Abstract
Motor adaptation is crucial for performing accurate movements in a changing environment and relies on the cerebellum. Although cerebellar involvement has been well characterized, the neurochemical changes in the cerebellum underpinning human motor adaptation remain unknown.
We used a novel Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopic Imaging (MRSI) technique to measure changes in the inhibitory neurotransmitter γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) in the human cerebellum during visuomotor adaptation. Participants (n = 17, 6 female) used their right hand to adapt to a rotated cursor in the scanner, compared with a control task requiring no adaptation. We spatially resolved adaptation-driven GABA changes at the cerebellar nuclei and cerebellar cortex in the left and the right cerebellar hemisphere independently and found that simple right-hand movements increase GABA in the right cerebellar nuclei and decreases GABA in the left. When isolating adaptation-driven GABA changes, we found that GABA in the left cerebellar nuclei and the right cerebellar nuclei diverged, though GABA change from baseline at the right cerebellar nuclei was not different from zero at the group level. Early adaptation-driven GABA fluctuations in the right cerebellar nuclei correlated with adaptation performance: Participants showing greater GABA decrease adapted better, suggesting early GABA change is behaviourally relevant. Early GABA change also correlated with functional connectivity change in a cerebellar network: Participants showing greater decreases in GABA showed greater strength increases in cerebellar network connectivity. Results were specific to GABA, to adaptation and to the cerebellar network.
This study provides first evidence for plastic changes in cerebellar neurochemistry during motor adaptation. Characterising these naturally occurring neurochemical changes may provide a basis for developing therapeutic interventions to facilitate human motor adaptation.
Keywords: cerebellum, GABA, visuomotor adaptation, spectroscopy, neuroimaging, functional connectivity
Introduction
As we move through the world we are constantly confronted with changes in our bodies and our environment. Muscle properties change due to fatigue, exercise or development and the objects we interact with vary in weight, size or how well we can manipulate them. Adapting our motor commands to these variations is thus crucial to interacting with our environment.
The cerebellum plays a key role in motor adaptation. Patient studies and brain stimulation suggest that an intact cerebellum function is a prerequisite for adaptation (Martin et al., 1996; Tseng et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2010; Diedrichsen et al. 2005, Jenkinson & Miall, 2010), with more severe cerebellar impairment resulting in worse adaptation (Tseng et al., 2007). Healthy participants show increased cerebellar activity early in adaptation (Seidler et al., 2006; Ichise et al., 2000) and in response to sensory prediction errors (Schlerf et al., 2012), as well as adaptation-related changes in cerebellar activity in visuomotor learning paradigms (Miall and Jenkinson 2005; Graydon et al., 2005). Though these studies provide a compelling argument for cerebellar involvement in adaptation, the physiological changes in the cerebellum during adaptation are not yet fully understood.
Adaptation may lead to decreases in the major inhibitory neurotransmitter γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA). GABA is found throughout the cerebellum and plays an important role in cerebellar plasticity (Ito, 2014). In the cerebellar cortex, GABA has been implicated in plasticity at parallel fibre synapses (Orts-Del’Immagine et al., 2018) as well as Purkinje cell synapses (He et al., 2015). GABAergic Purkinje cells, the sole output neurons of the cerebellar cortex, exert a constant inhibitory tone on the deep cerebellar nuclei via frequent simple spike firing. Strong climbing fibre input causes Purkinje cells to fire complex spikes, resulting in a pause in simple spike firing, hence releasing the deep cerebellar nuclei from inhibition (Thach, 1967). In non-human primates, Purkinje cells increased complex spike firing, and therefore decreased inhibition, after introduction of a perturbation, which returned to baseline as the monkeys learned to adapt their movements and errors decreased (Gilbert et al., 1977). However, despite the importance of GABA in motor adaptation, cerebellar GABA changes during human motor adaptation have not yet been investigated, likely due to the technical challenges presented by cerebellar MR in dealing with the low signal-to-noise ratio in the cerebellum, which is located deep in the cranium and close to brain stem structures that move during the cardiac cycle (Brooks et al., 2013; Diedrichsen et al., 2010).
In this within-subject, crossover study, we used a novel Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopic Imaging (MRSI) approach to quantify GABA in the human cerebellum while participants performed a visuomotor adaptation task with their right hand, compared with a non-rotated control using the same hand. We were able to spatially resolve adaptation-driven GABA changes at the cerebellar nuclei and in the cerebellar cortex in the left and the right cerebellar hemisphere independently. We hypothesised that adaptation would lead to a decrease in GABA in the right deep cerebellar nuclei, as the uncrossed pathways at the level of the cerebellum results in mainly ipsilateral hand representations (Buckner et al. 2011, Mottolese et al. 2013, Diedrichsen and Zotow, 2015). We hypothesised that the degree of GABA decrease would be correlated with adaptation on a subject-by-subject basis, such that greater GABA decreases related to greater adaptation. Finally, since previous studies have shown an increase in intrinsic functional connectivity in the cerebellum after adaptation (Albert et al., 2009; Nettekoven et al., 2020) and functional connectivity has been shown to correlate with GABA in a major network node (Stagg, 2014, Bachtiar 2018), we hypothesised that GABA decrease would correlate with increased cerebellar functional connectivity.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Seventeen healthy, right-handed participants (6 female; 18-34 years; mean age 23 years) participated in this within-subject, crossover study. Each participant attended two sessions on two separate days during which they performed a visuomotor task with either an adaptation component (rotation condition) or no adaptation component (control condition) during MRI. The order of the sessions was counterbalanced across the group. Participants had no history of neurological or psychiatric conditions and were not taking any psychoactive medications. All participants gave their written informed consent to participate, in line with central university research ethics committee approval (University of Oxford; R59564/RE002).
Experimental protocol
The experimental protocol for each session is shown in figure 1A. Participants performed a visuomotor task using a hand-held joystick (Nata Technologies, custom-built fMRI joystick, 60 Hz sampling rate, figure 1C left panel) to control a cursor on a screen, which either involved an adaptation component (rotation condition) or no adaptation component (control condition). Participants were instructed to make ballistic movements to ‘shoot’ with their cursor through one of eight targets (Figure 1C middle panel).
The target appeared at one of eight locations radially aligned around the starting position of the cursor and separated by 45°. Trials were grouped into epochs, with one epoch containing eight consecutive trials and all blocks containing five epochs (total: 5x8 = 40 trials; one block equates to one grey shaded region in figure 1B). The sequence of target presentation was fixed across participants to ensure equal task difficulty across participants. Participants had to perform the movement within a time window of 750 ms, after which the target disappeared, to ensure short, ballistic movements, minimising online corrections. No endpoint feedback was provided.
At the beginning of each session, participants performed a familiarisation block outside the scanner which consisted of 40 trials with no rotation imposed. They then performed 136 trials of no rotation in the scanner, serving as the baseline. Then, in the rotation condition, participants were required to adapt their centrifugal shooting movements to a rotation of the visual feedback which increased stepwise by 10 degrees after every block of 40 trials to maximally drive adaptation throughout the duration of the scanning session (Figure 1B red blocks). The imposed rotation reached a maximum of 80°, after which visual feedback was removed for four blocks of 40 trials each (blocks with crossed out eye in figure 1B) to assess aftereffect. In the control condition, participants performed the task without any rotation imposed for the full duration of the task (Figure 1B blue blocks). After the scan, participants performed 144 trials (i.e. 3.6 blocks) with no rotation imposed to probe retention of the previously learned compensatory movement.
