Abstract
Objective
The purpose of this study was to examine whether persisting hyperactivity into adulthood was associated with impaired family, friendship and partner relationships or poor coping skills in everyday life.
Methods
A 20-year community-based follow-up of 6-7-year-old boys showing pervasive hyperactivity (N=40) and unaffected controls (N = 25). At age 27 years, subjects were assessed with detailed interview techniques as well as self-report ratings.
Results
ADHD in adulthood was associated with problems in intimate relationships and negotiation skills. Antisocial behavior did not influence the association, but remitting childhood hyperactivity was not associated with social relationship difficulties in adulthood.
Conclusions
In an untreated, community-based sample of hyperactive children, the risk for unsatisfactory social relationships is largely confined to those subjects who still show ADHD in adulthood. The majority of subjects who experience childhood hyperactivity have positive social relationships in adulthood.
INTRODUCTION
The persistence of ADHD into adulthood has become the focus of widespread research attention and ADHD is currently considered a lifespan condition (Faraone et al., 2000; Wilens & Dodson, 2004). It has been suggested that as many as 60% of childhood cases may continue with significant ADHD symptoms as adults (Biederman et al., 2006; Rasmussen & Gillberg, 2000; Weiss, Hechtman, Milroy & Perlman, 1985). However, estimates of the proportion of children with ADHD who will have persisting symptoms vary considerably as a function of reporting source, attrition rate, and the criteria used to define the disorder in adulthood (Mannuzza, Klein & Moulton, 2003). There are several long-term prospective longitudinal studies on the persistence of ADHD into adulthood in the literature. Table 1 summarises these studies. Databases and published papers were searched for longitudinal studies on ADHD, hyperactivity and attention deficit. To be included, studies needed to be based on at least 30 subjects, use control groups, select participants using DSM criteria, retain at least 50% or more of their original samples into adulthood (above age 18 years), give details on subjects lost to follow up and have a low attrition rate.
Table 1. Prospective studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder into adulthood.
Investigators name and year of publication |
Location of study |
Sample size |
Age at baseline |
Diagnostic criteria | Age at follow-up |
Years of follow- up |
ADHD persistence |
Control group |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Mean | Baseline | Follow-up | Mean | Mean | n | % | |||
Weiss et al. (1985) | Montreal (Canada) |
61 children (59% PR) 90% boys |
6-12 years |
Not stated |
DSM III | 25.1 years | 15 | SYMPTOMS 1 | 41 children aged 25.2 |
|
42 | 66% | |||||||||
| ||||||||||
Mannuzza et al. (1993) | New York (USA) |
91 white boys (88% PR) |
9.3 ± 1.4 year |
DSM-II | DSM III DSM-III- R |
25.1 ± 1.3 years |
16.1 | FULL ADHD | 95 boys aged 25.6±1. 6 |
|
7 | 8% | |||||||||
| ||||||||||
IMPAIRMENT 2 | ||||||||||
10 | 11% | |||||||||
| ||||||||||
Mannuzza et al. (1998) | New York (USA) |
85 white boys (82% PR) |
7.3 years | DSM-II | DSM III DSM-III- R |
24.1 years | 17.0 year |
FULL ADHD | 73 boys 24.1 years (94% PR) |
|
3 | 4% | |||||||||
| ||||||||||
IMPAIRMENT 2 | ||||||||||
0 | 0% | |||||||||
| ||||||||||
Rasmussen et al. (2000) | Göteborg (Sweden) |
55 children (42% boys) |
7 years | DSM III | DSM-IV | 22 years | 15 years | 28 | 58% | 46 children (43% boys) |
| ||||||||||
Barkley et al. (2002) | Wisconsin (USA) |
158 hyper. children with 87% males (93% PR) |
4-12 years |
DSM-III R |
DSM-III R |
20.8 years | 13.8 years |
Not reported | 81 control (92% males) 90% PR |
|
| ||||||||||
Biederman et al (2006) | Boston (USA) |
140 children (80% PR) |
6-18 years |
DSM-III R |
DSM-IV | 21.6 years | 10 years | 78 | 70% | 120 (88% PR) |
PR= participation rate at follow-up
Weiss et al. reported persisting core symptoms in adults (restlessness, poor concentration, impulsivity), not persisting full diagnosis
Mannuzza et al. used the concept of “probable diagnosis” to define those cases were not than all criteria were reported but functional impairment was present
“Barkley et al. found that parents reported much higher rates of persistence than patients (66% vs 8%) and demonstrated that the definition of disorder (developmentally appropriate criteria vs DSM criteria) influenced persistence rates as well.
