Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jan 8.
Published in final edited form as: J Neurosci. 2009 Jul 8;29(27):8774–8783. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0115-09.2009

Minute effects of sex on the aging brain: a multi-sample MRI-study of healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease

Anders M Fjell 1,2,*, Lars T Westlye 1, Inge Amlien 1, Thomas Espeseth 1, Ivar Reinvang 1, Naftali Raz 3, Ingrid Agartz 4,5,6, David H Salat 7, Doug N Greve 7, Bruce Fischl 7,8, Anders M Dale 9,10,11, Kristine B Walhovd 1,2
PMCID: PMC2782778  NIHMSID: NIHMS133612  PMID: 19587284

Abstract

Age is associated with substantial macro-structural brain changes. While some recent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have reported larger age-effects in men than women, others find no sex differences. As brain morphometry is a potentially important tool in diagnosis and monitoring of age-related neurological diseases, e.g. Alzheimer’s disease (AD), it is important to know whether sex influences brain aging. We analyzed cross-sectional MR scans from 1143 healthy participants from seven subsamples provided by four independent research groups. In addition, 96 patients with mild AD were included. Estimates of cortical thickness continuously across the brain surface, as well as volume of 17 subcortical structures, were obtained by use of automated segmentation tools (FreeSurfer). In the healthy participants, no differences in aging slopes between women and men were found in any part of the cortex. Pallidum corrected for intracranial volume showed slightly higher age correlations for men. The analyses were repeated in each of the seven sub-samples, and the lack of age × sex interactions was largely replicated. Analyses of the AD sample showed no interactions between sex and age for any brain region. It is concluded that sex has negligible effects on the age-slope of brain volumes both in healthy participants and in AD.

Keywords: MRI, aging, sex, cerebral cortex, hippocampus, FreeSurfer

Introduction

More than 50 magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies of sex effects on brain structures have been published, partly motivated by observations of small, but consistent, sex differences in cognitive abilities (Halpern and Tan, 2001; Jones et al., 2003). More importantly, as brain morphometry emerges as a valuable tool in prediction and diagnosis of age-related neurological disorders, e.g. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Colliot et al., 2008; Dubois et al., 2007; Fischl et al., 2002), it is increasingly important to understand the factors that influence volumetric changes with increasing age. Although much is known about the general pattern of age effects on brain morphometry from cross-sectional studies (Courchesne et al., 2000; Fjell et al., in press; Fotenos et al., 2005; Jernigan et al., 2001; Raz et al., 2004a; Raz et al., 2007; Salat et al., 2004; Walhovd et al., 2005a; Walhovd et al., 2005b), the role of sex in brain aging is still controversial. Although extremely valuable longitudinal studies of aging exist (Raz et al., 2004b; Resnick et al., 2003), the age-spans sampled are small, and the present paper will focus on cross-sectional studies. Some studies have found more age-related regional volume differences in men than in women (Chung et al., 2006; Coffey et al., 1998; Cowell et al., 1994; Good et al., 2001; Gur et al., 2002b; Murphy et al., 1996; Nunnemann et al., 2007; Pruessner et al., 2001; Raz et al., 2004a; Resnick et al., 2000; Riello et al., 2005; Sowell et al., 2007; Xu et al., 2000), while others have found no sex differences (Greenberg et al., 2008; Lemaitre et al., 2005; Salat et al., 2004; Sowell et al., 2007), or a heterogeneous pattern across different brain structures (Cowell et al., 2007; Gunning-Dixon et al., 1998; Gur et al., 2002b; Murphy et al., 1996; Pruessner et al., 2001). The inconsistencies may be caused by differences in sample characteristics, scan quality, segmentation approach or statistical procedures. Limited attempts to replicate the pattern of sex difference in regional brain aging in samples drawn from the same population using the same equipment were unsuccessful (Raz et al., 2004a; Raz et al., 1997). We tested whether age affects the brains of women and men differentially by addressing these sources of variation. 1143 healthy participants from seven cross-sectional life-span samples were included in the combined sample. In addition, 96 AD patients were included in a separate sample. This made it possible to account for sample-and scan-related factors that could influence the results. It was tested how sex influences the magnitude and direction of age effects on cortical thickness and the volumes of 17 different subcortical brain structures. No previous studies have tested effects of sex in healthy aging or AD on such a large number of subcortical and cortical structures simultaneously.

Methods

Samples

Age-correlations across six of the seven healthy samples (n = 883) have been reported elsewhere (Fjell et al., in press), without analyses of sex effects. The details of each of the subsamples are described in Table 1, as well as in a previous publication from the project (Fjell et al., in press). The current sample consisted of 1143 participants (676 women, 467 men), with an age range of 76 years (18-94 years, mean = 46.8, SD = 19.2). All the healthy samples were well screened for diseases and history of neurological conditions, and for cognitive deficits/ dementia by standardized tests. 96 AD patients (59 women/ 37 men) were included from the Open Access Series of Imaging Studies (OASIS) database (www.oasis-brains.org), with an age range of 34 years (62-96 years, mean = 76.6, SD = 7.1). Details of recruitment and screening procedures for the AD group are given by Marcus and colleagues (Marcus et al., 2007). AD participants underwent the Washington University Alzheimer Disease Research Center’s full clinical assessment, yielding clinical dementia rating (CDR) (Berg, 1984, 1988; Morris, 1993; Morris et al., 2001). CDR of 0.5 or higher was taken to indicate mild or moderate AD.

Table 1.

