Abstract
The debilitating symptoms of Parkinson’s disease including the hallmark slowness of movement, termed bradykinesia, were described more than 100 years ago. Despite significant advances in elucidating the genetic, molecular and neurobiological changes in Parkinson’s disease, it remains conceptually unclear why exactly patients with Parkinson’s disease move slowly. To address this, we summarize behavioural observations of movement slowness in Parkinson’s disease and discuss these findings in a behavioural framework of optimal control. In this framework, agents optimize the time it takes to gather and harvest reward by adapting their movement vigour depending on the reward that is at stake and the effort that needs to be expended. Thus, slow movements can be favourable when the reward is deemed unappealing or the movement very costly. While reduced reward sensitivity, which makes patients less inclined to work for reward, has been reported in Parkinson’s disease, this appears to be mainly related to motivational deficits (apathy) rather than bradykinesia. Increased effort sensitivity has been proposed to underlie movement slowness in Parkinson’s disease. However, careful behavioural observations of bradykinesia are inconsistent with abnormal computations of effort costs due to accuracy constraints or movement energetic expenditure. These inconsistencies can be resolved when considering that a general disability to switch between stable and dynamic movement states can contribute to an abnormal composite effort cost related to movement in Parkinson’s disease. This can account for paradoxical observations such as the abnormally slow relaxation of isometric contractions or difficulties in halting a movement in Parkinson’s disease, both of which increase movement energy expenditure. A sound understanding of the abnormal behavioural computations mediating motor impairment in Parkinson’s disease will be vital for linking them to their underlying neural dynamics in distributed brain networks and for grounding future experimental studies in well-defined behavioural frameworks.
Keywords: vigour, optimal motor control, utility theory, dopamine
Introduction
Parkinson’s disease is a common neurodegenerative disorder defined clinically by the hallmark presence of bradykinesia, i.e. slowness of movements, combined with tremor, rigidity or both1,2. The clinical expression of symptoms in individual patients with Parkinson’s disease is very heterogeneous comprising gait problems, autonomic dysfunction, depression, apathy, anxiety and more. Neurobiologically, several neural systems are affected, most prominently midbrain dopaminergic neurons of the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc)3. While there is no curative treatment, several clinically effective symptomatic treatments exist including drugs restoring the neurotransmitter dopamine, electrical stimulation of the basal ganglia termed deep brain stimulation (DBS) and non-pharmacological treatments such as physiotherapy. There has been significant progress in elucidating molecular, genetic and neurobiological changes in Parkinson’s disease 1. However, in order to link the findings from increasingly sophisticated experimental techniques on one hand and patients’ clinical impairment on the other, there is a need to better understand the behavioural deficits of patients with Parkinson’s disease 4. In particular, we need to grasp the behavioural computations underlying any motor impairment that we wish to improve. In this review, we will summarize observations of the hallmark symptom of Parkinson’s disease, bradykinesia, and consider these observations in a behavioural framework borrowing concepts from optimal control and utility theory5–7. In particular, we will discuss how optimal decisions and movements may be based on computations of reward and effort and how this might go awry in Parkinson’s disease.
Abnormal movement control in Parkinson’s disease
There is a vast literature studying movements in Parkinson’s disease patients. As can be expected from the clinical diagnosis based on the presence of bradykinesia (see box 1 for a glossary), the most consistent finding is that movements are performed abnormally slowly2. However, this does not entail that patients fail to express any modulation of movement velocities. Healthy people scale movement velocities by the amplitude of the movement so that e.g. reaching to a target with a distance of 30 cm has a higher peak velocity than a 20 cm reach. Parkinson’s disease patients also scale movement velocities based on amplitudes and are capable of reaching to high distance targets with velocities that are comparable to that of healthy subjects reaching to lower distance targets. In other words, under certain constraints patients are able to reach relatively normal movement velocities. However, they usually express an abnormally flat velocity-to-amplitude slope so that they on average use velocities that are too low for a given amplitude (figure 1A). This impairment is not limited to a certain type of movement but has been demonstrated for a variety of different movements8–14. Electrophysiologically, it has been related to an abnormal activation of the agonist muscle in single-joint movements10,11,15–18 and impaired coordination of muscle activation in more complex, multi-joint movements19,20. During isometric contractions the analogous phenomenon can be observed, namely an abnormally slow change of force (yank) for a given peak force level21–25. Interestingly, differences in the amplitude (as opposed to the velocity) of ballistic movement or in the peak force (as opposed to the yank) of isometric contractions between Parkinson’s disease patients and healthy people are less pronounced and less consistently reported unless patients are severely affected26–29. Thus, many patients seem able to reach relatively normal amplitudes and force levels, but they only reach this state after a pathologically prolonged movement duration. This impairment becomes progressively worse in more severe disease stages (figure 1B) and can partly be ameliorated by therapies such as intake of dopaminergic medication (figure 1C) or DBS (figure 1D).
Box 1. Glossary.
Bradykinesia
The direct translation of bradykinesia is slow movement. Strictly, the related terms akinesia and hypokinesia reflect, respectively, fewer or no movements and movements of reduced amplitude, but the terms are often used interchangeably115. In the clinical assessment bradykinesia scores also incorporate movement interruptions, hesitations and amplitude decrement116. Here, we use bradykinesia in its general sense as slowness of movement.
Dynamical systems theory
A mathematical description of complex dynamical systems. For example, the behaviour of a pendulum can be described by a two-dimensional dynamical system with the state variables position and velocity. This approach can be extended to high-dimensional data and has been successfully applied for analysing simultaneous recordings of large populations of neurons during movement111.
Effort
The subjective work that must be exerted in order to achieve a goal. The overall effort assigned to an action is presumably not given by a single quantity, but consist of different costs, such as muscular energy, demand for resources and accuracy requirements42–44,46.
Reward
The term reward has often been used in a rather general and vague way in Neuroscience comprising reinforcer, appetite or pleasure117. In models of basal ganglia function reward is mostly used in terms of computations of incentivizing values e.g. of nutrient or money, that are closely related to the concept of utility (see below) and often contrasted to ‘motor’ functions solely concerned with movement. This can be problematic because, apart from general issues with putative value computations in the brain118, the contrast between reward and action is somewhat artificial in that computations of reward are only helpful if they inform our behaviour, i.e. our actions119. Here, we use the term reward for (explicit) incentivizing values, which in the reviewed Parkinson’s disease studies corresponds to money or collected points.