As apparent from Table 1, the risks associated with ADHD appear clear. However, most of the existing follow-up studies, (except the Swedish one), are based on patients referred to clinics and treated, so a poor outcome could be associated with factors leading to referral, such as parents’ ability to cope with child behavior, severity of disorder, coexistent problems, school relationship problems, as well as family and social background (Sayal, 2006; Wolf & Wasserstein 2001) - rather than the presence of hyperactivity per se. In order to fully understand the natural history, there is a need for more prospective, epidemiologically representative studies of untreated hyperactive children.
One purpose of epidemiology is the completion of the clinical picture. Hyperactive behavior is more common in childhood than the diagnosis of ADHD (Taylor et al 1991). In order to develop public health strategies, it will be desirable to know whether those with hyperactive behavior are at risk for later mental health problems even if they do not meet all the diagnostic criteria for ADHD.
The nature of any deficits in adult life also needs fuller understanding. Distinguished research on the adaptive functioning of adults with ADHD has stressed that their problems extend to poor academic grades, low engagement with further education, involvement with the juvenile justice system, and risky behavior, especially in driving (Barkley et al 2006). Some degree of uncertainty remains about the full long-term impact of hyperactivity (NICE, 2006). Clinical decisions could be better founded if there was more knowledge of social functioning of affected adults, and particularly of their ability to relate to significant others.
Social relationships are an important determinant of the quality of life, and need to be understood as part of the natural history of disorder. It has been shown that clinic referred adults with ADHD have poorer marital adjustment and family functioning (Eakin et al., 2004), as well as a higher incidence of separation and divorce, than normal controls (Biederman et al., 2006). Furthermore, it has been demonstrated that subjects who have been hyperactive in childhood have fewer close friends and report more problems with keeping friends compared with controls (Barkley, Fischer, Smallish & Fletcher, 2006).
In a previous paper from this prospective epidemiological study (Taylor, Chadwick, Heptinstall & Danckaerts 1996), it was suggested that childhood hyperactivity was a risk factor for development over the period from 7 to 17 years, even after allowing for the coexistence for conduct problems. This suggested a developmental pathway through which hyperactivity raised the likelihood of impaired social adjustment. Another study based on the current data set indicated that childhood hyperactivity predicts poorer mental health in adulthood (Stringaris et al., 2011).
Taking the above findings into account, we decided to examine social relationships in a never-medicated sample of adults, 20 years after they were ascertained as showing high levels of hyperactive behavior; and hypothesised that childhood hyperactivity would lead to disturbed social relationships in adulthood.
OBJECTIVES
The specific aims of the study were:
To document the extent to which adults who had been hyperactive in middle childhood had significantly different levels of satisfaction in family, friendship, and intimate relationships, as well impaired ability to negotiate, by comparison to those not hyperactive in their childhood.
To examine whether continuing presence of hyperactivity in adulthood influences the level of satisfaction in family, friendship and partner relationships, and the ability to negotiate.
To examine whether antisocial behavior influences the possible effect of hyperactivity on relationships in adulthood.
METHODS
Subjects (childhood study)
The original survey, from which all the subjects were taken, has been previously described (Taylor, Sandberg, Thorley, & Giles, 1991). As a brief summary, the subjects included all 6- and 7-year-old boys (3,215 boys), on the registers of mainstream schools in the London Borough of Newham, with the schools for severely learning disabled excluded.
The Rutter B(2) questionnaire was completed for 99% of the children by their class teachers, and the A(2) questionnaire for 80% by the parents. In total, 2462 had both screening questionnaires completed. Hyperactivity was defined as present if both the teacher and parent questionnaire gave a score of 3 or more on the ‘hyperactivity’ subscale. Conduct problems were defined as present if the score on the teacher scale was 9 or greater, or that on the parent scale was 13 or greater, and the score on the ‘conduct disorder’ subscale was greater than that for ‘emotional disorder’. These cut-offs had been validated in previous surveys and on the Isle of Wight studies (see Rutter et al., 1976).
