Abstract
Background
ADHD and autism, defined as traits or disorders, commonly co-occur. Developmental trajectories of ADHD and autistic traits both show heterogeneity in onset and course, but little is known about how symptom trajectories co-develop into adulthood.
Methods
Using data from a population cohort, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, we examined correlations between ADHD and autistic traits across development, using the social communication disorders checklist and ADHD subscale of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. We modelled joint developmental trajectories of parent-reported ADHD and autistic traits between 4 and 25 years, then characterised trajectory classes based on sociodemographic, perinatal, psychopathology, cognition and social functioning variables, and tested for associations with neurodevelopmental/psychiatric polygenic scores (PGS).
Results
Three classes of trajectories were identified; a typically developing majority with low-stable ADHD-autistic traits (87%), a male-predominant subgroup with child/adolescent-declining traits (6%), and a subgroup with late-emerging traits (6%). ADHD-autistic trait correlations were greatest in young adulthood for the two non-typically developing classes. There were higher rates of emotional and conduct problems, low IQ, childhood seizures and poor social functioning in the declining and late-emerging classes compared to the low-stable class. Emotional, conduct and peer problems were more prevalent during childhood in the childhood/adolescent-declining class compared to other classes, but were more prevalent in young adulthood in the late-emerging class. Neurodevelopmental/psychiatric PGS also differed: both non-typically developing classes showed elevated ADHD PGS compared to the low-stable group, and the late-emerging group additionally showed elevated schizophrenia PGS and decreased executive function PGS, whereas the declining group showed elevated broad depression PGS.
Conclusions
Distinct patterns of ADHD-autism co-development are present across development in the general population, each with different characterising factors and genetic signatures as indexed by PGS.
Keywords: ADHD, Autism, ALSPAC, trajectories, longitudinal, genetic
Introduction
Autism (or Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)) and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) are both highly heritable, neurodevelopmental conditions that frequently co-occur (Thapar, Cooper & Rutter, 2017). Although conceptualized as binary diagnostic categories for clinical purposes, they also can be viewed as traits on a continuum, akin to hypertension and blood pressure (Lubke, Hudziak, Derks, van Bijsterveldt & Boomsma, 2009; Wittkopf et al., 2022). Risk factors for ADHD and autistic trait scores in the general population and clinical disorders overlap, with no clear-cut threshold on trait measures beyond which adverse outcomes manifest (Lord, Elsabbagh, Baird & Veenstra-Vanderweele, 2018; Thapar & Cooper, 2016). ADHD trait scores in the general population and clinical ADHD share genetic influences (Larsson, Anckarsater, Råstam, Chang & Lichtenstein, 2012; Stergiakouli et al., 2015; Taylor et al., 2019), and are highly genetically correlated (rg=0.97) (Demontis et al., 2019). Similarly, evidence from twin and molecular genetic studies suggest the variation of autistic traits in the population (including variation in social communication and behaviours) and an autism diagnosis have a similar genetic aetiology (Colvert et al., 2015; Robinson et al., 2011; Robinson et al., 2016; Taylor et al., 2019). These studies highlight that ADHD and autism can be viewed as traits as well as categorical diagnoses.
ADHD and autism share much in common. Both typically onset in early development, are more common in males, share heritability, and share genetic aetiology (Lee et al., 2019; Thapar et al., 2017). They also have common risk factors such as preterm birth (Blencowe et al., 2013), and share common comorbidities, such as an increased risk of emotional problems in youth and later life (Thapar, Livingston, Eyre & Riglin, 2023). The co-occurrence of autism and ADHD is clinically important. Those with autism-ADHD comorbidity show greater impairment, worse outcomes and pose a greater treatment challenge (Hartman, Geurts, Franke, Buitelaar & Rommelse, 2016; Sokolova et al., 2017; Vaidya & Klein, 2022).
Although it is recognised that autism and ADHD can persist into adult life, both show developmental changes across the lifespan. To date, most studies of autism-ADHD co-occurrence focus on children, with fewer studies of co-occurrence in later life (Hartman et al., 2016; St Pourcain et al., 2011). Previous work indicates autism-ADHD co-occurrence changes across the lifespan, with suggestions that co-occurrence peaks in adolescence and early adulthood. This relationship may be stronger due to greater demands on executive function (EF, which is impaired in both autism and ADHD) and social skills during emerging adulthood (Hartman et al., 2016). The extent to which EF determines developmental patterns of ASD, ADHD and their co-occurrence is debated (Hartman et al., 2016).
One hypothesis is that there are subgroups with different developmental changes in autism-ADHD co-occurrence (Hartman et al., 2016). When autism and ADHD are considered separately, longitudinal studies have identified multiple sub-groups showing different patterns of change with age. In clinical samples, the majority show persistence of disorder, others show symptom decline with age or, for ADHD, symptoms can remit after childhood (McCauley, Elias & Lord, 2020; Orm, Øie, Fossum, Andersen & Skogli, 2021). Population-based studies show similar patterns, the majority in a consistently low ADHD or autism trait group, and subgroups with persistent and remitting patterns (Agnew-Blais et al., 2016; Pender, Fearon, St Pourcain, Heron & Mandy, 2021; Riglin et al., 2022; Riglin et al., 2021c). Multiple longitudinal studies have also observed late-onset forms of ADHD, first manifesting in adolescence and adult life (Agnew-Blais et al., 2016; Asherson & Agnew-Blais, 2019; Caye et al., 2016; Cooper et al., 2018; Moffitt et al., 2015; Riglin et al., 2022; Sibley et al., 2021). The same may apply to autism, at least for social communication symptoms (Riglin et al., 2021c), but late-onset autism is not well established. These studies that have examined ADHD and autistic developmental trajectories separately observe that the different subgroups show different patterns of association with genetic risks, as indexed by polygenic scores (composite of common genetic risk variants) and other correlates.
Together, these findings highlight the developmental heterogeneity of autism and ADHD. We hypothesise that subgroups in the general population will show different developmental patterns of ADHD-autistic trait co-occurrence across childhood, adolescence and early adulthood. We further hypothesise that these subgroups will show different genetic signatures, indexed by neurodevelopmental and psychiatric polygenic scores (PGS), vary in sociodemographic and perinatal features, psychopathology and child and adult functioning. We had two main aims:
-
1)
Identify and characterise heterogeneity in ADHD-autism co-developmental trajectories from childhood to early adulthood (25 years).
