loci. More precisely, this three-way linkage disequilibrium is characterized by a high frequency of choosier alleles, denoted C2 so that α2 > α1, being associated with the locally favoured combination of alleles at the E and T loci (E1T1 in population 1 and E2T2 in population 2). If we artificially reduce this three-way linkage disequilibrium at the end of each generation over the course of simulations, the ESS choosiness with a non-magic trait complex is no longer higher than the ESS choosiness with a magic trait (figure 3a and electronic supplementary material, figure S9). This highlights the importance of three-way linkage disequilibrium for the evolution of higher choosiness in the case of a non-magic trait complex.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Choosiness at the evolutionary equilibrium depending on the magnitude of the three-way linkage disequilibrium that is removed artificially over the course of simulations. In (a), we show the evolutionary stable choosiness, αESS, depending on the recombination rate rET and the proportion of the three-way linkage disequilibrium, DETC, removed artificially in the simulation. Over the course of the simulations, we reduce the magnitude of the three-way linkage disequilibrium by an amount that depends on the value Φ represented on the horizontal axis. At the end of each generation, we artificially reduce three-way linkage disequilibrium by transforming three-way linkage disequilibrium in each population k according to DETC, k=ΦDETC, magic, k+(1Φ)DETC, k, with DETC,magic,k = min [DTC,k(1 − 2t2,k), DEC,k(1 − 2e2,k)] being the equivalent measure to the three-way linkage disequilibrium if that formula were applied to the case of a magic trait. For Φ=1, we thus artificially set the three-way linkage disequilibrium to its lowest possible value. In (b) and (c), we represent the first-order contributions of indirect sexual selection (in green), indirect viability selection (in orange) and indirect epistatic selection (in purple), to the evolution of stronger choosiness than the ESS choosiness value favoured in the case of a magic trait, αESS,magic, depending on whether the three-way linkage disequilibrium is reduced to its lowest possible value over the course of simulations (c) or not (b). Choosiness α1 is set to be the ESS choosiness value obtained for rET = 0 (α1 = αESS,magic). For each combination of parameters, we know if a choosier allele C2, coding for α2 = α1 + 1, will increase or decrease in frequency based on the ESS value we were able to determine in the analysis done in (a). We, therefore, implement C2 at a frequency equal to 0.01 if it is destined to increase in frequency, or equal to 0.99 if it is destined to decrease in frequency. Over the course of the simulation, we measure the mean first-order contributions of linkage disequilibria to Δc2,2, corresponding to the first three terms of equation (2.1), while the frequency of the choosier allele c2,2 is between 0.05 and 0.95. These first-order contributions of linkage disequilibria correspond to first-order approximations of the effect of indirect sexual selection, indirect viability selection and indirect epistatic selection on the change in frequency of the choosier allele. See electronic supplementary material, figures S1 and S2 for more details on the effect of rET on the contributions of selection to the evolution of choosiness, shown in (b). Here, m = 0.01, s = 0.05 and rTC = 0.01. In (b) and (c), αESS,magic = 6.54 (estimated numerically; see position of the red squares in (a)).

Three-way linkage disequilibrium is established because, under secondary contact, higher choosiness reduces the risk of producing recombinant offspring. A choosier allele C2 is more likely to remain associated with the locally favoured allelic combination at the E and T loci than is a less choosy allele C1. For instance, in population 2, E2T2C2 females mate less often with maladapted E1T1 males than do less choosy E2T2C1 females, and thus produce recombinant offspring E2T1 and E1T2 less often. This results in the maintenance of the beneficial allelic association E2T2 with C2 rather than with C1, leading to the build-up of three-way linkage disequilibrium in the case of a non-magic trait complex (electronic supplementary material, figure S10). This contrasts with the case of a magic trait where there is no recombination between the E and T loci (rET = 0) and thus the build-up of an association akin to this three-way linkage disequilibrium cannot occur. Note that, with a magic trait, the formula for three-way linkage disequilibrium can still be applied (e.g. to measure selection for rET = 0 in figure 3), but results in a measure proportional to two-way linkage disequilibrium [25] (see also Mathematica notebook).