Behavioural analysis
Cursor movements were analyzed on a trial-by-trial basis using in-house software written in Matlab (Mathworks Inc, Natick, USA). The joystick position data (X and Y) was collected at a sample rate of 60 Hz. The kinematic data was filtered with a zero-phase filter with a 25 Hz cut-off and numerically differentiated to determine velocity. Trials that showed premeditated or incomplete movements were excluded from further analysis (mean number of rejected trials 30 ± 29).
For each trial, the angular error (measured in degrees) was calculated as the angle between a line connecting the starting position with the position of peak velocity of the cursor and the line connecting the starting position with the target. Positive values indicate a clockwise error (‘overshooting’) and negative values indicate a counter-clockwise error (‘undershooting’).
We quantified adaptation in the rotation condition by calculating the mean error across all rotation blocks, excluding the first epoch of each block (Galea et al., 2011). We normalised the mean error in the rotation condition to the mean error in the control condition to ensure that differences in the adaptation measure were specific to better adaptation to the imposed rotation. To do this, we calculated error change in the rotation condition by dividing the angular error in rotation blocks by angular error at baseline. We then calculated the difference between error change in the rotation condition and error change in the control condition by subtracting control from rotation error change. Positive change values therefore reflect a greater increase in error in the rotation condition than in the control condition. The normalisation formula applied to the rotation condition error was the same as the GABA normalisation, which is described in a later section. We refer to this normalised rotation error as adaptation error.
To quantify aftereffect, the mean error during all open loop trials of the rotation condition was normalised to the mean error during the control condition open loop trials. We refer to this normalised open loop measure as aftereffect. To quantify retention, the mean error in the first washout block of the rotation condition was normalised to the mean error in the first washout block after the control condition. We refer to this normalised washout error as retention error.
To determine whether participants adapted in the rotation condition, we used the R package lme4 (Bates et al. 2015) to construct a linear mixed effects model (LME) of error with epoch and target as fixed effects. To directly compare error reduction from the beginning of each block to the end of each block in the rotation condition and in the control condition, we constructed an LME model of the first and last epoch error in both conditions and tested for a significant condition x epoch interaction. In this LME model, we also entered target as a fixed effect to account for target-specific movement biases. For all LME models we allowed intercepts for different subjects to vary, to account for covarying residuals within subjects.
Neuroimaging acquisition
All Magnetic Resonance (MR) data were acquired on a 3T Siemens scanner (PRISMA) with a 32-channel head coil. Structural MRI data were acquired using a magnetisation prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence (Repetition Time 1900 ms, TE 3.96 ms, slice thickness 1.0 mm, in-plane resolution 1.0 x 1.0 mm2, flip angle = 8, FOV 256 x 232 mm2). The resting-state fMRI acquisition was matched to the UK Biobank resting-state fMRI parameters described in (Alfaro-Almagro et al. 2018). A multiband isotropic EPI acquisition (TR/TE = 735/39 ms, FOV = 210 x 210 mm2, bandwidth = 2030 Hz/Px, multiband acceleration factor 8, voxel dimension = 2.4 x 2.4 x 2.4 mm3, whole brain, acquisition time = 6:10 min for a total of 490 volumes) was performed before and immediately after the task. A separate “single-band reference scan” (SBRef) was acquired before the multiband acquisition (Moeller et al. 2010). This serves as the reference scan during pre-processing (e.g. during motion correction and registration steps), as it suffers from the same distortions as the multiband data, but has higher contrast (Alfaro-Almagro et al. 2018). Subjects fixated on a cross-hair image presented centrally on the screen during resting-state fMRI acquisition.
fMRI analysis
All MRI data processing was carried out using FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library, Version 6.00, Jenkinson et al. 2012). For resting-state functional data, pre-processing steps were largely matched to the UK Biobank processing pipeline (Alfaro-Almagro et al. 2018). The data was motion corrected using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al. 2012), slice-timing corrected using Fourier-space time-series phase-shifting, distortion-corrected using fieldmaps (B0 unwarping), stripped of non-brain voxels using BET (Smith 2002), grand-mean intensity normalised and highpass temporally filtered (Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight line fitting, with sigma = 50.0 s)).
To remove structured noise, functional data were cleaned using a single-subject ICA, implemented in MELODIC and the automated classifier FIX, which was trained on 40 hand-labelled Biobank datasets to identify noise ICA components (Beckmann et al., 2004; Salimi-Khorshidi et al. 2014; Griffanti 2014). Although these training datasets were matched in acquisition sequence and experimental parameters to the datasets acquired in our experiment, we checked its classification accuracy to ensure the suitability of the Biobank-trained FIX algorithm for our data by manually classifying four datasets and comparing our labels to the FIX-classification. The FIX-classified labels matched our hand classification at 99.8%, which is within the expected range of classification accuracy (Alfaro-Almagro et al. 2018).
Functional data were registered to the MNI-152 template, using the SBref image as an intermediate step. Functional data were first linearly registered to the SBref image using 6 degrees of freedom (FLIRT; Jenkinson et al. 2001; Jenkinson et al. 2002), then to the structural image using boundary-based registration (BBR; implemented in FLIRT), and finally to the MNI- 152 template using non-linear registration (FNIRT; Andersson et al., 2007; Andersson et al., 2019). Finally, spatial smoothing was applied with a 5 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.
Group Networks
In order to derive group networks, we applied a group ICA. For this, we concatenated the pre-processed functional data temporally across sessions and across subjects to create a single dataset. We then used the group-ICA to obtain 50 components. Visual inspection of the group-level components revealed that at a dimensionality of 50 the cerebellar network of interest included only cerebellar regions, whereas at a lower dimensionality, this network included regions of the visual cortex. We confirmed the cerebellar network (Figure 5A) as the network with the greatest spatial correlation with cerebellar regions active during performance of sensorimotor tasks (Hardwick et al., 2013). To determine the anatomical specificity of any effects we identified the Default Mode Network (DMN; Figure 5B) as a control network which we expected to be unaffected by task condition, and which does not spatially overlap with the task activation mask.
Dual regression
Subject-and-scan-specific networks were obtained via a dual regression approach (Nickerson 2017), masked by the group mean network.
Connectivity change
We tested whether change in network strength correlated with change in GABA. We obtained the network strength measure from the subject-and-scan-specific networks, which were derived through dual regression. Those networks were then masked by the group mean network and the within-mask z-values were extracted and averaged. This resulted in one value which reflects the network strength for each subject and for each fMRI timepoint (pre and post), from which we then calculated strength change. To do this, we used the same normalisation procedure as the one used for behavioural data, namely dividing the strength value of the subject’s network after task performance (post; Figure 1A) by network strength at baseline (pre; Figure 1A) and calculating the difference between rotation strength change and control strength. Positive change values therefore reflect a greater increase in network strength in the rotation condition than in the control condition. We refer to this measure as connectivity change.