Sample characteristics

Sample Country N (% f) Age mean (range) Education mean (range) Key publications Main screening instruments/inclusion criteria
1 Nor 69 (57) 51.3 (20-88) 15 (7-20) (Walhovd et al., 2005a) Health interview, MMSE > 26, BDI < 16, IQ > 85, RH only
2 Nor 208 (71) 46.8 (19-75) 14 (9-22) (Espeseth et al., 2008) Health interview, IQ > 85
3 Swe 106 (32) 41.6 (19-56) 14 (9-22) (Nesvag et al., 2008) Health interview, DSM-III-R, WASI vocabulary > 16a
4 USA 155 (65) 44.5 (18-93) 3.5 (1-5)c (Marcus et al., 2007) Health interview, CDR = 0b, MMSE > 25b, RH only
5 USA 154 (61) 44.4 (18-94) 3.4 (1-5)c Similar to Sample 4 Similar to Sample 4
6 USA 191 (60) 47.3 (18-81) 15.7 (12-21) (Raz et al., 2004a) Health interview, BIMCT > 30, GDQ < 15, RH only, neuroradiology,
7 Nor 260 (57) 48.8 (20-93) 15.5e (8-23) (Fjell et al., 2008b) Health interview, Neuropsychological evaluation, BDI < 16, IQ > 85, RH only
8d USA 96 (59) 76.6 (62-96) 2.8 (1-5) c Similar to Sample 4 Health interview, CDR ≥ 0.5, RH only

Nor: Norway

Swe: Sweden

% f: percentage of female participants

MMSE: Mini Mental Status Exam (Folstein et al. 1975)

BDI: Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, 1987)

BIMCT: Blessed Information-Memory-Concentration Test (Blessed et al. 1968)

CDR: Clinical Dementia Rating (Berg, 1984, 1988; Morris, 1993)

GDQ: Geriatric Depression Questionnaire (Auer and Reisberg, 1997)

RH: Right handed

WASI: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999)

a

Available for 70 participants

b

Available for participants ≥ 60 years only

c

Available for all participants ≥ 60 years, and sporadically for the rest. 1: less than high school grad., 2: high school grad., 3: some college, 4: college grad., 5: beyond college

d

Alzheimer patients

e

Missing for one participant

MR acquisition

All scans were obtained from 1.5T magnets from two different manufacturers (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany; General Electric CO. [GE], Milwaukee, WI), and from five different models (Siemens Avanto, Symphony, Sonata, Vision/ GE Signa). All participants within each sample were scanned on the same scanner. T1 weighted sequences were acquired (3D magnetization prepared gradient-echo for Siemens (MPRAGE)/ 3D spoiled gradient recalled pulse sequences (SPGR) for GE). In five of the samples (samples 1, 2, 4, 5, and 7) multiple scans were acquired within the same scanning session and averaged to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. The details of the sequences are presented in Table 2.

Table 2.

MRI parameters

Sample MRI scanner MRI protocol
Sample 1 Siemens Symphony Quantum Two 3D magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) T1-weighted sequences
TR/TE/TI/FA= 2730 ms/4 ms/1000 ms/7°
Matrix = 192×256
Scan time: 8.5 min per volume.
Each volume consisted of 128 sagittal slices (1.33×1×1 mm).
Sample 2 Siemens Sonata Two 3D magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) T1-weighted sequences
TR/TE/TI/FA= 2730 ms/ 3.43 ms/ 1000 ms/ 7°
Matrix: 256×256
Scan time: 8 min and 46 s per volume
Each volume consisted of 128 sagittal slices (1.33×1×1 mm)
Sample 3 General Electric Signa One 3D spoiled gradient recalled (SPGR) pulse T1-weighted sequence
TR/TE/FA=24 ms/ 6.0 ms/ 35°, number of excitations were 2
Matrix: 256×192
Each volume consisted of 1.5 mm coronal slices, no gap, FOV = 24 cm
Sample 4, 5 and 8 Siemens Vision 3–4 individual T1-weighted magnetization
prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) T1-weighted sequences
TR/TE/TI/FA = 9.7 ms/4.0 ms/20 ms/10°
Matrix = 256×256.
Each volume consisted of 128 sagittal slices (1.25×1×1 mm).
Sample 6 General Electric Signa One 3D spoiled gradient recalled (SPGR) pulse T1-weighted sequence
TR/TE/FA =24 ms/ 5.0 ms/ 30°
Matrix = 256×192
Each volume consisted of124 contiguous axial slices (1.30×.94×.86 mm), FOV = 22 cm
Sample 7 Siemens Avanto Two 3D magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) T1-weighted sequences
TR/TE/TI/FA= 2400 ms/ 3.61 ms/ 1000 ms/ 8°
Matrix: 192×192
Scan time: 7 min and 42 s per volume
Each volume consisted of 160 sagittal slices (1.25×1.25×1.20 mm)

FOV: Field of view

FA: Flip angle

TR: Repetition time

TE: Echo time

TI: Inversion time

MRI analysis

All datasets were processed and analysed with FreeSurfer 4.01 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) at the Neuroimaging Analysis Lab, Center for the Study of Human Cognition, Universitu of Oslo, with the additional use of computing resources from the ~4000 CPUs titan grid cluster (http://hpc.uio.no) run by the Research Computing Services Group at USIT, University of Oslo. The automated procedure for volumetric measures of the different brain structures is described in detail by Fischl and colleagues (Fischl et al., 2002; Fischl et al., 2004). A label was automatically assigned to each voxel in the MRI volume based on probabilistic information automatically estimated from a manually labelled training set. The training set included both healthy persons in the age range 18-87 years and a group of AD patients in the age range 60-87 years, and the classification technique employed a registration procedure that is robust to anatomical variability, including the ventricular enlargement typically associated with aging. The technique has been shown to be comparable in accuracy to manual labelling (Fischl et al., 2002; Fischl et al., 2004). We also applied a newly developed atlas normalization procedure, which has been shown to increase the robustness and accuracy of the segmentations across scanner platforms (Han and Fischl, 2007). Two limitations must be mentioned: The segmentation labelled ‘CSF’ in FreeSurfer represents CSF in an area superior to the brainstem, inferior to the fornix, and posterior to the 3rd ventricle. An example of the segmentation is presented in Supplementary Figure 1. However, it is important to note that this is less validated than the other CSF measures (ventricles), and that strong conclusions should thus not be drawn regarding CSF based on this measure. The measure is included in the whole-brain segmentation (the FreeSurfer label ‘CSF’), and we therefore report results for it. Second, the cortical volume estimates from the whole-brain segmentation approach are probably less accurate than the surface-based thickness calculations. However, to ease comparison with the subcortical volumes, the whole-brain procedure was used for estimating all volume (but not thickness) data. Intracranial volume (ICV) was calculated by use of an atlas normalization procedure described by Buckner and colleagues (Buckner et al., 2004).