The establishment of three-way linkage disequilibrium that occurs only alongside non-magic trait complexes contributes to indirect epistatic selection (non-additive fitnesses) favouring the evolution of higher choosiness, beyond the ESS choosiness promoted with a magic trait (purple bars in figure 3b, and see also time series in electronic supplementary material, figure S3). In other words, with a non-magic trait complex, viability and sexual selection act non-additively on choosiness via the association between the choosier allele C2 and the locally favoured combination of alleles at the E and T loci (E1T1 in population 1 and E2T2 in population 2). Obviously, if we artificially reduce the three-way linkage disequilibrium, indirect epistatic selection no longer favours the evolution of high choosiness (almost vanishing purple bars in figure 3c).

Additionally, three-way linkage disequilibrium contributes to the build-up of two-way linkage disequilibria between loci T and C during viability selection, and between loci E and C during sexual selection (see electronic supplementary material, text). These two-way linkage disequilibria then lead to indirect sexual and viability selection favouring the evolution of higher choosiness than the ESS choosiness established with a magic trait (green and orange bars in figure 3b, and see also time series in electronic supplementary material, figures S3 and S4). Indeed, if we artificially reduce the three-way linkage disequilibrium, indirect sexual and viability selection no longer favour the evolution of choosiness higher than with a magic trait (green and orange bars in figure 3c); this is because three-way linkage disequilibrium no longer leads to the build-up of positive two-way linkage disequilibria.

In analyses shown in the electronic supplementary material, we find that the difference in ESS choosiness evolving alongside a non-magic and a magic trait can even be amplified by a weak cost of choosiness (electronic supplementary material, text and figure S11). We also show that in a diploid version of the model, non-magic trait complexes can similarly promote the evolution of stronger assortative mate choice than do magic traits (electronic supplementary material, text and figures S12 and S13).

To summarize, a non-magic trait complex allows the establishment of an association among the choosiness, signal and ecological loci (three-way linkage disequilibrium). With few exceptions, detailed below, this association favours the evolution of higher choosiness than the ESS choosiness established with a magic trait (figure 2 and electronic supplementary material, figure S7). In other words, with a non-magic trait complex, this higher choosiness is favoured because it prevents the deleterious consequences of recombination between signal and ecological loci. This recombination does not occur in the case of a magic trait.

(c) . A pseudomagic trait complex can promote the evolution of higher choosiness than do unlinked loci

Although three-way linkage disequilibrium may ultimately favour the evolution of choosiness alongside any non-magic trait complex, a pseudomagic trait complex (rET < 0.5) can promote the evolution of higher choosiness than do physically unlinked loci (rET = 0.5; figure 2). This is because with the gene order ETC, recombination between the E and T loci degrades the linkage disequilibrium between the E and C loci and thus may decrease the contribution of indirect viability selection to the evolution of choosiness (orange bars for rET=0.2versus0.5 in figure 3b; see also electronic supplementary material, figures S1 and S2; intuitively, this is related to the advantage of a magic trait in preventing recombination). Likewise, with the gene order CET, recombination between the E and T loci degrades the linkage disequilibrium between the T and C loci and thus may decrease the contribution of indirect sexual selection to the evolution of choosiness. As a result, indirect selection favouring the evolution of high choosiness is the strongest alongside a pseudomagic trait complex (figure 2).

(d) . Tight linkage between the choosiness and pseudomagic trait loci favours higher choosiness

As noted above, the degree to which non-magic trait complexes (rET > 0) favour the evolution of higher choosiness than do magic traits (rET = 0) depends on the recombination rate, rTC, between the choosiness and the trait loci. This effect is especially pronounced when the choosiness modifier itself is tightly linked to the other loci (e.g. for rTC = 0.01 in figure 2; or for rCE = 0.01 in electronic supplementary material, figure S5 where we consider the gene order CET instead of ETC). This is because low recombination between the choosiness modifier locus and the other loci reduces the indirect selection that inhibits the evolution of very high choosiness and that, therefore, leads to intermediate choosiness [16] (see also electronic supplementary material, text).