Spectroscopic Imaging acquisition
Structural images were used to manually place a 65 x 25 x 15 mm3 MRSI slab in the cerebellum. The slab was positioned to cover anterior and superior areas of the right cerebellum which contain the hand motor representation (Buckner et al. 2011, Mottolese et al. 2013, Diedrichsen and Zotow, 2015), while avoiding contact with either the dura, to minimize significant macromolecule contamination, or with brainstem, to avoid excess physiological noise (Brooks et al., 2013).
Four MRSI scans were acquired during performance of the visuomotor task (time per scan = 9 min, total acquisition 36 min, TE 32 ms; Figure 1A). MRSI data was acquired using a nonwater suppressed semi-LASER DW-CRT sequence with metabolite cycling (Steel et al. 2018) at 5 x 5 x 15 mm3 resolution.
Spectroscopy analysis
MRSI data was reconstructed and pre-processed according to Steel et al., 2018 using in-house scripts. Briefly, pre-processing included: metabolite cycling reconstruction (Emir et al 2017), coil-combination (Walsh et al., 2000), and correction for frequency and phase shifts, residual water removal using HLSVD (Cabanes et al., 2001) and eddy currents correction using the unsuppressed water signal (Klose, 1990). Concentration of neurochemicals was quantified as in Steel et al., 2018, using LCModel (Provencher et al., 2001). Specifically, we used a chemical shift of 0.5 to 4.2 ppm, a basis set containing 30 metabolites, default LCModel macromolecules, soft constraints on metabolites were disabled (NRATIO set to 0), and a baseline stiffness setting (DKMTM) of 0.25 (raw spectrum, spectral fit and scaled basis spectra in figure 2C). Voxels in the metabolite maps were excluded if CRLB of the respective metabolite > 60%, LCModel defined linewidths (FWHM) > 20 Hz, or signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) < 15 (thresholded GABA shown maps in figure 2A). MRSI data analysis therefore yielded independent quantification of neurochemical concentrations at four timepoints. All neurochemical concentrations are expressed as a ratio to total Creatine (Cr + PCr, tCr).
Metabolite maps were registered to the MNI template by a linear registration to the structural scan (FLIRT; Jenkinson et al., 2001; Jenkinson et al., 2002), and then non-linearly warp to the MNI template (FNIRT; Andersson et al., 2007; Andersson et al., 2019). Overlap maps of the MRSI slab in MNI space for each condition are shown in figure 2B. The MRSI slab was reproducibly placed between sessions (mean spatial correlation coefficient between each subject’s pair of MRS voxels: r = 0.85 ± 0.06). The MRSI slab overlapped on average with 22 % of participant’s right cerebellar lobule VI (Mean: 22 ± 6.29 %) and with 20.6 % of the right cerebellar lobule VI (Mean: 20.6 ± 6.87 %). There was no significant difference between left lobule VI coverage compared to right lobule VI coverage (Paired t-test of percentage values: t(16) = 0.68, p = 0.51). Hence coverage of the left and right cerebellar lobules was similar and did not systematically differ, ensuring that the measurements were not biased to either hemisphere.
To address our hypotheses, we conducted an ROI-based analysis using the SUIT toolbox (Diedrichsen, 2006) for SPM12. We created SUIT-space ROIs for the left and right cerebellar nuclei and cortex separately (Figure 3C right panel and Figure 3I right panel). We then registered the SUIT-space ROI masks to each subject’s structural scan. The nuclei masks consisted of all deep cerebellar nuclei of the respective hemisphere. Because resolving GABA changes at the level of individual nuclei is not currently feasible, we did not perform the analysis at the level of individual nuclei, but rather quantified GABA from masks that include all deep cerebellar nuclei in the right and the left hemisphere, i.e. for the right hemisphere the mask included the right dentate, interposed and fastigial nucleus. The fastigial nucleus was included since animal studies have suggested that the output of the fastigial nucleus is relevant for upper limb movement (Brava et al., 1983; Licata et al., 1985).
Normalisation
To control for effects, we normalised GABA in the rotation condition to GABA in the control condition using the same normalisation procedure as was applied to the behavioural data. The normalised GABA value at each timepoint is therefore equivalent to taking the difference between the percentage change in rotation GABA and the percentage change in control GABA. The GABA data was normalised for each hemisphere separately, resulting in normalised values for left and right cerebellar hemisphere (GABAiLEFT and GABARight respectively).
Statistical Analysis
Mean ± SD are presented throughout. Statistical analyses of the data were conducted using R (RCoreTeam2013) and Statistics Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, Version 25, IBM Corp., Armork, NY, USA). Repeated Measures-ANOVAs were used to test for neurochemical changes in the cerebellum during the different experimental conditions in the left compared to the right cerebellum. Maunchly’s test of sphericity was used to test the homogeneity of variance. Where Mauchly’s test of sphericity was significant (p<0.05) in RM- ANOVAs, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied. Post hoc tests were calculated using Bonferroni-adjusted paired-samples t-tests.
To test relationships between neurochemical changes and behaviour, Pearson’s correlation coefficients (two-tailed) were calculated. Partial correlations were used to control for a behavioural index of no interest (two-tailed, unless otherwise specified). To determine whether two correlation coefficients were significantly different, Pearson’s correlation coefficients were converted into z-scores using Fisher’s Z-transformation. One-tailed z-tests were then performed on the z-scores given the standard error of each z-score (Myers et al., 2006).
Results
Participants adapted in the rotation condition and retained the aftereffect
We first tested whether participants adapted by reducing their error to the applied visuomotor perturbation in the rotation condition. As expected, we found that participants adapted within rotation blocks, indicated by a significant effect of epoch on error (LME of error in rotation blocks: χ2(1) = 5.78, p = 0.02; Figure 1B). To test whether this error reduction was specific to the rotation condition, we then compared error reduction in the rotation condition and error reduction in the control condition directly. In line with error reduction taking place exclusively in the rotation blocks of the rotation condition, we observed a significant interaction of epoch and condition in an LME (χ2(1) = 12.697, p < 0.001).
We next tested whether participants retained the adapted state. We observed an aftereffect in each of the four open loop blocks of the rotation condition, indicated by a significant negative error (Bonferroni-corrected one sample t-test of mean error for each open loop block, all p’s < 0.001). In contrast, no aftereffect was observed in any of the open loop blocks of the control condition (Bonferroni-corrected one sample t-test of mean error for each block, all p’s >0.99). Directly comparing the open loop blocks of the rotation condition and the control condition confirmed the presence of an aftereffect throughout the open loop blocks (Bonferroni-corrected paired t-test of mean error for each open loop block, all p’s < 0.001; Figure 1B).
We found that participants retained the compensatory movement in the washout phase of the rotation condition (Bonferroni-corrected one sample t-test of mean error for each block, all p’s < 0.01), but that there was no error during the washout phase of the control condition (Bonferroni-corrected one sample t-test of mean error for each block, all p’s > 0.28).
Because the rotation task imposed an adapted state that persisted throughout the washout phase, we tested for potential carry-over effects from the rotation session to the next session in this counterbalanced study. We found no evidence for carry-over effects in the second session, since there was no difference in baseline error of the second session between participants who had performed the rotation condition in the first session and participants who had performed the control condition in the first session (Paired t-test of baseline error: -4.090 ± 0.536, t(15) = -1.760, p = 0.099).