On the basis of the screening questionnaire ratings, three groups were selected: those with scores above cut-off for conduct disorder who also met criteria for pervasive hyperactivity (mixed; constituting 5.3% of the study population); those who met criteria for pervasive hyperactivity but not for conduct problems (hyperactive; amounting to 3.7%), and those not meeting criteria for either condition (control). The cases were stratified by behavioral group and then randomly sampled from the resulting groups, in order to give approximately equal numbers in each group for detailed study. Subjects were excluded if they had scores of 5 or greater on the emotional disorder subscale of either the parent or teacher scale. With respect to their hyperactive symptoms, the children with mixed hyperactivity and emotional symptoms were similar to those with hyperactivity only (Taylor, Sandberg, Thorley, & Giles, 1991). The children with mixed hyperactivity and emotional symptoms (15% of children with hyperactivity and 12% of controls) were excluded because they were considered to form an etiologically distinct group, which could have confounded the comparisons. For this reason, the results of the present study should not be generalized to hyperactive children with comorbid anxiety or depression.
THE FOLLOW-UP STUDY
Subjects (adulthood study)
For the present study, 121 subjects (79 hyperactive, 42 control). were initially selected because on the basis of their childhood bahavior they fell into either the hyperactive or control groups, and had been included in the detailed second wave of study Of these, the follow-up of the children of first-generation immigrant families (16 hyperactive, 9 controls) will be reported separately. The marked differences in the way they were identified by parents and teachers, by comparison to children, of native British families, made comparison unsatisfactory (Sonuga-Barke, Minocha, Taylor, & Sandberg, 1993). The sample for follow-up therefore consisted of 63 children with hyperactive behavior (‘mixed’ and ‘pure hyperactive’ combined), and 33 control children with no detected behavioral problems. Those who only met criteria for conduct problems were not included in the present analyses.
Of the 96 adults, 10 could not be traced, and 12 had refused permission for future contact when approached in a previous 10-year follow-up (Taylor, Chadwick, Heptinstall, & Danckaerts, 1996). Of the remaining 74, one had died (of meningitis) and eight declined to be interviewed. The subjects reported here therefore included 40 hyperactive cases (63% of the original target group) and 25 controls (76% of the original controls).
Procedure
A first follow up was carried out 9 years after the second stage of the original survey, when the subjects were aged 16 to 18 years (Taylor, Chadwick, Heptinstall, & Danckaerts, 1996). The second follow up reported here was carried out when the subjects were 25 to 30 years old. The follow-up was based on the groups who had received detailed study.
Tracing the subjects was undertaken by a variety of means, including previously recorded addresses, electoral records, and personal contacts. The reliance upon multiple methods was intended to reduce the bias that might be introduced by those who fail to be contacted by any one technique. When the young men and their families were contacted, permission was sought for interviews and a test session. A small sum of money was paid to them in recognition of the expenses involved. Informed consent was obtained before interviewing the participants.
Outcome Measures
1. Adult Functioning Interview
This is an investigator-based standardized interview schedule, administered to the subject by a trained interviewer. It is a modification of the Adult Personality Functioning Assessment (APFA) (Hill et al., 1989). The APFA interview itself includes investigator-based structured enquiry on aspects of psychosocial adjustment, including relationships with friends, partners and family members and ability to negotiate in social situations. These relationship measures are the subject of the present report. The interview also includes information about occupational and psychiatric outcomes: these will be the subjects of a separate report (Stringaris et al. 2011). It enquires about functioning over a period of 5 or 10 years, depending on a particular interpersonal domain. Pervasive dysfunction according to the APFA has been shown to be associated with the diagnosis of personality disorder (Hill, et al., 2000). In the present study three additional behavioral scales were included, i.e. those of (a) hyperactive (inattentive/restless) behavior, (b) defiant/antisocial bahavior, and (c) emotional disorder symptomatology, especially anxiety and depression. These three scales are in the format of the PACS interview (Chen & Taylor, 2006); which involves enquiring about behavior in specific situations by trained interviewers (in the present study psychology or social science graduates). Audio taped interviews, were used to make detailed behavioral ratings on the basis of the subject’s recollections of recent behaviors in particular situations.