Utility
A representation of the usefulness or desirability of an object, which amongst others depends on its value, probability of gain and the state of the actor120,121. In optimal foraging theory utility is equal to the capture rate which is given by the obtained energy subtracted by the spent energy and divided by time37. The actor can then decide between exploring and exploiting a current patch by comparing the local (current) capture rate with the global capture rate. In general, utility theory ranks different objects based on an individual’s choices.
Vigour
In everyday speech vigour is often used to described bodily or mental force, or the intensity of action. In Neuroscience the term has been used variably including the force, speed, amplitude and frequency of actions. The definition that we here use for movement execution is the velocity for a given movement extent (because velocity scales with movement extent such as amplitude, see figure 2A), which at its core optimizes the time that we need to reach the goal of a movement. This is central to optimizing the utility and effort cost in value-based decision-making and motor control theory5–7 and crucial for allowing skillful movement30. Vigour can also be defined in the context of decision-making as the ‘willingness’ to work harder or longer (e.g. should I collect a reward which requires a lot of effort, or choose the alternative which is less rewarding but easier to collect), or the propensity to work at all (as compared to resting), see figure 2 B&C.
Figure 1. Abnormal vigour in Parkinson’s disease.
A. Parkinson’s disease patients use abnormally low peak velocities for a given movement extent compared to healthy people. Based on male participants from Pfann et al.11 studying ballistic flexion movements over the elbow joint B. The more severe the disease, as indexed by bradykinesia scores, the lower the velocity that patients on average use for a given extent. Based on the median values from Warabi et al.12 studying flexion and extension movements of the wrist C. Dopaminergic medication increases the yank that patients exert for a given peak force. Based on Le Bouc et al.22 studying a manual gripping task D. Likewise DBS can increase the velocity for a given movement extent. Based on Baraduc et al.8 studying ballistic extension movements over the elbow joint. DBS, deep brain stimulation; Deg, degrees; dF, change in force; HC, healthy control; %MVC, percentage of maximum voluntary contraction; PD, Parkinson’s disease; s, seconds.
Together, this indicates that bradykinesia does not strictly reflect an inability to perform movements with a certain velocity, but that the velocity assigned to a given movement extent (e.g. the amplitude of a reaching movement) is reduced. This has been described as an impaired movement vigour10,30–33.
What is vigour?
The term vigour is often used somewhat vaguely and there is considerable heterogeneity in the way it is defined. The definition that most closely relates to the deficit observed in Parkinson’s disease is the velocity of a movement for a given movement extent34, see figure 2A. This definition is close to our intuitive understanding describing how a movement is expressed, determining the time it takes to reach the goal of an action (box 1). Vigour has also been defined as the propensity to exert effort when deciding what to choose or whether to engage in a task at all (figure 2B & C) putting modulation of vigour primarily in the context of decision-making rather than motor control. Even though decision-making and motor control are mainly studied in separate fields they share many commonalities including the computations underlying vigour modulations5,7,35,36.
Figure 2. Vigour.
A. Vigour can be represented by the movement velocity as a function of distance (rather than velocity per se, since velocity scales with distance). The higher a velocity for a given distance (reflected by the steepness of the slope) the higher the vigour. B. Another definition of vigour is the propensity to expend effort. This is typically measured by testing whether people prefer options which are effortful (represented by the height of the tree trunk) but lead to higher rewards (4 cherries) or options which are less effortful, but also lead to lower rewards (2 cherries). C. This can also be assessed by testing whether people want to engage in a task (sometimes tested by the frequency of reward collection, e.g. how often would they climb a tree to pick cherries) or rather rest (here indicated by a deck chair). D. When optimizing utility, increasing the reward can alter the optimal movement vigour. The maximum of the utility as a function of movement time is shifted to the left for high vs. low rewards, indicating that the movement speed that optimizes utility depends on the reward that can be acquired. E. The same can be observed when altering the effort costs of the movement. The equations and parameters for the plots in D&E are based on Shadmehr and Ahmed5 and correspond to caloric expenditure during a 50 m walk. F. Fast movements are related to lower accuracy. This relationship can be modulated through increases in control to reduce noise and thus improve accuracy 47. Throughout the figure the level of vigour is indicated by the opaqueness of the green colour.
An important modulator of vigour is the subjective value or utility that can be obtained when pursuing goals (box 1). The utility of an option does not only affect whether it is chosen but also how it is retrieved. Shadmehr, Ahmed and colleagues have developed a normative framework for vigour modulations during value-based decisions5–7. Like optimal foraging theory, this framework postulates that animals make decisions by computing a utility function that depends on the reward that can be obtained subtracted by the effort that has to be spent and divided by the total time it takes to acquire and harvest the reward. When considering caloric intake, the utility reflects the energy obtained (acquired minus spent) per unit time and is thus equivalent to the capture rate37. However, the term reward here refers to any incentivizing value, such as food, water or money (see also box 1). Ahmed and Shadmehr extended this framework by postulating that decision-making is not solely concerned with computations of subjective values for different rewards but also how these should be acquired. Since the movement vigour will both affect the effort that has to be spent and the time it takes to gather and harvest the reward, it directly feeds into the computation of utility. In particular, slow movements can be ‘costly’ despite being closer to the optimum regarding energy consumption, since reward acquisition is delayed (reward is discounted in time) and the increased time to gather the reward constitutes an opportunity cost (no other rewards can be obtained). This model predicts that increasing the utility e.g. by increasing the reward value (figure 2D) or reducing the effort cost (figure 2E), should result in increased movement vigour, which has been demonstrated in several studies38–41.
Effort costs are not only related to movement energy expenditure as described above, but also neural costs and the requirement of resources for a specific movement42–44. Furthermore, what can also be considered a cost of fast movements is the deterioration of movement accuracy, which is termed speed-accuracy trade-off45. This is because faster movements require a larger motor command, which increases motor noise46, and accuracy can only be preserved at the cost of increased control in order to reduce this noise47,48, see figure 2F.
In summary, modulations of vigour can be conceptualized as a process optimizing which options to choose and the time we should take to reach a certain goal by considering the reward that can be acquired given the associated costs such as missed opportunities and effort costs. Could abnormal computations of rewards or costs underlie bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease?