-
2)
Test for associations between different patterns of co-development and neuropsychiatric polygenic scores known to be associated with ADHD, autism and executive function.
Methods
Sample
The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) study is an ongoing longitudinal study which recruited pregnant woman in the Avon region of South-West England, due to give birth between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992 (Boyd et al., 2013; Fraser et al., 2013; Northstone et al., 2019). The core sample consisted of 14,541 mothers and of these pregnancies, 13,988 children were alive at 1 year. Following the initial recruitment, an additional 913 children were recruited in three phases. Data were collected from families repeatedly and managed using REDCap (Harris et al., 2009). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Informed consent for the use of data collected via questionnaires and clinics was obtained from participants following the recommendations of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee at the time. The study website contains details of all the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data/). Full details of the ALSPAC study can be found in the Supplementary Material. For families with multiple births, we included the oldest sibling.
Measures and data collection
ADHD and autistic traits used to generate trajectories
ADHD traits were measured using the parent-rated 5-item Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (Goodman, 1997) ADHD subscale. This was completed at ages 4, 7, 8, 9, 12, 13, 17, and 25 years. Additionally, the self-rated questionnaire was completed by ALSPAC offspring at age 25. It is designed to measure hyperactive and inattentive symptoms, ranges from 0-10, and has been validated against a DSM-5 diagnosis of ADHD in childhood and young adulthood (Riglin et al., 2021a). Previous studies indicate SDQ scores have configural, metric, scalar, and residual invariance across ages 7 to 16 years (Speyer, Auyeung & Murray, 2022), as well as internal consistency of SDQ subscales in adolescents/young adults in a clinical setting (Brann, Lethbridge & Mildred, 2018). For reference, between ages 4 to 17 years, scores between 0-5 are classified as low, scores of 6-7 as slightly raised, or scores of 8-10 as high ADHD symptoms (Goodman, 1997). At age 25, ≥4 is classified as raised ADHD symptoms for parent-reports, and ≥5 as slightly raised and ≥6 as high ADHD symptoms in self-reports (Riglin et al., 2021a). Questionnaires with >2 items missing were excluded. Details of imputation of missing items is in Supplementary Material.
Autistic traits were measured using the parent-rated 10-item Social Communication Disorder Checklist (SCDC) (Skuse, Mandy & Scourfield, 2005). This was completed at ages 7, 10, 13, 17, and 25 years, and has previously been shown to have acceptable measurement invariance across these ages (Riglin et al., 2021c). It is designed to measure social reciprocity and verbal/nonverbal communication and has high sensitivity and specificity for an autism diagnosis (Bölte, Westerwald, Holtmann, Freitag & Poustka, 2011). The questionnaire ranges from 0-24, with a score of ≥9 recommended as the cut-point for pervasive developmental disorders, such as autism (Skuse et al., 2005). Details of imputation of missing items is in Supplementary Material.
Trajectory validators
Since trajectories were derived exclusively on parent-rated ADHD (SDQ-ADHD) and autistic (SCDC) traits to retain consistency of informant across ages, we also report on self-rated ADHD and autistic traits in young adulthood using the self-rated SDQ-ADHD subscale (detailed above) and the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin & Clubley, 2001). The AQ has been validated as a measure of clinical autism in adults, and ranges from 0-50, with a score of ≥32 indicating likely autism (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). We also examined childhood ADHD diagnosis at age 7 using the 18-item Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA) ADHD section (Goodman et al., 2000). The DAWBA is a structured diagnostic interview that assesses the 18 DSM ADHD diagnostic symptoms and was completed by parents as a questionnaire. We also report on childhood autism diagnosis, which was reported by parents when offspring were approximately 9 years old.
Characterising trajectory classes
To describe trajectory classes, we selected measures known to be associated with ADHD, autism or both.
Pre-/Perinatal variables
We investigated the frequency of preterm birth and low birth weight in each trajectory group. Preterm birth was defined as birth before 37-weeks gestation. Low birth weight was defined as a birth weight of less than 2,500g. Both measures were recorded at birth from obstetric records, ALSPAC measurements, and birth notification records.
Socio-demographic variables
We investigated sex, recorded at birth, and family income in each trajectory group, since previous evidence has shown links between socio-economic status and ADHD (Michaёlsson et al., 2022; Russell, Ford, Williams & Russell, 2016), and conflicting evidence for associations with autism depending on country (Kelly et al., 2017). Family income was reported by mothers when children were aged 11 as the average household income including social benefits each week on a 10-point scale from <£120 to ≥£800. We binarized family income for the purpose of our analysis, classifying low family income as those with <£430 weekly income (the £430 - £479 strata was the median income category in the cohort).
Psychopathology, cognitive ability and childhood seizures
Parent-rated emotional problems and conduct problems were measured using the emotional problems and conduct problem subscales of the SDQ (Goodman, 1997) at ages 7 (child), 13 (early adolescence) and 17 (late adolescence). Both parent and self-ratings were obtained at 25 years (early adulthood). Each subscale score ranges from 0-10. A cut-point of ≥4 indicates the presence of conduct problems and a cut-point of ≥5 indicates the presence of emotional problems in childhood and adolescence (Goodman, 1997; Goodman, Ford, Simmons, Gatward & Meltzer, 2000). Additionally, self-reported anxiety symptoms were measured at age 21 using the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Assessment-7 (GAD-7) (Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams & Lowe, 2006). Self-reported mental wellbeing was measured at age 23 using the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale (WEMWBS) (Tennant et al., 2007). Depressive symptoms were self-reported at age 25 using the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) (Angold, Costello, Messer & Pickles, 1995). Total symptom scores were calculated for each scale and we used recommended cut-points for these measures to estimate probable depression (SMFQ ≥12) (Eyre et al., 2021), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD-7 ≥10) (Spitzer et al., 2006), and poor mental wellbeing (WEMWBS ≤40) (Warwick Medical School., 2021).
Childhood IQ was measured in ALSPAC at age 8 using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – full IQ. An IQ score of <80 was considered low IQ. As epilepsy commonly co-occurs with ADHD and autism and itself is considered a neurodevelopmental disorder, we included a measure of childhood seizures. The occurrence of seizures thought to be due to epilepsy up to the age of 11 was determined by asking parents of children in ALSPAC about any history of seizures and the cause of the seizure(s) at 18, 30, 42, 57, 69, 81, 103 months (approximately 2.5, 3.5, 4.8, 5.8, 6.8 and 8.6 years) and finally at 11 years.