GABA is stable across sessions
Because the MRSI approach employed in this study has not been used before in the cerebellum, we first quantified the reliability of our GABA measurements at baseline across sessions. The intraclass-correlation coefficient (ICC) of the baseline GABA measurements was 0.45 (F(16,16.5) = 2.63; p = 0.03), indicating a ‘fair’ reliability between sessions. To check whether GABA quantification was affected by the number of MRSI voxels included in an ROI, we looked for a relationship between GABA:tCr and number of voxels in our ROIs, and found no relationship in either the deep cerebellar nuclei masks (r = -0.13, p = 0.29) or the cerebellar cortex masks (r = -0.1 p = 0.42). Further, there was no difference in the number of voxels surviving quality assessment across the slab between the two conditions (t(16) = 1.140, p = 0.27).
GABA changes in the cerebellar nuclei during movement execution
We first investigated GABA changes over time between conditions, regions, and hemispheres, via a four-way RM-ANOVA with within-subject factors of condition (Rotation, Control), region (Nuclei, Cortex), hemisphere (Left, Right) and timepoint (Base, Early, Late, After). There was no main effect on GABA:tCr of condition (F(1,16) = 1.145, p = 0.300, partial η2 =.067) or timepoint (F(3,48) = 1.391,p = 0.257, partial η2 =.08). However, GABA:tCr differed between regions (F(1,16) = 463.532, p < 0.0001, partial η2 =.967) and between hemispheres (F(1,16) = 71.816, p < 0.0001, partial η2 =.818), with higher GABA:tCr in the cerebellar nuclei (0.554 ± 0.014) than in the cerebellar cortex (0.348 ± 0.012) and higher GABA:tCr in left (0.478 ± 0.013) compared to the right cerebellar hemisphere (0.424 ± 0.012). There was a significant interaction between region and hemisphere (F(1,16) = 21.403, p < 0.001, partial η2 =0.572), driven by a greater difference in GABA:tCr between the hemispheres in the nuclei than in the cortex. Finally, we observed a significant three-way interaction between condition, hemisphere and timepoint (F(3,48) = 4.544, p = 0.007, partial η2 = 0.221). All other interactions were non-significant (all p > 0.1).
To understand this three way interaction, we performed RM-ANOVAs on GABA:tCr from the two conditions and the two regions separately, with within-subject factors of hemisphere (Left, Right) and timepoint (Base, Early, Late, After; Figure 3).
In the control condition, there were no bilateral task-related changes in GABA:tCr in the cerebellar nuclei (main effect of timepoint F(3,48) = 0.793,p = 0.504, partial η2 =0.047), but there was a significant main effect of hemisphere (F(1,16) = 9.551, p = 0.007, partial η2 =0.374) and a significant hemisphere*time interaction (F(3,48) = 3.943, p = 0.014, partial η2 =0.198; Figure 3A). Post hoc t-tests demonstrated that GABA:tCr was higher in the left than right nuclei at baseline (t(16.000) = 3.552, p = 0.011, 0.829 ± 0.963). However, during control task performance, GABA decreased in the left nuclei and increased in the right nuclei, so that there was no significant difference between the hemispheres during task performance (Early: t(16.000) = 2.204, p = 0.170, 0.636 ± 1.190; Late: t(16.000) = -0.438, p = 1.000, -0.125 ± 1.179; Figure 3A shows GABA:tCr timecourse normalised to baseline). In the cerebellar cortex, performance of the control task had no significant effects on GABA:tCr (main effect of hemisphere: F(1,16) = 3.166, p = 0.094, partial η2 =0.165; main effect of timepoint: F(3,48) = 0.793,p = 0.275, partial η2 =0.017; hemisphere*time interaction: F(3,48) = 3.943, p = 0.277, partial η2 =0.074; Figure 3G).
In the rotation condition, there were no task-related changes in GABA:tCr in the cerebellar nuclei (main effect of timepoint: F(3,48) = 2.528,p = 0.068, partial η2 =0.136; hemisphere*time interaction: F(3,48) = 0.539, p = 0.658, partial η2 =0.033; Figure 3B) or the cortex (main effect of timepoint: F(3,48) = 0.644,p = 0.591, partial η2 =0.039; hemisphere*time interaction: F(3,48) = 0.539, p = 0.658, partial η2 =0.033; Figure 3H).
Isolating adaptation reveals GABA diverges in the left and right cerebellar nuclei
Given the significant effects of movement on GABA:tCr in the cerebellar nuclei, we wished to isolate any effects of adaptation over and above movement on inhibition. In particular, we wanted to account for any GABA fluctuations not driven by adaptation, but by movement execution or other learning processes such as use-dependent learning. To do this we normalised GABA:tCr during the rotation condition to GABA:tCr during control by subtraction (Figure 3C and Figure 3I). To test the hypothesis that GABA would be differentially affected in the left and right nuclei during adaptation, we then performed a two-way RM-ANOVA with within-subject factors of hemisphere (Left, Right) and timepoint (Early, Late, After). In line with our hypothesis, we observed significant GABA:tCr modulation in left compared to right nuclei (significant hemisphere*time interaction: F(2,32) = 4.490, p = 0.014, partial η2 = .236; main effect of timepoint: F(2,32) = 0.054, p = 0.948, partial η2 = 0.003; Figure 3C).
Finally, to test the neurochemical specificity of the observed GABA:tCr changes in the cerebellar nuclei, we performed an equivalent RM-ANOVA to investigate adaptation-related changes in Glutamate:tCr. There was no main effect of timepoint on Glutamate:tCr, and no hemisphere*time interaction (main effect of timepoint: F(2,32) = 0.054, p = 0.948, partial η2 = 0.003, F(2,32) = 4.490, p = 0.014, partial η2 = 0.236; Figure 3F), suggesting that these findings were specific to GABA and not a reflection of more general processes.
“Additionally, although not the main focus of the experiment, we tested for GABA changes for each hemisphere separately. For this, we used a one-sample t-test of GABA change from baseline to timepoint Late separately for each condition and each hemisphere. In the control condition, there was a significant GABA change in the left cerebellar nuclei (t(16) = -2.46, p = 0.03) and a non-signficant GABA change in the right cerebellar nuclei (t(16) = 1.88; p = 0.08). In the rotation condition, there was no significant GABA change in the right cerebellar nuclei (t(16) = 1.36, p = 0.19), or in the left cerebellar nuclei (t(16) = 1.98, p = 0.07). GABA change during isolated adaptation was significantly different from zero for the left cerebellar nuclei, (t(16) = 3.75, p < 0.002), but not for the right cerebellar nuclei (t(16) = -0.38, p = 0.14).”