Cortical thickness was obtained by reconstructing representations of the gray/white matter boundary (Dale et al., 1999; Dale, 1993) and the cortical surface, and the distance between these surfaces at each point across the cortical mantle was calculated. This method uses both intensity and continuity information from the entire 3D MR volume in segmentation and deformation procedures to construct representations of cortical thickness. The maps were created using spatial intensity gradients across tissue classes and are therefore not simply reliant on absolute signal intensity. The maps are not restricted to the voxel resolution of the original data and are thus capable of detecting submillimeter differences between groups (Fischl and Dale, 2000). This has been validated using MR and histology (Kuperberg et al., 2003; Rosas et al., 2002), and the results are not biased across scanner platforms (Han and Fischl, 2007; Han et al., 2006). Maps were smoothed using a circularly symmetric Gaussian kernel across the surface with a fullwidth at half maximum (FWHM) of 15 mm and averaged across participants using a non-rigid high-dimensional spherical averaging method to align cortical folding patterns (Fischl et al., 1999b). This procedure provides accurate matching of morphologically homologous cortical locations among participants on the basis of each individual’s anatomy while minimizing metric distortion, resulting in a measure of cortical thickness for each person at each point on the reconstructed surface. Statistical comparisons of surface maps were generated by computing a general linear model (GLM) of the effects of each variable on thickness at each vertex, which were mapped on the semi-inflated surface of the average brain of the sample (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999a).

Statistics

Effect of sample was regressed out from all variables prior to statistical analysis. To test main effects of sex and sex × age interactions, general linear models (GLM) were used, with brain structure (17 levels) as the within subjects factor, and sex and age as between subjects factors. The analyses were done on uncorrected and ICV-corrected (residuals) volumes. ICV correction controls for sex differences in body size as well as in brain size at the earliest stages of development when ICV is affected by brain growth. In addition, group comparisons were done with corrections for total brain volume (TBV), adjusting the regional volumes for the differences in brain sinze since previous studies have used this approach. This will not give a good approximation of age effects on the different structures, so these are not reported. TBV was quantified as the sum of all brain structures, not including CSF/ ventricular system.

The GLMs were followed by post hoc paired samples t-tests, and correlations with age for each sex separately. Since Pearson coefficients are not symmetrically distributed around other values than 0, the coefficients from the correlation analyses were z-transformed (Z = ½ ln (1+r / 1-r), where ln is the base-e logarithm, SD = 1/√(n-3)) and compared between women and men by t-tests. To test for non-linear relationships, multiple regressions with age and age2 as simultaneous predictors were run for the ICV-corrected structures for women and men separately. Finally, GLMs were computed to test interactions between sex and age in the AD sample for the 17 volume estimates. Post hoc multiple regressions with age, sex and age × sex were run, with cognitive status (CDR and Mini Mental Status Exam [MMSE] score) as additional predictors to control for possible gender differences in cognitive function.

Results

Subcortical analyses

For the 13 uncorrected hemispheric brain volumes (cerebral cortex, cerebral WM, hippocampus, amygdala, putamen, caudate, pallidum, accumbens, thalamus, lateral ventricles, inferior lateral ventricles, cerebellum cortex, cerebellum WM), ANCOVA with hemisphere × sex × structure (13 volumes) with age and sample as covariates established that sex and hemisphere did not interact (F [1,1139] = .001, p = .973). Thus, the sum of left and right hemisphere was used in the rest of the analyses. The effect of sex on the volume of the different brain structures are shown in Figure 1. A GLM with 17 uncorrected volumes, sex, and age was run. Significant effects of sex (F [1,998] = 290.99, p < 10-56) and age (F [75,998] = 4.65, p < 10-29) were found, in addition to structure × sex (F [8.28, 8263.38] = 8.60, p < 10-11), structure × age (F [621.00, 8263.38] = 8.58, p = .000), and structure × sex × age (F [563.04, 8263.38] = 1.30, p < 10-5) interactions. However, sex and age did not interact significantly (F [68,998] = 1.214, p = .119). Post hoc t-tests for each structure are presented in table 3. Men had significantly larger volume than women for all structures. The largest differences were found for cerebral WM and cortex, cerebellum cortex, amygdala, and thalamus, in addition to TBV.

Figure 1. Volumetric differences between women and men.

Figure 1

The bars illustrate the mean volume for women (red) and men (blue) separately for each of the brain volumes tested. The 95% confidence intervals are indicated. In the upper row, volumes are corrected for the effects of sample by means of regression analyses, and the standardized residuals are shown. In the middle row, the volumes are corrected for intracranial volume (ICV) in addition to sample, and in the final row they are corrected for total brain volume and sample. Men had significantly larger uncorrected volumes for all structures, and for several ICV-corrected structures, although the differences were much smaller in the latter cases.

* p < .05

Table 3. Volume differences between women and men.

T-tests of mean differences in volume are presented for the various brain structures included in the present paper. Results are shown for analyses done on the residuals after the effect of sample is regressed out, when the effects of sample and ICV are regressed out, and finally when the effects of sample and total brain volume are regressed out. Negative difference score and t-value indicate larger volume for men and positive indicate larger for women. Men had significantly larger uncorrected volumes for all structures and significantly larger ICV-corrected volumes for 10 structures.