Effects of reward and effort on vigour in Parkinson’s disease
Several studies have assessed how Parkinson’s disease patients choose among options with varying levels of monetary reward and effort costs. Le Heron and colleagues49 tested Parkinson’s disease patients with and without apathy, ON and OFF dopaminergic medication and a healthy control group performing a reward-effort trade-off task. Participants could accept or reject offers where different levels of isometric grip-force had to be produced in order to obtain different amounts of monetary reward. While apathy mainly reduced acceptance rates for low-reward offers, dopaminergic medication had a different effect, increasing the acceptance for high reward, high effort options. In a related study Le Bouc et al.22 investigated the reward-effort trade-off in Parkinson’s disease patients ON and OFF dopaminergic medication and a healthy control group using two tasks. First, in an incentive force task, patients produced grip forces, which were multiplied by varying levels of monetary reward shown on the screen. Second, in a binary choice task, participants chose between a low reward - low effort option and a variable high reward - high effort option. Effort was reflected by the required peak grip force. The high reward - high effort option was adjusted in a staircase procedure resulting in equivalence levels between effort and reward. As expected, irrespective of monetary incentives patients OFF medication used a significantly lower yank for a given force level compared to patients ON medication and healthy people (shown in figure 1C). Furthermore, the effort (in this study defined as peak force) participants were willing to exert for higher rewards was significantly lower in patients OFF medication compared to ON medication and healthy people in both tasks, see figure 3A. Thus, patients were effectively less willing to produce higher forces for higher rewards. To further disentangle these effects, Le Bouc et al. used a computational model that incorporated several free parameters including reward sensitivity and motor activation rate (the latter parameter reflected how neural drive results in muscle activation). Using model comparison, they found that in the model that best explained the data, dopamine modulated both the motor activation rate and reward sensitivity. However, these parameters had distinct effects on the observed behaviour. A reduction in motor activation rate in patients OFF medication reflected the observed flattened yank-force slope (corresponding to our definition of movement vigour, see figure 1C & 2A) irrespective of reward and correlated with patients’ bradykinesia scores. In contrast, reductions in reward sensitivity reflected the slope between reward and effort (figure 3A) and correlated with patients’ apathy scores rather than bradykinesia severity. Thus, while reduced reward sensitivity has repeatedly been reported in Parkinson’s disease patients 22,49–55 it appears to be more closely related to impaired motivation (apathy)56,57, arguing against a central role of abnormal reward sensitivity underlying bradykinesia. Further evidence for distinct mechanisms underlying motivational and movement deficits comes from the clinical observation that Parkinson’s disease patients can loose their motivation e.g. to pursue their hobbies and engage in activities despite significant improvement in bradykinesia after DBS surgery58,59.
Figure 3. Effects of reward and effort on movement in Parkinson’s disease.
A. Parkinson’s disease patients express an abnormally flat slope between exerted force and reward resulting in lower force levels when rewards are high. This reduced ‘reward-sensitivity’ is improved by dopamine and mainly observed in patients with apathy. Based on Le Bouc et al.22 studying a manual gripping task during varying levels of reward. B. Parkinson’s disease patients are able to reach targets with a certain required velocity (S, slow; M, medium; F, fast; VF, very fast) like healthy people, but need a larger number of trials. This is particularly pronounced when the movement demands acceleration and deceleration in close succession (compare e.g. reaching with fast (F) velocities to a 12 cm vs. 16 cm distant target). Based on Mazzoni et al.31 studying reaching movements. Copyright 2007 Society for Neuroscience. C. Using a computational model Baraduc and colleagues showed that Parkinson’s disease patients show an abnormally narrow motor range resulting in long movement duration despite otherwise normal movement parameters. Based on Baraduc et al.8 studying ballistic extension movements over the elbow joint. ag, agonist; ant., antagonist; DBS, deep brain stimulation. HC, healthy control; PD, Parkinson’s disease.
Could increased sensitivity to effort costs underlie bradykinesia?
Mazzoni and colleagues studied seven patients with Parkinson’s disease ON medication31. Patients had to perform reaching movements to targets with varying distances. While feedback regarding their endpoint-accuracy was given, the critical determinant of trial validity depended on whether the velocity fell within a specified range. Patients had to complete twenty trials of each trial type (different combinations of distances and velocities) in order to successfully finish the experiment. The main result from the study was that patients were able to successfully complete the task but required a larger number of trials (termed ‘trials to criterion’) compared to healthy participants, see figure 3B. In other words, the range of possible movement velocities was similar to that of healthy controls, but patients were more likely to use lower velocities. Parkinson’s disease patients needed particularly many trials when these required a high amount of velocity changes for a given movement duration (e.g., fast movements to close targets require large acceleration and deceleration in close succession), which was measured as the average absolute acceleration. This measure correlated with the energetic cost of the movement (power expenditure of the arm) and with the patients’ clinical motor impairment. Since in this study patients were able to expend the required energetic movement cost, but were less likely to do so (i.e. need a higher number of trials), it was concluded that slow movements in Parkinson’s disease were due to an increased sensitivity to energetic movement costs rather than an inability to exert this effort. Another important finding was that patients and healthy controls did not differ regarding their movement accuracy or other kinematic parameters arguing against a speed-accuracy trade-off underlying bradykinesia. This finding is in line with several other studies reporting similar kinematic measures such as movement trajectories, variability and end-point accuracy in Parkinson’s disease patients and healthy people16,24,29,60,61. For example, Baraduc et al.8 tested Parkinson’s disease patients performing reaching movements to varying distances without visual feedback. As illustrated in figure 1D, patients showed abnormally low vigour, i.e. low velocities for a given distance, which could be ameliorated through therapeutic DBS. The authors then analysed the observed data using a model of optimal motor control in which motor commands are transformed to kinematics using a set of differential equations (see Baraduc et al.8 for more details). The optimal motor command yielded the movement trajectory that minimized the neuromuscular cost of the movement given movement amplitude and duration. This analysis showed that the same model was able to account for the movements of patients and healthy people, but that the groups differed regarding their range of motor commands (which in this study corresponded to the range from baseline to maximal population motoneuron activity, similar to the motor activation rate during isometric contractions in Le Bouc et al.22). This range was abnormally narrow in Parkinson’s disease patients leading to slower movement velocities despite otherwise normal model parameters, see figure 3C. In other words, given their abnormal motor command range, patients performed optimal (i.e. cost minimizing) movement trajectories suggesting that except from abnormally low vigour, patients are able to execute normal movements. Thus, the results of these studies argue against increased control costs to account for impaired movement accuracy underlying bradykinesia and are in line with increased sensitivity to movement energetic costs8,31,62. Another way to put this is that Parkinson’s disease patients might have reduced implicit motivation 31,63 (since it is controlled outside of awareness, in contrast to explicit motivation) to expend energy for selected movements. This would result in abnormally slow movements unless patients are explicitly motivated by extrinsic factors such as task instructions31 or urgency64. For example, a patient who is immobilized by their disease might suddenly be able to move rapidly when faced with immediate danger, e.g. running out of a burning house, a phenomenon termed paradoxical kinesia 64,65. Thus, the lack of implicit motivation causing bradykinesia due to reduced energy invested in movement may be overcome by significant extrinsic motivation. Interestingly, this energy cost account of bradykinesia resembles a hypothesis that has been put forward over 40 years ago, ascribing bradykinesia to an impaired ‘energization’ of movement10. Yet, there are observations that argue against bradykinesia being related to energy preservation.