Adaptation-driven early GABA change correlates with adaptation
Prior studies have shown that early change in motor cortical GABA during learning of a motor sequence correlates with subsequent learning-related changes in reaction times on the task (Kolasinski, 2019; Floyer-Lea et al., 2016). We therefore hypothesised that early change in the right cerebellar nuclei GABA:tCr would correlate with error during adaptation. We observed a significant correlation between change in GABA:tCr in the right nuclei and adaptation error (Figure 4B; r = 0.64 p = 0.006), such that participants showing an early decrease in right nuclei GABA:tCr (Timepoint: Early in Figure 1A) adapted their right-hand movements better to the rotation. This relationship was specific both neurochemically (change in Glutamate:tCr and adaptation r = -0.10 p = 0.693; zdiff = 2.27, p = 0.011; Figure 4C) and anatomically (change in left nuclei GABA:tCr and adaptation r = -0.16, 0.537; zdiff = 2.43, p < 0.01). To test whether our relationship between GABA:tCr and adaptation was specific to that behaviour, we tested for a relationship between GABA:tCr change and retention error, which demonstrated a significant correlation (r = -0.49 p = 0.0455). However, as expected, our metrics of adaptation and retention are highly correlated (R = -0.8185 p < 0.0001). We therefore calculated partial correlations between early GABA:tCr change and the two behavioural metrics. GABA:tCr change and adaptation were still significantly correlated when controlling for retention (r = 0.47, p = 0.03 [one-tailed]), but GABA:tCr change did not correlate with retention when controlling for adaptation (R = 0.07, p = 0.39). Additionally, we tested whether GABA change from baseline to timepoint Early was significantly different from zero. We found no significant difference (t(16) = 0.81, p = 0.43).
Adaptation-driven GABA changes correlate with cerebellar connectivity change
Finally, we wanted to determine whether our behaviour-related changes in GABA correlated with changes in cerebellar functional connectivity. We did not demonstrate a significant group mean change in cerebellar network strength after adaptation (all p > 0.05). However, there was a significant correlation between early change in right nuclei GABA:tCr and the change in cerebellar functional connectivity (r = -0.4939 p = 0.0439; Figure 5D). This relationship was both neurochemically (Glutamate change and cerebellar connectivity change: r = 0.09 p = 0.71; zdiff = -1.69 p = 0.0459; Figure 5E) and anatomically specific (GABA change and connectivity change in DMN, r = 0.2707 p = 0.29; zdiff = -2.17 p = 0.015; Figure 5C). There was no relationship between cerebellar connectivity change and adaptation error (r = -0.11 p = 0.67).
Discussion
In this study, we used a novel, spatially-resolved MRSI technique to quantify cerebellar GABA to investigate neurochemical changes during visuomotor adaptation. We found that simple movement of the right hand leads to diverging GABA timecourses in the right cerebellar nuclei compared to GABA in the left. When controlling for these movement-related GABA changes, in line with our hypotheses we found GABA in the left cerebellar nuclei increased compared to GABA in the right cerebellar nuclei during adaptation. Although GABA change from baseline at the right cerebellar nuclei was not different from zero at the group level, early adaptation-driven fluctuations in right cerebellar nuclei GABA correlates with performance in the righthand adaptation task: participants who show a greater GABA decrease adapt better, suggesting this early GABA change is behaviourally relevant. Finally, while we did not demonstrate a group mean change in cerebellar connectivity, the adaptation-driven right cerebellar nuclei GABA decrease correlated with functional connectivity changes in a network that has previously been shown to increase in strength in response to adaptation (Albert et al., 2009; Nettekoven et al., 2020). Participants who show a greater early decrease in GABA in the right cerebellar nuclei also show a greater increase in cerebellar network connectivity. These findings are anatomically specific and neurochemically specific: control analyses show no adaptation-related changes in the cerebellar cortex and no concentration changes in glutamate. Our results are also behaviourally specific and network-specific: The relationship between adaptation and right nuclear GABA change holds when controlling for retention, but there is no correlation between retention and right nuclear GABA change controlling for adaptation, suggesting this relationship is specific to adaptation. Further, right nuclear GABA change correlates with strength change in a cerebellar network, but not with strength change in the Default Mode Network, confirming the specificity of our results for functional connectivity of the cerebellum.
Movement-related increase in GABA in ipsilateral nuclei
When groups of parallel fibres activate and cause high enough input to Purkinje cells so that Purkinje cells fire simple spikes, this results in GABA release at the cerebellar nuclei. Simple spike responses can occur in response to a large variety of vestibular and motor signals in vivo (Barmack and Yakhnitsa, 2013; Dean et al., 2010). For example, increases in simple spike firing have been shown during eye movements in the direction of their preferred response (Medina and Lisberger, 2008; Yang and Lisberger, 2014). There may also be population coding of hand movements within the CS as has been described for eye movements (Thier et al., 2000; Popa et al., 2019).
While the increase in GABA in the right compared to the left cerebellar nuclei observed in this study was unexpected, there are a number of plausible reasons for this GABA change during simple movement execution. Perhaps the most parsimonious explanation would be that GABA increase in the right nuclei could reflect increased simple spike firing of Purkinje cells, leading to higher GABA concentration in the deep cerebellar nuclei. In the control condition participants performed simple ballistic wrist movements with veridical visual feedback, and participants therefore made no systematic movement errors. In response to these simple movements, converging parallel fibre activity may have caused Purkinje cells in the right cerebellum to fire simple spikes and, due to the absence of movement errors in the control condition, a lack of complex spikes may have increased simple spike activity of the same Purkinje cells.
Adaptation-related change in GABA in ipsilateral nuclei
Unexpected target movements that result in a visual error have been shown to lead to complex spike firing on some trials (Medina and Lisberger, 2008; Yang and Lisberger, 2014). After trials where a complex spike occurred, reduced simple spike firing was found on the next trial, conversely if no complex spike occurred, simple spike firing showed a slight increase on the next trial (Yang and Lisberger, 2014). Similar decreases in simple spike firing rate after a complex spike have been observed following saccades, with a less pronounced increase in simple spikes when no complex spike was present on the previous trial (Herzfeld et al., 2018).
The systematic movement errors that occurred in our adaptation condition might therefore be expected to result in reduced simple spike firing after the complex spike signal transmitted the error information, thereby reducing inhibitory tone (GABA) in the nuclei. Consistent with this, complex spike firing has been shown to serve as a “teaching signal” (Ma et al., 2016), leading to long-term depression (LTD) of parallel fibre-Purkinje cell synapses of those parallel fibres active simultaneously with the climbing fibre input (Ito and Kano, 1982; Albus, 1971; Marr, 1969). LTD in turn has been shown to result in decreased simple spike firing (Ito and Kano, 1982; Gao et al., 2003; Yang and Lisberger, 2014) and improve motor performance (Medina and Lisberger, 2008). Hence it is plausible that a reduction in GABA concentration at the deep cerebellar nuclei could be tied to successful adaptation.
In line with the hypothesis that these adaption-related changes in GABA concentration are related to cerebellar plasticity, we demonstrated a correlation between the extent of early GABA changes in the right nuclei and adaptation. A greater decrease in GABA was related to better adaptation.
There are several potential explanations for the GABA increase in the contralateral nuclei during adaptation. One possible reason may be that the contralateral GABA increase may suppress output from the side of the cerebellum that is non-relevant to the adaptation task. Since the output would be relayed by the cerebellar nuclei, it is plausible that increased GABA in the contralateral cerebellar nuclei may serve to suppress output from the contralateral cerebellar hemisphere.
