Abstract
The COVID-19 pandemic (including lockdown) is likely to have had profound but diverse implications for mental health and well-being, yet little is known about individual experiences of the pandemic (positive and negative) and how this relates to mental health and well-being, as well as other important contextual variables. Here, we analyse data sampled in a large-scale manner from 379,875 people in the United Kingdom (UK) during 2020 to identify population variables associated with mood and mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic, and to investigate self-perceived pandemic impact in relation to those variables. We report that while there are relatively small population-level differences in mood assessment scores pre- to peak- UK lockdown, the size of the differences is larger for people from specific groups, e.g. older adults and people with lower incomes. Multiple dimensions underlie peoples’ perceptions, both positive and negative, of the pandemic’s impact on daily life. These dimensions explain variance in mental health and can be statistically predicted from age, demographics, home and work circumstances, pre-existing conditions, maladaptive technology use and personality traits (e.g., compulsivity). We conclude that a holistic view, incorporating the broad range of relevant population factors, can better characterise people whose mental health is most at risk during the COVID-19 pandemic.
Keywords: pandemic, coronavirus, COVID-19, mental health, wellbeing, mood, depression, anxiety, impulsive, compulsive, compulsivity, predictors, population, machine learning
Introduction
The Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic has brought about unprecedented change in peoples’ lives due to the direct and indirect consequences of illness, physical distancing and socio-economic restructuring. These changes have likely affected mood and mental health in widespread, profound but idiosyncratic ways1. Expert groups have posited that the impact of the pandemic on mental health is modulated by a variety of factors including (i) aspects of demographics such as age or ethnicity, (ii) social networks, (iii) financial and occupational circumstances (iv), being shielded or having carer responsibilities, (v) pre-existing mental health symptoms, (vi) maladaptive online technology use, (vii) personality traits and (viii) tendency towards compulsive behaviours2–7.
There have been urgent calls to study these relationships as they are critical to inform policy and healthcare decisions, and to guide researchers and clinicians. However, pre-existing knowledge about how pandemics affect mental health is limited. Previous work on the relationship between COVID-19 and mental health has focused on relatively narrow aspects of mental health, failing to consider the diverse psycho-socio-economic variables that are likely to modulate impact, and not taking into account self-perceived impact of the pandemic (positive and negative)8–12. Consequently, it remains unclear which segments of society have been most affected or whether expert opinions align with population perspectives. Additionally, extant studies predominantly have used promotional materials that explicitly mention COVID-19, and target established cohorts, yielding high likelihood of recruitment bias from already nonrepresentative sub-populations13.
Addressing these issues presents a major methodological challenge. There are likely to be multivariate relationships between the ways people have been affected by the pandemic and their psycho-socio-economic profiles. Many of the relevant variables, e.g., personality or psychiatric traits and technology use, covary14,15, and it is not clear what the major dimensions of the impact are. In such a context, identifying, disentangling and mapping the key variables can only be achieved in a data-driven multivariate manner, necessitating the analysis of large-scale population data.
To address this challenge, we applied a combination of multivariate and machine learning methods to analyse our large-scale dataset, comprising a survey of mental health and wellbeing variables completed by 379,875 people, ~90% resident within the UK (see supplementary figure 7) and the remainder from around the globe, since late December 2019 and concentrated around January and May 2020. This broader database includes responses to a questionnaire instrument comprehensively probing self-perceived pandemic-impact in 79,779 out of the 112,046 respondents in May-June 2020.
Our analyses comprised several distinct steps designed to address some of the most pressing questions at this time (Figure 1a). Specifically, we first confirmed whether there were differences in population distributions of standard depression, anxiety, tiredness, and sleep measures between January 2020 (immediately prior to the COVID-19 outbreak reaching the UK), and May 2020 (during peak lockdown in the UK). We examined how the scale of these differences covaried with relevant population factors such as age, education, occupational and socioeconomic status. Next, we quantified prevalent views and estimated in a data-driven manner the dimensionality of self-perceived impact of the pandemic on daily life during lockdown in May 2020. We tested the degree to which there were both positive and negative aspects of self-perceived impact, and whether these had significant explanatory value in terms of the standard mental health assessment measures. Finally, we used multivariate analyses to quantify the relative and collective importance of population factors in predicting the idiosyncratic ways that people’s daily lives have been perceptibly affected by the pandemic.
Results
Respondents and pre-analysis filtering
The study website, where participants completed cognitive tests and a detailed questionnaire (Supplementary methods), was made available online from December 26th 2019 at https://gbit.cognitron.co.uk. Articles describing the study were placed on the BBC2 Horizon page, BBC Homepage, BBC News Homepage and circulated on mobile news meta-apps from January 1st 2020, in the UK. A second promotional drive, placing articles in the same prominent locations, was launched on May 2nd 2020, aligned with a BBC2 Horizon documentary focused on concurrently collected cognitive data, analysis of which is outside the scope of this article. This produced large peaks in data collection (Figure 1b) around the two launches (UK pre-pandemic, December 25th-January 31st N= 243,875; mid-pandemic, May 2nd to June 30th N= 125,177), with smaller, but not insubstantial quantities of data in the intervening (early-pandemic, February 1st to May 1st, N=10,003) and pre-launch (N=820) periods.
Participants aged under 16 were excluded prior to analysis because they were presented with an abbreviated questionnaire for ethical reasons. Also, as a quality control people who completed the questionnaire in under 4 minutes were excluded; this threshold was decided by the study team prior to database lock based on the approximate minimum time taken to complete the survey while reading the questions. This resulted in 233,268 pre-pandemic, 9,049 Early-pandemic and 112,046 mid-pandemic participants in the analyses presented here.