By contrast, when the choosiness modifier is loosely linked to the trait loci, a non-magic trait complex may even lead to slightly lower choosiness than the ESS choosiness established with a magic trait (e.g. for rTC = 0.5 but the effect is so small that this is not visible in figure 2; see electronic supplementary material, figure S7). This is because recombination between the E and T loci degrades the linkage disequilibrium that causes indirect viability selection on choosiness (as detailed in electronic supplementary material, figures S1 and S2).

(e) . Pseudomagic trait complexes and the invasion of large-effect choosiness mutations

Now that we have explained why the choosiness at evolutionary equilibrium can be higher for a non-magic trait complex, and in particular for a pseudomagic trait complex, than for a magic trait, we turn to the investigation of the implication of non-magic trait complexes for the spread or loss of large-effect choosiness mutations (figure 4; see electronic supplementary material, figure S14 for a log-scale highlighting the slight increase or the decrease in frequency of these mutations). We track the change in frequency of choosier alleles for different mutation effects, when ancestral choosiness in the population is low, but high enough to maintain polymorphism at the E and T loci.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Time series of the invasion of mutant alleles at the choosiness locus for different mutation effect sizes and different genetic architectures. Choosiness α1 is set to be the lowest choosiness value that maintains polymorphism at the T locus for all recombination rates tested (α1 = 0.13; estimated numerically). We then show the invasion in population 2 of a mutant allele coding for a choosiness α2 = α1 + Δα. We observe the same invasion dynamics in population 1 (not shown). Brackets show the time points where choosiness becomes a neutral trait because polymorphism at the T locus is lost (bottom right panel). Here, m = 0.01 and s = 0.05.

For rTC = 0.5, mutant alleles that code for higher choosiness, but that are not overly choosy, can invade (figure 4). The recombination rate between loci encoding the ecological trait and the mating signal (rET) has little effect on the dynamics of invasion, unless the mutation effect size is very small. This is not surprising given that the ESS choosiness depends very little on rET (electronic supplementary material, figure S7).

For rTC = 0.01, a non-magic trait complex (rET > 0; especially pseudomagic trait complexes with intermediate rET) can favour the invasion of choosier mutant alleles than does a magic trait (rET = 0) (figure 4). This is in line with the high ESS choosiness obtained with a non-magic trait complex (figure 2). Interestingly, a pseudomagic trait complex allows the evolution of very high choosiness through the spread of a single large-effect mutation (so that Δα ≥ 100 in figure 4) which far overshoots the choosiness value at evolutionary equilibrium (see also pairwise invasibility plots in electronic supplementary material, figure S15).

With a non-magic trait complex, the invasion of choosier mutant alleles occurs only if choosiness is initially strong enough to maintain polymorphism. If mating is initially random, the quick loss of polymorphism after secondary contact, especially when loci are physically unlinked, impedes the invasion of any choosier alleles (electronic supplementary material, figure S16; in contrast, with a magic trait, polymorphism is always maintained, even under random mating). Assuming that polymorphism at the T locus is initially maintained, we get qualitatively the same invasion time series as in the symmetric case (in figure 4) if migration is asymmetric (electronic supplementary material, figure S17), viability selection is asymmetric (electronic supplementary material, figure S18) or choosiness is asymmetric (electronic supplementary material, figure S19). Such asymmetric conditions cause the loss of polymorphism at the mating signal locus to occur more easily than under symmetric conditions (electronic supplementary material, figure S20). If polymorphism is maintained at the outset, however, pseudomagic trait complexes predominantly favour the evolution of higher choosiness than does a magic trait.