Simultaneous, but distinct processes have been shown to underlie adaptation (Smith et al., 2006), including explicit and implicit processes (Taylor et al., 2014; Taylor et al., 2011; McDougle et al., 2015; McDougle & Taylor, 2019), and the adaptation paradigm has been suggested to influence which processes are favoured (Fleury et al., 2019). Indeed, there is evidence that implicit adaptation in visuomotor tasks is limited (Morehead et al., 2015; Morehead et al., 2017), and it is possible that the adaptation paradigm used in this study led to the use of explicit strategies to counter the rotation. Further, although participants adapted substantially during the task adaptation was incomplete, as evidenced by the aftereffect. Both the recruitment of explicit strategies, as well as the inability to fully adapt could potentially influence the physiological changes observed. Further research is required to determine the effect of different task paradigms on the differential recruitment of explicit and implicit processes and on enabling asymptotic adaptation, and future studies should explore how paradigm choice influences GABA changes observed in the cerebellum.
Cerebellar connectivity change is correlated with GABA in the nuclei
The adaptation-related GABA change in the right cerebellar nuclei was correlated with functional connectivity change in a cerebellar network, such that a decrease in GABA was associated with stronger cerebellar network connectivity. This cerebellar network has previously been shown to increase in strength after adaptation, compared to after a control task (Albert et al., 2009; Nettekoven et al., 2020), in a performance-relevant manner (Nettekoven et al., 2020). We did not see a similar change here, perhaps because the restingstate scan in this study was not acquired immediately after the rotation blocks, but after interspersed open loop blocks, which may have diluted a strength increase of the cerebellar network after adaptation.
Previous work from our lab and others has shown that GABA measured from a key node of a functional network is related to functional connectivity within that network (Stagg et al., 2014; Bachtiar et al., 2015; Kapogiannis et al., 2013). This finding therefore suggests that the relationship between local inhibition and connectivity may be a general property of functional networks, even in the context of the highly different physiology and anatomy of the cerebellar and cerebral cortices.
No GABA changes in the cerebellar cortex
In the cerebellar cortex, GABA is released by inhibitory interneurons in the molecular layer and the granule layer. Molecular layer interneurons, called basket and stellate cells, receive input from parallel fibres and inhibit Purkinje cells, resulting in di-synaptic inhibition in parallel with the monosynaptic excitatory input from granule cells to Purkinje cells (Rieubland et al., 2014; Arlt et al., 2020). In the granule layer, Golgi cells receive input from mossy fibres as well as granule cells and inhibit granule cells and other Golgi cells (Hull and Regehr, 2012). Although there is evidence that plasticity at molecular layer interneurons could regulate plasticity of parallel fibre-Purkinje cell synapses and influence adaptation in vivo (Rowan et al., 2018), there is no consensus yet on how precisely inhibitory interneurons in the cerebellar cortex contribute to adaptation learning.
We did not see significant changes in GABA in the cerebellar cortex during simple movement execution or during adaptation. It is possible that GABA concentration changes in the cerebellar cortex were not detectable with MRSI. It is also possible that the placement of the MRSI grid did not optimally or only incompletely cover the right-hand representation in the cerebellar cortex spanning right lobules 5 and 6 in the anterior cerebellum (Diedrichsen and Zotow, 2015). Future studies should probe the role of GABA in the cerebellar cortex directly by optimizing voxel size and placement such that potential changes in GABA in the right-hand representation in the cerebellar cortex are captured. Although not yet technically feasible, it would also be interesting to resolve changes at individual cerebellar nuclei and individual cerebellar lobules using finer MRSI resolution.
Conclusions
In this study, we present (to the best of our knowledge) the first investigation of cerebellar GABA changes during human visuomotor adaptation. We found movement-related GABA changes in the left and right cerebellar nuclei. When we controlled for these, we isolated GABA changes in the nuclei that were driven by adaptation. We found relationships between adaptation-driven GABA change in the right cerebellar nuclei and adaptation performance, and between adaptation-driven GABA change in the right cerebellar nuclei and change in cerebellar network connectivity. These results show the first evidence for plastic changes in cerebellar neurochemistry during a cerebellar learning task and provide the basis for developing therapeutic interventions that facilitate these naturally occurring changes to amplify cerebellar-dependent learning.
Significance Statement.
Despite motor adaptation being fundamental to maintaining accurate movements, its neurochemical basis remains poorly understood, perhaps because measuring neurochemicals in the human cerebellum is technically challenging. Using a novel magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging method, this study provides evidence for GABA changes in the left compared to the right cerebellar nuclei driven by both simple movement and motor adaptation. Though right cerebellar GABA changes were not significantly different from zero at the group-level, the adaptation-driven GABA fluctuations in the right cerebellar nuclei correlated with adaptation performance, and with functional connectivity change in a cerebellar network. These results show the first evidence for plastic changes in cerebellar neurochemistry during a cerebellar learning task. This provides the basis for developing therapeutic interventions that facilitate these naturally occurring changes to amplify cerebellar-dependent learning.
Acknowledgments
The research was supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Oxford Biomedical Research Centre and the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. CJS holds a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship, funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (102584/Z/13/Z). HJB holds a Wellcome Principal Research Fellowship (110027/Z/15/Z). The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust (203139/Z/16/Z). For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest statement:
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author Contributions:
CRN: Designed research, performed research, analyzed data, wrote the first draft of the paper and edited the paper
LM: Performed research, analyzed data, edited the paper
WTC: Contributed analytic tools and edited the paper
UE: Contributed analytic tools and edited the paper
JC: Performed the research and edited the paper
HJB: Edited the paper
NJ: Designed the research and edited the paper
CJS: Designed research, performed research, wrote the paper and edited the paper
Data Availability
MRI data, MRSI data and behavioural will be shared on the data sharing platform of the Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging which is currently under development. In the interim, data is available upon request.