A critical question was whether the cross-sectional population samples were well matched between these three epochs. Plotting the sociodemographic distributions (Supplementary figures 1-8 & Supplementary table 1) confirmed generally close correspondences in sex (“what is your sex?” with response options “male, female or other”), handedness, first language, UK residence, ethnicity, education level, earnings, and occupational status across all three epochs. However, the smaller early-pandemic sample, when the study website was not being actively promoted, had a marked skew towards younger age; therefore, we did not analyse this period further herein and rather focused on the two very closely matched, larger N epochs.
The two epochs of interest captured a broad and inclusive cross section of the population, with >50 participants per age year per subset up to and including 85. Participants above this age were combined into an 86+ category. Overall, ~14.3% of participants identified as from minority ethnic groups. This inclusivity compares favourably to established UK bioresource cohorts, which often under-represent such groups (e.g. 5.4% ethnic minority groups in the UK Biobank)16. There were 45.8% females, 53.3% males and 0.9% indicating other.
Differences from Pre- to Mid-Pandemic in mean population mood scores
Mental health self-assessment items were scored on a frequency scale in response to the question of how often the participant had experienced the specified symptom (e.g., feeling down or depressed) over the past weeks. Raw % responses for the Pre and mid-Pandemic epochs are reported in (Figure 2). On visual inspection, the largest difference was for anxiety, with the number of people reporting feeling anxious or on edge ‘several times a week’ increasing from 24% to 33% and the number of people reporting ‘never’ decreasing from 18 to 8%. Other aspects of mental health measures showed modest reductions whereas sleep showed a small increase.
Responses were transformed into a numeric scale from 0 (never) to 6 (>hourly) in order to enable statistical analysis of differences in mean mood assessment scores across the mid vs pre-pandemic epochs whilst accounting for population factors. More specifically, age, sex, handedness, education level, first language, country of residence, occupational status and income were factored out using a general linear model (GLM) from the entire dataset for responses reporting the frequencies over the past month of anxiety, depression, problems concentrating, insomnia, hours slept and tiredness. The residual scores were grouped by pre- vs mid-pandemic epoch and compared using F tests. When analysing large-scale data, negligible scaled effects can have significant statistical values, necessitating a focus on effect size to gauge relevance. Here we report mean differences in standard deviation (SD) units, i.e., equivalent to Cohen’s D, and conform to Sawilowsky’s revised17 criteria, whereby effects of 0.1SDs=very small, 0.2SDs=small, 0.5SDs=medium, 0.8SDs=large, 1.2SDs=very large and 2.0SDs=huge.
The mean differences in mental health measures were generally in the small to very small effect size range. The most pronounced difference was anxiety score, which was greater in the mid-pandemic epoch (0.28 SDs, F(1,3.4425e+05)=6090.60 p<0.0001). Mean depression score was lower by a very small margin (-0.08 SDs, F(1,3.4425e+05)=537.75 p<0.0001) and problems concentrating were greater by a small margin (0.08 SDs, F(1,3.4425e+05)= 507.88 p<0.0001). Problems getting to sleep or staying asleep showed a negligible-scaled difference (0.003 SDs, F(1,3.4425e+05)=0.628 p=0.4280); however, reported hours slept per night increased (0.14 SDs, F(1,3.4425e+05)=2035.70 p<0.0001) and a decrease in tiredness was evident (-0.15 SDs, F(1,3.4425e+05)=1797.30 p<0.0001). Analysis of day-by-day data during the 31 days after January and the 31 days after May promotion launches (where daily number of participants was high) confirmed that the observed mean differences in mental health measures were consistent throughout those months (supplementary figure 9).
Population factors covary with differences in mental health scores pre- and mid-pandemic
We next examined whether differences in mental health pre- to mid-pandemic lockdown were greater for select sociodemographic sub-populations. GLMs were conducted on each of the six item scores. These included interactions of epoch (pre vs mid) with age, sex, handedness, ethnicity, employment status, first language, country of residence, education level and earnings, while partialling out the main effects. There were numerous statistically significant interactions (Supplementary tables 2-16 & Figure 3) spanning from very small to large effect sizes. Most notable were the relationships with age. There was a simple linear relationship between difference in anxiety score mid- vs pre-pandemic and age, (+0.40SDs for 60-80-year-olds, +0.10SDs for teenagers). This relationship also was evident for depression scores, with older adults also showing a small increase (+0.14SDs for 80+) whereas teenagers and young adults showed a small decrease (-0.12SDs). It should be noted that in both cases, anxiety and depression scores already were higher by a large margin for teenagers and young adults than older adults pre-pandemic lockdown (pre-phase 16-26y minus 76-86y: anxiety 0.89SDs, depression 0.82SDs). There was a greater difference in hours slept for younger adults (+0.49 hrs at 16) vs older adults (~0SD difference at 60+). The pre- to mid-pandemic differences in anxiety score was greatest for retired people (0.38SDs), followed by homemakers (0.31SDs) and workers (0.29SDs). The difference in anxiety scores was greatest for those identifying their sex as other (0.39SDs), followed by females (+0.33SDs) then males (+0.22SDs). Difference in hours slept was greatest for students (+0.37SDs), contrasting with retired people (-0.01SDs and homemakers (+0.02SDs) and disabled people (~0SDs). The relationship between differences in mood scores across epochs and earnings, handedness, first language, and country of residence were generally of very small to negligible scale.
Self-perceived pandemic impact at the level of individual questions
The results of the cross-epoch analyses of mental health assessment measures accorded with the view that population factors such as age, sex and occupational status may moderate the impact of the pandemic on mood and mental health. Complementing this, in May 2020, the online questionnaire was extended with a bespoke PanDemic General Impact Scale (PD-GIS), designed to probe self-perceived current and longer-term impact of the pandemic on day-to-day life by measuring levels of agreement with positive and negative statements. This optional section of the questionnaire was collected in the same session. Those who did versus did not complete the PD-GIS had very similar mental health scores (i.e. of very small effect size difference), indicating that sampling bias was negligible (Supplementary results 1).