Reliability checks were carried out throughout the investigation by another researcher listening to and rating randomly selected tapes. The inter-rater agreements in terms of kappas were .74, .81, .79 and .67, for overall satisfaction with friendships, negotiation skills, partner relationships and family relationships, respectively.
2. Psychiatric outcome was measured by the SADS interview, and the cognitive function assessed by CANTAB (these are subjects of separate reports). Algorithmic diagnoses were generated from the SADS. Telephone or postal enquiry was made where possible from the men’s parents.
3. A diagnostic conference was held about each study participant by the research team (chaired by an experienced psychiatrist, ET), which was blind to the person’s childhood hyperactivity levels. All information was gathered from the subjects and informants, inquiring about past and present problems, level of impairment and developmentally inappropriateness of symptoms over the year prior to the assessment and systematically presented for DSM-IV diagnostic judgements. All members of the research team remained blind to the findings of previous assessments until the diagnosis had been recorded.
Analysis
Mean differences between the hyperactive and non-hyperactive groups as defined in childhood - and, within the previously hyperactive, between those who shared ADHD at outcome and those who did not – were compared by analysis of variance; and by analysis of covariance with antisocial behaviour as a covariate. Where a scale was dichotomised at the level of those showing ‘good’ function, chi-square comparisons were made.
RESULTS
Sample characteristics
On average, the subjects were 27.6 years old (SD = 1.2) and had 0.5 children (SD = 0.8); 83.4% were in paid employment, while 13.3% were either unable to work, in full-time education/training or caring for others (3.3% missing data). The average age of leaving school was16 years (SD = 1.1), leaving home 20.5 years (SD = 3.6), and starting employment 16.9 years (SD = 2.4). The level of attrition described earlier under “Subjects” led us to consider whether the 65 children followed up in adulthood were representative of the original 96 in terms of their characteristics at the outset of the study. Table 2 describes IQ, socio-economic status, and level of behavior problems for the 65 children who were followed up, and compares them with the 31 children lost to attrition. No systematic differences were found.
Table 2. Comparison of those followed up and those not contacted on baseline measures.
HYPERACTIVE Followed up Mean [SD] |
HYPERACTIVE Not interviewed Mean [SD] |
p | CONTROL Followed up Mean [SD] |
CONTROL Not interviewed Mean [SD] |
p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | 40 | 23 | 25 | 8 | ||
IQ | 100 [14.8] |
97 [17.1] |
.39 | 103 [14.2] |
102 [8.9] |
.90 |
CRS | 12 [4.7] |
13 [4.8] |
.11 | 2.3 [2.8] |
4.1 [2.2] |
.10 |
B2 Total | 14 [6.9] |
12 [6.4] |
.39 | 4.2 [3.3] |
6.0 [3.7] |
.19 |
A2 Total | 15 [5.9] |
14 [7.5] |
.47 | 9.5 [5.2] |
8.8 [4.8] |
.73 |
SES | 4.1 [1.0] |
4.5 [0.9] |
.08 | 3.8 [1.1] |
4.6 [0.8] |
.09 |
The table shows the mean scores and standard deviations on variables measured at the first contact with the project, at age app. 7 years; according to whether contact was achieved at follow-up.