References
- Albert NB, Robertson EM, Miall RC. The Resting Human Brain and Motor Learning. Curr Biol. 2009;19(12):1023–1027. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albus JS. A theory of cerebellar function. 1971 doi: 10.1016/0025-5564(71)90051-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Alfaro-Almagro F, Jenkinson M, Bangerter NK, Andersson JL, Griffanti L, Douaud G, et al. Smith SM. Image processing and Quality Control for the first 10,000 brain imaging datasets from UK Biobank. Neuroimage. 2018;166:400–424. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.034. (October 2017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S. Non-linear registration aka spatial normalisation. 2007 (tech. rep. No. June). Retrieved from http://fmrib.medsci.ox.ac.uk/analysis/techrep/tr07ja2/tr07ja2.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Andersson JD, Matuskey D, Finnema SJ. Positron emission tomography imaging of the γ-aminobutyric acid system. Neurosci Lett. 2019;691:35–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.08.010. (May 2018) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arlt C, Häusser M. Microcircuit Rules Governing Impact of Single Interneurons on Purkinje Cell Output In Vivo. Cell Rep. 2020;30(9):3020–3035.:e3. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bachtiar V, Near J, Johansen-Berg H, Stagg CJ. Modulation of GABA and resting state functional connectivity by transcranial direct current stimulation. Elife. 2015;4:1–9. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08789. (September 2015) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67(1):arXiv: 1406.5823. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barmack NH, Yakhnitsa V. In: Handb cerebellum cerebellar disord. Manto M, Schmahmann JD, Rossi F, Gruol DL, Koibuchi N, editors. 2013. Vestibulocerebellar Connections; pp. 357–375. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann CF, Smith SM. Probabilistic Independent Component Analysis for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2004;23(2):137–152.:arXiv: 978-087893-286-3. doi: 10.109/TMI.2003.822821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks JC, Faull OK, Pattinson KT, Jenkinson M. Physiological noise in brainstem fMRI. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013 OCT;7:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL. Human functional connectivity: New tools, unresolved questions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(24):10769–10770. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005987107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Thomas Yeo BT. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(5):2322–2345. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabanes E, Confort-Gouny S, Le Fur Y, Simond G, Cozzone PJ. Optimization of residual water signal removal by HLSVD on simulated short echo time proton MR spectra of the human brain. J Magn Reson. 2001;150(2):116–125. doi: 10.1006/jmre.2001.2318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coesmans M, Weber JT, De Zeeuw CI, Hansel C. Bidirectional parallel fibre plasticity in the cerebellum under climbing fibre control. Neuron. 2004;44(4):691–700. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Zeeuw CI, Lisberger SG, Raymond JL. Diversity and dynamism in the cerebellum. Nat Neurosci. 2020 doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00754-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dean P, Porrill J, Ekerot CF, Jörntell H. The cerebellar microcircuit as an adaptive filter: Experimental and computational evidence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(1):30–43. doi: 10.1038/nrn2756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J. Neural Correlates of Reach Errors. J Neurosci. 2005;25(43):9919–9931. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, Verstynen T, Schlerf J, Wiestler T. Advances in functional imaging of the human cerebellum. Curr Opin Neurol. 2010;23(4):382–387. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32833be837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, Zotow E. Surface-based display of volume-averaged cerebellar imaging data. PLoS One. 2015;10(7):1–18. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dyke K, Pépés SE, Chen C, Kim S, Sigurdsson HP, Draper A, et al. Jackson SR. Comparing GABA-dependent physiological measures of inhibition with proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy measurement of GABA using ultra-high-field MRI. Neuroimage. 2017;152:360–370. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.011. (October 2016) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emir UE, Burns B, Chiew M, Jezzard P, Thomas MA. Non-water-suppressed short-echo-time magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging using a concentric ring k-space trajectory. NMR Biomed. 2017;30(7):1–12. doi: 10.1002/nbm.3714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emir UE, Sood J, Chiew M, Thomas MA, Lane SP. High-resolution metabolic mapping of the cerebellum using 2D zoom magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging. Magn Reson Med. 2021;85(5):2349–2358. doi: 10.1002/mrm.28614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrant M, Nusser Z. Variations on an inhibitory theme: Phasic and tonic activation of GABA A receptors. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(3):215–229. doi: 10.1038/nrn1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleury L, Prablanc C, Priot AE. Do prism and other adaptation paradigms really measure the same processes? Cortex. 2019;119:480–496. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyer-Lea A, Wylezinska M, Kincses T, Matthews PM. Rapid modulation of GABA concentration in human sensorimotor cortex during motor learning. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95(3):1639–1644. doi: 10.1152/jn.00346.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea JM, Vazquez A, Pasricha N, Orban De Xivry JJ, Celnik P. Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: The motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(8):1761–1770. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao W, Dunbar RL, Chen G, Reinert KC, Oberdick J, Ebner TJ. Optical imaging of long-term depression in the mouse cerebellar cortex in vivo. J Neurosci. 2003;23(5):1859–1866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-05-01859.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert PF, Thach WT. Purkinje cell activity during motor learning. Brain Res. 1977;128(2):309–328. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90997-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffanti L, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Beckmann CF, Auerbach EJ, Douaud G, Sexton CE, et al. Smith SM. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. Neuroimage. 2014;95:232–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansel C, de Jeu M, Belmeguenai A, Houtman SH, Buitendijk GHH, Andreev D, et al. Elgersma Y. αCaMKII Is Essential for Cerebellar LTD and Motor Learning. Neuron. 2006;51(6):835–843. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardwick RM, Rottschy C, Miall RC, Eickhoff SB. A quantitative meta-analysis and review of motor learning in the human brain. Neuroimage. 2013;67:283–297. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He Q, Duguid I, Clark B, Panzanelli P, Patel B, Thomas P, et al. Smart TG. Interneuron- and GABAA receptor-specific inhibitory synaptic plasticity in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nat Commun. 2015 May;6 doi: 10.1038/ncomms8364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzfeld DJ, Kojima Y, Soetedjo R, Shadmehr R. Encoding of error and learning to correct that error by the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21(5):736–743. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0136-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hull C, Regehr WG. Identification of an Inhibitory Circuit that Regulates Cerebellar Golgi Cell Activity. Neuron. 2012;73(1):149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, version 27.0. 2020 [Google Scholar]
- Ichise T, Kano M, Hashimoto K, Yanagihara D, Nakao K, Shigemoto R, et al. Aiba A. mGluR1 in cerebellar Purkinje cells essential for long-term depression, synapse elimination, and motor coordination. Science (80-) 2000;288(5472):1832–1835. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M. Cerebellar Microcircuitry. Encycl neurosci. 2009:723–728. doi: 10.1016/B978-008045046-9.01306-1. [M.] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M, Kano M. Long-lasting depression of parallel fibre-Purkinje cell transmission induced by conjunctive stimulation of parallel fibres and climbing fibres in the cerebellar cortex. Neurosci Lett. 1982;33(3):253–258. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(82)90380-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jalali R, Miall RC, Galea JM. No consistent effect of cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation on visuomotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2017;118(2):655–665. doi: 10.1152/jn.00896.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images. Neuroimage. 2002;17(2):825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. Review FSL. Neuroimage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med Image Anal. 2001;5(2):143–156. doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson N, Miall RC, Chris R. Disruption of saccadic adaptation with repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the posterior cerebellum in humans. Cerebellum. 2010 doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0193-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson JF, Belyk M, Schwartze M, Pinheiro AP, Kotz SA. The role of the cerebellum in adaptation: ALE meta-analyses on sensory feedback error. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40(13):3966–3981. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jörntell H, Ekerot CF. Reciprocal bidirectional plasticity of parallel fibre receptive fields in cerebellar Purkinje cells and their afferent interneurons. Neuron. 2002;34(5):797–806. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00713-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kapogiannis D, Reiter DA, Willette AA, Mattson MP. Posteromedial cortex glutamate and GABA predict intrinsic functional connectivity of the default mode network. Neuroimage. 