We sought to determine which individual items of the PD-GIS people were most likely to agree with (Figure 4). Unexpectedly, some of the strongest agreement was with positive statements. >70% of respondents indicated they were spending less and saving more, ~50% had more time as were commuting less, >45% more relaxed than before, >65% enjoying the simpler things in life, ~55% had more time to read for pleasure, >80% were, more in touch with loved ones they previously had trouble finding time for using apps, >75% noted more pleasant environment with ~70% reporting more wildlife and >65% a greater sense of community. There was strong agreement that technology, science and healthcare would advance more rapidly (>50%) and that things would change but not necessarily for the worse (>60%), but strong disagreement that economic impact would be temporary (60%).
Amongst the strongest agreement with negative statements was concern for health of loved ones (>85%), which was higher than concern about one’s own health (>50%), loss of leisure and health activities (>75%) and disconnectedness (60%) and loss of daily structure (>45%). The strongest disagreement was predominantly with negative statements, e.g., loss of employment (~65%), loss of productivity (~70%), reduced attention to personal hygiene (~65%), loss of access to basics (~70%), and items pertaining to increased conflict at home (~65%). ~30% of people reported drinking more alcohol. ~40% worried about having a less healthy lifestyle.
These measures of overall agreement or disagreement were contextualised by substantial population variability. Therefore, peoples’ self-perceptions of the impact of the pandemic on their daily lives were highly variable and included an unforeseen degree of positivity.
Self-perceived impact of the pandemic at the level of latent variables
We examined whether the profile of population variability in self-perceived pandemic impact was best captured by one or more latent variables by applying principal component analysis to PD-GIS responses (supplementary figure 10). Permutation testing18 indicated an optimal 7 component model. After varimax rotation the first component had heavy loadings for questions pertaining to positive perception of more free time, less stress and reduced tiredness (Figure 4 right). The second component represented questions pertaining to disruption of normal life and increased loneliness. Based on the item loading matrix subsequent components were labelled 3: increased health concern. 4: positive outlook. 5: more conflict at home. 6: improved environment. 7: more time for people at home.
Predicting standard mental health measures from self-perceived impact components
Component scores from the seven self-perceived impact components were cross correlated with the standard mental health assessment measures. The robustness of the relationship was corroborated by multivariate analysis with canonical correlation analysis, which produced seven statistically significant correlation modes (all p<0.001), with small-large scaled canonical r values (Figure 5). Train-test analysis demonstrated that these substantial canonical correlations were not a consequence of overfit (supplementary figure 11).
Bivariate correlations showed that the strongest relationships with anxiety for the PD-GIS components were health concerns followed by disruption of normal life. Depression and insomnia also correlated most strongly with disruption of normal life and health concerns. Irritability correlated with conflict at home. Positive PD-GIS components generally had small correlation effect sizes in the expected direction, e.g., participants indicating they had slept more and were less stressed and tired showed small benefits in terms of mental health scores.
Population factors predict self-perceived pandemic impact
Together the above results demonstrated the utility of the PD-GIS in providing a multi-dimensional assay of self-perceived pandemic impact and the relevance to mental health measures. This enabled us to examine how population variables relate to pandemic impact. We used GLMs to examine the relationships of PD-GIS scores with sociodemographic and economic variables, home context, cohabitees and work arrangements (Supplementary tables 17-24 for full results). In accordance with the mental health assessment analysis, age was amongst the most prominent predictors, having strong correlations with all seven selfperceived pandemic impact components. Examined at a yearly grain (Figure 6), these differences spanned the medium to very large effect size range but were often non-linear. Older adults scored lower for more time and less stress, and higher for increased health concerns. Younger people scored higher for disrupted daily life. Teenagers and early 20s scored higher for increased conflict at home. People of working age had less positive longer-term outlooks. People of older working age scored higher for perceiving improved environment and those of middle working age scored higher for having more time for people at home, i.e., despite having a similar likelihood of living alone to young adults.
In addition to age, the most prominent predictors of self-perceived pandemic impact were occupational status and cohabitees (Figure 7). Healthcare workers benefited the least from more time and less stress and reported greater increases in health concerns. This was balanced by reporting more positive outlook for the future and the least disrupted lifestyles. Furloughed people benefited the most from more time and less stress, followed by home workers, but along with students, reported the most disrupted lifestyles. Disabled or shielded people had some of the most negative perceptions, reporting little benefit of more time and less stress, having the greatest increase in health concerns, and being least likely to report improved environment. Retired people also reported heightened health concerns and were least likely to report having more time for people at home, i.e., even having accounted for levels of living alone. Disabled or shielded and home workers scored highest in terms of more time for people. People living with small children also perceived disproportionate impact, scoring lowest in terms of more time and less stress and highest for increased conflict at home, with this somewhat balanced by more time for people at home. People living alone scored highest for disruption of lifestyle, and as expected, lowest for more time for people and conflict at home.
Small-medium relationships were evident for other population variables. Interestingly, type of house had small-negligible relationships with self-perceived impact scores whereas quality of outside space predicted more positive report across many components, e.g., indicating more time and less stress, less disruption of lifestyle, less health concerns, less conflict at home and greater perception of improved environment (Figure 8).
Higher education level predicted lower positive outlook. Males were less likely to report increased health concerns than females or people selecting other, females were more likely to report improved environment, and people indicating ‘other’ had more positive outlook. Differences between ethnicities were generally in the small-negligible range, with the exception of health concerns, with people indicating Asian, Hispanic and Other scoring highest.