Childhood and adult social functioning variables
The presence or absence of peer problems was measured using the peer problems subscale of SDQ (Goodman, 1997), parent-rated at ages 7 (childhood), 13 (early adolescence) and 17 (late adolescence), and parent and self-rated at 25 years (young adulthood). A cut-point of ≥4 indicates the presence of peer problems in childhood and adolescence (Goodman, 1997). We investigated the frequency of those not in education, employment or training (NEET) in each trajectory group using a self-report data supplied from ALSPAC participants at age 25. Alcohol abuse was measured using self-report questionnaires in ALSPAC participants at age 22 using the DSM-5 criteria for alcohol use disorder (AUD). We binarized the variable whereby we included anyone meeting mild, moderate or severe AUD criteria. Cannabis abuse was measured using the self-report six-item CAST (Cannabis Abuse Screening Test (Legleye, Piontek & Kraus, 2011)) at age 22. As done previously in the ALSPAC cohort (Heron et al., 2013), we categorised those with a non-zero CAST score as ‘cannabis abuse’.
Genetic liability
We used polygenic scores (PGS) to index the common genetic liability for a variety of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions. Summary statistics from the following studies were used to generate PGS: i) ADHD - Demontis et al. (2019), ii) ASD - Grove et al. (2019), iii) schizophrenia - Trubetskoy et al. (2022), iv) bipolar disorder - Stahl et al. (2019), v) broad depression - Howard et al. (2019), vi) major depression - Wray et al. (2018), and vii) executive function - Hatoum et al. (2020). These PGS were chosen since previous evidence suggests shared genetic aetiology between these disorders and ADHD/ASD (Larsson et al., 2013; Riglin et al., 2021b). Additionally, the disorders indexed by these PGS are well-established comorbidities or cognitive aspects of ADHD and autism (Thapar et al., 2023). Details of the generation of PGS and GWAS from which PGS were derived from, are supplied in the Supplementary Material. Each PGS was standardised to aid interpretation.
Statistical analysis
Correlation between ADHD and autistic traits
Correlations between ADHD and autistic traits at approximately ages 7, 9/10, 13, 17 and 25 years were examined using Spearman’s rank correlation. We used the subset of the cohort who had complete SDQ-ADHD and SCDC questionnaires at age 7 (n=7,156). To minimise attrition bias, we imputed any subsequent incomplete values using multiple imputation, using the mice package in R (van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2011). SDQ-ADHD values were imputed using the SDQ-ADHD scores from all other timepoints, SCDC scores at age 7, and SCDC scores at the same timepoint and the prior timepoint as predictors (e.g., age 25 SDQ-ADHD scores were imputed using all other SDQ-ADHD scores and age 7, age 17 and age 25 SCDC scores). The converse was carried out for missing SCDC scores. Predictive mean matching was used to impute scores, generating 60 imputed datasets which were pooled and analysed.
Parallel-process growth mixture modelling
We used parallel-process growth mixture modelling (GMM) in MPlus, v.8.8 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998-2017) to model trajectories of parent-reported ADHD and autistic traits between 4 and 25 years. Parallel-process GMM aims to group individuals into classes based on patterns of change of two outcomes across multiple timepoints (note that the pattern of change for each outcome within a class is not required to be the same/similar in this model). Individuals in the same class are assumed to have the same growth curve, however within-class heterogeneity is modelled, unlike latent class growth analysis (LCGA). The average age in years at each questionnaire completion was used as the time metric for the model. Given the large gap between the last two time points and akin to previous ADHD and autism trajectory work in the same dataset (Riglin et al., 2022; Riglin et al., 2021c), models were fitted with piecewise growth models with single intercept and two linear slope factors, one for ages 4-17 for ADHD traits and 7–17 for autistic traits, and another for ages 17 and 25 for ADHD and autistic traits. The variance of the second slopes were fixed to zero to avoid nonidentification, as only two time points were included in these growth factors. The GMM therefore included one intercept, one slope for ages 4/7–17, and a second slope for ages 17–25 each for ADHD and autistic traits in parallel (see Supplementary Figure 1). Residual variances were constrained to be time-invariant for SCDC and SDQ-ADHD, and between-class covariances were also constrained due to difficulties running models with freed covariances. The primary sample included individuals for whom SCDC or SDQ-ADHD data were available from at least one timepoint (n=11,316). Models were fit using maximum likelihood estimator and full information maximum likelihood was used to handle missing data.
Starting with a single k-class model, k+1 solutions were fitted until the optimum model was reached, as determined primarily using Lo-Mendell-Rubin (LMR) tests, supported by Akaike information criterion (AIC), Bayesian information criterion (BIC), model entropy and smallest class size information. The LMR test quantifies whether a k-class model substantially improves model fit enough to justify the additional parameters introduced compared to k-1 class model. Lower values on AIC and BIC indicate better model fit and higher model entropy values indicate better distinguishability of classes when testing for association of auxiliary variables with trajectory classes. Once the optimal model was determined, the modelling was repeated with an increased number of random start values (n=1000) to ensure there were no problems with the local maxima and the correct solutions were obtained. Class sizes are reported based on the estimated model with Ns rounded to the nearest integer.
Correlations between ADHD and autistic traits were examined again, as described previously, this time stratified by trajectory class membership. Differences between correlations at age 7 and age 25 were tested using the “cocor” package in R (Diedenhofen & Musch, 2015), using Silver’s method (Silver, Hittner & May, 2004).
Characterising trajectory profiles
We next characterised trajectory classes across a range of variables known to be associated with ADHD and/or autism. We used the manual bias-corrected three-step approach (Heron, Croudace, Barker & Tilling, 2015; Vermunt, 2010; Wickrama, 2016), implemented in MPlus, to test for associations of trajectory classes with auxiliary binary variables. This method uses multinomial regression and accounts for measurement error in class assignment. Associations with auxiliary variables were conducted on all available data. Sample sizes for each analysis are supplied in Supplementary Table 1. Additionally, we used DCAT in MPlus to estimate proportions ±standard error (SE) for binary variables (Supplementary Table 3, Supplementary Figure 3), and the BCH (Bolck-Croon-Hagenaars) method to estimate means ±SE and a Wald’s Chi-squared test for equality of means for continuous auxiliary variables (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2021; Bakk & Kuha, 2021) since other three-step methods have been found to be unsuitable when modelling continuous dependent variables.