Is bradykinesia caused by reduced willingness to energize movement?
People tend to move at velocities near the optimum regarding energy consumption5. Not only moving faster, but also moving slower can increase energy expenditure, since the metabolic cost increases linearly with movement duration5,66. The very slow movements observed in Parkinson’s disease will therefore not reduce, but increase the energetic cost of the movement. In line with this, Parkinson’s disease patients spend more energy when walking compared to healthy people67. Furthermore, rigidity, an increased resistance to passive movement perceived as muscle stiffness and another hallmark motor symptom of Parkinson’s disease 1,68,69, comes at the cost of increased energy expenditure. Finally, during isometric contractions Parkinson’s disease patients do not only show a reduced activation rate (positive yank) but also strongly reduced relaxation rate (negative yank), which has been demonstrated in a multitude of studies27–29,70–73. For example, in the study by Le Bouc et al.22 discussed above both activation and relaxation rate in Parkinson’s disease patients were abnormally low particularly OFF dopaminergic medication (figure 4A), increasing the energetic cost of the movement (figure 4B) even if the exerted peak force is slightly lower. An increase in the absolute exerted force has also been observed in Parkinson’s disease patients performing externally-paced isometric contractions74 and precision grips75,76. Together, these observations indicate that bradykinesia does not necessarily reduce the energy invested in movements.
Figure 4. Effort costs of movement.
A. Parkinson’s disease patients do not only show an abnormally slow increase in force (positive yank, left column), but also an abnormally slow decrease in force (negative yank, right column) leading to larger absolute force that is exerted (area under the curve) even if the peak force is lower. Based on Le Bouc et al.22 studying a manual gripping task. B. Since people usually use movement durations near the optimum regarding its energetic cost, slowing down movements results in increased costs. The cost here reflects the instantaneous movement cost (neural drive leading to muscle activation) integrated over the movement duration. Based on Le Bouc et al.22 C. Movements are presumably related to a composite cost consisting of different effort costs, here shown for three dimensions. For example, movement 1 (black) has higher costs regarding effort 2 and 3, but lower costs regarding effort 1 compared to movement 2 (green) and Parkinson’s disease patients might be impaired in exerting a particular effort cost altering the composite cost compared to healthy people. HC, healthy control; PD, Parkinson’s disease.
Another challenge of the energy-preservation account of bradykinesia is the assumption that patients could use higher velocities if they were ‘willing’ to do so. For example, Mazzoni et al.31, observed that Parkinson’s disease patients were able to reach targets with the required velocity but needed a larger number of trials. An explanation for this could be, that the chosen vigour (velocity for a given movement extent) assigned to a movement is probabilistic and that the probability distribution is shifted to lower velocities in Parkinson’s disease. Irrespective of the exact mechanisms underlying this shift, drawing more samples from this distribution will lead to sufficient trials with the required velocity at some point. While the authors addressed this to some extent by computing (non-normalized) probability distributions, and the seven included patients all were able to reach the required velocities it is questionable whether a patient with severe bradykinesia OFF medication would be able to do so. Finally, it is important to note that, while motor function in Parkinson’s disease can fluctuate strongly, the occurrence of paradoxical kinesia is rare and most patients will not have experienced this phenomenon even when faced with immediate danger e.g. in war times77. The more common mild urgency-related improvement in bradykinesia might be more closely related to general arousal78 and, similar to the well-known beneficial effects of external cueing64,79–81, might be less dependent on SNc innervation of the basal ganglia78.
So why are Parkinson’s disease patients slow if not due to reduced willingness to expend energy? Bradykinesia could still be in line with an inability to exert effort (or increased effort sensitivity), but this effort might not be simply reflected by a (directly measurable or computable) change in movement energy. The observation that patients are also impaired in relaxing a contraction27–29,70–73, halting, correcting, or decelerating a movement71,82,83 suggests that motor impairment in Parkinson’s disease includes a general impairment in transitioning between stable and dynamic movement states. While stabilizing the current body position can be useful, e.g., when stabilizing body posture against external perturbations to prevent falls, it can become detrimental when it needs to be changed during or in anticipation of voluntary movement. Be it a crouching tiger attacking its prey, a sprinter commencing from the starting block or simply a person releasing a firm handshake this transition is central to physiological motor control. A bias towards the stable state is also consistent with the observed rigidity in Parkinson’s disease, i.e. muscle co-contractions that reinforce a postural state69. The processes underlying the shift between stable and dynamic motor states could be viewed as another effort cost to be evaluated when determining the vigour of an intended action; in addition to efforts reflecting specific properties of a movement (such as required movement energy or end-point accuracy). It is currently not well-known how the brain computes effort costs based on e.g. neural energy demands and allocation of resources in contrast to more easily computable movement energy costs48,84. However, it seems likely that the brain computes a composite cost reflecting different efforts for decision-making and motor control42–44,46, illustrated in figure 4C. Parkinson’s disease patients might be particularly impaired in exerting an effort that does not directly reflect movement energy altering the composite effort related to movement compared to healthy people.
It seems difficult to disentangle whether Parkinson’s disease patients have increased sensitivity to computed effort or are impaired in exerting it, because their effects on behaviour would be indistinguishable in most cases. However, there are clinical observations arguing for the latter. Parkinson’s disease patients do not only move more slowly compared to healthy people, but this deteriorates over time during repetitions (e.g. finger tapping) so that movements become slower and smaller (termed decrement or sequence effect2). This characteristic phenomenon of Parkinson’s disease85 is unlikely to be related to abnormal effort computations, since this would require the computation of separate (increasing) effort costs for each iteration of repetitive movements. Ultimately, both increased effort sensitivity and difficulties in its implementation might contribute to bradykinesia. Elucidating which exact efforts are particularly costly or difficult to exert for Parkinson’s disease patients will be a crucial step in better understanding motor impairment in Parkinson’s disease.
Outlook
In this paper, we discussed how bradykinesia could result from abnormal utility computations based on the rewards and efforts associated with an action (summarized in figure 5). Behavioural observations mainly suggest that Parkinson’s disease patients move slowly because of an abnormal computation of, or an inability to exert, a composite effort reflecting the disparate costs incurred by the transitioning to and from a dynamic movement state and by the execution of specific movements themselves. The motivation for this review was to discuss behavioural accounts of bradykinesia in terms of the putative computations underlying the observed movement deficits borrowing concepts from optimal control and utility theory. It should be noted that Parkinson’s disease patients suffer from a variety of other debilitating symptoms including anxiety, depression, cognitive dysfunction and more, which are beyond the scope of the current review.