Self-perceived pandemic impact in people with pre-existing conditions
Next, we tested the prediction that people with pre-existing conditions would be more adversely affected. GLMs estimated differences in PD-GIS scores for people reporting established diagnoses of mental health and neurological conditions, and conditions predisposing to COVID-19 vulnerability relative to controls (Supplementary tables 25-31 & Figure 9) after factoring out potentially confounding effects of the above population variables. There was high comorbidity between obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) and anxiety, and between anxiety and depression within the cohort. These participants were modelled as separate categories. Disorders with under 100 representatives were moved to ‘other neurological’ and ‘other psychiatric’ categories. Small and statistically significant relationships were evident for disrupted lifestyle, which scored higher for people reporting diagnoses of depression. Notably, increased health concerns scored higher for people with pre-existing conditions that predispose to COVID-19 risk including diabetes, lung, heart and weakened immune system, but were greater still for people with anxiety disorders and OCD (~0.49SDs). People with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder scored marginally higher for conflict at home (~0.18SDs). There was a trend towards lower scores for improved environment across conditions, with this being greatest for people with bipolar disorder (~-0.22SDs) and people with Parkinson’s Disease (~-0.25SDs).
Role of technology use and personality traits in self-perceived pandemic impact
Finally, linear modelling was applied using a robust train-test pipeline to quantify multivariate associations of online technology use, personality traits, and compulsivity with self-perceived COVID-19 impact after factoring out sociodemographic variables (Full results in Figure 10). Correlating predicted by true PD-GIS scores for the test datasets showed that a substantial proportion of the variance was explained by these traits, with the relationship being strongest for health concerns (mean r=0.32 p<0.0001) followed by disrupted lifestyle (mean r=0.24 p<0.0001) and then appreciation of improved environment (mean r=0.22 p<0.0001). Bivariate correlations showed that a combination of small-scaled correlations formed the basis of this multivariate relationship. The most prominent predictors related to negative PD-GIS components of disrupted lifestyle and more health concerns-specifically, personality traits including ‘self-security’ and ‘conscientiousness’ were associated with reduced scores, whereas technology addiction (i.e. problematic usage of the internet), stress from technology, and compulsivity (reward drive and rigidity) were associated with increased scores.
Discussion
By analysing UK data at large scale collected from individuals immediately prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, and around the time of first UK lockdown, our study provides insights into the idiosyncratic ways that mental health and wellbeing have changed, including the relationship to self-perceived impact on everyday life and the population variables that modulate vulnerability and resilience.
On average, we observed only subtle differences in conventional mental health measures pre- to peak-UK lockdown. Anxiety rates showed the most pronounced increase. Depression rates were if anything slightly lower whilst in parallel, people reported sleeping more and being less tired. Notably, prior studies in this area have provided mixed results, with some reporting subtle and others more pronounced pandemic impact12,19,20. Part of this incongruency may relate to differences in baseline calculation approaches, with a reliance on old historical data or baseline data collected after the pandemic was entrenched in many prior studies. The incongruency in prior work also likely pertains to the application of statistical models that do not account for the breadth of relevant population variables.
Age was the most prominent population variable accounting for pandemic impact. Older people showed disproportionate negative differences in standard mental health measures; e.g., greater increases in anxiety, less increase in sleep and concomitant reduction in tiredness, and in the case of depression, increases as opposed to decreases. Conversely, younger people on average already had higher anxiety and depression scores prior to the pandemic, and their scores remained higher mid-lockdown. These findings reinforce the message advocated in a recent position statement2 and elsewhere21,22 that there is cause for concern about the mental health of both younger and older people during the pandemic. Whilst considerable attention has been drawn to the impact of the pandemic in younger people, we believe that the disproportionate impact on older people has been overlooked.
Regarding interventions for mitigating the impact of the pandemic on mental health, useful insights can be drawn from how people perceive their everyday lives to have been affected. In this respect, results from the analysis of the PD-GIS scale are informative. First, the ways in which people consider themselves to have been affected are complex and highly idiosyncratic. Seven principal components were required to capture variance in self-perceived impact. Second, the positive and negative dimensions segregated from each other but also explained substantial variance in the standard mental health measures, providing different potential targets for mental health interventions both during and beyond the pandemic. Finally, along with the mental health measures, the dimensions of self-perceived impact covaried with key population variables like age. Therefore, although some potential approaches to mitigating the mental health impact of the pandemic may be generic, it is likely that others should be tailored to the individual. We believe that intersecting scales such as the PD-GIS with relevant demographic and other contextual variables provides a promising avenue for enabling such tailoring.
Notable amongst the broader positive associations was access to pleasant outdoor space, which was a marked statistical determinant of more positive self-perceived pandemic impact, relating to being less stressed and tired, fewer health concerns, a more positive outlook, and a less disrupted lifestyle. These data are consistent with prior work in non-COVID-19 literature suggesting that access to green space is associated with mental health benefits and with reduced stress23,24, but also extend that association to resilience against the negative conditions brought about by the pandemic..
In terms of self-perceived negative impact of the pandemic, of particular note were the components ‘increased health concerns’ and ‘disrupted lifestyle’, as these had the strongest relationships with standard mental health measures. Age again featured prominently. It was clear that self-perceived negative impact came in different forms e.g., for older adults-who had the highest health concerns, young adults and teens-who reported being the most disrupted and having greater conflict at home, and those of middle working age-who reported more time for people at home and improved environment but reduced positive outlook. These results highlight that age is an important factor to consider when attempting to design interventions to mitigate the pandemic impact on mental health.
Demographic characteristics, work, environment and social circumstances also all had robust associations with self-perceived pandemic impact, varying in scale from small to very large. Most notably, health workers showed very large differences to the broader population, being less relaxed and having less free time, but also reporting better sleep and greater work engagement. These results are in keeping with disproportionate psychological effects of outbreaks in healthcare workers reported in previous literature on other infections25, as well as initial data available regarding COVID-1926,27, but also indicate that perceived benefits of such work are experienced by healthcare workers. Maximisation of workforce wellbeing amongst healthcare professionals is especially crucial during the pandemic28,29, but also beyond it given the escalating backlog of more routine care being deferred due to the pandemic. People who had been furloughed were less stressed and tired compared with healthcare workers, whereas those with loss of income reported more life disruption compared with those who had retained their jobs. Finally, in terms of living circumstances, cohabiting with parents was associated with the largest increases in conflict at home during the lockdown, as was living with younger children.