Sensitivity analysis
We checked whether trajectory classes were similar in males and females separately, as reported in the Supplementary material. Further, we repeated analysis of the characterising of trajectory classes using the original continuous variables which had been binarized for the main analysis (i.e., self-report SDQ-ADHD, AQ, gestational age, birthweight, all parent and self-rated subscales of SDQ (emotional, conduct and peer problems), GAD7, SMFQ, WEMWBS and IQ. We report mean ±SE of each measure by trajectory class as well as testing for equality of means using BCH in MPlus (Supplementary Table 4).
Results
The correlation between ADHD and autistic traits between 7 and 25 years in the whole cohort are shown in Figure 1 and the correlation matrix presented in the Supplementary Table 2. Overall correlations were relatively stable across development (rs = 0.4-0.5).
Parallel process growth mixture modelling
Results of GMM of SDQ-ADHD and SCDC in 11,316 individuals showed a 3-class model as the preferred fit (Table 1). Whilst BIC, AIC, log-likelihood values and LMR tests indicated a better fit with each added class (between 1-4), the 4-class model included a small class (3%) below the recommended size of 5% (see Supplementary Figure 2 for trajectories in the 4-class model). An entropy value of 0.87 for the 3-class model indicates high class separation. The chosen model included the following classes: Class 1, low-stable ADHD-autistic traits (87%) below the recommended cut-points for SCDC and SDQ-ADHD at all ages; Class 2, child/adolescent-declining ADHD-autistic traits (6%) above SCDC and SDQ-ADHD cut-points in childhood but falling below by late-adolescence; and Class 3, late-emerging ADHD-autistic traits (6%) below SCDC and SDQ-ADHD cut-points in childhood but rising above cut-points by late-adolescence (Figure 2).
Table 1. Model fit of growth mixture models with 1-4 classes.
1 CLASS | 2 CLASS | 3 CLASS | 4 CLASS | 5 CLASS | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
AIC | 389586 | 385487 | 383553 | 381949 | 380450 |
BIC | 389718 | 385670 | 383788 | 382235 | 380787 |
Entropy | - | 0.90 | 0.87 | 0.85 | 0.83 |
Log-likelihood | -194775 | -192718 | -191745 | -190936 | -190179 |
LMR-LRT p-value | - | <0.00001 | 0.05 | 0.02 | 0.55 |
Class 1 N (%) | 11316 (100%) | 10386 (92%) | 9892 (87%) | 9192 (81%) | 9026 (80%) |
Class 2 N (%) | - | 930 (8%) | 700 (6%) | 1193 (11%) | 851 (8%) |
Class 3 N (%) | - | - | 724 (6%) | 592 (5%) | 637 (6%) |
Class 4 N (%) | - | - | - | 340 (3%) | 553 (5%) |
Class 5 N (%) | - | - | - | - | 249 (2%) |
Akaike information criterion (AIC); Bayesian information criterion (BIC); Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted likelihood ratio test (LMR-LRT).
ADHD and autistic trait correlations increased with age when stratified by trajectory class (Figure 1). In the declining and late-emerging symptom classes, the correlation increased from ages 7 to 25 (declining: rs = 0.39 to rs = 0.65 (p<0.001); late-emerging: rs = 0.41 to rs = 0.63 (p<0.001)) whereas there was no change in the low-stable class (rs = 0.37 to rs = 0.35, (p=0.18)).
There was a higher proportion of individuals with an ADHD or autism diagnosis during childhood in the declining trait group, compared to both the late-emerging and low-stable groups (Table 2). The proportion of individuals with high ADHD and autistic traits in adulthood did not differ between the declining and late-emerging classes, but both were higher than the low-stable group (Table 2). For estimated proportions per class see Supplementary Table 3.
Table 2. Associations between additional autism and ADHD variables with trajectory classes.
Autism/ADHD variables | Declining vs. Low-stable | Late-emerging vs. Low-stable | Late-emerging vs. Declining | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR | 95% CI | P-value | OR | 95% CI | P-value | OR | 95% CI | P-value | |
Autism diagnosis age 9 (parent-report) | 68.1 | 32.0, 145.0 | <0.001 | 2.6 | 0.3, 19.9 | 0.36 | 0.04 | 0.01, 0.25 | 0.001 |
ADHD diagnosis age 7 (DAWBA) | 210.8 | 45.3, 981.8 | <0.001 | 48.9 | 9.0, 266.4 | <0.001 | 0.2 | 0.1, 0.4 | <0.001 |
High autistic traits age 25 (self-rated AQ) | 5.1 | 2.3, 11.5 | <0.001 | 5.2 | 2.4, 11.2 | <0.001 | 1.0 | 0.4, 2.9 | 0.98 |
High ADHD traits age 25 (self-rated SDQ-ADHD) | 3.3 | 2.0, 5.3 | <0.001 | 3.3 | 2.1, 5.0 | <0.001 | 1.0 | 0.5, 2.0 | 0.97 |
Characterising trajectory classes
There are substantially more males in the child/adolescent-declining group (73%) compared to the low-stable (49%, OR = 0.38 (0.30,0.47), p<0.001) and late-emerging (54%, OR = 0.37 (0.26, 0.51) p<0.001) groups. The child/adolescent-declining group also had the highest rate of pre-term birth (8%, vs. 5% in low-stable (OR = 1.79 (1.26, 2.53), p=0.001) and 5% in late-emerging (OR = 2.11 (1.05, 4.23), p=0.04) groups). Low family income was also more common in both the declining and late-emerging symptom groups than the low-stable group (Figure 3).
Psychopathology, cognition and seizures
There were higher odds of emotional and conduct problems at all ages (parent and self-rated), childhood epileptic seizures and low IQ in both the late-emerging and child/adolescent-declining groups, compared to the low-stable symptom group (Figure 3, Supplementary Table 3). Parent-rated emotional and conduct problems were more common at age 7 in the child/adolescent-declining group than other classes, however a higher proportion of individuals in the late-emerging group had emotional and conduct problems at age 25, compared to the other classes (Figure 3).
Social functioning
There were higher odds of peer problems (parent and self-rated) at all ages and NEET status at age 25 in both the late-emerging and child/adolescent-declining groups, compared to the low-stable symptom group, and higher rates of substance abuse at age 22 in the late-emerging group compared to the low-stable group, but no difference compared to the declining symptom group (Figure 3, Supplementary Table 3).