IQ: score on 4 subtests of WISC-R
CRS: score on hyperactivity items of Conners’ Classroom Rating Scale
B2 Total: sum of behaviour problems from Rutter B(2) teacher rating scale
A2 Total: behaviour problems from Rutter A(2) parent rating scale
SES: socioeconomic status from Registrar-Generals classification of occupations (range 1-6, 1 denoting highest occupational level)
Comparison between adults identified as hyperactive in childhood and their controls
When the subjects identified as hyperactive in childhood and the controls were compared for age, employment status, number of children, age leaving school and starting employment, no statistically significant differences in any of these aspects were found. By age, those designated hyperactive (M = 27.7, SD = 1.1) were only slightly older than the controls (M = 27.4, SD = 1.5; t(63) = 3.76, p >0.5), and had left home at a younger age (M = 20, SD = 3.7) than the controls (M = 21.3, SD = 3.6), t(45) = −1.21, p >0.5). There was no significant difference regarding the employment status, (χ2 (3) = 1.5, p >0.5). The key measure for social relationships is the overall level of satisfaction with friendships. In order to arrive at this rating, several specific aspects were enquired about: several aspects of social relationships were measured, such as joint activities and levels of discord with friends. In the sample as a whole, 55% reported having four or more friends with whom they engaged in joint activities and 52% had two or three friends they could confide in. The overall perceived level of satisfaction with friendships was significantly correlated with the number of friends with whom the subject engaged in joint activities (r = 0.45, p < .001), had a confiding relationship (r = 0.57, p < .001), and had received practical help from (r = 0.54, p < .001), as well as with the subject’s own level of sociability (r = 0.40, p < .001). For each friendship, the interviewer enquires further about the quality of relation in order to arrive at the judgement of overall satisfaction. There was no significant effect of persisting hyperactivity with regard to the overall level of satisfaction with friendships (F=2.22, df=1, 63, P=0.14) (table 3) and those with a ‘good’ level of satisfaction were as common in the previously hyperactive as in the controls (χ2 (1) = 1.7, p =0.19).
Table 3.
Childhood hyperactivity |
Controls | p | |
---|---|---|---|
Satisfaction with friends | 2.67 [0.500] |
2.87 [1.088] |
0.59 |
Family support | 2.38 [0.744] |
1.77 [0.898] |
0.87 |
Ability to negotiate | 3.67 [1.000] |
2.97 [0.983] |
0.69 |
Satisfaction in partner and intimate
relationships |
4.00 [0.707] |
3.13 [1.332] |
0.70 |
When the degree of support the subjects both received from and gave to other members of their family was examined it was found that a majority (81.2%) reported their families giving them practical and emotional support. A third stated that they were supported by both of their parents, as well as siblings, 28.1% had received support only from their mothers and siblings, while the rest either received support only from by their mothers or did not receive support from anyone in the family. As shown in table 3, there was no statistically significant difference between previously hyperactive and controls on the on the overall level of support from the family (F=0.62, df=1, 58, P=0.80) and those with a ‘good’ level of support were as common in the previously hyperactive as in the controls (χ2 (1) = 5.2, p =0.27).
Another aspect examined was the subjects’ ability to handle negotiations, defined as the ability to secure rights and obtain their goals using discussion, assertiveness or even humour, instead of aggression and discord. Again, those identified as hyperactive in childhood did not differ significantly from controls in their level of ability to negotiate (F=2.44, df=1, 63, P=0.12) (table 3), and were no less likely to be ‘good’ negotiation (χ2 (1) = 0.25, p = 0.62).
Regarding partner and other intimate relationships in the sample as a whole, the average age the subjects began dating was 15.2 years (SD = 2.4), and the first steady relationship 17.8 years (SD = 2.8), which on average lasted for 27.8 months (SD = 31.2). At the time of the assessment, 41% had no current relationship, 21.7% were in a steady relationship, 35% had been in a cohabiting relationship/marriage for more than 6 months and the rest were in shorter-lasting cohabitations. Among those 56.7% with cohabitations lasting more than 6 months, 20% had cohabited with more than one partner, 37% with one partner only, and 10% had never had a steady relationship. Concerning the level of satisfaction in partner and intimate relationships, as shown in table 3, there was no significant difference between previously hyperactive and controls (F=0.63, df=1, 62, P=0.43) and those identified as hyperactive in childhood were no less likely to show a ‘good’ level of satisfaction (χ2 (1) = 0.41, p = 0.84).
Comparison between hyperactive and non-hyperactive adults
According to the DSM-IV diagnostic ratings at age 27 years, 22.5% of all subjects met the full criteria for ADHD in adulthood. In accordance with the second aim of the study, the subjects with adult ADHD and those without hyperactivity were compared, and results are in table 4. Those with adult ADHD reported poorer negotiation skills and less satisfaction in partner and other intimate relationships, compared with the subjects without hyperactivity. In contrast, both groups did not differ with regard to the level of satisfaction with friendships or support received from the family, and both groups were similar to the controls.
Table 4.