Structure Corrected for sample Corrected for ICV Corrected for sample and TBV
Difference (z) t (p<) Difference (z) t (p<) Difference (z) t (p<)
3rd ventricle -0.455 -7.761 (10-13) -0.189 -3.154 (.01) -0.703 -12.458 (10-32)
4th ventricle -0.354 -5.978 (10-08) -0.054 -0.893 (n.s.) -0.271 -4.540 (10-06)
Accumbens -0.327 -5.506 (10-07) -0.043 -0.722 (n.s.) 0.131 2.190 (.05)
Amygdala -0.838 -15.283 (10-47) -0.253 -4.247 (10-03) -0.083 -1.377 (n.s.)
Brainstem -0.902 -16.731 (10-55) -0.268 -4.490 (10-04) -0.294 -4.948 (10-6)
Caudate -0.586 -10.164 (10-22) 0.007 0.121 (n.s.) -0.034 -0.561 (n.s.)
Cerebellum Cortex -0.889 -16.432 (10-53) -0.363 -6.143 (10-07) -0.169 -2.824 (.005)
Cerebellum WM -0.626 -10.938 (10-25) -0.041 -0.682 (n.s.) -0.014 -0.225 (n.s.)
Cerebral Cortex -0.830 -15.114 (10-46) -0.238 -3.988 (10-03) 0.159 2.658 (.01)
Cerebral WM -0.914 -17.009 (10-57) -0.013 -0.222 (n.s.) -0.093 -1.556 (n.s.)
CSF -0.483 -8.264 (10-15) -0.107 -1.781 (n.s.) -0.472 -8.067 (10-14)
Hippocampus -0.719 -12.785 (10-34) -0.169 -2.821 (.005) 0.125 2.088 (.05)
Inferior lateral vent -0.228 -3.808 (10-02) -0.072 -1.194 (n.s.) -0.565 -9.785 (10-21)
Lateral ventricle -0.315 -5.306 (10-06) -0.016 -0.259 (n.s.) -0.559 -9.665 (10-20)
Pallidum -0.724 -12.872 (10-34) -0.197 -3.292 (.005) -0.123 -2.046 (.05)
Putamen -0.597 -10.386 (10-23) -0.166 -2.765 (.01) 0.040 0.666 (n.s.)
Thalamus -0.820 -14.904 (10-45) -0.124 -2.065 (.05) 0.055 0.922 (n.s.)
Total brain volume -0.988 -18.798 (10-68) -0.216 -3.615 (.001) NA NA

ICV: Intra Cranial Volume

TBV: Total brain volume

Correlations between age and structure, split by sex, are presented in table 4. The difference in coefficient strength was assessed by t-tests of Fisher’s z-transformed coefficients. Only for CSF did the age correlations differ significantly between the sexes (.31 vs. .49 for women and men, respectively). Scatterplots are presented in Figure 2 (cerebrum cortex and WM), Figure 3 (subcortical structures) and Figure 4 (CSF compartments). To test whether the lack of sex × age interactions was caused by reduced sensitivity due to merging of different sub-samples, the GLM was re-run in each of the 7 samples separately. For Sample 2 (F [28, 128] = 2.33, p < .001) and Sample 7 (F [36, 159] = 1.96, p < .05), significant age × sex interactions were found, while this was not seen in the other five samples (p ranging from .21 to 1.00). In sample 2, CSF correlated significantly different with age between the sexes (r = .25 vs .51 in women and men, respectively, z = 1.98, p < .05). In sample 7, cerebellum cortex correlated significantly different (r = -.45 vs. -.63 in women and men, respectively, z = 2.03, p < .05).

Table 4. Age correlations for women and men.

Pearson correlations between age and the various neuroanatomical structures are presented for women and men separately. T-tests of Fisher’s z-transformed coefficients were done, and the z-scores for the pair wise contrasts as well as the significance levels are presented. Results are shown for analyses done on the residuals after the effect of sample is regressed out, and when the effects of sample and ICV are regressed out. The only difference was found for CSF, where the correlations for men were significantly higher than for women.

Structure Corrected for sample Corrected for sample and ICV
Women Men diff (z) Women Men diff (z)
3rd ventricle 0.55 0.61 1.50 0.59 0.63 1.05
4th ventricle 0.11 0.08 0.50 0.15 0.10 0.84
Accumbens -0.62 -0.61 0.27 -0.60 -0.61 0.26
Amygdala -0.58 -0.51 1.65 -0.55 -0.52 0.70
Brainstem -0.19 -0.15 0.68 -0.10 -0.12 0.34
Caudate -0.32 -0.33 0.19 -0.26 -0.35 1.65
Cerebellum Cortex -0.53 -0.53 0.00 -0.48 -0.53 1.11
Cerebellum WM -0.38 -0.33 0.95 -0.33 -0.34 0.19
Cerebral Cortex -0.69 -0.66 0.91 -0.67 -0.69 0.62
Cerebral WM -0.34 -0.25 1.64 -0.31 -0.30 0.18
CSF 0.31 0.46 2.93 0.36 0.50 2.86
Hippocampus -0.53 -0.49 0.90 -0.50 -0.49 0.22
Inferior lateral vent 0.51 0.58 1.65 0.53 0.60 1.71
Lateral ventricle 0.58 0.59 0.25 0.63 0.62 0.27
Pallidum -0.54 -0.56 0.48 -0.49 -0.58 2.09
Putamen -0.71 -0.68 0.96 -0.68 -0.68 0.00
Thalamus -0.60 -0.57 0.76 -0.62 -0.61 0.27
Total brain volume -0.62 -0.55 1.77 -0.67 -0.66 0.30

ICV: Intra Cranial Volume

Bold p < .01 for the difference between the coefficients

Underlined italic p < .05 for the difference between the coefficients

Figure 2. Scatterplots of age effects on cerebrum.

Figure 2

The scatterplots show the individual data points and the aging pattern for women (red) and men (blue) for cerebral cortex and white matter after ICV-corrections of the volumes (the x-axes denote z-scores of the residuals). The numbers above each plot is the R2 for women (red) and men (blue), respectively, including the contributions from both linear and nonlinear terms. In addition, the three-dimensional renderings of several of the structures included in the present paper are shown above the plots, based on the average brain of about 70 participants below 40 years.

Figure 3. Scatterplots of age effects on subcortical structures.

Figure 3

The scatterplots show the individual data points and the aging pattern for women (red) and men) for selected subcortical structures after ICV-corrections of the volumes (the x-axes denote z-scores of the residuals). The numbers above each plot is the R2 for women (red) and men (blue), respectively, including the contributions from both the linear and the nonlinear terms. Above the plots, the three-dimensional renderings of the structures are shown, based on the average brains of about 70 participants below 40 years. Note that these are re-scaled for the figure, and so their size cannot be visually compared. The age effects did not differ between women and men.

Figure 4. Scatterplots of aging patterns on CSF compartments.

Figure 4

The scatterplots show the individual data points and the aging pattern for women (red) and men) for the measured CSF compartments after ICV-corrections of the volumes (the x-axes denote z-scores of the residuals). The numbers above each plot is the R2 for women (red) and men (blue), respectively, including the contributions from both the linear and the nonlinear terms. CSF differed in age-slope between women and men.