Genetic liability
Mean standardised PGS for each group are presented in Figure 4, with results from Wald’s Chi-squared test for equality of means presented in Supplementary Table 4. PGS for all traits lie at a Z-score of around 0 for the low-stable symptoms group. In the childhood/adolescent-declining class, ADHD (mean diff = 0.14, p = 0.02) and broad depression (mean diff = 0.16, p = 0.01) (but not major depression) PGS were higher compared to the low-stable group. In the late-emerging class schizophrenia PGS was higher (mean diff = 0.15, p = 0.03) and executive function PGS was lower (mean diff = -0.24, p = 0.001) than the low-stable symptom group, with weaker evidence for elevated ADHD PGS (mean diff = 0.14, p=0.06).
Sensitivity analysis
The best-fit classes for sex-stratified GMMs were less clear than in the overall analysis (see all models fits in Supplementary Tables 6 and 7), however both one (best fit for males) and two-class (best fit for females) solutions (shown in Supplementary Figures 4-6) show overall similar trajectory patterns for ADHD and autistic traits between males and females.
Analysis of auxiliary variables associated with class membership using the original continuous variables in the overall cohort is shown in Supplementary Table 4. This analysis showed similar patterns of association as when variables were binarized (as in Figure 3) for all variables except for anxiety, measured by the GAD7, for which the late-emerging group had a higher self-rated mean score at age 25 (mean = 7.4 ±SE 0.6) compared to the declining symptom group (mean = 4.9 ±SE 0.7, χ2=6.7, p=0.01). In the main analysis there was no difference in the proportion of individuals with probable generalised anxiety disorder between these two groups (Figure 3).
Discussion
In this study we investigated the co-development of ADHD-autistic traits from childhood to young adulthood in a large, prospective longitudinal birth cohort. Using parallel-process GMM we derived three distinct sub-groups of hyperactive-inattentive and social communication disorder symptoms: one with low-stable ADHD-autistic traits, reflecting the typically developing majority (87%), and two smaller sub-groups both with 6% prevalence, one with child/adolescent-declining ADHD-autistic traits, and the other with late-emerging ADHD-autistic traits.
These trajectory classes are similar to those observed previously in this cohort when social communication and ADHD traits were examined separately (Riglin et al., 2022; Riglin et al., 2021c). The patterns of ADHD development are also similar to those observed in other cohorts (Agnew-Blais et al., 2016; Caye et al., 2016; Moffitt et al., 2015). In clinical settings, many autistic or ADHD individuals show persistence of symptoms and disorder. Given this is a population cohort with very few meeting diagnostic criteria for autism or ADHD, we suggest that those with persistent symptoms are likely represented by the declining symptom class, as a 4-class solution (Supplementary Figure 2) did identify a very small subgroup with persistent ADHD-autistic traits who were mostly present in the declining symptom class in the 3-class solution.
It now is well recognised that ADHD and autism commonly co-occur and show a high level of correlation. Our findings suggest that autism and ADHD co-develop in parallel from childhood to early adulthood. However, our findings also suggest that the patterns of developmental change maybe more complex than previously thought. Whilst ADHD-autistic trait correlations were stable for the total cohort, this was not the case in the non-typically developing subgroups (declining and late-emerging). Here, symptom correlations increased with age, as previously hypothesised by Hartman et al. (2016). Whilst most clinicians recognise the need to assess autism in children with ADHD, and vice versa, if our findings extend to clinical populations, they would indicate that autism may be missed in some adults with ADHD, and vice versa, since the relationship between symptoms increase over time and therefore was less clear-cut earlier in development. If this finding is replicated in other studies, it may suggest that assessments of neurodevelopmental comorbidity should be repeated across the lifespan, not only at initial assessment in childhood.
The child/adolescent-declining group and the late-emerging group both showed higher rates of emotional and conduct problems, lower IQ, higher chance of childhood seizures and worse social functioning as indicated by peer problems and NEET status at age 25. However, some characteristics differentiated the two non-typically developing subgroups. Firstly, the child/adolescent-declining group showed the typical male predominance, concurring with previous literature that, in females, ADHD symptoms are less likely decline with age (Owens, Hinshaw, Lee & Lahey, 2009). This group also had higher rates of preterm birth than the other classes. Secondly, whilst both non-typically developing groups had higher emotional and conduct problems throughout life than the low-stable group, the late-emerging group showed much higher rates of these psychopathologies and poor social functioning in early adulthood (age 25) than those with childhood/adolescent-declining symptoms. This declining group rather showed higher rates of these problems during childhood and early adolescence (ages 7 and 13). This indicates that emotional and behavioural problems appear to be concurrent with ADHD-autistic traits, such that problems peak when ADHD-autistic traits peak. It is possible that this could be an artefact of parents being unable to distinguish different psychopathologies or difficulties, however, we saw little to no differences in self-rated ADHD and autistic trait scores nor self-reported psychopathology (e.g., emotional problems) between late-emerging and declining groups.
Social functioning was also impaired in both non-typically developing groups and this was observed across all ages, with both groups showing peer problems across development and higher rates of being not in education or employment (NEET) in young adulthood. However, like emotional and conduct problems, peer problems were highest at ages when ADHD-autistic traits peaked. Rates of substance abuse were also higher in the late-emerging symptom group compared to the typically developing group.
Analysis of the genetic risk score profiles of these three subgroups, as indexed by PGS, suggested elevated ADHD genetic liability for the two non-typically developing sub-groups, as well as decreased executive function PGS for both, compared to the typically developing majority. PGS for broad depression, but not major depressive disorder, was elevated in the childhood/adolescent-declining subgroup only, whereas in the late-emerging subgroup only, we saw elevated genetic liability for schizophrenia and lower PGS for executive function. This indicates that these two subgroups may have subtly different genetic architecture, with regards to common genetic risk variants (SNPs) associated with these psychiatric/neurodevelopmental disorders. It is interesting that the late-emerging trait group has on average higher schizophrenia PGS, and schizophrenia typically first onsets after late-adolescence/young adulthood. This may indicate that the timing of onset of different types of symptoms is associated with different genetic risk profiles. Conversely, higher depression (broadly defined) PGS are associated with declining ADHD-autistic traits, yet depressive disorder more frequently onsets after adolescence rather than in childhood (Thapar, Eyre, Patel & Brent, 2022). However, PGS associated with different psychiatric/neurodevelopmental disorders are highly correlated and non-specific, i.e., they cross diagnostic boundaries (Lee et al., 2019; Leppert et al., 2020). Thus, caution is needed in interpreting what different PGS profiles mean. Also, as PGS are weak indicators of genetic liability, and rare genetic variants also play an important role in neurodevelopmental disorders, these findings require further investigation.