In addition to revealing important differences in self-perceived pandemic impact contingent on demographic and vocational and home contexts, the study sheds light on mental health resilience and vulnerability in the general population. We hypothesised that the self-perceived impact of the COVID-19 pandemic would be associated with psychiatric and neurologic disorders1,2, and dimensional traits8. Although overall, people with psychiatric and neurologic conditions were less likely to report increased connectedness during the pandemic, these generalised associations were of small scale. More selective associations were observed for particular disorders: elevated health worries in anxiety disorders and obsessive-compulsive disorder, and increased conflict at home in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder30,31. The pandemic outbreak for people with OCD may reinforce underlying health anxieties and serve to reinforce underlying belief systems; an initial study in OCD patients indeed reported worsening of symptom severity longitudinally following the outbreak of the pandemic32. Guidance to help management of OCD during the pandemic is available33. A previous survey of parents reported worsening of their children’s ADHD symptoms during the pandemic34. which has been proposed to reflect the emotional dysregulation inherent to the disorder30,35–37. The observation here of self-reported increases in conflict in the home for people with ADHD accord closely with this view.
Personality, technology use and compulsivity traits were strongly interrelated and collectively accounted for substantial variation in pandemic impact. Most notably, people who reported having increased health concerns tended to have higher insecurity, cognitive rigidity, reward drive, technology addiction and stress from technology. A similar pattern of association was evident for people reporting more disrupted lifestyle. These results accords with the notion that certain traits are prominent in shaping resilience, whereas others engender vulnerability38. A crucial aspect of these data is that whereas certain conventional personality traits have been previously related to resilience, including during COVID-19 lockdown39, the roles of compulsivity and technology use have received very limited attention in this context. In part, this may be due to a historic lack of available, validated trans-diagnostic compulsivity instruments40. Here, compulsivity (reward drive and cognitive rigidity)41,42 were shown to associate with vulnerability, robustly associating with negative self-perceived pandemic impact.
Technology use bore important statistical relationships with positive and negative dimensions of self-perceived pandemic impact. Prior studies proposed that a subset of people develop problematic usage of online technology43–46, and a consensus statement suggested this would be especially impactful during lockdown3. Here, we found that negative COVID-19 impact was strongly linked not with time spent using online technology per se, but rather with maladaptive online behaviours. Relatedly, the most prominent technology stressor was reading news articles. Conversely, the benefits of using technology to stay connected were prominent in the questionnaire. Looking forward, one might infer that encouraging people to simply limit screen time could be counterproductive; more nuanced approaches that develop foster healthy online behaviours47,48, whilst minimising maladaptive use3,45,46,49, have relevance through and beyond the recovery phase of the pandemic.
The primary limitations of this study pertain to cross-sectional analysis, albeit within the context of a longitudinal study. We believe this limitation is mitigated by the large scale of data, the close matching of participant demographic profiles pre vs mid pandemic, and the rigorous application of multivariate statistical analyses to take into account diverse potentially confounding population factors. They also are counterbalanced by the ability to compare data at scale just prior to the pandemic and midway through the first lockdown. A further limitation is the focus on self-report measures, although we note that this limitation applies to much pandemic-related mental health research, since face-to-face data collection exposes research participants and study teams to potential infection risks.
Finally, a critical consideration for any survey-based study is how representative the sample is of the general population. All sampling approaches, probabilistic or otherwise, have some degree of bias as participants must be accessible and motivated to respond. We believe a key strength of our study is its use of a large-scale non-probabilistic approach to capture baseline in the weeks immediately prior to the pandemic in the UK rather than relying on historical data or baseline data collected after the pandemic was already entrenched. We sought to maximise inclusivity through the high visibility to the general public of the BBC. Furthermore, the bias analysis showed only negligible scaled differences in mental health measures between those who did vs did not choose to complete the PD-GIS scale. The resultant sample compares favourably to bioresource-based and random sampling studies in terms of inclusivity of minority groups and age range. More importantly, stratified, formally representative population samples are often constrained to one or two measures of interest, including those deployed to date in the COVID-19 context. In contrast, our inferences are based on analysis models where broad psycho-socio-demographic variables are accounted for, this being enabled by unusually large sample size. Future research should quantitatively compare in greater detail the relative biases present in mainstream-media based vs. other sampling approaches.
Looking forward, we are recontacting this cohort at 6 and 12 months to plot change in the idiosyncratic impact of the pandemic and its aftermath in vivo12,50 including more detailed assessment of identified vulnerable sub-groups. We also note that the current paper focused on data from people aged 16 and above – we intend to examine data from younger people in separate work. It is vital that research examines the impact of the pandemic in younger people using this and related methodologies adapted for that purpose2. Regarding domains of self-perceived impact, the PD-GIS may not capture all domains that people find important in terms of pandemic impact. A future article will report topic modelling of free text reports of pandemic impact collected alongside the current dataset.
In summary, our results demonstrate value of measuring multiple dimensions when quantifying pandemic impact on mental health and highlight the necessity of incorporating the broad psycho-socio-economic context when seeking to understand, predict or mitigate such impact. The largest associations explaining mental health during the pandemic and self-perceived pandemic impact on daily life related to age, occupation and home context, and external space, followed by medium associations with personality traits, compulsivity and maladaptive technology use, and smaller but still notable associations for mental health and neurological disorders and demographic characteristics. The ways in which different vulnerable groups report being impacted by the pandemic can provide rich information to help guide future resource provision, research, and clinical interventions, in order to maximise outcomes and minimise untoward effects of the COVID-19 pandemic as it continues to manifest, especially in periods of lockdown or social restrictions. To this end, we are making our dataset available for detailed analysis by the broader research community.