4. Discussion

Our model shows the fragility of the intuitive prediction that magic traits, subject to divergent selection and pleiotropically affecting reproductive isolation, necessarily favour stronger reproductive isolation than other genetic architectures. We show that, although magic traits favour divergence and the maintenance of polymorphism, pseudomagic trait complexes, characterized by separate physically linked loci being subject to divergent selection and affecting reproductive isolation, have the potential to promote the evolution of stronger assortative mate choice than do magic traits, provided that polymorphism at the ecological and mating signal loci is maintained. In the case of a pseudomagic trait complex, this strong assortative mate choice is favoured because it ultimately diminishes the risk of recombining the genes that underlie favourable complexes of ecological and mating traits. It thus reduces the production of recombinant offspring with lower average fitness than the parental forms. This recombination risk does not exist in the case of magic traits (which rely on pleiotropy, by definition), explaining why this strong assortative mate choice is favoured in the case of pseudomagic trait complexes and in the case of non-magic trait complexes with physically unlinked loci (although in the latter case polymorphism is particularly difficult to maintain, and the spread of a single large-effect choosiness mutation can only slightly overshoot the ESS choosiness value).

With a magic trait, a single pleiotropic locus is both under ecologically divergent selection and involved in assortative mating [11]. Functionally, this is analogous to a situation where linkage disequilibrium between loci controlling ecological and mating traits is maximized and cannot be broken by recombination. Nevertheless, one should not make the mistake of equating linkage disequilibrium between a subset of loci with increased reproductive isolation [7]. Clearly, incipient species are characterized not only by genes involved in premating isolation but also by other genes, such as neutral genes and genes involved in postzygotic isolation, and one must consider the linkage disequilibrium among all of these loci to infer the likelihood of speciation. Importantly, the extent to which linkage disequilibrium between all genes is maintained depends on the gene flow between incipient species and thus on the strength of assortative mate choice [23]. Therefore, whether a given genetic architecture favours speciation strongly depends on the strength of assortative mate choice that can evolve.

In a previous study [23], the authors concluded that it is not necessarily important to identify whether co-localizing mating and ecological components are pleiotropic or merely tightly linked, in terms of evolutionary divergence. By assessing the impact of genetic linkage on the build-up of premating isolation via the evolution of strong assortative mate choice, our study shows that a very different conclusion applies to reproductive isolation more broadly. We predict that a pseudomagic trait complex characterized by very tight linkage (e.g. for rET ∈ [0.01, 0.1]) is a genetic architecture that is prone to favour the evolution of strong reproductive isolation, provided that polymorphism at the ecological and mating signal loci is maintained after secondary contact. Although a magic trait is more likely to maintain polymorphism in this situation (especially under random mating), the strength of assortative mate choice that can evolve with such a magic trait is limited. By contrast, a pseudomagic trait complex more easily leads to the loss of polymorphism at the mating signal locus upon secondary contact, but if it does not, then it can lead to the evolution of stronger assortative mate choice than does a magic trait. In this respect, a pseudomagic trait complex is more likely to maintain species-specific allelic combinations, and may be more effective in the promoting later stages of speciation than a magic trait.

The recombination rate between loci involved in premating isolation can evolve upon secondary contact, which we did not consider in our model. For example, chromosomal rearrangements (e.g. inversions) have suppressed recombination between loci that may be involved in premating isolation in some taxa [28,29], as predicted by population genetics theory [30]. Our study highlights that this local suppression of recombination may not necessarily favour the establishment of strong premating isolation, but may instead inhibit the evolution of mate choice that leads to reduced gene flow across the genome.

Our model also emphasizes the importance of the location in the genome of genetic loci encoding choosiness, i.e. encoding the strength of assortative mate choice. Under secondary contact, pseudomagic trait complexes favour the evolution of strong assortative mate choice only when choosiness loci are tightly linked to loci forming pseudomagic trait complexes. This raises the question of whether the evolution of high choosiness could be limited by the genetic architecture and position of loci encoding choosiness. For instance, if choosiness is a quantitative trait encoded by loci distributed uniformly along the genome, then the evolution of choosiness is likely to be limited by number of choosiness loci that are tightly linked to ecological and mating signal loci, rather than by indirect selection favouring choosiness. Such a prediction calls for more empirical investigation of the genetic basis of choosiness, which has received little attention so far. Indeed, intraspecific variation in choosiness is required to assess quantitative trait loci along genomes. Because models predict that choosiness alleles may spread uniformly through incipient species during speciation with gene flow (because choosiness operates through a ‘one-allele’ mechanism [7]), there may be limitations to the dissection of the genetic basis of choosiness if most alleles go to fixation. However, by measuring preference behaviours in hybrids that may show variation in choosiness this limitation could conceivably be overcome (as in [29] in the case of the preference for conspecific versus heterospecific mates), shedding light on the constraints to the evolution of choosiness alongside pseudomagic trait complexes. Before the genetic architecture of choosiness can be assessed, however, it is necessary to develop a standardized measure of the choosiness [31], as a stronger preference can be independent of choosiness when there is variation in the deviation between the preference and the trait values of the individuals.