These results should be viewed considering several limitations. First, the SCDC exclusively captures social and communication impairments, omitting the third phenotypic domain of autism: restricted and repetitive behaviours, including stereotyped movements, inflexible adherence to routines and sensory hypo/hyperreactivity (American Psychiatric Association., 2013). Therefore, it is possible that other domains of autism have different developmental trajectories and associations with ADHD symptom co-occurrence and outcomes. Also, unlike the SDQ-ADHD, which was first obtained in ALSPAC at age 4, the SCDC measure was first obtained at age 7 years, and therefore earlier measures could contribute to greater symptom variability and perhaps the emergence of different subgroups. Second, all measures used to create ADHD-autistic trait trajectories are based on parent-report, since self-report was only available at age 25. Indeed, we see variability in SDQ subscale scores at age 25 depending on the reporter – for example conduct and peer problems at age 25 are more common in the late-emerging than the childhood/adolescent-declining class when reported by parents, but there is little difference in self-report, therefore indicating the possibility of rater effects, such that trajectory classes may only reflect symptoms as observed by parents. Additionally, since diagnoses through health records were not available, we relied on parental report of an autism diagnosis at age 9 and the self-report AQ questionnaire at age 25 as trajectory class validators. Whilst parent-reported autism diagnosis does not include those diagnosed after 9 years, it remains useful in confirming that the questionnaire measure (SCDC) does identify clinical autism, as rates were higher in those with elevated ADHD-autistic traits in childhood (child/adolescent declining class), compared to the other classes which do not. Third, as with many longitudinal studies, ALSPAC suffers from non-random attrition, with those at elevated risk of psychopathology, including those with high genetic risk for ADHD and depression, being more likely to drop-out (Taylor et al., 2018). However, previous work on separate autism (Riglin et al., 2021c) and ADHD (Riglin et al., 2022) trajectories in ALSPAC revealed similar patterns of results, when using different approaches to account for missing data. Finally, we must acknowledge the issue of multiple testing when looking at associations between auxiliary variables with ADHD-autism trajectory classes which may increase the chance of false positive associations. Thus, replication in other population cohorts and clinical samples is required.
Nevertheless, our findings do suggest that developmental trajectories of ADHD and social communication traits appear to co-develop in parallel, but that developmental patterns of emergence and decline, as well as their correlations, across the life-course show heterogeneity. If shown in clinical populations, different patterns of co-development will be an important consideration for clinicians following up autistic or ADHD children through to adult life.
Conclusion
In summary, we observe heterogeneity in the joint trajectories of ADHD and autistic traits in the general population and differential patterns of symptom co-occurrence from childhood to young adulthood. Trajectory sub-groups indicate that autism and ADHD traits are reciprocal, whereby in most cases, they decline, persist or emerge together. Further, while both non-typically developing ADHD-autism trajectory sub-groups differ to the typically developing population in terms of sociodemographic and perinatal factors, psychopathology, child and adult function and genetic risk score profiles, there are some differences in the correlates and polygenic risk score profiles between the declining and late-emerging subgroups.
Supplementary Material
Key points.
Autism and ADHD are both highly heritable neurodevelopmental conditions that frequently co-occur and have heterogeneous developmental patterns. However, there is little known about patterns of ADHD-autism co-occurrence through development, particularly into adulthood.
This work shows heterogeneous patterns of ADHD-autistic trait co-occurrence and development from childhood to young adulthood, characterised by different timings of symptom emergence, psychopathology, social functioning and genetic risk score profiles as indexed by polygenic scores.
These different patterns of symptom co-development will be an important consideration for clinicians following up autistic or ADHD children through to adult life.
Acknowledgements
We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.
Funding
The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 217065/Z/19/Z) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and AT and KT will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. This research was funded in whole, or in part, by the Wellcome Trust (204895/Z/16/Z). For the purpose of Open Access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright license to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf). ES, RB and KT work in a unit that receives funding from the University of Bristol and the UK Medical Research Council (MC_UU_00011/1 and MC_UU_00011/3). GWAS data was generated by Sample Logistics and Genotyping Facilities at Wellcome Sanger Institute and LabCorp (Laboratory Corporation of America) using support from 23andMe.
Abbreviations
- ADHD
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
- PGS
Polygenic score
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors report no conflicting interests.
References
- Agnew-Blais JC, Polanczyk GV, Danese A, Wertz J, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L. Evaluation of the persistence, remission, and emergence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:713–720. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 2013.