ADULT ADHD | NOT ADULT ADHD | p | |
---|---|---|---|
Satisfaction with friends | 2.79 [0.802] |
2.64 [0.954] |
0.589 |
Family support | 2.15 [0.689] |
1.92 [0.896] |
0.382 |
Ability to negotiate | 3.36 [0.929] |
2.80 [0.850] |
0.03 |
Satisfaction in partner and intimate relationships |
3.93 [0.917] |
3.03 [1.197] |
0.01 |
Comparisons between those hyperactive only in childhood and those with hyperactivity in childhood and ADHD in adulthood were also carried out. Those identified as hyperactive in childhood with ADHD in adulthood did not differ significantly from those hyperactive in childhood only and from controls in their level of ability to negotiate (F=3.50, df=1, 38, P=0.07), satisfaction with relationships (F=3.47, df=1, 38, P=0.07) or family support (F=3.09, df=1, 37, P=0.09). These results might suggest that persistence of ADHD could be linked to adverse social outcomes , but the number of participants is too small to base conclusions.
Finally, in order to examine whether antisocial behavior had an influence on the associations, a global measure of antisocial behavior was created. The number of offences found in criminal records, self-reported aggressions and fights and use of weapons were added in order to create this measure. Using an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with hyperactivity levels in adulthood as predictor, antisocial behavior as covariate and relationship satisfaction levels as dependent variables, antisocial behavior was not found to be significantly related to intimate partner relationships (F(1,61) = 0.43); negotiation skills (F(1,61) = 0.60); family support (F(1,61) = 1.4), or friendships (F(1,61) = 0.72), and the predictiveness of hyperactivity was unaltered.
DISCUSSION
This study shows that adult ADHD is associated with difficulties in social relationships, especially intimate partner relationships, and with poor negotiation skills. These findings generally corroborate and extend those of previous studies suggesting that children with ADHD are at an increased risk for social dysfunction in later life (Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2006). Adults with ADHD having difficulties in romantic relationships (Halversted, 2002), more psychological maladjustment (Murphy & Barkley, 1996), and impairment in a number of areas of functioning (Wilens & Dodson, 2004) has been documented. Previous follow-ups of our sample also support the view that persistence of hyperactivity is a key influence on adult psychopathology and poor outcome (Taylor, Chadwick, Heptinstall, & Danckaerts 1996).
Another interesting finding from this study was that the participants who had been hyperactive in childhood, but did not have ADHD in adulthood, were not obviously less satisfied with their social relationships than those who had never been hyperactive. The rather negative picture of the social outcome of children referred to specialist clinics, and diagnosed with ADHD, should not necessarily be generalized to the broader range of hyperactive children in the community. Most hyperactive children may have a different adult outcome from that reported in clinical-referred ADHD children (Mannuzza, et al.,1998; Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2004).
It is possible that the method of self-report may have underestimated the degree of impairment in relationships if people with hyperactivity lack insight into any problems that may be present. This explanation would not, of course, diminish the significance of the differences we found in adults who showed diagnosable ADHD.
The question arises why milder degrees of childhood hyperactivity are not associated with relationship dissatisfaction in adulthood in most cases. This may be due to the overall decrease of hyperactivity from childhood to adulthood. In a previous follow-up of this sample hyperactivity was found to diminish between the ages of 7 and 17 years (Taylor, Chadwick, Heptinstall & Danckaerts 1996). Consistent with this, Biederman et al. (2000) reported that hyperactivity symptoms declined at a higher rate than inattention symptoms, and Asherson’s (2009) meta-analysis on the prevalence of adult ADHD shows that a high proportion of children grow out of the disorder - either due to maturational changes in brain function and self-regulation, or because of learnt coping skills. Moreover, adult life offers more opportunities to choose an environment which is more suitable for someone who has ADHD. Hyperactive adults may find supportive friends and partners who can help them to improve poor self-discipline, overreacting to frustration, difficulty in self-organisation and establishing and keeping routines.
The association of hyperactivity and antisocial behavior has been documented in clinic-referred samples (Mannuzza et al., 1993; Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2004). However, antisocial behavior is known to be frequently associated with both childhood hyperactivity and with referral to services (Woodward, Dowdney, & Taylor, 1997).