1: Lateral ventricles, 2: 3rd ventricle, 3: Inferior lateral ventricles, 4: CSF, 5: 4th ventricle, 6: Brainstem, 7: Cerebral WM, 8: Cerebellum WM, 9: Cerebellum cortex, 10: Cerebellum Cortex, 11: Hippocampus, 12: Pallidum, 13: Amygdala, 14: Thalamus, 15: Accumbens, 16: Total brain volume (TBV), 17: Cerebral cortex, 18: Putamen

The analyses were repeated with ICV-corrected volumes. Main effects of sex (F [1, 998] = 14.22, p < .001) and age (F [75, 998] = 6.59, p < 10-47) were confirmed. In addition, significant structure × sex (F [8.20, 8187.37] = 3.82, p < .001), structure × age (F [615.28, 8187.37] = 7.97, p < .000), and structure × sex × age (F [557.86, 8187.37] = 1.33, p < 10-6) interactions were found. Age and sex did not interact (F [68, 998] = 0.90, p = .71). T-tests showed significantly larger volumes for the 3rd ventricle, amygdala, brainstem, cerebellum cortex, cerebral cortex, hippocampus, pallidum, putamen, and thalamus, in addition to TBV, for men (Table 3). Significant differences in age correlations were observed only for CSF (.36 vs .59 for women and men, respectively) and pallidum (-.49 vs. -.58) (Table 4). The GLM was re-run in each of the 7 samples separately, with no significant age × sex interactions (p’s from .22 to .85). The ANOVA for the total sample was repeated for the participants over 60 years of age, to test whether age × sex interactions could be found in this age span. There were still no significant age × sex interaction (F [25, 247] = 0.65, p = .90). The residual variance for each variable was calculated for each sex separately. An ANOVA showed that the residual variance was highly and positively related to age (F [75,998] = 135.91, p < .0001), but no age × sex interaction was seen (F [68, 998] = 1.22, p = .12).

GLM with TBV-corrected volumes confirmed main effects of sex (F [1, 998] = 53.344, p < 10-12). T-tests showed significantly larger TBV-corrected volumes for women for cerebral cortex and accumbens area, while larger volumes for men were found for the ventricular system (3rd ventricle, 4th ventricle, lateral and inferior lateral ventricles, CSF), brainstem, cerebellum cortex, and pallidum (Table 3).

Effects of introducing a quadratic term were investigated for the ICV-corrected volumes (Table 5). A non-linear fit yielded significantly more explained variance for both women and men for most structures. For pallidum, age2 did not increase amount of explained variance for either of the groups. For cerebral cortex, adding age2 as a predictor increased the amount of explained variance from 45 to 48% (p < 10-6) in women, while the amount of explained variance did not increase significantly for men. The effect of age on cortical volume in women was decreasing with advancing age. For amygdala and cerebellum cortex, increasing effect of age in higher age was seen for men only. Generally, however, the amount of variance explained by age was very similar for men and women, as is illustrated in Figure 5. CSF and pallidum seemed to be more related to age in men than in women, and when the non-linear component was added cerebral white matter also seemed to be more related to age in the sample of men than in the sample of women.

Table 5. R2 from multiple regressions with age and age2 as predictors.

All structures were significantly related to age for both women and men. Bold characters indicate that the contribution from age2 significantly (p < .05) increased the amount of explained variance.

Structure Corrected for sample and ICV
Women Men
3rd ventricle .45 .48
4th ventricle .05 .02
Accumbens .41 .41
Amygdala .30 .29
Brainstem .04 .06
Caudate .21 .16
Cerebellum Cortex .23 .29
Cerebellum WM .12 .16
Cerebral Cortex .48 .47
Cerebral WM .28 .40
CSF .16 .29
Hippocampus .33 .34
Inferior lateral vent .37 .44
Lateral ventricle .48 .46
Pallidum .24 .34
Putamen .51 .50
Thalamus .39 .39
Total brain volume .46 .50

ICV: Intra Cranial Volume

Figure 5. Correlations with age in women and men.

Figure 5

The lines illustrate the Pearson correlations between volume and age for women (red) and men (blue). The values are sorted by the coefficient strength for the raw volumes in women. With the exception of CSF, where men showed higher correlations, none differed significantly between women and men.

Cortical analyses

GLMs were run to test for sex differences in cortical thickness and effects of aging (see Figure 6). When age was regressed out, only minute and scattered effects of sex were seen, going in both directions (yellow-red: women thicker, blue-green: men thicker). When a commonly used criterion for correction for multiple comparisons were used (False Discovery Rate [FDR] < .05), no effects survived. The same was found when comparing the effects of age between women and men. Some scattered areas of steeper age-slope in women than men (yellow-red) were seen, but did not survive correction for FDR. To test whether lack of effects could be due to merging of data from different samples, sex × age interactions were tested for each of the 7 samples independently. No effects survived the FDR < .05 criterion for any of the samples.

Figure 6. Testing of effects of sex on cortical thickness.

Figure 6

The figure shows that no effects of sex on cortical thickness were found when conventional procedures for multiple comparisons (false discovery rate [FDR] < .05) were used. Further, there were no significant differences in the age-slopes of cortical thickness between women and men when the same threshold was used (FDR < .05).

Top panel: The effects of sex on cortical thickness is shown as color-coded p-value maps, and projected onto a semi-inflated template brain. Blue-green indicates thinner cortex for men than women, while red-yellow indicate that women have thicker cortex (p < .05, uncorrected). The analyses were corrected for the effects of age and subsample. The middle panel illustrates that all effects vanish if a conventionally used procedure for correction for multiple comparisons are used (FDR < .05).

Bottom panel: Red-yellow areas indicate that women have more age-related reduction in cortical thickness than men, while blue-green areas indicate the opposite. The analyses are corrected for the effect of subsample. When the results were thresholded at FDR < .05, all effects vanished.

Alzheimer’s disease

ANCOVA with sex, age and 17 uncorrected neuroanatomical volumes, and MMSE and CDR as covariates, yielded an effect of sex (F[1,47]=35.54, p = 10-6, men larger), but no age × sex interaction (F[17,47]=1.58, p = .11). For ICV-corrected volumes, trends were observed for sex (F[1,47]=3.90, p = .054), age (F[28,47]=1.53, p = .10), and sex × age (F[17,47]=1.73, p = .07). Finally, analysis of TBV-corrected volumes yielded no significant results (all p’s > .59). Post hoc multiple regression analyses were run, with each of the 17 structures as dependent variables in turn, and CDR, MMSE, age, sex, and age × sex interaction as predictors. No sex × age interactions were significant.