- Angold A, Costello EJ, Messer SC, Pickles A. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International journal of methods in psychiatric research. 1995 [Google Scholar]
- Asherson P, Agnew-Blais J. Annual research review: Does late-onset attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder exist? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2019;60:333–352. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Using the bch method in mplus to estimate a distal outcome model and an arbitrary secondary model. Mplus Web Notes: No 21. 2021 [Google Scholar]
- Bakk Z, Kuha J. Relating latent class membership to external variables: An overview. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2021;74:340–362. doi: 10.1111/bmsp.12227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (aq): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:5–17. doi: 10.1023/a:1005653411471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blencowe H, Lee ACC, Cousens S, Bahalim A, Narwal R, Zhong N, Chou D, Say L, Modi N, Katz J, Vos T, et al. Preterm birth–associated neurodevelopmental impairment estimates at regional and global levels for 2010. Pediatric Research. 2013;74:17–34. doi: 10.1038/pr.2013.204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bölte S, Westerwald E, Holtmann M, Freitag C, Poustka F. Autistic traits and autism spectrum disorders: The clinical validity of two measures presuming a continuum of social communication skills. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:66–72. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1024-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Davey Smith G. Cohort profile: The ‘children of the 90s’--the index offspring of the avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:111–127. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brann P, Lethbridge MJ, Mildred H. The young adult strengths and difficulties questionnaire (sdq) in routine clinical practice. Psychiatry Research. 2018;264:340–345. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caye A, Rocha TB, Anselmi L, Murray J, Menezes AM, Barros FC, Gonçalves H, Wehrmeister F, Jensen CM, Steinhausen HC, Swanson JM, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: Evidence from a birth cohort supporting a late-onset syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:705–712. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colvert E, Tick B, Mcewen F, Stewart C, Curran SR, Woodhouse E, Gillan N, Hallett V, Lietz S, Garnett T, Ronald A, et al. Heritability of autism spectrum disorder in a uk population-based twin sample. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:415–423. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.3028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper M, Hammerton G, Collishaw S, Langley K, Thapar A, Dalsgaard S, Stergiakouli E, Tilling K, Davey Smith G, Maughan B, O’donovan M, et al. Investigating late-onset adhd: A population cohort investigation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018;59:1105–1113. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demontis D, Walters RK, Martin J, Mattheisen M, Als TD, Agerbo E, Baldursson G, Belliveau R, Bybjerg-Grauholm J, Bækvad-Hansen M, Cerrato F, et al. Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics. 2019;51:63–75. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedenhofen B, Musch J. Cocor: A comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0121945. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eyre O, Bevan Jones R, Agha SS, Wootton RE, Thapar AK, Stergiakouli E, Langley K, Collishaw S, Thapar A, Riglin L. Validation of the short mood and feelings questionnaire in young adulthood. J Affect Disord. 2021;294:883–888. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.07.090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A, Golding J, Davey Smith G, Henderson J, Macleod J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, et al. Cohort profile: The avon longitudinal study of parents and children: Alspac mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:97–110. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman R, Ford T, Simmons H, Gatward R, Meltzer H. Using the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (sdq) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;177:534–539. doi: 10.1192/bjp.177.6.534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grove J, Ripke S, Als TD, Mattheisen M, Walters RK, Won H, Pallesen J, Agerbo E, Andreassen OA, Anney R, Awashti S, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nature Genetics. 2019;51:431–444. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (redcap)--a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42:377–381. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartman CA, Geurts HM, Franke B, Buitelaar JK, Rommelse NNJ. Changing asd-adhd symptom co-occurrence across the lifespan with adolescence as crucial time window: Illustrating the need to go beyond childhood. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:529–541. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatoum AS, Morrison CL, Mitchell EC, Lam M, Benca-Bachman CE, Reineberg AE, Palmer RHC, Evans LM, Keller MC, Friedman NP. Genome-wide association study of over 427,000 individuals establishes executive functioning as a neurocognitive basis of psychiatric disorders influenced by gabaergic processes. bioRxiv. 2020:674515. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.06.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heron J, Barker ED, Joinson C, Lewis G, Hickman M, Munafò M, Macleod J. Childhood conduct disorder trajectories, prior risk factors and cannabis use at age 16: Birth cohort study. Addiction. 2013;108:2129–2138. doi: 10.1111/add.12268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heron JE, Croudace TJ, Barker ED, Tilling K. A comparison of approaches for assessing covariate effects in latent class analysis. Longitudinal and Life Course Studies. 2015;6(4) doi: 10.14301/llcs.v6i4.322. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Howard DM, Adams MJ, Clarke TK, Hafferty JD, Gibson J, Shirali M, Coleman JRI, Hagenaars SP, Ward J, Wigmore EM, Alloza C, et al. Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22:343–352. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0326-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly B, Williams S, Collins S, Mushtaq F, Mon-Williams M, Wright B, Mason D, Wright J. The association between socioeconomic status and autism diagnosis in the united kingdom for children aged 5–8 years of age: Findings from the born in bradford cohort. Autism. 2017;23:131–140. doi: 10.1177/1362361317733182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsson H, Anckarsater H, Råstam M, Chang Z, Lichtenstein P. Childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as an extreme of a continuous trait: A quantitative genetic study of 8,500 twin pairs. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:73–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsson H, Rydén E, Boman M, Långström N, Lichtenstein P, Landén M. Risk of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia in relatives of people with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203:103–106. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.120808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee PH, Anttila V, Won H, Feng Y-CA, Rosenthal J, Zhu Z, Tucker-Drob EM, Nivard MG, Grotzinger AD, Posthuma D, Wang MMJ, et al. Genomic relationships, novel loci, and pleiotropic mechanisms across eight psychiatric disorders. Cell. 2019;179:1469–1482.:e1411. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legleye S, Piontek D, Kraus L. Psychometric properties of the cannabis abuse screening test (cast) in a french sample of adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;113:229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leppert B, Millard LaC, Riglin L, Davey Smith G, Thapar A, Tilling K, Walton E, Stergiakouli E. A cross-disorder prs-phewas of 5 major psychiatric disorders in uk biobank. PLOS Genetics. 2020;16:e1008185. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Elsabbagh M, Baird G, Veenstra-Vanderweele J. Autism spectrum disorder. Lancet. 2018;392:508–520. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31129-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubke GH, Hudziak JJ, Derks EM, Van Bijsterveldt T, Boomsma DI. Maternal ratings of attention problems in adhd: Evidence for the existence of a continuum. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:1085–1093. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181ba3dbb. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mccauley JB, Elias R, Lord C. Trajectories of co-occurring psychopathology symptoms in autism from late childhood to adulthood. Dev Psychopathol. 2020;32:1287–1302. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420000826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michaёlsson M, Yuan S, Melhus H, Baron JA, Byberg L, Larsson SC, Michaёlsson K. The impact and causal directions for the associations between diagnosis of adhd, socioeconomic status, and intelligence by use of a bi-directional two-sample mendelian randomization design. BMC Medicine. 2022;20:106. doi: 10.1186/s12916-022-02314-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE, Houts R, Asherson P, Belsky DW, Corcoran DL, Hammerle M, Harrington H, Hogan S, Meier MH, Polanczyk GV, Poulton R, et al. Is adult adhd a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from a four-decade longitudinal cohort study. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172:967–977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14101266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. Muthén & Muthén; Los Angeles, CA: 1998-2017. [Google Scholar]