Methods
Recruitment
Starting from December 26th, 2019, participants were recruited to the study website, where they completed cognitive tests and a detailed questionnaire. The sampling approach was large-scale and non-probabilistic. Specifically, to maximise visibility and inclusiveness articles describing the study were placed on the BBC2 Horizon, BBC homepage, BBC News homepage and circulated on mobile news meta-apps from January 1st, 202051 in the UK. To maximise representativeness of the sample there were no inclusion or exclusion criteria. Analyses here exclude data from participants under 16 years old, as they completed a briefer questionnaire, and those who responded to the questionnaire unfeasibly fast (<4 minutes). Cognitive test data will be reported separately. The study was approved by the Imperial College Research Ethics Committee (17IC4009) and participants provided informed consent prior to starting the test. Data were collected from individuals at a particular time point, rather than data being collected serially from the same individuals.
Data collection
Data52 were collected via our custom server system, which produces study-specific cognitive testing and survey websites (https://gbws.cognitron.co.uk) on the Amazon EC2. Questionnaires and tests were programmed in JavaScript and HTML5. They were deliverable via personal computers, tablets and smartphones. The questionnaire included scales quantifying sociodemographic, lifestyle, online technology use, personality, and mental health (Supplementary methods)42,53–56. Participants could enrol for longitudinal follow up, scheduled for 3, 6 and 12 months. People returning to the site outside of these timepoints were navigated to a different URL. On May 2nd, 2020, the questionnaire was augmented-in light of the COVID-19 pandemic-with an extended mood scale, and an instrument comprising 47 items quantifying self-perceived effects on mood, behaviour and outlook (Pandemic General Impact Scale PD-GIS). PD-GIS specifically asked about changes due to the pandemic (increases and decreases). Questions regarding pre-existing psychiatric and neurological conditions, lockdown context, having the virus, and free text fields were added. This coincided with further promotion via BBC2 Horizon and BBC Homepage.
Data processing and statistical analysis
Analyses were conducted in MATLAB R2020a. Participants with missing data were retained as some questions were contingent on others; therefore, observations per analysis vary with data availability. The questionnaire was organised into the following scales: demographics and lifestyle, online technology use, mood, personality, compulsive traits, and pandemic impact. Where appropriate, scales were summarised in the following steps. Agree-disagree and frequency items were filtered for missing data casewise within scale, converted to numeric, rank transformed to normality, and subscale scores estimated using principal component analysis. Components with eigenvalues>1 were varimax rotated and component scores estimated by regression. While there are other methods for determining factor fit, this is widely used in the literature, and is convenient. PCAs are reported in Supplementary figures 12-16 and supplementary tables 32-41.
Cohort demographics were segmented into pre-pandemic, early-pandemic, and mid-pandemic epochs (Figure 1). General linear modelling tested how population variables statistically predicted differences in mood, anxiety, and sleep, between the pre-pandemic and mid-pandemic epochs. To enable this, responses were converted to a numeric scale, ranging as follows. 0 (never), 1 (rarely), 2 (once or twice a week), 3 (several time a week), 4 (every day), 5 (hourly) and 6 (more often). These two epochs were analysed because they had large sample sizes, and comprised participants with similar demographic distributions, whereas the Early-Pandemic epoch comprised generally younger aged individuals.
Subscales of the pandemic impact instrument (PD-GIS supplementary figure 10) quantified self-perceived impact across 7 psycho-socio-economic dimensions. These were identified by applying a permutation-based approach, whereby eigenvalues for the true data were cross compared to 10,000 permutations in which the linkage across measures was destroyed18. Components with eigenvalues >95% of the permutations at the corresponding index were retained. The rotated component scores were cross related to the mental health self-assessment in a multivariate manner using canonical correlation analysis (CCA), which finds linear combinations of the two multivariate matrices that have maximum correlation with each other57, and in a bivariate manner using Pearson’s correlation.
General linear modelling (GLM) determined the relationship of sociodemographic variables, home context, cohabitees and work arrangements to the seven components of the PD-GIS. Further GLMs examined carers and people reporting psychiatric and neurologic diagnoses (for groups with N>90 members) with the sociodemographics factored out.
Due to the expected high shared variance between online technology use, personality traits and compulsivity, their multivariate relationships with the PD-GIS were quantified using a linear regression support vector machine regression model58 in MATLAB, specifically, the fitrlinear function, which by default uses support vector machines with a ridge penalty, and optimises using dual stochastic gradient descent. To evaluate generalisability of the model and degree of overfit, a train-test pipeline was applied, whereby the model was trained by fitting the trait measures onto the scores for each individual PD-GIS component with 50% of the data, and the r value then estimated between predicted and true data when the trained model was applied to the same data, and the other 50% of the data, to which it was naive. This process was repeated 100 times with different random splits and the mean and standard deviation correlation value reported.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This study was conducted in collaboration with BBC2 Horizon. The study was supported by the UK Dementia Research Institute and Biomedical Research Centre at Imperial College London. Technology development was supported by EU-CIG EC Marie-Curie CIG and NIHR grant II-LB-0715-20006 to AH. ES’s role was supported by MRC grant MR/R005370/1 to AH. WT is supported by the EPSRC Center for Doctoral Training in Neurotechnology under supervision of AH. This research was funded in part by Wellcome [110049/Z/15/Z and 110049/Z/15/A] (Grant to SC). For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. MM is in part supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. We would like to acknowledge COST Action CA16207 ‘European Network for Problematic Usage of the Internet’, supported by COST (European Cooperation in Science and Technology); and the support of the National UK Research Network for Behavioural Addictions (NUK-BA).