Discussion

The results from the present large-scale multi-sample study indicate that sex exerts minute and unstable effects on age differences in brain morphometry, both in healthy aging and in AD. Sex did not influence the age-slopes of regional cortical thickness, while there was a small effect of sex on the age-slope of CSF and pallidum in healthy controls when corrected for ICV. Sex and age interacted in predicting brain morphometry uncorrected for ICV in two subsamples, but the effects were not replicable across other subsamples. This fits with the inconsistency of effects seen in previous research. Some find larger age-slopes for men for at least some structures (Chung et al., 2006; Coffey et al., 1998; Cowell et al., 1994; Good et al., 2001; Gur et al., 2002b; Murphy et al., 1996; Nunnemann et al., 2007; Pruessner et al., 2001; Raz et al., 2004a; Resnick et al., 2000; Riello et al., 2005; Sowell et al., 2007; Xu et al., 2000), while others do not observe differences (Greenberg et al., 2008; Lemaitre et al., 2005; Salat et al., 2004; Sowell et al., 2007). The large number of analyses conducted across multiple samples and brain regions increased the susceptibility to Type I errors. This may have contributed to the between-samples inconsistency seen for the uncorrected volumes, i.e. the significant age × sex interactions found for samples 2 and 7. While the effect in sample 2 would have survived a Bonferroni correction with a factor of almost 50, the effect in sample 7 would not survive any correction for multiple comparisons, even though the n was as high as 260.

The sex differences in age-slope for CSF could indicate nonspecific sex effects on global atrophic processes. However, the age-slopes for ICV-corrected total brain volumes were almost identical between the sexes (-.67 vs. -.66 for females and males, respectively), which may speak against this interpretation. Further, as noted above, the CSF measure capture only a proportion of the total CSF, is less well validated than the other CSF measures, and should be interpreted with caution. It should also be stressed that the present data are cross-sectional, and thus can only address the issue of age-differences, not aging per se. However, it interesting that a longitudinal study did not find different rates of volumetric change for women and men over four years (Resnick et al., 2003).

With its 1143 healthy adults and 96 AD patients, the present study is among the largest MR-studies to date. Of the 44 MR-studies testing effects of sex on brain morphometry reported in the present paper, the median n was 116, while the median n for the 29 who tested for sex differences in aging was 140. Coupled with the use of validated, automated segmentation techniques, covering the entire brain, the present results could provide a firm basis for conclusion. Still, the most unique aspect of the present data is that separate analyses could be done in eight different samples, confirming the conclusions from the total sample.

As argued in the introduction, the discrepant results in previous literature can be caused by one or more of four different factors: (1) sample characteristics, (2) scan quality, (3) segmentation approach or the (4) statistical procedures used: (1-2) To examine the influence of sample characteristics and scan quality, seven healthy sub-samples were used, from six different scanners, four different research groups, and three different countries. If differences in sample characteristics and/ or scan quality are responsible for the discrepancies in previous literature, it is likely that this would have been reflected across the sub-samples included in the present study. Some inconsistencies across samples were found, but different age-slopes for men and women were generally not seen. Differences in sample characteristics/ scan quality may have contributed to the observed inconsistencies in previous literature, but did not appear to have a major impact on the results in the present study. (3) Further, the normalization procedure for head size had small but notable effects on the sex × age interactions. The significance of the interaction term changed for some samples when the volumes were ICV-corrected, indicating that different procedures can explain some of the discrepancies in previous literature. (4) Finally, it is possible that the segmentation procedure used may affect the degree to which sex differences are observed. It is interesting to note that the present results are in accordance with findings from the first surface-based study that mapped sex differences in thickness across the brain surface, where age × sex interactions were largely not found (Sowell et al., 2007). Surface-based methods are likely to improve anatomical correspondence between the participants, which may enhance sensitivity to group differences. Further, the cortical analysis used in the present study is not restricted to anatomically defined areas. This stands in contrast to many volumetric studies, and increases the likelihood of detecting unexpected group differences. Finally, the methods applied in the present paper have been validated using both histological and manual methods (Fischl et al., 2002; Fischl et al., 2004; Kuperberg et al., 2003; Rosas et al., 2002), and have been shown to be sensitive to various morphometric effects, e.g. of schizophrenia (Kuperberg et al., 2003; Nesvag et al., 2008), mild cognitive impairment and AD (Du et al., 2007; Fjell et al., 2008a) and prenatal opiate exposure (Walhovd et al., 2007). Further, the present results show high correlations between brain volumes and age, and replicated the established finding of larger raw volumes in men is replicated.

Effects of sex on cortical thickness and brain volumes

Sex differences in cortical thickness were not found. Sowell and colleagues found thicker cortex in women than men in posterior temporal and inferior parietal regions, independently of differences in brain or body size (Sowell et al., 2007). They studied a relatively large sample (176 participants), and p-values of the three regions that differed were just below .05 (.048-.017). Thus, it seems that sex differences in cortical thickness, even when observed, are statistically small or non-existent, and that very large samples are necessary to detect them in a reliable way. Since the cortical surface is larger in men, it is likely that cortical volume measures, especially when no corrections for head or brain size are applied, will distinguish more between the sexes than thickness. Group comparisons of volumetry confirmed previous findings of larger brains in men, especially for brainstem, cerebral WM and cerebral cortex (Allen et al., 2005; Courchesne et al., 2000; Ge et al., 2002; Goldstein et al., 2001; Kruggel, 2006; Luders et al., 2005; Luders et al., 2002; Raz et al., 2004a; Smith et al., 2007). The effects of sex on ICV-corrected volumes were much smaller, even though men had larger residual volumes of 3rd ventricle, brain stem, cerebellum cortex, thalamus, putamen, pallidum, hippocampus, amygdala, and the cerebral cortex, in addition to TBV. Except for cerebellum cortex (3% explained variance), sex explained between 0.3% and 1.7% of the variance in the structures that were significantly different. This supports previous studies showing larger ICV-corrected brain volumes for men (Chen et al., 2007; Kruggel, 2006; Resnick et al., 2000; Resnick et al., 2003), but that the differences are small, and only applies to some structures (Blatter et al., 1995; Courchesne et al., 2000; Eberling et al., 2003; Ge et al., 2002; Gur et al., 2002a; Ikram et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2007). Further, as noted by Raz and colleagues, corrections for ICV may not be appropriate in testing sex differences in morphometry as it includes some of the brain variance shared with ICV since the early months of life (Raz et al., 2004a). The present study also replicated earlier findings that in women’s brains the cerebral cortex and the hippocampus occupy a larger part of the TBV than in men’s (Chen et al., 2007; Filipek et al., 1994; Goldstein et al., 2001; Gur et al., 1999; Im et al., 2006; Kruggel, 2006; Luders et al., 2006; Luders et al., 2005; Luders et al., 2002; Riello et al., 2005; Sowell et al., 2007). Men had larger relative volume of brainstem, cerebellum cortex, and pallidum, in addition to all parts of the ventricular system. However, with exception of CSF compartments, all differences were small. Sex explained about 0.6% of the variance in cortical and about 0.4% of the variance in hippocampal TBV-corrected volume. Thus, even though these effects were statistically significant due to the very large sample, it is unlikely that they have any cognitive implications.