Figure 5. Summary.
To optimize movement vigour an agent has to take into account the reward that can be obtained and the different effort costs (such as muscular energy expenditure, motor state transitions, resource allocation, accuracy requirements, etc.) that need to be expended to reach the goal. An increased composite effort cost in Parkinson’s disease can lead to abnormally slow movements despite otherwise normal (i.e., utility optimizing) motor control mechanisms.
A sound understanding of the behavioural impairment in Parkinson’s disease is vital for therapeutic advances. While the effect of dopamine on reward computations has been studied quite extensively, we hope that this review sparks interest in further delineating how alterations in distinct effort costs might affect motor impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Beyond the general implications for improving our understanding of patients’ symptoms, this might also contribute to improving therapy. In particular novel therapeutic techniques, such as adaptive DBS, which adapts stimulation to changes in patients’ clinical states 86–89, offer the opportunity to directly target specific neural signals. To this end it will be vital to link the behavioural impairment to changes at the neural implementation level. There has been significant progress in our understanding of the (physiological and pathological) implementation of reward- and effort-based movement control in cortical and subcortical networks of the brain5,90. For example, there is a wealth of studies linking midbrain dopaminergic innervation of cortico – basal ganglia networks (in particular ventral tegmental area and ventral striatum) to reward prediction errors, action outcomes and value91–94. Even though several studies have demonstrated a gradient from ‘reward’-related computations in more ventral loops to movement-related activity in more dorsal (SNc and dorsal striatum) loops95–97 kinematic correlates have also been recorded in ventral areas98–100 and it remains to be elucidated how exactly this is altered in Parkinson’s disease101,102. Given the functional architecture of the basal ganglia and the effects of dopamine release on neural excitability and plasticity30,103–105, cortico – basal ganglia networks would be well-suited to mediate vigour modulations30,33,35,106,107. This might both be possible through gain modulation of downstream pre-motor areas30,106 or feedback connections to cortical areas thought to be involved in effort cost computations and evaluations57. Furthermore, abnormal modulation of certain oscillatory frequency bands (in particular the 13-30 Hz beta band in the subthalamic nucleus) has been demonstrated in Parkinson’s disease. Since this is strongly modulated prior to and during movement, related to patients’ motor impairment and reduced by therapeutic dopaminergic medication and DBS, it is a strong candidate neural feedback marker for adaptive DBS. There is evidence that beta activity more closely reflects the transitioning between stable and dynamic movement states than movement energetic costs108–110. Thus, restoring physiological beta activity modulation in Parkinson’s disease might improve patients’ ability to flexibly adapt their behaviour. Finally, dynamical systems theory has made significant contributions to linking multidimensional cortical population dynamics to movement control over the last years 111. This framework might be particularly suited for studying motor impairment in Parkinson’s disease, since it is concerned with the relationship between neural state transitions and behaviour 111,112. Basal ganglia architecture resembles a recurrent negative feedback system106. Such systems are particularly suited for stabilizing (cortical) attractor states and allowing state changes when necessary, e.g. during purposeful movement48,113. Recently cortical neural ‘null’ spaces have been proposed to allow movement preparation without overt movement114. In order to allow muscle activation during movement execution the neural trajectories transition to ‘output-potent’ spaces114. Thus, these neural state transitions might resemble the stable vs. dynamic movement states discussed above. It remains to be shown whether dynamical systems theory could be a helpful framework for improving our understanding of the neural basis of motor impairment in Parkinson’s disease.
There is an ever-increasing wealth of studies elucidating neural activity and network patterns in cortico-basal ganglia networks and their modulation by midbrain neurotransmitter systems. To avoid widening the gap between our understanding of neural dynamics and their effects on behaviour, careful examinations of behaviour and the underlying computations will be vital for advancing Parkinson’s disease research. Grounding experimental studies in well-defined behavioural frameworks will allow us to gain important insights into physiological control of movement and how this might go awry in patients with Parkinson’s disease and other brain disorders.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank David Meder for helpful discussions and comments on previous versions of the manuscript.
Funding information
Medical Research Council (MC_UU_00003/2).
Abbreviations
- DBS
deep brain stimulation
- SNc
substantia nigra pars compacta
Footnotes
Competing interest statement:
The authors report no competing interests.
Data availability
This review article does not include any newly acquired data.
References
- 1.Bloem BR, Okun MS, Klein C. Parkinson’s disease. Lancet. 2021 Jun 12;397(10291):2284–2303. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)00218-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bologna M, Paparella G, Fasano A, Hallett M, Berardelli A. Evolving concepts on bradykinesia. Brain. 2020 Mar 1;143(3):727–750. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fearnley JM, Lees AJ. Ageing and Parkinson’s disease: substantia nigra regional selectivity. Brain. 1991 Oct;114(Pt 5):2283–301. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.5.2283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Krakauer JW, Ghazanfar AA, Gomez-Marin A, MacIver MA, Poeppel D. Neuroscience Needs Behavior: Correcting a Reductionist Bias. Neuron. 2017 Feb 8;93(3):480–490. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Shadmehr R, Ahmed AA. Vigor: Neuroeconomics of Movement Control. MIT Press; 2020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Shadmehr R, Huang HJ, Ahmed AA. A Representation of Effort in Decision-Making and Motor Control. Curr Biol. 2016 Jul 25;26(14):1929–34. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Shadmehr R, Reppert TR, Summerside EM, Yoon T, Ahmed AA. Movement Vigor as a Reflection of Subjective Economic Utility. Trends Neurosci. 2019 May;42(5):323–336. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Baraduc P, Thobois S, Gan J, Broussolle E, Desmurget M. A common optimization principle for motor execution in healthy subjects and parkinsonian patients. J Neurosci. 2013 Jan 9;33(2):665–77. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1482-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Berardelli A, Sabra AF, Hallett M. Physiological mechanisms of rigidity in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1983 Jan;46(1):45–53. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.46.1.45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hallett M, Khoshbin S. A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. Brain. 1980 Jun;103(2):301–14. doi: 10.1093/brain/103.2.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pfann KD, Buchman AS, Comella CL, Corcos DM. Control of movement distance in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2001 Nov;16(6):1048–65. doi: 10.1002/mds.1220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Warabi T, Noda H, Yanagisawa N, Tashiro K, Shindo R. Changes in sensorimotor function associated with the degree of bradykinesia of Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1986 Dec;109(Pt 6):1209–24. doi: 10.1093/brain/109.6.1209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Draper IT, Johns RJ. The Disordered Movement in Parkinsonism and the Effect of Drug Treatment. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp. 1964 Dec;115:465–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.KA Flowers. Visual "closed-loop" and "open-loop" characteristics of voluntary movement in patients with Parkinsonism and intention tremor. Brain. 1976 Jun;99(2):269–310. doi: 10.1093/brain/99.2.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Evarts EV, Teravainen H, Calne DB. Reaction time in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1981 Mar;104(Pt 1):167–86. doi: 10.1093/brain/104.