Therefore, it may only be possible to examine the impact of hyperactivity accurately in epidemiological studies. To our knowledge there exist only two larger epidemiological studies. Bussing et al. (2010) screened 1615 children in a school district and followed them up for 8 years. In their study childhood ADHD was found to be associated with persistence in nearly half of the sample, together with an increased risk for comorbidity, functional impairment and reduced quality of life. Sub-threshold ADHD increased the risk for grade retention, but social relationships were not analysed.
The Christchurch Health and Development Study (Fergusson et al (2007)) examined the mediating role of conduct problems in a general population sample and found that any association between early attentional problems and the adverse outcome of substance misuse was mediated via the association between conduct and attentional problems. By contrast, our epidemiological data do not suggest that antisocial behavior determines the relationship between ADHD and social dysfunction.
It has to be noted that none of the children in our sample had received medication for symptoms of ADHD. At that time, and in this part of London, the diagnosis was not used and medication was not prescribed to any of the children. If a referral to a mental health service was made, then the offer was of family therapy, which was usually unacceptable to the parents. Hence, the course of hyperactivity was not confounded by the effects of specific therapeutic intervention. It is uncertain whether medication is a protective factor in the longer term and more research is needed to clarify its effect on the long-term course of ADHD symptoms. Our results do not support the treatment of young people with subdiagnostic levels of hyperactivity. Neither do they provide evidence for screening for hyperactivity in the general population. The majority of untreated children in our sample appeared to have a positive outcome regarding social relationships. However, reverse causality cannot be ruled out. Relationship problems may influence emotional wellbeing and increase the likelihood of affected individuals continuing to display high levels of hyperactivity. Whether this can happen in adulthood is not known. To date, little is known about the long-term developmental course of hyperactivity and accurate knowledge about factors involved in both negative and positive outcome is needed. Future well-designed, long-term studies should contribute to gaining a better understanding of these factors, which in turn would help to define high risk groups and eventually lead the way to new targets for intervention.
Limitations of the study
This study has controlled for antisocial and emotional problems, but like in other long-term epidemiological studies there remains the possibility for other unmeasured, uncontrolled confounding factors. The rather small sample size entails limited power, and the attrition rate of around 30% might have had an effect on the validity of the results. Social relationships were documented through self-reports, which could have exaggerated, or understated, the associations with hyperactivity.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the Medical Research Council, initially as a project grant and later as part of the work of its Child Psychiatry Unit and Centre for Genetic, Social and Developmental Psychiatry. We are grateful to Dr G. Thorley for his work in the initial survey, the schools and education department in Newham and the community health department. We would also like to thank the Alicia Koplowitz Foundation for its support.
References
- •.Asherson P. Review: prevalence of adult ADHD declines with age. Evidence Based Mental Health. 2009;12(4):128. doi: 10.1136/ebmh.12.4.128-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish l, Fletcher K. The persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111(2):279–289. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. Young adult follow-up of hyperactive children: Antisocial activities and drug use. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(2):195–211. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00214.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. Young adult outcome of hyperactive children: adaptive functioning in major life activities. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent, Psychiatry. 2006;45(2):192–202. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000189134.97436.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV. Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157(5):816–818. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Spencer T, Wilens TE, Silva JM, Snyder LE, Faraone SV. Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 10-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36(2):167–179. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Biederman J, Faraone SV, Spencer TJ, Mick E, Monuteaux MC, Aleardi M. Functional impairments in adults with self-reports of diagnosed ADHD: a controlled study of 1001 adults in the community. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67(4):524–40. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v67n0403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Bussing R, Mason D, Bell L, Bell L, Porter P. Adolescent Outcomes of Childhood Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in a Diverse Community Sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 49(6):595–605. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Chen W, Taylor E. Parental Account of Children’s Symptoms (PACS) and the ADHD Phenotype: Relevance for Quantitative Trait Locus Studies. In: Oades RD, editor. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (AD/HD) and the Hyperkinetic syndrome (HKS): Current Ideas and Ways Forward. Essen, Germany: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- •.Cumyn L, French L, Hechtman L. Comorbidity in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;54(10):673–83. doi: 10.1177/070674370905401004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Eakin L, Minde K, Hechtman L, Ochs E, Krane E, Bouffard R, Greenfield B, Looper K. The marital and family functioning of adults with ADHD and their spouses. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2004;8(1):1–10. doi: 10.1177/108705470400800101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Faraone SV, Biederman J, Spencer T, Wilens T, Seidman LJ, Mick E, Doyle AE. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: an overview. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48(1):9–20. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00889-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM. Conduct and attentional problems in childhood and adolescence and later substance use, abuse and dependence: results of a 25-year longitudinal study. Drug Alcohol Dependence. 2007;88(1):S14–26. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Fischer M, Barkley RA, Smallish L, Fletcher K. Young adult foliow-up of hyperactive children: Self-reported psychiatric disorders, comorbidity, and the role of childhood conduct problems and teen CD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;30(5):463–475. doi: 10.1023/a:1019864813776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Halversted J. Romantic relationships for women with ADHD. In: Nadau K, Quinn P, editors. Understanding women with ADHD. Advantage Books; Silver Spring, MD: 2002. pp. 409–424. [Google Scholar]
- •.Hill J, Harrington R, Fudge H, Rutter M, Pickles A. Adult personality functioning assessment (APFA). An investigator-based standardised interview. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1989;155:24–35. doi: 10.1192/bjp.155.1.24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Hill J, Fudge H, Harrington R, Pickles A, Rutter M. Complementary approaches to the assessment of personality disorder. The Personality Assessment Schedule and Adult Personality Functioning Assessment compared. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;176:434–439. doi: 10.1192/bjp.176.5.434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, Lapadula M. Adult outcome of hyperactive boys: Educational achievement, occupational rank, and psychiatric status. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1993;50(7):565–576. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820190067007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, Lapadula M. Adult psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;155(4):493–498. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.4.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Moulton JL. Persistence of Attention– Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder into adulthood: What have we learned from the prospective follow-up studies? Journal of Attention Disorders. 2003;7(2):93–100. doi: 10.1177/108705470300700203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Murphy K, Barkley RA. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adults: Comorbidities and adaptive impairments. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1996;37(3):393–401. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(96)90022-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.NICE . The National Institute for Health & Clinical Excellence. The British Psychological Society and The Royal College of Psychiatrists; London: 2006. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, Diagnosis and management of ADHD in children, young people and adults. [Google Scholar]
- •.Rasmussen P, Gillberg C. Natural outcome of ADHD with developmental coordination disorder at age 22 years: A controlled, longitudinal, community-based study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39(11):1424–1431. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200011000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Rutter M, Tizard J, Yule W, Graham P, Whitmore K. Research report: Isle of Wight Studies, 1964-1974. Psychological Medicine. 1976;6(2):313–332. doi: 10.1017/s003329170001388x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Sayal K. Annotation: Pathways to care for children with mental health problems. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47(7):649–659. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01543.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Sonuga-Barke EJS, Minocha K, Taylor EA, Sandberg S. Interethnic bias in teachers’ ratings of childhood hyperactivity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1993;1:187–200. [Google Scholar]
- •.Stringaris A, Hawkins A, McLoughlin L, Moyá J, Ashershon P, Sandberg S, Taylor E. Childhood Hyperactivity as a Risk Factor for Adult Psychopathology: Results from a 20 Year Prospective Community-Based Study in London. 2011. (submitted for publication)
- •.Taylor E, Sandberg S, Thorley G, Giles S. The epidemiology of Childhood Hyperactivity London. Oxford University Press; United Kingdom: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- •.Taylor E, Chadwick O, Heptinstall E, Danckaerts M. Hyperactivity and conduct problems as risk factors for adolescent development. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35(9):1213–1226. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199609000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Weiss G, Hechtman L, Milroy T, Perlman T. Psychiatric status of hyperactives as adults: A controlled prospective 15-year follow-up of 63 hyperactive children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1985;24(2):211–220. doi: 10.1016/s0002-7138(09)60450-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Wilens TE, Dodson W. A Clinical Perspective of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder into Adulthood. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2004;65(10):1301–1313. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v65n1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Wolf LE, Wasserstein J. Adult ADHD: Concluding thoughts. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2001;931:396–408. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- •.Woodward L, Dowdney L, Taylor E. Child and family factors influencing the clinical referral of children with hyperactivity: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38(4):479–485. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01533.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]