- Northstone K, Lewcock M, Groom A, Boyd A, Macleod J, Timpson N, Wells N. The avon longitudinal study of parents and children (alspac): An update on the enrolled sample of index children in 2019. Wellcome Open Res. 2019;4:51. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15132.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orm S, Øie MG, Fossum IN, Andersen PN, Skogli EW. Declining trajectories of co-occurring psychopathology symptoms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder: A 10-year longitudinal study. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:724–759. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.724759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owens EB, Hinshaw SP, Lee SS, Lahey BB. Few girls with childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder show positive adjustment during adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2009;38:132–143. doi: 10.1080/15374410802575313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pender R, Fearon P, Pourcain B, Heron J, Mandy W. Developmental trajectories of autistic social traits in the general population. Psychological Medicine. 2021:1–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721002166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riglin L, Agha SS, Eyre O, Bevan Jones R, Wootton RE, Thapar AK, Collishaw S, Stergiakouli E, Langley K, Thapar A. Investigating the validity of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire to assess adhd in young adulthood. Psychiatry Res. 2021a;301:113984. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riglin L, Leppert B, Langley K, Thapar AK, O’donovan MC, Davey Smith G, Stergiakouli E, Tilling K, Thapar A. Investigating attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder traits in the general population: What happens in adult life? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2021b;62:449–457. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riglin L, Wootton RE, Livingston LA, Agnew-Blais J, Arseneault L, Blakey R, Agha SS, Langley K, Collishaw S, O’donovan MC, Davey Smith G, Stergiakouli E, et al. “Late-onset” adhd symptoms in young adulthood: Is this adhd? J Atten Disord. 2022:10870547211066486. doi: 10.1177/10870547211066486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riglin L, Wootton RE, Thapar AK, Livingston LA, Langley K, Collishaw S, Tagg J, Davey Smith G, Stergiakouli E, Tilling K, Thapar A. Variable emergence of autism spectrum disorder symptoms from childhood to early adulthood. Am J Psychiatry. 2021c;178:752–760. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20071119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson EB, Koenen KC, Mccormick MC, Munir K, Hallett V, Happé F, Plomin R, Ronald A. Evidence that autistic traits show the same etiology in the general population and at the quantitative extremes (5%, 2.5%, and 1%) Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68:1113–1121. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson EB, Pourcain B, Anttila V, Kosmicki JA, Bulik-Sullivan B, Grove J, Maller J, Samocha KE, Sanders SJ, Ripke S, Martin J, et al. Genetic risk for autism spectrum disorders and neuropsychiatric variation in the general population. Nat Genet. 2016;48:552–555. doi: 10.1038/ng.3529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell AE, Ford T, Williams R, Russell G. The association between socioeconomic disadvantage and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (adhd): A systematic review. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;47:440–458. doi: 10.1007/s10578-015-0578-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sibley MH, Arnold LE, Swanson JM, Hechtman LT, Kennedy TM, Owens E, Molina BSG, Jensen PS, Hinshaw SP, Roy A, Chronis-Tuscano A, et al. Variable patterns of remission from adhd in the multimodal treatment study of adhd. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2021;179:142–151. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2021.21010032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver NC, Hittner JB, May K. Testing dependent correlations with nonoverlapping variables: A monte carlo simulation. The Journal of Experimental Education. 2004;73:53–69. [Google Scholar]
- Skuse DH, Mandy WP, Scourfield J. Measuring autistic traits: Heritability, reliability and validity of the social and communication disorders checklist. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:568–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.187.6.568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokolova E, Oerlemans AM, Rommelse NN, Groot P, Hartman CA, Glennon JC, Claassen T, Heskes T, Buitelaar JK. A causal and mediation analysis of the comorbidity between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (adhd) and autism spectrum disorder (asd) J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47:1595–1604. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3083-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speyer LG, Auyeung B, Murray AL. Longitudinal invariance of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire across ages 4 to 16 in the alspac sample. Assessment. 2022:10731911221128948. doi: 10.1177/10731911221128948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Lowe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The gad-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:1092–1097. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pourcain B, Mandy WP, Heron J, Golding J, Davey Smith G, Skuse DH. Links between co-occurring social-communication and hyperactive-inattentive trait trajectories. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:892–902.:e895. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stahl EA, Breen G, Forstner AJ, Mcquillin A, Ripke S, Trubetskoy V, Mattheisen M, Wang Y, Coleman JRI, Gaspar HA, De Leeuw CA, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 30 loci associated with bipolar disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51:793–803. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0397-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stergiakouli E, Martin J, Hamshere ML, Langley K, Evans DM, Pourcain B, Timpson NJ, Owen MJ, O’donovan M, Thapar A, Davey Smith G. Shared genetic influences between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (adhd) traits in children and clinical adhd. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54:322–327. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.01.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor AE, Jones HJ, Sallis H, Euesden J, Stergiakouli E, Davies NM, Zammit S, Lawlor DA, Munafo MR, Davey Smith G, Tilling K. Exploring the association of genetic factors with participation in the avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47:1207–1216. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyy060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor MJ, Martin J, Lu Y, Brikell I, Lundström S, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P. Association of genetic risk factors for psychiatric disorders and traits of these disorders in a swedish population twin sample. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:280–289. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.3652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tennant R, Hiller L, Fishwick R, Platt S, Joseph S, Weich S, Parkinson J, Secker J, Stewart-Brown S. The warwick-edinburgh mental well-being scale (wemwbs): Development and uk validation. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2007;5:63. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-5-63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Cooper M. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Lancet. 2016;387:1240–1250. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00238-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Cooper M, Rutter M. Neurodevelopmental disorders. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2017;4:339–346. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30376-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Eyre O, Patel V, Brent D. Depression in young people. The Lancet. 2022;400:617–631. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(22)01012-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar A, Livingston LA, Eyre O, Riglin L. Practitioner review: Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder-the importance of depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2023;64:4–15. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trubetskoy V, Pardiñas AF, Qi T, Panagiotaropoulou G, Awasthi S, Bigdeli TB, Bryois J, Chen C-Y, Dennison CA, Hall LS, Lam M, et al. Mapping genomic loci implicates genes and synaptic biology in schizophrenia. Nature. 2022;604:502–508. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04434-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaidya CJ, Klein C. Comorbidity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorders: Current status and promising directions. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2022;57:159–177. doi: 10.1007/7854_2022_334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. Mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in r. Journal of Statistical Software. 2011;45:1–67. [Google Scholar]
- Vermunt JK. Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Political Analysis. 2010;18:450–469. [Google Scholar]
- Warwick Medical School. Collect, score, analyse and interpret wemwbs. University of Warwick; 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Wickrama K, L TK, ON CW, L FO. Higher-order growth curves and mixture modeling with mplus : A practical guide. 2016.
- Wittkopf S, Langmann A, Roessner V, Roepke S, Poustka L, Nenadić I, Stroth S, Kamp-Becker I. Conceptualization of the latent structure of autism: Further evidence and discussion of dimensional and hybrid models. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2022 doi: 10.1007/s00787-022-02062-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wray NR, Ripke S, Mattheisen M, Trzaskowski M, Byrne EM, Abdellaoui A, Adams MJ, Agerbo E, Air TM, Andlauer TMF, Bacanu SA, et al. Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat Genet. 2018;50:668–681. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0090-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.