Conclusion

Previous studies of effects of sex on brain and aging have yielded inconsistent results. In the present cross-sectional study, we aimed to overcome several sources of discrepancy by basing the analyses on a very large pool of participants from seven different samples. The main finding was that most brain structures were not differentially affected by aging in men and women who were healthy or had Alzheimer’s disease. The few observed differences were small and unstable across subsamples. Thus, from the present data, it seems unlikely that men and women differ in the rates of age-related volume loss.

Supplementary Material

Supp1

Acknowledgments

The present papers was funded by the following grants: The Norwegian Research Council (177404/W50 to K.B.W., 175066/D15 to A.M.F., 154313/V50 to I.R., 177458/V50 to T.E.), University of Oslo (to K.B.W. and A.M.F.); the National Institutes of Health (R01-NS39581, R37-AG11230, and R01-RR13609); the Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery (MIND) Institute; The Wallenberg Foundation and the Swedish Medical Research Council (2008-2167). The National Center for Research Resources (P41-RR14075, R01 RR16594-01A1 and the NCRR BIRN Morphometric Project BIRN002, U24 RR021382), the National Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering (R01 EB001550, R01EB006758), the National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01 NS052585-01) as well as the Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery (MIND) Institute, and is part of the National Alliance for Medical Image Computing (NAMIC), funded by the National Institutes of Health through the NIH Roadmap for Medical Research, Grant U54 EB005149. Additional support was provided by The Autism & Dyslexia Project funded by the Ellison Medical Foundation. We thank the developers of the OASIS (Open Access Series of Imaging Studies) database for access to MRI data constituting samples 4 and 5 of the present work. According to Marcus et al. (2007), the OASIS database is supported by NIH grants P50 AG05681, P01 AG03991, P20 MH071616, RR14075, RR 16594, BIRN002, the Alzheimer’s Association, the James S. McDonnell Foundation, the Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery Institute, and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.

Footnotes

Conflicts of interest: Anders M. Dale is a founder and holds equity in CorTechs Labs, Inc, and also serves on the Scientific Advisory Board. The terms of this arrangement have been reviewed and approved by the University of California, San Diego in accordance with its conflict of interest policies.