2013;64(1):112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klose U. In vivo proton spectroscopy in presence of eddy currents. Magn Reson Med. 1990 doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910140104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ma Y, Shaik MA, Kozberg MG, Kim SH, Portes JP, Timerman D, Hillman EM. Resting-state hemodynamics are spatiotemporally coupled to synchronized and symmetric neural activity in excitatory neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(52):E8463–E8471. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1525369113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marr JE. A Theory of Cerebellar Cortex. J Physiol. 1969;202:437–470. doi: 10.2307/1776957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medina JF, Lisberger SG. Links from complex spikes to local plasticity and motor learning in the cerebellum of awake-behaving monkeys. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11(10):1185–1192. doi: 10.1038/nn.2197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT. Throwing while looking through prisms. Brain. 1996 doi: 10.1093/brain/119.4.1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDougle SD, Taylor JA. Dissociable cognitive strategies for sensorimotor learning. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1) doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07941-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDougle SD, Taylor JA. Dissociable cognitive strategies for sensorimotor learning. Nat Commun. 2019;10 doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07941-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moeller S, Yacoub E, Olman CA, Auerbach E, Strupp J, Harel N, Uğurbil K. Multiband multislice GE-EPI at 7 tesla, with 16-fold acceleration using partial parallel imaging with application to high spatial and temporal whole-brain FMRI. Magn Reson Med. 2010;63(5):1144–1153. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mottolese C, Richard N, Harquel S, Szathmari A, Sirigu A, Desmurget M. Mapping motor representations in the human cerebellum. Brain. 2013;136(1):330–342. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morehead RJ, Taylor JA, Parvin DE, Ivry RB. Characteristics of implicit sensorimotor adaptation revealed by task-irrelevant clamped feedback. J Cogn Neurosci. 2017 doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morehead JR, Qasim SE, Crossley MJ, Ivry R. Savings upon re-aiming in visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci. 2015 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1046-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers L, Sirois MJ. Spearman Correlation Coefficients, Differences between. Wiley StatsRef Stat Ref. 2014 doi: 10.1002/9781118445112.stat02802. Online 1-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nettekoven C, Brady S, Clarke W, Emir UE, Levenstein J, Petitet P, et al. Stagg CJ. GABA relates to functional connectivity changes and retention in visuomotor adaptation. bioRxiv. 2020:1–29.:arXiv: arXiv:1011.1669v3. doi: 10.1101/2020.12.22.423981. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nickerson LD, Smith SM, Öngür D, Beckmann CF. Using dual regression to investigate network shape and amplitude in functional connectivity analyses. Front Neurosci. 2017 MAR;11:1–18. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orts-Del’Immagine A, Pugh JR. Activity-dependent plasticity of presynaptic GABAB receptors at parallel fibre synapses. Synapse. 2018;72(5):1–12. doi: 10.1002/syn.22027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popa LS, Streng ML, Ebner TJ. Purkinje Cell Representations of Behavior: Diary of a Busy Neuron. Neuroscientist. 2019;25(3):241–257. doi: 10.1177/1073858418785628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Provencher SW. Automatic quantitation of localized in vivo1H spectra with LCModel. NMR Biomed. 2001;14(4):260–264. doi: 10.1002/nbm.698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Development Core Team 3.0.1. A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2013. Retrieved from http://www.r-project.org. [Google Scholar]
- Rieubland S, Roth A, Häusser M. Structured connectivity in cerebellar inhibitory networks. Neuron. 2014;81(4):913–929. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson EM, Miall RC. Visuomotor adaptation during inactivation of the dentate nucleus. Neuroreport. 1999;10(5):1029–1034. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904060-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowan MJ, Bonnan A, Zhang K, Amat SB, Kikuchi C, Taniguchi H, et al. Christie JM. Graded Control of Climbing-Fibre-Mediated Plasticity and Learning by Inhibition in the Cerebellum. Neuron. 2018;99(5):999–1015.:e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salimi-Khorshidi G, Douaud G, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Griffanti L, Smith SM. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: Combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. Neuroimage. 2014;90:449–468. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlerf J, Ivry RB, Diedrichsen J. Encoding of sensory prediction errors in the human cerebellum. J Neurosci. 2012;32(14):4913–4922. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4504-11.2012.arXiv:NIHMS150003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidler RD, Noll DC, Chintalapati P. Bilateral basal ganglia activation associated with sensorimotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res. 2006;175(3):544–555. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0571-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semyanov A, Walker MC, Kullmann DM, Silver RA. Tonically active GABAA receptors: Modulating gain and maintaining the tone. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27(5):262–269. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simpson R, Devenyi GA, Jezzard P, Hennessy TJ, Near J. Advanced processing and simulation of MRS data using the FID appliance (FID-A)—An open source, MATLAB-based toolkit. Magn Reson Med. 2017;77(1):23–33. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17(3):143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23(SUPPL 1):S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stagg CJ, Bachtiar V, Amadi U, Gudberg CA, Ilie AS, Sampaio-Baptista C, et al. Johansen-Berg H. Local GABA concentration is related to network-level resting functional connectivity. Elife. 2014;2014(3):1–9. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stagg CJ, Bachtiar V, Johansen-Berg H. The role of GABA in human motor learning. Curr Biol. 2011;21(6):480–484. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steel A, Chiew M, Jezzard P, Voets NL, Plaha P, Thomas MA, et al. Emir UE. Metabolite-cycled density-weighted concentric rings k-space trajectory (DW-CRT) enables high-resolution 1 H magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging at 3-Tesla. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26096-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JA, Klemfuss NM, Ivry RB. An explicit strategy prevails when the cerebellum fails to compute movement errors. Cerebellum. 2010;9(4):580–586. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0201-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JA, Ivry RB. Flexible cognitive strategies during motor learning. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011;7(3):arXiv: e1001096. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor JA, Krakauer JW, Ivry RB. Explicit and implicit contributions to learning in a sensorimotor adaptation task. J Neurosci. 2014;34(8):3023–3032.:arXiv: NIHMS150003. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3619-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thach WT. Somatosensory receptive fields of single units in cat cerebellar cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1967;30(4):675–696. doi: 10.1152/jn.1967.30.4.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thier P, Dicke PW, Haas R, Barash S. Encoding of movement time by populations of cerebellar Purkinje cells. Nature. 2000 doi: 10.1038/35011062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Towers SK, Gloveli T, Traub RD, Driver JE, Engel D, Fradley R, et al. Whittington MA. α5 subunit-containing GABAA receptors affect the dynamic range of mouse hippocampal kainate-induced gamma frequency oscillations in vitro. J Physiol. 2004 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.071191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng Y-w, Diedrichsen J, Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ. Sensory Prediction Errors Drive Cerebellum-Dependent Adaptation of Reaching. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98(1):54–62. doi: 10.1152/jn.00266.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tunik E, Schmitt PJ, Grafton ST. BOLD coherence reveals segregated functional neural interactions when adapting to distinct torque perturbations. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97(3):2107–2120. doi: 10.1152/jn.00405.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waddell KW, Zanjanipour P, Pradhan S, Xu L, Welch EB, Joers JM, et al. Gore JC. Anterior cingulate and cerebellar GABA and Glu correlations measured by 1H J-difference spectroscopy. Magn Reson Imaging. 2011;29(1):19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2010.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh DO, Gmitro AF, Marcellin MW. Adaptive reconstruction of phased array MR imagery. Magn Reson Med. 2000 doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1522-2594(200005)43:5<682::AID-MRM10>3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner S, Schorn CF, Bock O, Theysohn N, Timmann D. Neural correlates of adaptation to gradual and to sudden visuomotor distortions in humans. 2014 doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3824-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang Y, Lisberger SG. Role of plasticity at different sites across the time course of cerebellar motor learning. J Neurosci. 2014;34(21):7077–7090. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0017-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
MRI data, MRSI data and behavioural will be shared on the data sharing platform of the Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging which is currently under development. In the interim, data is available upon request.