We appreciate that our modelling approach comes with its limitations, including a limited number of loci, a geographical context of secondary contact and a specific mating rule (phenotype matching). As a whole, however, our model highlights the importance of genetic architecture for the evolution of assortative mating and thus the build-up of premating isolation. Speciation with gene flow has been traditionally studied under the assumption that ‘magic’ genes encode traits involved in premating isolation [8,1317], and some authors have speculated that non-magic trait complexes could mimic the role of magic traits in the speciation process [18,21,22]. Our study shows that by reducing the risk of recombination, strong assortative mate choice can evolve alongside a non-magic trait complex, and even more so alongside a pseudomagic trait complex, thus promoting the build-up of stronger premating isolation than would occur alongside a magic trait. We hope that the predictions of our model will stimulate further empirical investigations assessing the genetic architecture underlying premating isolation.

Acknowledgements

We thank B. Lerch, K. Xu and two anonymous reviewers for comments that have helped improve our manuscript.

Data accessibility

The Mathematica notebook for this article is archived on Zenodo: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7631806 [24].

Supplementary text and figures are provided in the electronic supplementary material [32].

Authors' contributions

T.G.A.: conceptualization, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, writing—original draft; R.B.: conceptualization, supervision, validation, writing—review and editing; M.R.S.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, supervision, validation, writing—review and editing.

All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Conflict of interest declaration

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

This research was supported by grant no. DEB-1939290 from the National Science Foundation (to M.R.S.).