Footnotes
Author contributions | AH designed and instigated this study, arranged promotion of the study URL, oversaw design of the questionnaire and programming of assessment software, conducted all statistical analyses, created figures, and authored the article. PH contributed to the design of the questionnaire and the study, programmed the server-side software, oversaw data collection and curation, and edited the article. ES contributed to the design of the study questionnaire, developed custom visualisation software for the project and edited the article. MM contributed to the design of the study questionnaire and edited the article. KI contributed to the design of the study questionnaire and edited the article. WT contributed to the design of the questionnaire and the running of the study, programmed the custom questionnaire software with AH, contributed to data collection and curation, and edited the article. JG contributed to the design of the study questionnaire and edited the article. SC contributed to the design of the study and of the study questionnaire, provided input on the analyses, and co-wrote all versions of the article.
Declaration of interests | SC previously consulted for Promentis. He receives honoraria for journal editorial work from Elsevier. JG has received research grants from the TLC Foundation for Body-Focused Repetitive Behaviors, Biohaven, Promentis, and Avanir Pharmaceuticals. MM has received grant income from Takeda Pharmaceuticals, Johnson & Johnson and Lundbeck. AH is owner and founder of Future Cognition Ltd and H2 Cognitive Designs Ltd, which develop custom cognitive assessment software for other university-based research groups. PJH is owner and co-founder of H2 Cognitive Designs Ltd. The authors report no other conflicts of interest.
Data availability
All statistical models and data, both raw and filtered for analysis are available via the UK Data Service: COVID-19 impact dataset: Great British Intelligence Test, 2020 https://reshare.ukdataservice.ac.uk/854451/. No limitations are applied on use of the shared data. A key detailing the location of data and models for main display items is provided in the supplementary information. A Source Data file is provided with this paper.
Code Availability
Native MATLAB functions were used to generate all statistical models in this study these and are available via the UK Data Service: COVID-19 impact dataset: Great British Intelligence Test, 2020 https://reshare.ukdataservice.ac.uk/854451/.
References
- 1.Pfefferbaum B, North CS. Mental Health and the COVID-19 Pandemic. N Engl J Med. 2020;383:510–512. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp2008017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Holmes EA, et al. Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: a call for action for mental health science. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7:547–560. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kiraly O, et al. Preventing problematic internet use during the COVID-19 pandemic: Consensus guidance. Compr Psychiatry. 2020;100:152180. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Pareek M, et al. Ethnicity and COVID-19: an urgent public health research priority. Lancet. 2020;395:1421–1422. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30922-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Brooks SK, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet. 2020;395:912–920. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chen S, et al. The early impact of COVID-19 on mental health and community physical health services and their patients’ mortality in Cambridgeshire and Peterborough, UK. J Psychiatr Res. 2020;131:244–254. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wang Q, Xu R, Volkow ND. Increased risk of COVID-19 infection and mortality in people with mental disorders: analysis from electronic health records in the United States. World Psychiatry. 2020 doi: 10.1002/wps.20806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ioannidis K, Askelund AD, Kievit RA, van Harmelen AL. The complex neurobiology of resilient functioning after childhood maltreatment. BMC Med. 2020;18:32. doi: 10.1186/s12916-020-1490-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Philip KEJ, Polkey MI, Hopkinson NS, Steptoe A, Fancourt D. Social isolation, loneliness and physical performance in older-adults: fixed effects analyses of a cohort study. Sci Rep. 2020;10:13908. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-70483-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bu F, Steptoe A, Fancourt D. Who is lonely in lockdown? Cross-cohort analyses of predictors of loneliness before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Public Health. 2020;186:31–34. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2020.06.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Iob E, Steptoe A, Fancourt D. Abuse, self-harm and suicidal ideation in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic. Br J Psychiatry. 2020;217:543–546. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2020.130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fancourt Daisy, Steptoe A, Bu F. Trajectories of depression and anxiety during enforced isolation due to COVID-19; longitudinal analyses of 59,318 adults in the UK with and without diagnosed mental illness. medRxiv. 2020 [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pierce M, et al. Says who? The significance of sampling in mental health surveys during COVID-19. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7:567–568. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30237-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Chamberlain SR, Stochl J, Redden SA, Grant JE. Latent traits of impulsivity and compulsivity: toward dimensional psychiatry. Psychological medicine. 2018;48:810–821. doi: 10.1017/s0033291717002185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chamberlain SR, et al. Fractionation of impulsive and compulsive trans-diagnostic phenotypes and their longitudinal associations. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2019;53:896–907. doi: 10.1177/0004867419844325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fry A, et al. Comparison of Sociodemographic and Health-Related Characteristics of UK Biobank Participants With Those of the General Population. Am J Epidemiol. 2017;186:1026–1034. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sawilowsky S. New Effect Size Rules of Thumb. Journal of modern applied statistical methods. 2009 [Google Scholar]
- 18.Horn JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika. 1965 doi: 10.1007/BF02289447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Niedzwiedz CL, et al. Mental health and health behaviours before and during the initial phase of the COVID-19 lockdown: longitudinal analyses of the UK Household Longitudinal Study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2020 doi: 10.1136/jech-2020-215060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Twenge JM, Joiner TE. U.S. Census Bureau-assessed prevalence of anxiety and depressive symptoms in 2019 and during the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic. Depress Anxiety. 2020 doi: 10.1002/da.23077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Richardson SJ, et al. Research with older people in a world with COVID-19: identification of current and future priorities, challenges and opportunities. Age Ageing. 2020 doi: 10.1093/ageing/afaa149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.D’cruz M, Banerjee D. ‘An invisible human rights crisis’: The marginalization of older adults during the COVID-19 pandemic - An advocacy review. Psychiatry Res. 2020;292:113369. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kondo MC, Jacoby SF, South EC. Does spending time outdoors reduce stress? A review of real-time stress response to outdoor environments. Health Place. 2018;51:136–150. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nutsford D, Pearson AL, Kingham S. An ecological study investigating the association between access to urban green space and mental health. Public Health. 2013;127:1005–1011. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2013.