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Godaux E, Koulischer D, Jacquy J. Parkinsonian bradykinesia is due to depression in the rate of rise of muscle activity. Ann Neurol. 1992 Jan;31(1):93–100. doi: 10.1002/ana.410310117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hallett M, Shahani BT, Young RR. Analysis of stereotyped voluntary movements at the elbow in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1977 Dec;40(12):1129–35. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.40.12.1129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Teasdale N, Phillips J, Stelmach GE. Temporal movement control in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1990 Oct;53(10):862–8. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.53.10.862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rand MK, Van Gemmert AW, Hossain AB, Stelmach GE. Coordination deficits during trunk-assisted reach-to-grasp movements in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2014 Jan;232(1):61–74. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3720-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Seidler RD, Alberts JL, Stelmach GE. Multijoint movement control in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2001 Oct;140(3):335–44. doi: 10.1007/s002210100829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hammond KG, Pfeiffer RF, LeDoux MS, Schilling BK. Neuromuscular rate of force development deficit in Parkinson disease. Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon) 2017 Jun;45:14–18. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2017.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Le Bouc R, Rigoux L, Schmidt L, et al. Computational Dissection of Dopamine Motor and Motivational Functions in Humans. J Neurosci. 2016 Jun 22;36(25):6623–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3078-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Park JH, Stelmach GE. Force development during target-directed isometric force production in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci Lett. 2007 Jan 29;412(2):173–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Stelmach GE, Teasdale N, Phillips J, Worringham CJ. Force production characteristics in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 1989;76(1):165–72. doi: 10.1007/BF00253633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wierzbicka MM, Wiegner AW, Logigian EL, Young RR. Abnormal most-rapid isometric contractions in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1991 Mar;54(3):210–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.54.3.210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Corcos DM, Chen CM, Quinn NP, McAuley J, Rothwell JC. Strength in Parkinson’s disease: relationship to rate of force generation and clinical status. Ann Neurol. 1996 Jan;39(1):79–88. doi: 10.1002/ana.410390112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Jordan N, Sagar HJ, Cooper JA. A component analysis of the generation and release of isometric force in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992 Jul;55(7):572–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.7.572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kunesch E, Schnitzler A, Tyercha C, Knecht S, Stelmach G. Altered force release control in Parkinson’s disease. Behav Brain Res. 1995 Feb;67(1):43–9. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)00111-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Stelmach GE, Worringham CJ. The preparation and production of isometric force in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 1988;26(1):93–103. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(88)90033-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Dudman JT, Krakauer JW. The basal ganglia: from motor commands to the control of vigor. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2016 Apr;37:158–166. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mazzoni P, Hristova A, Krakauer JW. Why don’t we move faster? Parkinson’s disease, movement vigor, and implicit motivation. J Neurosci. 2007 Jul 4;27(27):7105–16. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0264-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Meder D, Herz DM, Rowe JB, Lehericy S, Siebner HR. The role of dopamine in the brain - lessons learned from Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage. 2019 Apr 15;190:79–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Turner RS, Desmurget M. Basal ganglia contributions to motor control: a vigorous tutor. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010 Dec;20(6):704–16. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.08.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Choi JE, Vaswani PA, Shadmehr R. Vigor of movements and the cost of time in decision making. J Neurosci. 2014 Jan 22;34(4):1212–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2798-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Carland MA, Thura D, Cisek P. The Urge to Decide and Act: Implications for Brain Function and Dysfunction. Neuroscientist. 2019 Oct;25(5):491–511. doi: 10.1177/1073858419841553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Cisek P, Kalaska JF. Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2010;33:269–98. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Charnov EL. Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theor Popul Biol. 1976 Apr;9(2):129–36. doi: 10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kawagoe R, Takikawa Y, Hikosaka O. Expectation of reward modulates cognitive signals in the basal ganglia. Nat Neurosci. 1998 Sep;1(5):411–6. doi: 10.1038/1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Reppert TR, Lempert KM, Glimcher PW, Shadmehr R. Modulation of Saccade Vigor during Value-Based Decision Making. J Neurosci. 2015 Nov 18;35(46):15369–78. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2621-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Summerside EM, Shadmehr R, Ahmed AA. Vigor of reaching movements: reward discounts the cost of effort. J Neurophysiol. 2018 Jun 1;119(6):2347–2357. doi: 10.1152/jn.00872.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Yoon T, Geary RB, Ahmed AA, Shadmehr R. Control of movement vigor and decision making during foraging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018 Oct 30;115(44):E10476–E10485. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1812979115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Morel P, Ulbrich P, Gail A. What makes a reach movement effortful? Physical effort discounting supports common minimization principles in decision making and motor control. PLoS Biol. 2017 Jun;15(6):e2001323. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2001323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Rigoux L, Guigon E. A model of reward- and effort-based optimal decision making and motor control. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012;8(10):e1002716. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Todorov E, Jordan MI. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat Neurosci. 2002 Nov;5(11):1226–35. doi: 10.1038/nn963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Fitts PM. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol. 1954 Jun;47(6):381–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Harris CM, Wolpert DM. Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature. 1998 Aug 20;394(6695):780–4. doi: 10.1038/29528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Manohar SG, Chong TT, Apps MA, et al. Reward Pays the Cost of Noise Reduction in Motor and Cognitive Control. Curr Biol. 2015 Jun 29;25(13):1707–16. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Manohar SG, Muhammed K, Fallon SJ, Husain M. Motivation dynamically increases noise resistance by internal feedback during movement. Neuropsychologia. 2019 Feb 4;123:19–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Le Heron C, Plant O, Manohar S, et al. Distinct effects of apathy and dopamine on effort-based decision-making in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2018 May 1;141(5):1455–1469. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Chong TT, Bonnelle V, Manohar S, et al. Dopamine enhances willingness to exert effort for reward in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex. 2015 Aug;69:40–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Skvortsova V, Degos B, Welter ML, Vidailhet M, Pessiglione M. A Selective Role for Dopamine in Learning to Maximize Reward But Not to Minimize Effort: Evidence from Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. J Neurosci. 2017 Jun 21;37(25):6087–6097. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2081-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Drew DS, Muhammed K, Baig F, et al. Dopamine and reward hypersensitivity in Parkinson’s disease with impulse control disorder. Brain. 2020 Aug 1;143(8):2502–2518. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Muhammed K, Manohar S, Ben Yehuda M, et al. Reward sensitivity deficits modulated by dopamine are associated with apathy in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2016 Oct;139(Pt 10):2706–2721. doi: 10.1093/brain/aww188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Schmidt L, d'Arc BF, Lafargue G, et al. Disconnecting force from money: effects of basal ganglia damage on incentive motivation. Brain. 2008 May;131(Pt 5):1303–10. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Shiner T, Seymour B, Symmonds M, Dayan P, Bhatia KP, Dolan RJ. The effect of motivation on movement: a study of bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47138. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Le Heron C, Holroyd CB, Salamone J, Husain M. Brain mechanisms underlying apathy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2019 Mar;90(3):302–312. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2018-318265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Pessiglione M, Vinckier F, Bouret S, Daunizeau J, Le Bouc R. Why not try harder? Computational approach to motivation deficits in neuro-psychiatric diseases. Brain. 2018 Mar 1;141(3):629–650. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Zoon TJ, de Bie RM, Schuurman PR, van den Munckhof P, Denys D, Figee M. Resolution of apathy after dorsal instead of ventral subthalamic deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol. 2019 May;266(5):1267–1269. doi: 10.1007/s00415-019-09232-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Zoon TJC, van Rooijen G, Balm G, et al. Apathy Induced by Subthalamic Nucleus Deep Brain Stimulation in Parkinson’s Disease: A Meta-Analysis. Mov Disord. 2021 Feb;36(2):317–326. doi: 10.1002/mds.28390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Berardelli A, Hallett M, Rothwell JC, et al. Single-joint rapid arm movements in normal subjects and in patients with motor disorders. Brain. 1996 Apr;119(Pt 2):661–74. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Desmurget M, Grafton ST, Vindras P, Grea H, Turner RS. The basal ganglia network mediates the planning of movement amplitude. Eur J Neurosci. 2004 May;19(10):2871–80. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03395.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Gepshtein S, Li X, Snider J, Plank M, Lee D, Poizner H. Dopamine function and the efficiency of human movement. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014 Mar;26(3):645–57. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Niv Y, Rivlin-Etzion M. Parkinson’s disease: fighting the will? J Neurosci. 2007 Oct 31;27(44):11777–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4010-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ballanger B, Thobois S, Baraduc P, Turner RS, Broussolle E, Desmurget M. “Paradoxical kinesis” is not a hallmark of Parkinson’s disease but a general property of the motor system. Mov Disord. 2006 Sep;21(9):1490–5. doi: 10.1002/mds.20987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Glickstein M, Stein J. Paradoxical movement in Parkinson’s disease. Trends Neurosci. 1991 Nov;14(11):480–2. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90055-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Huang HJ, Kram R, Ahmed AA. Reduction of metabolic cost during motor learning of arm reaching dynamics. J Neurosci. 2012 Feb 8;32(6):2182–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4003-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Christiansen CL, Schenkman ML, McFann K, Wolfe P, Kohrt WM. Walking economy in people with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2009 Jul 30;24(10):1481–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.22621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Jankovic J. Parkinson’s disease: clinical features and diagnosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008 Apr;79(4):368–76. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.131045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Mazzoni P, Shabbott B, Cortes JC. Motor control abnormalities in Parkinson’s disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. 2012 Jun;2(6):a009282. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a009282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Neely KA, Planetta PJ, Prodoehl J, et al. Force control deficits in individuals with Parkinson’s disease, multiple systems atrophy, and progressive supranuclear palsy. PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e58403. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Poon C, Robichaud JA, Corcos DM, Goldman JG, Vaillancourt DE. Combined measures of movement and force variability distinguish Parkinson’s disease from essential tremor. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011 Nov;122(11):2268–75. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Robichaud JA, Pfann KD, Vaillancourt DE, Comella CL, Corcos DM. Force control and disease severity in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2005 Apr;20(4):441–450. doi: 10.1002/mds.20350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Wing AM. A comparison of the rate of pinch grip force increases and decreases in parkinsonian bradykinesia. Neuropsychologia. 1988;26(3):479–82. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(88)90100-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Tinaz S, Pillai AS, Hallett M. Sequence Effect in Parkinson’s Disease Is Related to Motor Energetic Cost. Front Neurol. 2016;7:83. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Fellows SJ, Noth J. Grip force abnormalities in de novo Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2004 May;19(5):560–5. doi: 10.1002/mds.10710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Fellows SJ, Noth J, Schwarz M. Precision grip and Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1998 Sep;121(Pt 9):1771–84. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.9.1771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Schlesinger I, Erikh I, Yarnitsky D. Paradoxical kinesia at war. Mov Disord. 2007 Dec;22(16):2394–7. doi: 10.1002/mds.21739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Anzak A, Tan H, Pogosyan A, et al. Subcortical evoked activity and motor enhancement in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Neurol. 2016 Mar;277:19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2015.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Georgiou N, Bradshaw JL, Iansek R, Phillips JG, Mattingley JB, Bradshaw JA. Reduction in external cues and movement sequencing in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994 Mar;57(3):368–70. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.3.368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Rocha PA, Porfirio GM, Ferraz HB, Trevisani VF. Effects of external cues on gait parameters of Parkinson’s disease patients: a systematic review. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2014 Sep;124:127–34. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2014.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Thobois S, Ballanger B, Baraduc P, et al. Functional anatomy of motor urgency. Neuroimage. 2007 Aug 1;37(1):243–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Angel RW, Alston W, Higgins JR. Control of movement in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 1970;93(1):1–14. doi: 10.1093/brain/93.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Manza P, Amandola M, Tatineni V, Li CR, Leung HC. Response inhibition in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis of dopaminergic medication and disease duration effects. NPJ Parkinsons Dis. 2017;3:23. doi: 10.1038/s41531-017-0024-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Westbrook A, Frank MJ, Cools R. A mosaic of cost-benefit control over cortico-striatal circuitry. Trends Cogn Sci. 2021 Aug;25(8):710–721. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Ling H, Massey LA, Lees AJ, Brown P, Day BL. Hypokinesia without decrement distinguishes progressive supranuclear palsy from Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2012 Apr;135(Pt 4):1141–53. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Guidetti M, Marceglia S, Loh A, et al. Clinical perspectives of adaptive deep brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2021 Sep-Oct;14(5):1238–1247. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2021.07.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Meidahl AC, Tinkhauser G, Herz DM, Cagnan H, Debarros J, Brown P. Adaptive Deep Brain Stimulation for Movement Disorders: The Long Road to Clinical Therapy. Mov Disord. 2017 Jun;32(6):810–819. doi: 10.1002/mds.27022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Tinkhauser G, Pogosyan A, Little S, et al. The modulatory effect of adaptive deep brain stimulation on beta bursts in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2017 Apr 1;140(4):1053–1067. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Merk T, Peterson V, Kohler R, Haufe S, Richardson RM, Neumann WJ. Machine learning based brain signal decoding for intelligent adaptive deep brain stimulation. Exp Neurol. 2022 May;351:113993. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2022.113993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Shadmehr R, Krakauer JW. A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp Brain Res. 2008 Mar;185(3):359–81. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1280-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Bayer HM, Glimcher PW. Midbrain dopamine neurons encode a quantitative reward prediction error signal. Neuron. 2005 Jul 7;47(1):129–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Gershman SJ, Uchida N. Believing in dopamine. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019 Nov;20(11):703–714. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0220-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Montague PR, Dayan P, Sejnowski TJ. A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. J Neurosci. 1996 Mar 1;16(5):1936–47. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-05-01936.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Berke JD. What does dopamine mean? Nat Neurosci. 2018 Jun;21(6):787–793. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0152-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Howe MW, Dombeck DA. Rapid signalling in distinct dopaminergic axons during locomotion and reward. Nature. 2016 Jul 28;535(7613):505–10. doi: 10.1038/nature18942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Menegas W, Babayan BM, Uchida N, Watabe-Uchida M. Opposite initialization to novel cues in dopamine signaling in ventral and posterior striatum in mice. Elife. 2017 Jan 5;6 doi: 10.7554/eLife.21886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Parker NF, Cameron CM, Taliaferro JP, et al. Reward and choice encoding in terminals of midbrain dopamine neurons depends on striatal target. Nat Neurosci. 2016 Jun;19(6):845–54. doi: 10.1038/nn.4287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Hughes RN, Bakhurin KI, Petter EA, et al. Ventral Tegmental Dopamine Neurons Control the Impulse Vector during Motivated Behavior. Curr Biol. 2020 Jul 20;30(14):2681–2694.:e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Lederman J, Lardeux S, Nicola SM. Vigor Encoding in the Ventral Pallidum. eNeuro. 2021 Jul-Aug;8(4) doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0064-21.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Saunders BT, Richard JM, Margolis EB, Janak PH. Dopamine neurons create Pavlovian conditioned stimuli with circuit-defined motivational properties. Nat Neurosci. 2018 Aug;21(8):1072–1083. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0191-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Dodson PD, Dreyer JK, Jennings KA, et al. Representation of spontaneous movement by dopaminergic neurons is cell-type selective and disrupted in parkinsonism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016 Apr 12;113(15):E2180-8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515941113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Panigrahi B, Martin KA, Li Y, et al. Dopamine Is Required for the Neural Representation and Control of Movement Vigor. Cell. 2015 Sep 10;162(6):1418–30. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Yagishita S, Hayashi-Takagi A, Ellis-Davies GC, Urakubo H, Ishii S, Kasai H. A critical time window for dopamine actions on the structural plasticity of dendritic spines. Science. 2014 Sep 26;345(6204):1616–20. doi: 10.1126/science.1255514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Zhai S, Shen W, Graves SM, Surmeier DJ. Dopaminergic modulation of striatal function and Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 2019 Apr;126(4):411–422. doi: 10.1007/s00702-019-01997-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Gerfen CR, Surmeier DJ. Modulation of striatal projection systems by dopamine. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:441–66. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Yin HH. The Basal Ganglia in Action. Neuroscientist. 2017 Jun;23(3):299–313. doi: 10.1177/1073858416654115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Klaus A, Alves da Silva J, Costa RM. What, If, and When to Move: Basal Ganglia Circuits and Self-Paced Action Initiation. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2019 Jul 8;42:459–483. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Fischer P, Pogosyan A, Green AL, et al. Beta synchrony in the cortico-basal ganglia network during regulation of force control on and off dopamine. Neurobiol Dis. 2019 Jul;127:253–263. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2019.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Herz DM, Bange M, Gonzalez-Escamilla G, et al. Neural underpinnings of action adaptation in the subthalamic nucleus. bioRxiv. 2022 doi: 10.1101/2022.06.28.497904. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Gilbertson T, Lalo E, Doyle L, Di Lazzaro V, Cioni B, Brown P. Existing motor state is favored at the expense of new movement during 13-35 Hz oscillatory synchrony in the human corticospinal system. J Neurosci. 2005 Aug 24;25(34):7771–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1762-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Vyas S, Golub MD, Sussillo D, Shenoy KV. Computation Through Neural Population Dynamics. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2020 Jul 8;43:249–275. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-092619-094115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Magnusson JL, Leventhal DK. Revisiting the “Paradox of Stereotaxic Surgery”: Insights Into Basal Ganglia-Thalamic Interactions. Front Syst Neurosci. 2021;15:725876. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2021.725876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Sauerbrei BA, Guo JZ, Cohen JD, et al. Cortical pattern generation during dexterous movement is input-driven. Nature. 2020 Jan;577(7790):386–391. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1869-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Kaufman MT, Churchland MM, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Cortical activity in the null space: permitting preparation without movement. Nat Neurosci. 2014 Mar;17(3):440–8. doi: 10.1038/nn.3643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Schilder JC, Overmars SS, Marinus J, van Hilten JJ, Koehler PJ. The terminology of akinesia, bradykinesia and hypokinesia: Past, present and future. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2017 Apr;37:27–35. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2017.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, et al. Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord. 2008 Nov 15;23(15):2129–70. doi: 10.1002/mds.22340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Salamone JD, Correa M. The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron. 2012 Nov 8;76(3):470–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Hayden BY, Niv Y. The case against economic values in the orbitofrontal cortex (or anywhere else in the brain) Behav Neurosci. 2021 Apr;135(2):192–201. doi: 10.1037/bne0000448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Fine JM, Hayden BY. The whole prefrontal cortex is premotor cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2022 Feb 14;377(1844):20200524. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Glimcher PW, Dorris MC, Bayer HM. Physiological utility theory and the neuroeconomics of choice. Games Econ Behav. 2005 Aug;52(2):213–256. doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Bernoulli D. Exposition of a new theory on the measurement of risk. Econometrica. 1954;22:23–36. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This review article does not include any newly acquired data.