References

  • 1.Jiang Y, Bolnick DI, Kirkpatrick M. 2013. Assortative mating in animals. Am. Nat. 181, E125-E138. ( 10.1086/670160) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Janicke T, Marie-Orleach L, Aubier TG, Perrier C, Morrow EH. 2019. Assortative mating in animals and its role for speciation. Am. Nat. 194, 865-875. ( 10.1086/705825) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Weis AE, Nardone E, Fox GA. 2014. The strength of assortative mating for flowering date and its basis in individual variation in flowering schedule. J. Evol. Biol. 27, 2138-2151. ( 10.1111/jeb.12465) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rubin MJ, Schmid KM, Friedman J. 2019. Assortative mating by flowering time and its effect on correlated traits in variable environments. Ecol. Evol. 9, 471-481. ( 10.1002/ece3.4765) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Coyne JA, Orr HA. 2004. Speciation. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kopp M, et al. 2018. Mechanisms of assortative mating in speciation with gene flow: connecting theory and empirical research. Am. Nat. 191, 1-20. ( 10.1086/694889) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Felsenstein J. 1981. Skepticism towards Santa Rosalia, or why are there so few kinds of animals? Evolution 35, 124-138. ( 10.2307/2407946) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dieckmann U, Doebeli M. 1999. On the origin of species by sympatric speciation. Nature 400, 354-357. ( 10.1038/22521) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Geritz S, Kisdi E. 2000. Adaptive dynamics in diploid, sexual populations and the evolution of reproductive isolation. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 1671-1678. ( 10.1098/rspb.2000.1194) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.De Cara MA, Barton NH, Kirkpatrick M. 2008. A model for the evolution of assortative mating. Am. Nat. 171, 580-596. ( 10.1086/587062) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gavrilets S 2004. Fitness landscapes and the origin of species. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kirkpatrick M, Ravigné V. 2002. Speciation by natural and sexual selection: models and experiments. Am. Nat. 159(Suppl. 3), S22-S35. ( 10.1086/338370) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Matessi C, Gimelfarb A, Gavrilets S. 2001. Long-term buildup of reproductive isolation promoted by disruptive selection: how far does it go? Selection 2, 41-64. ( 10.1556/Select.2.2001.1-2.4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Otto SP, Servedio MR, Nuismer SL. 2008. Frequency-dependent selection and the evolution of assortative mating. Genetics 179, 2091-2112. ( 10.1534/genetics.107.084418) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pennings PS, Kopp M, Meszéna G, Dieckmann U, Hermisson J. 2008. An analytically tractable model for competitive speciation. Am. Nat. 171, E44-E71. ( 10.1086/523952) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Servedio MR. 2011. Limits to the evolution of assortative mating by female choice under restricted gene flow. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 179-187. ( 10.1098/rspb.2010.1174) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Aubier TG, Kokko H, Joron M. 2019. Coevolution of male and female mate choice can destabilize reproductive isolation. Nat. Commun. 10, 5122. ( 10.1038/s41467-019-12860-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Servedio MR, Van Doorn GS, Kopp M, Frame AM, Nosil P. 2011. Magic traits in speciation: ‘magic’ but not rare? Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 389-397. ( 10.1016/j.tree.2011.04.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hawthorne DJ, Via S. 2001. Genetic linkage of ecological specialization and reproductive isolation in pea aphids. Nature 412, 904-907. ( 10.1038/35091062) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ferris KG, Barnett LL, Blackman BK, Willis JH. 2017. The genetic architecture of local adaptation and reproductive isolation in sympatry within the Mimulus guttatus species complex. Mol. Ecol. 26, 208-224. ( 10.1111/mec.13763) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rundle HD, Nosil P. 2005. Ecological speciation. Ecol. Lett. 8, 336-352. ( 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00715.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.McKinnon JS, Pierotti ME. 2010. Colour polymorphism and correlated characters: genetic mechanisms and evolution. Mol. Ecol. 19, 5101-5125. ( 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2010.04846.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Servedio MR, Bürger R. 2020. The effectiveness of pseudomagic traits in promoting divergence and enhancing local adaptation. Evolution 74, 2438-2450. ( 10.1111/evo.14056) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Aubier TG, Bürger R, Servedio MR. 2023. Code for: The effectiveness of pseudomagic traits in promoting premating isolation. Zenodo. ( 10.5281/zenodo.7631806) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 25.Barton NH, Turelli M. 1991. Natural and sexual selection on many loci. Genetics 127, 229-255. ( 10.1093/genetics/127.1.229) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Servedio MR, Hermisson J. 2020. The evolution of partial reproductive isolation as an adaptive optimum. Evolution 74, 4-14. ( 10.1111/evo.13880) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Westram AM, Stankowski S, Surendranadh P, Barton N. 2022. What is reproductive isolation? J. Evol. Biol. 35, 1143-1164. ( 10.1111/jeb.14005) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Poelstra JW, et al. 2014. The genomic landscape underlying phenotypic integrity in the face of gene flow in crows. Science 344, 1410-1414. ( 10.1126/science.1253226) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Merrill RM, Rastas P, Martin SH, Melo MC, Barker S, Davey J, McMillan WO, Jiggins CD. 2019. Genetic dissection of assortative mating behavior. PLoS Biol. 17, e2005902. ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.2005902) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Trickett AJ, Butlin RK. 1994. Recombination suppressors and the evolution of new species. Heredity 73, 339-345. ( 10.1038/hdy.1994.180) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Edward DA. 2015. The description of mate choice. Behav. Ecol. 26, 301-310. ( 10.1093/beheco/aru142) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Aubier TG, Bürger R, Servedio MR. 2023. The effectiveness of pseudomagic traits in promoting premating isolation. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6431854) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Citations

  1. Aubier TG, Bürger R, Servedio MR. 2023. The effectiveness of pseudomagic traits in promoting premating isolation. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6431854) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Data Availability Statement

The Mathematica notebook for this article is archived on Zenodo: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7631806 [24].

Supplementary text and figures are provided in the electronic supplementary material [32].


Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES