Abstract
Objectives
To examine whether during a period of limited e-cigarette regulation and rapid growth in their use, smoking began to become renormalised among young people.
Design
Interrupted time-series analysis of repeated cross-sectional time-series data.
Setting
Great Britain
Participants
248,324 young people aged approximately 13 and 15, from three national surveys during the years 1998-2015.
Intervention
Unregulated growth of e-cigarette use (following the year 2010, until 2015).
Primary and Secondary outcome measures
Primary outcomes were prevalence of self-reported ever smoking and regular smoking. Secondary outcomes were attitudes towards smoking. Tertiary outcomes were ever use of cannabis and alcohol.
Results
In final models, no significant change was detected in the pre-existing trend for ever smoking (OR = 1.01; CI = 0.99 to1.03). There was a marginally significant slowing in the rate of decline for regular smoking (OR = 1.04; CI: 1.00 to1.08), accompanied by a larger slowing in the rate of decline of cannabis use (OR = 1.21, CI = 1.18 to1.25) and alcohol use (OR = 1.17; CI = 1.14 to1.19). In all models and subgroup analyses for smoking attitudes, an increased rate of decline was observed after 2010 (OR = 0.88; CI = 0.86 to 0.90). Models were robust to sensitivity analyses.
Conclusions
There was a marginal slowing in the decline in regular smoking during the period following 2010, when e-cigarettes were emerging but relatively unregulated. However, these patterns were not unique to tobacco use and the decline in the acceptability of smoking behaviour among youth accelerated during this time. These analyses provide little evidence that renormalisation of youth smoking was occurring during a period of rapid growth and limited regulation of e-cigarettes from 2011-2015.
Background
Electronic cigarettes, first developed in China, have proliferated in many countries in the last decade. In the UK, adults’ use of e-cigarettes rose rapidly from 2011 before plateauing since 2013 (1). Some argue that e-cigarettes appear to have had small, but important, positive population level impacts on adult smoking cessation rates (2, 3). Although this remains contested (4, 5), their harm reduction potential has led many to support their use as an alternative to smoking (6). However, public health communities remain divided in approaches to harm reduction and views on the extent to which e-cigarettes should be regulated (7). While Public Health England has supported less restrictive policies (8), the Centre for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) in the USA has highlighted potential harms of e-cigarettes, supporting a more restrictive approach to their use (9). In North America, policies have included banning e-cigarette use wherever tobacco use is prohibited (7), while in other countries, such as Australia, sales of e-cigarettes containing nicotine remain illegal (10), citing concerns of smoking renormalisation (11).
Growth of e-cigarette use among young people has been framed to some extent as a potential public health problem in its own right, due to some evidence from animal models that nicotine may impair adolescent brain development (12). However, the most commonly expressed concern among those calling for greater regulation relates to their potential impact on young people’s smoking. Unlike adult use of e-cigarettes which has largely been limited to smokers or ex-smokers (13), emerging international evidence indicates increasing numbers of adolescents who have never used tobacco are experimenting with e-cigarettes (14–16). These studies show that by 2015, experimentation with e-cigarettes was more common than experimentation with tobacco. Notably, they also show that experimentation is not translating into widespread regular e-cigarette use to date (17, 18). Nevertheless, a perception that e-cigarette proliferation may renormalise smoking (19), through leading young people to view smoking as a socially acceptable behaviour, has been cited in policy documents in several countries as a rationale to support more restrictive policies. The European Union Tobacco Products Directive (TPD, 20) has regulated e-cigarettes in partial alignment with tobacco, contending “Electronic cigarettes can develop into a gateway to nicotine addiction and ultimately traditional tobacco consumption, as they mimic and normalise the action of smoking. For this reason, it is appropriate to adopt a restrictive approach to advertising electronic cigarettes and refill containers” (p.7; 20) while the Australian government has stated: “…the Department is concerned about evidence suggesting that e-cigarettes may provide a gateway to nicotine addiction or tobacco use (particularly among youth), and may re-normalise smoking.” (p. 1; 21)
Much success in maintaining a continuous downward trajectory in youth smoking in the past 20 years has been achieved through policies that aim to reverse the normalisation of smoking (22). The renormalisation hypothesis (23) assumes that growing prevalence and visibility of e-cigarette use will reverse tobacco control successes through increasing the extent to which smoking is once again seen as a “normal” behaviour, accepted and accommodated by the non-smoking majority, including young people. However, the hypothesis that e-cigarettes will renormalise smoking in young people is premised on an assumption that tobacco use and e-cigarette use are viewed by young people as sufficiently similar for one to renormalise the other. By contrast, some argue that e-cigarettes may denormalise smoking (24), through social display of an alternative behaviour, leading to displacement away from tobacco use for some young people who would otherwise have become smokers. From this perspective, alignment of e-cigarettes with tobacco in terms of regulatory frameworks paradoxically risks creating a perception that they are synonymous, potentially creating conditions for renormalisation to occur.
To date, national surveys in a number of countries have shown that smoking rates among young people have continued to fall in recent years despite the growth of e-cigarette use (17, 25–28). However, few attempts have been made to model whether this decline has occurred at a faster rate (as would be expected were displacement to be taking place), or a slowed rate since the emergence of e-cigarettes (as would be expected were renormalisation to be taking place), or to examine changes in young people’s attitudes toward smoking as a normative behaviour. To date, only one US study has tested these changes in trend, finding no evidence of change in trend for youth smoking during the period of rapid growth, but limited regulation, of e-cigarette use (29). The aim of the current study was therefore to examine these competing hypotheses by examining trends of smoking and smoking attitudes of young people in the UK since 1998, with a focus on whether these trends change significantly after 2010 and until 2015; the period of time when e-cigarettes were emerging, but largely unregulated (i.e. before the introduction of TPD (20). Changes in trend for tobacco use and smoking attitudes were accompanied by analyses of trends for alcohol and cannabis use, to examine the extent to which change in trend during this period is unique to tobacco or reflective of broader substance use trajectories which are less likely to have changed as a direct consequence of e-cigarettes.
Methods
Population-sampled data
Nationally representative samples of secondary school students were used from England, Scotland and Wales from the following repeated cross-sectional surveys: the annual Smoking Drinking and Drug Use Among Young People in England Survey (SDDU), the biennial Scottish Adolescent Lifestyle and Substance Use Survey (SALSUS), and for Wales, the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) survey (from 1998-2013) and the School Health Research Network (SHRN) survey (2015). The HBSC survey takes place every two to four years, with SHRN developed from the 2013 survey and a SHRN survey conducted in 2015 (as of 2017, HBSC is integrated into the larger SHRN survey). Further details about sampling strategies and procedures used for these surveys, including access to SDDU and SALSUS data, are available elsewhere (26, 27, 30–48).
Measures
Sociodemographic information
All surveys asked young people to indicate whether they were male or female. SALSUS only surveys pupils in S2 and S4 (i.e. pupils aged approximately 13 and 15 years respectively). SDDU and HBSC/SHRN datasets collect data from 11-16 year olds, but for comparability with Scotland were limited to approximately equivalent school year groups (i.e. Years 9 and 11). As not all surveys provide an age variable, year group was used as a proxy for age. In SALSUS and SDDU, socioeconomic status (SES) was indicated by a binary variable representing whether or not students reported receiving free school meals. In HBSC/SHRN, the Family Affluence Scale (FAS; 49), which measures material affluence, was used to indicate SES. As material markers of deprivation shift substantially over time, a relative measure of SES was derived whereby the sample was divided into “high” and “low” affluence within the survey year in question.
Primary outcomes: Ever smoking and regular smoking
Two binary variables were derived to indicate whether students had ever smoked and whether they smoked regularly (i.e. weekly or more). In SDDU and SALSUS, participants were asked to indicate which of the following statements best described them: “I have never smoked”, “I have only ever smoked once”, “I used to smoke but I never smoke a cigarette now”, “I sometimes smoke a cigarette now, but I don’t smoke as many as one a week”, “I usually smoke between one and six cigarettes a week”, “I usually smoke more than six cigarettes a week”. To indicate ever smoking, those who reported “I have never smoked” were compared to all others. For regular use, those who reported smoking between one and six cigarettes a week, or more, were compared to all others. In HBSC/SHRN, students were asked at what age they “smoked a cigarette (more than just a puff)” with the following response options: “never” or a range of ages. Those who reported “never” were compared to all others. Students in HBSC/SHRN were also asked, “how often do you smoke at present?” with response options: “every day”, “at least once a week but not every day”, “less than once a week” or “I do not smoke”. To indicate regular use, those who reported use at least once a week or more frequently were compared to all others.
Secondary outcomes: Smoking attitudes
In SALSUS (from 2006) and SDDU (from 2003), students were asked “Do you think it is OK for someone your age to do the following?: Try a cigarette to see what it is like”. In SDDU in 1999 and 2001, the question wording was slightly different (‘Try smoking once’). Hence, analyses were run with and without these earlier years as a sensitivity analysis. In SDDU only (from 2003), students were also asked whether it was OK for someone their age to smoke cigarettes once a week. Response options for both items were: “it’s OK”, “it’s not OK” and “I don’t know”. For each smoking attitudes measure, two dichotomous variables were created which coded “I don’t know” as “yes” as well as “no”, respectively.
Tertiary outcomes: Alcohol and cannabis use
Falsifiability checks included replicating analyses for binary indicators of ever alcohol use and ever cannabis use. For alcohol use, both SALSUS and SDDU asked students “Have you ever had a proper alcoholic drink – a whole drink, not just a sip?” with responses of “yes” or “no”. From 2002, HBSC/SHRN surveys asked students ‘At what age did you do the following things? If there is something that you have not done, choose the ‘never’ category.” Responses other than “never” for the category “drink alcohol (more than a small amount)” were classed as ever drinkers. To measure ever cannabis use in SALSUS and SDDU, students were presented with a grid listing a range of drugs and asked which, if any, they have ever used with response options “yes” or “no”. In HBSC, pupils were asked how many times they have used cannabis in their lifetime with response options of “never”, “once to twice”, “3 to 5 times”, “6 to 9 times”, “10 to 19 times”, “20 to 39 times” and “40+ times”. A binary variable distinguished ever users from never users.
Statistical Analyses
Segmented time series regression analyses were used. 1998 was selected a priori as the starting time point when youth smoking peaked before commencing a period of approximately linear decline (50). Proliferation of e-cigarettes was viewed as a naturally occurring intervention, with 2010 treated as the “intervention” point as surveys of the general population in the UK began to identify emergence of e-cigarette use from 2011 (1). While not an intervention in the traditional sense of the term, the emergence of e-cigarettes represents an important industry-driven ‘event’ within the tobacco control system with potential to alter its trajectories, positively and negatively (51). The following core statistical model was used for Yki, the smoking status of individual i at time k:
Where πki was the expected value of Yki; time was a continuous variable indicating time from the start of the study to the end of the period of observation; intervention was coded 0 for pre-intervention time points (prior to, and including, year 2010) and 1 for post-intervention time points (from 2011) and postslope was coded 0 up to the last point before the intervention phase and coded sequentially from 1,2… thereafter. β0 estimated the baseline level of the outcome at time 0 (beginning of the period); β1 estimated the structural trend, independently from the policy intervention; β2 estimated the immediate impact of the intervention and β3 reflects the change in trend/slope after the intervention; β4 is the set of parameters corresponding to the country dummy variables. Data were analysed with all countries’ data combined with year group and gender included as covariates. A time2 covariate was also included to allow for non-linear trajectories in a separate quadratic model. Models were repeated for all outcomes. Females (as opposed to males), older adolescents and less affluent groups have typically reported higher prevalence rates of smoking, and the role of e-cigarettes in exacerbating or reducing these inequalities is of significant interest. For subgroup analyses, models were therefore stratified by gender, year group, and (where available) SES. Interaction effects by year group and gender were also investigated. In pre-specified sensitivity analyses, models were also run: with England data only (the country with largest number of data points); with data points excluded for when a survey was conducted at a different time of year (e.g. SALSUS 2002 and 2006); and with survey weights applied. The extent of non-response was deemed to be sufficiently trivial for the analysis to be conducted on a complete-case basis, with data unavailable on the primary outcomes of ever and regular smoking for only 1.9% and 1.8% of pupils within the final dataset respectively. While models were a priori assumed linear, examination of trends over time pointed towards non-linearity for some outcomes. Entry of a quadratic term to the model which allowed for structural departures from linearity changed the size and direction of odds ratios in models, revealing sensitivity to these assumptions. Quadratic models are therefore reported alongside linear models and are referred to from here on as the final models. As most trends were clearly linear from the turn of the millennium, a further post-hoc sensitivity analysis involved exclusion of the earliest time points and modelling of “pre-intervention” trend from 2001-10 only. All analyses were run using STATA/SE 14.2.
Results
For primary and tertiary outcomes, data from at least one UK country were available for each of 18 time-points, representing 248,324 survey respondents. For smoking attitudes, at least 15 time-points were available representing 162,324 survey respondents. Across all outcomes, prevalence rates decreased over the study period (see supplementary material Tables 1-14). From 1998 to 2015, among children aged 13 and 15, the percentage of ever smokers decreased from 60% (n = 6,346) to 19% (n = 35,840) while regular smokers decreased from 19% (n = 6,360) to 5% (n = 35,789; note 2015 did not include data from England; see supplementary Tables 1 and 3 respectively). Perceptions of smoking also changed over time: the percentage of participants who reported that trying a cigarette was “OK” declined from 70% (n = 3,595) in 1999 to 27% (n = 23,729) in 2015 (see supplementary Table 5). The percentage of young people in England reporting that it was “OK” to smoke weekly declined from 36% (n = 3,962) in 2003 to 14% (n = 2,399) in 2014 (where including those who responded “I don’t know” as “not OK”, see supplementary Table 9). With “I don’t know” responses coded as “OK”, the percentage of participants who reported that trying a cigarette was “OK” declined from 79% (n = 3,595) in 1999 to 42% in 2015 (n = 23,729; see supplementary Table 7) and the percentage of participants in England who reported it was “OK” to smoke weekly declined from 47% (n = 3,962) in 2003 to 23% (n = 6,346) in 2014 (n = 2,399; see supplementary Table 10; from here on, analyses are reported with “I don’t know” coded as “not OK”, and sensitivity analyses where “I don’t know” was coded as “OK” can be found in supplementary material Tables 7-8, 17 and 18). Between 1998 and 2015, ever cannabis use decreased from 29% (n = 4,970) to 9% (n = 34,977) and ever alcohol use from 79% (n = 3,682) to 48% (n = 35,255; see supplementary materials Tables 11-14).
Table 1 shows model results, adjusted for covariates, for ever smoked and regular smoking (for the whole sample and subgroups based on gender and year group). Table 2 shows model results, adjusted for covariates, for smoking attitudes (for the whole sample and subgroups based on gender and year group, see Table 15 and 16 in supplementary material for subgroup analyses by SES and for England only).
Table 1. Odds ratios of linear and quadratic models for ever smoked and regular smoking between 1998-2015 among students in England, Scotland and Wales.
Ever smoked | Regular smoking | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Linear | p | Quadratic | p | Linear | p | Quadratic | p | ||
Whole sample (n=242,855; 243,111) |
Year | 0.91 [0.91 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 0.95 [0.93 to 0.96] | <0.001 | 0.93 [0.93 to 0.94] | <0.001 | 0.98 [0.96 to 1.00] | 0.025 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | <0.001 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | <0.001 | |
Level | 0.89 [0.84 to 0.95] | <0.001 | 0.89 [0.84 to 0.95] | <0.001 | 0.79 [0.71 to 0.88] | <0.001 | 0.80 [0.72 to 0.90] | <0.001 | |
Post-slope | 0.97 [0.96 to 0.99] | <0.001 | 1.01 [0.99 to 1.03] | 0.231 | 0.98 [0.96 to 1.01] | 0.231 | 1.04 [1.00 to 1.08] | 0.028 | |
Male only subgroup (n=121,879; 122,042) |
Year | 0.92 [0.92 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 0.93 [0.91 to 0.95] | <0.001 | 0.94 [0.94 to 0.95] | <0.001 | 0.95 [0.92 to 0.98] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | 0.621 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | 0.583 | |
Level | 0.88 [0.81 to 0.96] | 0.005 | 0.88 [0.81 to 0.96] | 0.005 | 0.83 [0.70 to 0.97] | 0.022 | 0.83 [0.70 to 0.97] | 0.023 | |
Post-slope | 0.98 [0.95 to 1.00] | 0.034 | 0.98 [0.95 to 1.01] | 0.212 | 1.00 [0.96 to 1.04] | 0.921 | 1.01 [0.96 to 1.06] | 0.794 | |
Female only subgroup (n=120,976; 121,069) |
Year | 0.90 [0.89 to 0.90] | <0.001 | 0.96 [0.94 to 0.98] | <0.001 | 0.92 [0.92 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 1.00 [0.98 to 1.03] | 0.922 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [0.99 to 1.00] | <0.001 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 1.00] | <0.001 | |
Level | 0.90 [0.83 to 0.98] | 0.014 | 0.90 [0.83 to 0.98] | 0.018 | 0.76 [0.65 to 0.89] | <0.001 | 0.78 [0.67 to 0.91] | 0.002 | |
Post-slope | 0.97 [0.95 to 0.99] | 0.009 | 1.05 [1.01 to 1.08] | 0.003 | 0.97 [0.93 to 1.01] | 0.139 | 1.07 [1.02 to 1.12] | 0.009 | |
13 year olds only (n=126,960; 127,100) |
Year | 0.89 [0.89 to 0.90] | <0.001 | 0.96 [0.94 to 0.98] | <0.001 | 0.91 [0.90 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 1.02 [0.98 to 1.05] | 0.378 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [0.99 to 1.00] | <0.001 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 1.00] | <0.001 | |
Level | 0.82 [0.74 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 0.83 [0.75 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 0.73 [0.57 to 0.92] | 0.009 | 0.76 [0.60 to 0.97] | 0.027 | |
Post-slope | 0.99 [0.96 to 1.01] | 0.304 | 1.07 [1.03 to 1.10] | <0.001 | 1.00 [0.95 to 1.07] | 0.902 | 1.14 [1.06 to 1.23] | <0.001 | |
15 year olds only (n=115,895; 116,011) |
Year | 0.93 [0.93 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 0.94 [0.92 to 0.95] | <0.001 | 0.94 [0.93 to 0.94] | <0.001 | 0.97 [0.95 to 0.99] | 0.003 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | 0.390 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | 0.009 | |
Level | 0.92 [0.85 to 0.99] | 0.035 | 0.92 [0.85 to 0.99] | 0.036 | 0.79 [0.70 to 0.90] | <0.001 | 0.80 [0.70 to 0.90] | <0.001 | |
Post-slope | 0.96 [0.94 to 0.98] | <0.001 | 0.96 [0.94 to 0.99] | 0.012 | 0.98 [0.95 to 1.01] | 0.240 | 1.01 [0.97 to 1.06] | 0.497 |
Table 2. Odds ratios of linear and quadratic models for smoking attitudes among students in England and Scotland (trying smoking is ‘OK’, from 1999-2015) and England only (smoking weekly is ‘OK’, from 2003-2014).
Trying smoking is OKa (don’t know = not OK) |
Smoking weekly is OKb (don’t know = not OK) |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Linear | p | Quadratic | p | Linear | p | Quadratic | p | ||
Whole sample (n=165,199; 35,890) |
Year | 0.91 [0.91 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 0.87 [0.85 to 0.89] | <0.001 | 0.91 [0.90 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 0.72 [0.65 to 0.81] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | <0.001 | - | - | 1.01 [1.01 to 1.02] | <0.001 | |
Level | 1.03 [0.97 to 1.10] | 0.330 | 1.05 [0.98 to 1.12] | 0.143 | 1.15 [1.00 to 1.32] | 0.045 | 1.10 [0.96 to 1.27] | 0.152 | |
Post-slope | 0.92 [0.90 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 0.88 [0.86 to 0.90] | <0.001 | 0.95 [0.90 to 1.00] | 0.032 | 0.82 [0.75 to 0.89] | <0.001 | |
Male only subgroup (n=82,270; 18,042) |
Year | 0.92 [0.91 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 0.84 [0.81 to 0.87] | <0.001 | 0.92 [0.91 to 0.94] | <0.001 | 0.67 [0.57 to 0.79] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.01 [1.00 to 1.01] | <0.001 | - | - | 1.02 [1.01 to 1.03] | <0.001 | |
Level | 1.08 [0.99 to 1.18] | 0.090 | 1.11 [1.01 to 1.22] | 0.024 | 1.33 [1.10 to 1.62] | 0.004 | 1.27 [1.04 to 1.54] | 0.020 | |
Post-slope | 0.91 [0.89 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 0.84 [0.81 to 0.88] | <0.001 | 0.88 [0.82 to 0.94] | <0.001 | 0.71 [0.62 to 0.81] | <0.001 | |
Female only subgroup (n=82,929; 17,848) |
Year | 0.90 [0.90 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 0.89 [0.86 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 0.90 [0.88 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 0.77 [0.66 to 0.91] | 0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | 0.492 | - | - | 1.01 [1.00 to 1.02] | 0.067 | |
Level | 0.98 [0.90 to 1.07] | 0.718 | 0.99 [0.90 to 1.08] | 0.789 | 1.02 [0.84 to 1.22] | 0.866 | 0.99 [0.82 to 1.19] | 0.917 | |
Post-slope | 0.93 [0.91 to 0.95] | <0.001 | 0.92 [0.88 to 0.95] | <0.001 | 1.01 [0.95 to 1.08] | 0.720 | 0.92 [0.81 to 1.04] | 0.180 | |
13 year olds only (n=85,713; 18,721) |
Year | 0.90 [0.89 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 0.89 [0.86 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 0.90 [0.89 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 0.78 [0.65 to 0.94] | 0.007 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.00] | 0.459 | - | - | 1.01 [1.00 to 1.02] | 0.107 | |
Level | 0.95 [0.86 to 1.05] | 0.321 | 0.95 [0.87 to 1.05] | 0.349 | 1.22 [0.97 to 1.53] | 0.086 | 1.19 [0.94 to 1.49] | 0.145 | |
Post-slope | 0.94 [0.92 to 0.97] | <0.001 | 0.93 [0.90 to 0.97] | <0.001 | 0.93 [0.85 to 1.01] | 0.092 | 0.84 [0.73 to 0.98] | 0.022 | |
15 year olds only (n=79,486; 17,169) |
Year | 0.93 [0.92 to 0.94] | <0.001 | 0.86 [0.83 to 0.89] | <0.001 | 0.91 [0.90 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 0.70 [0.60 to 0.80] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 1.00 [1.00 to 1.01] | <0.001 | - | - | 1.01 [1.01 to 1.02] | <0.001 | |
Level | 1.09 [1.01 to 1.19] | 0.034 | 1.13 [1.04 to 1.23] | 0.006 | 1.11 [0.94 to 1.32] | 0.218 | 1.06 [0.90 to 1.26] | 0.473 | |
Post-slope | 0.89 [0.87 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 0.83 [0.80 to 0.86] | <0.001 | 0.96 [0.90 to 1.02] | 0.144 | 0.80 [0.71 to 0.89] | <0.001 |
available for England and Scotland only
available for England only
As indicated by the final quadratic models in Table 1, for the whole sample, change in the rate of decline for ever smoking post-2010 was not significant, though a marginally significant (p = 0.03) slowing in the rate of decline occurred for regular smoking. For subgroup analyses, the slowing decline in regular smoking post-2010 was limited to groups for whom rates had declined rapidly prior to 2010 (i.e. females and 13 year olds, see Figure 1). Similarly, there was a significant slowing in the rate of decline post-2010 among these subgroups for ever smoked; though a significant increase in the rate of decline for 15 year olds (see figure 1 supplementary material). For smoking attitudes, there was consistent evidence across all subgroups of an increased rate of decline in the percentage of young people saying that trying smoking is “OK” and weekly smoking is “OK”, except for subgroup analyses of attitudes of smoking weekly for females (see Figure 2 and Table 2).
For ever and regular smoking, there was a significant reduction in prevalence at the intervention point (referred to as ‘level’ in the table, and from here on) for the whole sample and all subgroups. Changes in trend were robust to pre-specified sensitivity analyses, although some England-only models differed slightly from the whole group models (see supplementary Tables 15 and 16). In post-hoc sensitivity analyses modelling trends from 2001 to account for non-linearity (not shown in tables), the statistical evidence for a change in trend for regular smoking weakened (OR = 0.99; CI = 0.94 to 1.03), and magnitude for change in trend lowered for ever cannabis use (OR = 1.05; CI = 1.01 to 1.09) and alcohol use (OR = 1.05; CI = 1.02 to 1.08) though remaining significant, although other findings were not materially altered. For sensitivity analyses of smoking attitudes (trying smoking is “OK”) not including years 1999 and 2001, no differences in results were found for change in decline (OR = 0.85; CI = 0.81 – 0.88; p < 0.001); although level became significant (OR = 1.08; CI = 1.01 – 1.16; p = 0.030).
Inclusion of time*gender interaction terms showed that for all outcomes (with the exception of alcohol use) changes in the secular decline over time was significantly greater for females than for males (see supplementary Table 20). For gender, there was no significant effect modification for level or post-slope terms, except for attitudes towards smoking weekly where the rate of decline increased at a significantly faster rate for males than for females (though with a significant increase in trend for males). Inclusion of time*year group interaction terms showed that for all outcomes (with the exception of attitudes towards smoking weekly), the changes in the secular decline over time was greater for 13 year olds than for 15 year olds. Change in level post-2010 was more negative among 13 year olds students for ever smoked and ever alcohol use. For post-slope terms, declines in prevalence for ever smoked, regular smoking and positive attitudes towards trying smoking were greater for 15 year olds than 13 year olds.
Decreases in rates of decline post-2010 were observed for alcohol use and cannabis and in greater magnitude than change in regular smoking (OR = 1.17; CI = 1.14 to 1.19 and OR = 1.21, CI = 1.18 to 1.25, respectively; see Table 3). These were generally consistent across all subgroups (see Table 19 as well as Figures 5-6 in supplementary material).
Table 3. Odds ratios of linear and quadratic models for ever drunk alcohol and ever cannabis use between 1998-2015 for England, Scotland and Wales.
Ever drunk alcohol | Ever used cannabis | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Linear | p | Quadratic | p | Linear | p | Quadratic | p | ||
Whole sample (n = 239,190; 239,457) |
Year | 0.91 [0.90 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 1.10 [1.08 to 1.12] | <0.001 | 0.92 [0.92 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 1.08 [1.07 to 1.10] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 0.99] | <0.001 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 0.99] | <0.001 | |
Level | 0.90 [0.85 to 0.95] | <0.001 | 0.85 [0.81 to 0.90] | <0.001 | 0.98 [0.90 to 1.07] | 0.661 | 1.01 [0.93 to 1.10] | 0.761 | |
Post-slope | 0.96 [0.94 to 0.97] | <0.001 | 1.17 [1.14 to 1.19] | <0.001 | 1.00 [0.98 to 1.03] | 0.667 | 1.21 [1.18 to 1.25] | <0.001 | |
Male only subgroup (n = 119,989; 120,025) |
Year | 0.91 [0.90 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 1.06 [1.03 to 1.09] | <0.001 | 0.93 [0.92 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 1.06 [1.04 to 1.09] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 0.99] | <0.001 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 0.99] | <0.001 | |
Level | 0.89 [0.82 to 0.96] | 0.004 | 0.85 [0.79 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 0.92 [0.82 to 1.04] | 0.185 | 0.94 [0.84 to 1.06] | 0.337 | |
Post-slope | 0.96 [0.94 to 0.98] | <0.001 | 1.12 [1.09 to 1.16] | <0.001 | 1.02 [0.99 to 1.05] | 0.175 | 1.20 [1.15 to 1.24] | <0.001 | |
Female only subgroup (n = 119,201; 119,432) |
Year | 0.90 [0.90 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 1.15 [1.12 to 1.18] | <0.001 | 0.92 [0.91 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 1.11 [1.08 to 1.14] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 0.99 [0.98 to 0.99] | <0.001 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 0.99] | <0.001 | |
Level | 0.91 [0.84 to 0.98] | 0.016 | 0.85 [0.79 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 1.06 [0.93 to 1.20] | 0.396 | 1.11 [0.98 to 1.26] | 0.110 | |
Post-slope | 0.96 [0.94 to 0.97] | <0.001 | 1.22 [1.18 to 1.26] | <0.001 | 0.99 [0.95 to 1.02] | 0.359 | 1.23 [1.17 to 1.28] | <0.001 | |
13 year olds only (n = 124,842; 123,608) |
Year | 0.90 [0.90 to 0.90] | <0.001 | 1.11 [1.08 to 1.13] | <0.001 | 0.90 [0.89 to 0.90] | <0.001 | 1.16 [1.12 to 1.21] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 0.99] | <0.001 | - | - | 0.98 [0.98 to 0.99] | <0.001 | |
Level | 0.88 [0.82 to 0.95] | 0.001 | 0.84 [0.78 to 0.91] | <0.001 | 1.07 [0.89 to 1.28] | 0.480 | 1.14 [0.95 to 1.37] | 0.165 | |
Post-slope | 0.95 [0.93 to 0.97] | <0.001 | 1.17 [1.14 to 1.21] | <0.001 | 0.99 [0.95 to 1.04] | 0.746 | 1.34 [1.26 to 1.42] | <0.001 | |
15 year olds only (n = 114,348; 115,849) |
Year | 0.92 [0.91 to 0.92] | <0.001 | 1.08 [1.05 to 1.11] | <0.001 | 0.93 [0.92 to 0.93] | <0.001 | 1.06 [1.04 to 1.08] | <0.001 |
Year2 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 0.99] | <0.001 | - | - | 0.99 [0.99 to 0.99] | <0.001 | |
Level | 0.92 [0.84 to 1.00] | 0.049 | 0.87 [0.80 to 0.95] | 0.002 | 0.96 [0.87 to 1.06] | 0.432 | 0.99 [0.90 to 1.09] | 0.776 | |
Post-slope | 0.96 [0.94 to 0.98] | <0.001 | 1.13 [1.09 to 1.17] | <0.001 | 1.01 [0.98 to 1.03] | 0.645 | 1.17 [1.13 to 1.20] | <0.001 |
Discussion
The current study is the first to test whether proliferation of e-cigarettes during a period of limited regulation led to changes in smoking trajectories as well as smoking attitudes among young people. Our results provide little evidence that renormalisation of smoking occurred during this period. The rate of decline for ever smoking prevalence did not slow. While decreases for regular smoking did slow, this was specific to groups where the level of decline prior to 2010 was greatest, possibly reflecting a floor effect in the data. Slowing declines were also found, to a greater magnitude, for cannabis and alcohol use, suggesting change in trend was not unique to tobacco use, but reflected wider changes in youth substance use trajectories. What is more, positive perceptions of smoking attitudes declined at a faster rate following the proliferation of e-cigarettes, suggesting that attitudes towards smoking hardened while e-cigarettes were emerging rather than softening, as would be expected were smoking becoming renormalised. These findings are consistent with a previous study in the US that found little change in smoking trends among adolescents during a period of growth in e-cigarette use (27). Our study is, however, unique in that it is the first to test these changes in the UK population, and to understand them in the context of broader substance use trajectories. It is the first internationally to test the renormalisation hypothesis by examining changes in trends for youth attitudes toward smoking. Although it is unclear to what extent our findings can generalise to other countries, the UK is often referred to as a country comparable to the US with regards to the tobacco epidemic (23).
This study benefits from the use of a large, nationally representative sample of school-age children from England, Scotland and Wales, covering a long time period (17 years). It also benefited from investigating smoking attitudes, contributing to understanding underlying theoretical mechanisms of renormalisation hypothesis, and locating changes in smoking within the context of wider youth substance use trajectories to assess whether or not findings were unique to smoking outcomes. Nevertheless, it does suffer some substantial limitations. Survey intervals and methods used varied. While all surveys used two-stage cluster sampling, recruiting schools and then pupils, the absence of school identifiers within some datasets precluded adjustment for clustering. Smoking typically exhibits a moderate to high degree of intra-cluster correlation (52). Hence, adjusting for clustering would likely have led to change in trends, such as that for smoking regularly where significance was borderline (p=0.03), becoming non-significant. It would likely have had less of an impact on results for smoking attitudes, which had p values typically below 0.001. Robust country-specific analyses were only possible for England as this country provided the most frequently occurring data points before and after the intervention time point. Stratification by SES based on free school meal entitlement was only possible for England and Scotland data, as survey data from Wales did not contain an equivalent indicator of SES, and findings from these subgroups are presented with caution. Events other than the increased use of e-cigarettes might have contributed to the increased decline in positive smoking attitudes observed in the current study, and causality cannot be asserted. The fact that estimates are available only on an annual or biennial basis limit our ability to understand covariance between e-cigarettes and tobacco use over time.
Nevertheless, the study has important implications. It demonstrates the success of public health efforts in reducing smoking among young people. With the average prevalence levels of ever smokers having decreased by nearly 40 percentage points for adolescents within two decades, it is no surprise many fear a reversal in this progress. However, given the limited evidence for the renormalisation of youth smoking, it is perhaps unhelpful for policy on e-cigarette regulation to be justified on the sole basis that they renormalise smoking (20). Some evidence from animal models suggests that nicotine use during adolescence can inhibit brain development. Because of this, use of e-cigarettes among young people has been described as a potential concern in its own right. While evidence to date suggests that regular use among non-smokers is rare (17), continued conflation with the normalisation of tobacco may be an unhelpful distraction from the need to consider whether youth e-cigarette use does become a potential problem in isolation from its links to tobacco.
Understanding young people’s perceptions of e-cigarettes, the ways in which they are viewed as similar or different to cigarettes, and how these vary according to regulatory frameworks, is an important direction for future research. It remains to be seen whether trajectories of e-cigarette use, smoking and smoking attitudes will change (positively or negatively) as a result of increased e-cigarette regulations such as the marketing restrictions and product labelling brought in by the European Union Tobacco Products Directive. Within the UK, while regulatory frameworks have to date been similar, Welsh and Scottish Governments (53) have pursued (but not yet implemented) more restrictive regulatory frameworks than in England (54). Wales was the only country whose government attempted (unsuccessfully) to ban vaping in public places, while Scotland are considering further restrictions on marketing of e-cigarettes. Future research focusing on how divergences in policy impact young people’s use of and attitudes toward tobacco, and e-cigarettes, would further enhance our understandings of these issues.
While this policy landscape is shifting, so are the products themselves. E-cigarettes have been described as mimicking behavioural aspects of smoking; as discussed, the renormalisation hypothesis is premised on the assumption that cigarettes and e-cigarettes are viewed as similar to each other (23). However, e-cigarettes have changed substantially over time and now resemble traditional cigarettes less than early “cig-a-like” models, which may decrease perceived similarity. Saebo and Scheffels (23) state that the normalisation of e-cigarettes can occur during the simultaneous continued de-normalisation of cigarette use, and appears to be reflected in findings here. However, newer products entering the market have been described by some as showing particular popularity among young people in the US (55). Hence, while neither widespread regular youth vaping, nor the renormalisation of smoking, appear to have occurred during the period investigated here, ongoing monitoring of young people’s e-cigarette use, and links to smoking, remains a public health priority.
Supplementary Material
What is already known about this topic
E-cigarette experimentation is increasing among young people who have not previously used tobacco, leading to fears that e-cigarettes may renormalise smoking.
However, e-cigarette experimentation is not translating into regular e-cigarette use and smoking rates among young people continue to fall.
It has not been tested whether the proliferation of e-cigarettes has renormalised, or displaced, smoking behaviour and smoking attitudes among young people.
What this study adds
While the rate of decline for regular smoking did marginally slow between 2011-2015, this was also found for cannabis and alcohol use. Furthermore, the decline in the perceived acceptability of smoking behaviour accelerated during this period.
Our findings do not support the hypothesis that e-cigarettes renormalised youth smoking during a period of growing but largely unregulated use in the UK.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Chris Roberts for supplying the HBSC data for Wales.
Funding: This work presents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) in England under its Public Health Research Board (grant number 16/57/01). The views expressed in this presentation are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the National Health Service (NHS), the NIHR or the Department of Health for England. The work was also undertaken with the support of The Centre for the Development and Evaluation of Complex Interventions for Public Health Improvement (DECIPHer), a UKCRC Public Health Research Centre of Excellence. Joint funding (MR/KO232331/1) from the British Heart Foundation, Cancer Research UK, Economic and Social Research Council, Medical Research Council, the Welsh Government and the Wellcome Trust, under the auspices of the UK Clinical Research Collaboration, is gratefully acknowledged. LM and LG acknowledge support from the Medical Research Council and the Chief Scientist Office (MC_UU_12017/13 and MC_UU_12017/14) of the Scottish Government Health Care Directorates (SPHSU13 and SPHSU14). The School Health Research Network is a partnership between DECIPHer at Cardiff University, Welsh Government, Public Health Wales and Cancer Research UK, funded by Health and Care Research Wales via the National Centre for Health and Wellbeing Research.
Footnotes
Competing interest: All authors have completed the ICMJE uniform disclosure form at www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf. LB declares a secondment post with Cancer Research UK and all other authors report no support from any organisation for the submitted work; no financial relationships with any organisations that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous three years; no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.
Transparency declaration: The lead author affirms that the manuscript is an honest, accurate, and transparent account of the study being reported. No important aspects of the study have been omitted and any discrepancies from the study as originally planned have been explained.
Data Sharing: Detailed information and access to data for the Smoking Drinking and Drug Use Among Young People in England Survey and the Scottish Adolescent Lifestyle and Substance Use Survey, are available from the UK data archive: http://www.data-archive.ac.uk
Ethical Approval: Ethical approval was provided by the School of Social Sciences Research Ethics Committee at Cardiff University (SREC/2188).
References
- 1.West R, Beard E, Brown J. Trends in electronic cigarette use in England. Smoking Toolkit Study: University College London; 2018. Oct 04, 2018. Report No.: Contract No.: STS140122. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bullen C, Howe C, Laugesen M, McRobbie H, Parag V, Williman J, et al. Electronic cigarettes for smoking cessation: a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet. 2013;382(9905):1629–37. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61842-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.West R, Shahab L, Brown J. Estimating the population impact of e-cigarettes on smoking cessation in England. Addiction. 2016;111(6):1118–9. doi: 10.1111/add.13343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kalkhoran S, Glantz SA. E-cigarettes and smoking cessation in real-world and clinical settings: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Respir Med. 2016;4:116–128. doi: 10.1016/S2213-2600(15)00521-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Beard E, West R, Michie S, Brown J. Association between electronic cigarette use and changes in quit attempts, success of quit attempts, use of smoking cessation pharmacotherapy, and use of stop smoking services in England: time series analysis of population trends. BMJ. 2016 Sep 13;354:i4645. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i4645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Abrams D, Axtell B, Bartsch P, Bauld L, Borland R, Castonguay A. Statement from specialists in nicotine science and public health policy. 2014 [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bauld L, McNeill A, Hajek P, Britton J, Dockrell M. E-cigarette use in public places: striking the right balance. Tob Control. 2017a;26(e1):e5–e6. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2016-053357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.McNeill A, Brose L, Calder R, Bauld L, Robson D. Evidence review of e-cigarettes and heated tobacco products 2018. A report commissioned by Public Health England. London: Public Health England; 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Fairchild AL, Lee JS, Bayer R, Curran J. E-Cigarettes and the Harm-Reduction Continuum. The N Eng J Med. 2018;378(3):216. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1711991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Douglas H, Hall W, Gartner C. E-cigarettes and the law in Australia. Aust Fam Physician. 2015;44(6):415. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Parliament of the Commonwealth of Australia. Report on the Inquiry into the Use and Marketing of Electronic Cigarettes and Personal Vaporisers in Australia. Canberra: House of Representatives Standing Committee on Health, Aged Care and Sport; 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 12.US Department of Health and Human Services. E-cigarette use among youth and young adults: A report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health; 2016a. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Office for National Statistics. Adult smoking habits in the UK 2017. 2018 Jan 26; Available from: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/healthandsocialcare/healthandlifeexpectancies/bulletins/adultsmokinghabitsingreatbritain/2017/pdf.
- 14.Bialous SA, Sarma L. Electronic cigarettes and smoking cessation: a quandary? The Lancet. 2014;383(9915):407–8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60143-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dutra LM, Glantz SA. Electronic cigarettes and conventional cigarette use among US adolescents: a cross-sectional study. JAMA pediatrics. 2014;168(7):610–7. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.5488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Grana RA, Ling PM, Benowitz N, Glantz S. Electronic cigarettes. Circulation. 2014;129(19):e490–e2. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.114.008545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bauld L, MacKintosh AM, Eastwood B, Ford A, Moore G, Dockrell M, et al. Young people’s use of e-cigarettes across the United Kingdom: findings from five surveys 2015–2017. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017b;14(9):973. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14090973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Collins LK, Villanti AC, Pearson JL, Glasser AM, Johnson AL, Niaura RS, et al. Frequency of youth e-cigarette, tobacco, and poly-use in the United States, 2015: update to Villanti et al.,“frequency of Youth e-cigarette and tobacco use patterns in the United States: measurement precision is critical to inform public health”. Nicotine Tob Res. 2017;19(10):1253–4. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntx073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hsu R, Myers AE, Ribisl KM, Marteau TM. An observational study of retail availability and in-store marketing of e-cigarettes in London: potential to undermine recent tobacco control gains? BMJ open. 2013;3(12):e004085. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-004085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Official Journal of the European Union. The Tobacco Products Directive: Official Journal of the European Union. 2014 Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/health/sites/health/files/tobacco/docs/dir_201440_en.pdf.
- 21.Care DoHaS, editor. Australian Government. Standing Committee on Health, Aged Care and Sport - Inquiry into the Use and Marketing of Electronic Cigarettes and Personal Vaporisers in Australia. 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Jarvis MJ, Sims M, Gilmore A, Mindell J. Impact of smoke-free legislation on children's exposure to secondhand smoke: cotinine data from the Health Survey for England. Tob Control. 2011 doi: 10.1136/tc.2010.041608. tc. 2010.041608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sæbø G, Scheffels J. Assessing notions of denormalization and renormalization of smoking in light of e-cigarette regulation. Int J Drug Policy. 2017;49:58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2017.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.McNeill A, Brose LS, Calder R, Hitchman SC, Hajek P, McRobbie H. E-cigarettes: an evidence update. London: Public Health England; 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Centre for Disease Control and Prevention Newsroom. E-cigarette use triples among middle and high school students in just one year 2015. 18 May 23; Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/media/releases/2015/p0416-E-cigarette-use.html.
- 26.Moore G, Hewitt G, Evans J, Littlecott HJ, Holliday J, Ahmed N, et al. Electronic-cigarette use among young people in Wales: evidence from two cross-sectional surveys. BMJ open. 2015;5(4):e007072. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-007072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.de Lacy E, Fletcher A, Hewitt G, Murphy S, Moore G. Cross-sectional study examining the prevalence, correlates and sequencing of electronic cigarette and tobacco use among 11–16-year olds in schools in Wales. BMJ open. 2017;7(2):e012784. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-012784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Levy DT, Warner KE, Cummings KM, et al. Examining the relationship of vaping to smoking initiation among US youth and young adults: a reality check. Tobacco Control. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2018-054446. Published Online First: 20 November 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dutra LM, Glantz SA. E-cigarettes and national adolescent cigarette use: 2004–2014. Pediatrics. 2017:e20162450. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2000. UK Data Service; 2002. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2001. UK Data Service; 2003. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2002. 2nd Edition. UK Data Service; 2004. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2003. UK Data Service; 2005. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2004. UK Data Service; 2005. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2005. UK Data Service; 2007. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2006. 2008. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2007. UK Data Service; 2008. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2008. UK Data Service; 2009. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2009. UK Data Service; 2010. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2010. UK Data Service; 2011. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2011. UK Data Service; 2012. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.National Centre for Social Research, National Foundation for Educational Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2012. UK Data Service; 2013. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.National Centre for Social Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2013. UK Data Service; 2015. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.NatCen Social Research. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young People, 2014. UK Data Service; 2015. [01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Office for National Statistics. Social Survey Division: Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young Teenagers, 1998. UK Data Service; 2000. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Office for National Statistics: Social Survey Division. Smoking, Drinking and Drug Use among Young Teenagers, 1999. UK Data Service; 2001. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ipsos MORI Scotland. Scottish Schools Adolescent Lifestyle and Substance Use Survey Trend Data, 1990-2013. UK Data Service; 2015. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ipsos MORI Scotland. Scottish Schools Adolescent Lifestyle and Substance Use Survey, 2015. UK Data Service; 2017. [Accessed 01 June 2018]. [data collection] [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Currie C, Molcho M, Boyce W, Holstein B, Torsheim T, Richter M. Researching health inequalities in adolescents: the development of the Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children (HBSC) family affluence scale. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(6):1429–36. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Scholes S, Mindell J. Health Survey for England 2015 Children’s smoking and exposure to other people’s smoke. 2016 [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hawe P, Shiell A, Riley T. Theorising interventions as events in systems. American journal of community psychology. 2009 Jun 1;43(3–4):267–76. doi: 10.1007/s10464-009-9229-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Shackleton N, Hale D, Bonell C, Viner RM. Intraclass correlation values for adolescent health outcomes in secondary schools in 21 European countries. SSM-population health. 2016 Dec 1;2:217–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2016.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.HM Government. Health (Tobacco, Nicotine etc. and Care) (Scotland) Act 2016. 2018 May 22; Available from: http://www.legislation.gov.uk/asp/2016/14/introduction/enacted.
- 54.Department of Health and Social Care. Towards a smoke-free generation: a tobacco control plan for England. London: Department of Health; 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Huang J, Duan Z, Kwok J, et al. Vaping versus JUULing: how the extraordinary growth and marketing of JUUL transformed the US retail e-cigarette market. Tobacco Control. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2018-054382. Published Online First: 31 May 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.