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kisely S, et al. Occurrence, prevention, and management of the psychological effects of emerging virus outbreaks on healthcare workers: rapid review and meta-analysis. BMJ. 2020;369:m1642. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m1642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Braquehais MD, et al. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on the mental health of healthcare professionals. QJM. 2020 doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcaa207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Spoorthy MS, Pratapa SK, Mahant S. Mental health problems faced by healthcare workers due to the COVID-19 pandemic-A review. Asian J Psychiatr. 2020;51:102119. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Baldwin DS. What is the role of a psychiatrist in the COVID-19 pandemic? Medicine (Abingdon) 2020 doi: 10.1016/j.mpmed.2020.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Simons G, Baldwin DS. COVID-19: doctors must take control of their wellbeing. BMJ. 2020;369:m1725. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m1725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cortese S, Coghill D. Twenty years of research on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): looking back, looking forward. Evid Based Ment Health. 2018;21:173–176. doi: 10.1136/ebmental-2018-300050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Posner J, Polanczyk GV, Sonuga-Barke E. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. 2020;395:450–462. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)33004-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Davide P, et al. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on patients with OCD: Effects of contamination symptoms and remission state before the quarantine in a preliminary naturalistic study. Psychiatry Res. 2020;291:113213. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fineberg NA, et al. How to manage obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) under COVID-19: A clinician’s guide from the International College of Obsessive Compulsive Spectrum Disorders (ICOCS) and the Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders Research Network (OCRN) of the European College of Neuropsychopharmacology. Compr Psychiatry. 2020;100:152174. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Zhang J, et al. Acute stress, behavioural symptoms and mood states among school-age children with attention-deficit/hyperactive disorder during the COVID-19 outbreak. Asian J Psychiatr. 2020;51:102077. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hirsch O, Chavanon M, Riechmann E, Christiansen H. Emotional dysregulation is a primary symptom in adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) J Affect Disord. 2018;232:41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171:276–293. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Cortese S. Pharmacologic Treatment of Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder. N Engl J Med. 2020;383:1050–1056. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1917069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Grant JE, Chamberlain SR. Obsessive-Compulsive Personality Disorder. American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2019. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Morales-Vives F, Dueñas JM, Vigil-Colet A, Camarero-Figuerola M. Psychological Variables Related to Adaptation to the COVID-19 Lockdown in Spain. Front Psychol. 2020;11:565634. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hook RW, et al. Trans-diagnostic measurement of impulsivity and compulsivity: A review of self-report tools. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2020 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.10.007. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Albertella L, et al. Compulsivity is measurable across distinct psychiatric symptom domains and is associated with familial risk and reward-related attentional capture. CNS Spectr. 2019:1–8. doi: 10.1017/S1092852919001330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Chamberlain SR, Grant JE. Initial validation of a transdiagnostic compulsivity questionnaire: the Cambridge-Chicago Compulsivity Trait Scale. CNS Spectr. 2018:1–7. doi: 10.1017/S1092852918000810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ioannidis K, et al. Problematic internet use (PIU): Associations with the impulsive-compulsive spectrum. An application of machine learning in psychiatry. J Psychiatr Res. 2016;83:94–102. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Tiego J, et al. Problematic use of the Internet is a unidimensional quasi-trait with impulsive and compulsive subtypes. BMC Psychiatry. 2019;19:348. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2352-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.King DL, Delfabbro PH, Billieux J, Potenza MN. Problematic online gaming and the COVID-19 pandemic. J Behav Addict. 2020;9:184–186. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Saunders JB, et al. Gaming disorder: Its delineation as an important condition for diagnosis, management, and prevention. J Behav Addict. 2017;6:271–279. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bland AR, et al. COVID-19 induced social isolation; implications for understanding social cognition in mental health. Psychological medicine. 2020:1–3. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720004006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Vatansever D, Wang S, Sahakian BJ. COVID-19 and promising solutions to combat symptoms of stress, anxiety and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020 doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00791-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Håkansson A, Fernández-Aranda F, Menchón JM, Potenza MN, Jiménez-Murcia S. Gambling During the COVID-19 Crisis - A Cause for Concern. J Addict Med. 2020;14:e10–e12. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0000000000000690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wang C, et al. A longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;87:40–48. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hampshire A. Great British Intelligence Test Protocol. Research Square. 2020 [Google Scholar]
- 52.Hampshire A. UK Data Service, Colchester, Essex; UK: 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Oshio A, Taku K, Hirano M, Saeed G. Resilience and Big Five personality traits: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018 doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.048. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The Patient Health Questionnaire-2: validity of a two-item depression screener. Med Care. 2003;41:1284–1292. doi: 10.1097/01.MLR.0000093487.78664.3C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Lowe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:1092–1097. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.John OP, Naumann LP, S CJ. In: Handbook of personality: Theory and research. Robins RW, John OP, Pervin LA, editors. Guilford Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hotelling H. Relations Between Two Sets of Variates. Biometrika. 1936 [Google Scholar]
- 58.Drucker H, Burges CC, Kaufman L, Smola AJ, Vapnik VN. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 9, NIPS 1996 Ch. Support Vector Regression Machines. MIT Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All statistical models and data, both raw and filtered for analysis are available via the UK Data Service: COVID-19 impact dataset: Great British Intelligence Test, 2020 https://reshare.ukdataservice.ac.uk/854451/. No limitations are applied on use of the shared data. A key detailing the location of data and models for main display items is provided in the supplementary information. A Source Data file is provided with this paper.
Native MATLAB functions were used to generate all statistical models in this study these and are available via the UK Data Service: COVID-19 impact dataset: Great British Intelligence Test, 2020 https://reshare.ukdataservice.ac.uk/854451/.