References

  1. Allen JS, Bruss J, Brown CK, Damasio H. Normal neuroanatomical variation due to age: the major lobes and a parcellation of the temporal region. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26:1245–1260. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.023. discussion 1279-1282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berg L. Clinical Dementia Rating. Br J Psychiatry. 1984;145:339. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berg L. Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988;24:637–639. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blatter DD, Bigler ED, Gale SD, Johnson SC, Anderson CV, Burnett BM, Parker N, Kurth S, Horn SD. Quantitative volumetric analysis of brain MR: normative database spanning 5 decades of life. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1995;16:241–251. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Buckner RL, Head D, Parker J, Fotenos AF, Marcus D, Morris JC, Snyder AZ. A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage. 2004;23:724–738. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen X, Sachdev PS, Wen W, Anstey KJ. Sex differences in regional gray matter in healthy individuals aged 44-48 years: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neuroimage. 2007;36:691–699. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chung SC, Tack GR, Yi JH, Lee B, Choi MH, Lee BY, Lee SY. Effects of gender, age, and body parameters on the ventricular volume of Korean people. Neurosci Lett. 2006;395:155–158. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Coffey CE, Lucke JF, Saxton JA, Ratcliff G, Unitas LJ, Billig B, Bryan RN. Sex differences in brain aging: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Neurol. 1998;55:169–179. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.2.169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Colliot O, Chetelat G, Chupin M, Desgranges B, Magnin B, Benali H, Dubois B, Garnero L, Eustache F, Lehericy S. Discrimination between Alzheimer disease, mild cognitive impairment, and normal aging by using automated segmentation of the hippocampus. Radiology. 2008;248:194–201. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2481070876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Courchesne E, Chisum HJ, Townsend J, Cowles A, Covington J, Egaas B, Harwood M, Hinds S, Press GA. Normal brain development and aging: quantitative analysis at in vivo MR imaging in healthy volunteers. Radiology. 2000;216:672–682. doi: 10.1148/radiology.216.3.r00au37672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cowell PE, Sluming VA, Wilkinson ID, Cezayirli E, Romanowski CA, Webb JA, Keller SS, Mayes A, Roberts N. Effects of sex and age on regional prefrontal brain volume in two human cohorts. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:307–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05281.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cowell PE, Turetsky BI, Gur RC, Grossman RI, Shtasel DL, Gur RE. Sex differences in aging of the human frontal and temporal lobes. J Neurosci. 1994;14:4748–4755. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-08-04748.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dale AM, Sereno MI. Improved localization of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1993;5:162–176. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Du AT, Schuff N, Kramer JH, Rosen HJ, Gorno-Tempini ML, Rankin K, Miller BL, Weiner MW. Different regional patterns of cortical thinning in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2007;130:1159–1166. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dubois B, Feldman HH, Jacova C, Dekosky ST, Barberger-Gateau P, Cummings J, Delacourte A, Galasko D, Gauthier S, Jicha G, Meguro K, O’Brien J, Pasquier F, Robert P, Rossor M, Salloway S, Stern Y, Visser PJ, Scheltens P. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:734–746. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Eberling JL, Wu C, Haan MN, Mungas D, Buonocore M, Jagust WJ. Preliminary evidence that estrogen protects against age-related hippocampal atrophy. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:725–732. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(02)00056-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Filipek PA, Richelme C, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS., Jr The young adult human brain: an MRI-based morphometric analysis. Cereb Cortex. 1994;4:344–360. doi: 10.1093/cercor/4.4.344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:341–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fischl B, Salat DH, van der Kouwe AJ, Makris N, Segonne F, Quinn BT, Dale AM. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage. 2004;23(Suppl 1):S69–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage. 1999;9:195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM. High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;8:272–284. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4&#x0003c;272::AID-HBM10&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Amlien I, Bjornerud A, Reinvang I, Gjerstad L, Cappelen T, Willoch F, Due-Tonnessen P, Grambaite R, Skinningsrud A, Stenset V, Fladby T. Morphometric Changes in the Episodic Memory Network and Tau Pathologic Features Correlate With Memory Performance in Patients With Mild Cognitive Impairment. American Journal of Neuroradiology. 2008;29:1–7. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Amlien I, Espeseth T, Reinvang I, Raz N, Agartz I, Salat D, Greve D, Fischl B, Dale A, Walhovd KB. High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cerebral Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn232. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fotenos AF, Snyder AZ, Girton LE, Morris JC, Buckner RL. Normative estimates of cross-sectional and longitudinal brain volume decline in aging and AD. Neurology. 2005;64:1032–1039. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000154530.72969.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ge Y, Grossman RI, Babb JS, Rabin ML, Mannon LJ, Kolson DL. Age-related total gray matter and white matter changes in normal adult brain. Part I: volumetric MR imaging analysis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2002;23:1327–1333. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Goldstein JM, Seidman LJ, Horton NJ, Makris N, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Jr, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT. Normal sexual dimorphism of the adult human brain assessed by in vivo magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:490–497. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.6.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. 2001;14:21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Greenberg DL, Messer DF, Payne ME, Macfall JR, Provenzale JM, Steffens DC, Krishnan RR. Aging, gender, and the elderly adult brain: an examination of analytical strategies. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29:290–302. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gunning-Dixon FM, Head D, McQuain J, Acker JD, Raz N. Differential aging of the human striatum: a prospective MR imaging study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1998;19:1501–1507. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gur RC, Gunning-Dixon F, Bilker WB, Gur RE. Sex differences in temporo-limbic and frontal brain volumes of healthy adults. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:998–1003. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.9.998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gur RC, Gunning-Dixon FM, Turetsky BI, Bilker WB, Gur RE. Brain region and sex differences in age association with brain volume: a quantitative MRI study of healthy young adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002;10:72–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gur RC, Turetsky BI, Matsui M, Yan M, Bilker W, Hughett P, Gur RE. Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: correlations with cognitive performance. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4065–4072. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04065.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Halpern DF, Tan U. Stereotypes and steroids: using a psychobiosocial model to understand cognitive sex differences. Brain Cogn. 2001;45:392–414. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2001.1287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Han X, Fischl B. Atlas renormalization for improved brain MR image segmentation across scanner platforms. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2007;26:479–486. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2007.893282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Han X, Jovicich J, Salat D, van der Kouwe A, Quinn B, Czanner S, Busa E, Pacheco J, Albert M, Killiany R, Maguire P, Rosas D, Makris N, Dale A, Dickerson B, Fischl B. Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: the effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer. Neuroimage. 2006;32:180–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Ikram MA, Vrooman HA, Vernooij MW, van der Lijn F, Hofman A, van der Lugt A, Niessen WJ, Breteler MM. Brain tissue volumes in the general elderly population. The Rotterdam Scan Study. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29:882–890. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Im K, Lee JM, Lee J, Shin YW, Kim IY, Kwon JS, Kim SI. Gender difference analysis of cortical thickness in healthy young adults with surface-based methods. Neuroimage. 2006;31:31–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jernigan TL, Archibald SL, Fennema-Notestine C, Gamst AC, Stout JC, Bonner J, Hesselink JR. Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiol Aging. 2001;22:581–594. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00217-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jones CM, Braithwaite VA, Healy SD. The evolution of sex differences in spatial ability. Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:403–411. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kruggel F. MRI-based volumetry of head compartments: normative values of healthy adults. Neuroimage. 2006;30:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kuperberg GR, Broome MR, McGuire PK, David AS, Eddy M, Ozawa F, Goff D, West WC, Williams SC, van der Kouwe AJ, Salat DH, Dale AM, Fischl B. Regionally localized thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:878–888. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lemaitre H, Crivello F, Grassiot B, Alperovitch A, Tzourio C, Mazoyer B. Age- and sex-related effects on the neuroanatomy of healthy elderly. Neuroimage. 2005;26:900–911. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Luders E, Narr KL, Thompson PM, Rex DE, Woods RP, Deluca H, Jancke L, Toga AW. Gender effects on cortical thickness and the influence of scaling. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006;27:314–324. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Luders E, Narr KL, Thompson PM, Woods RP, Rex DE, Jancke L, Steinmetz H, Toga AW. Mapping cortical gray matter in the young adult brain: effects of gender. Neuroimage. 2005;26:493–501. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Luders E, Steinmetz H, Jancke L. Brain size and grey matter volume in the healthy human brain. Neuroreport. 2002;13:2371–2374. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000049603.85580.da. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Marcus DS, Wang TH, Parker J, Csernansky JG, Morris JC, Buckner RL. Open Access Series of Imaging Studies (OASIS): cross-sectional MRI data in young, middle aged, nondemented, and demented older adults. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1498–1507. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller JP, McKeel DW, Price JL, Rubin EH, Berg L. Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:397–405. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.3.397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Murphy DG, DeCarli C, McIntosh AR, Daly E, Mentis MJ, Pietrini P, Szczepanik J, Schapiro MB, Grady CL, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI. Sex differences in human brain morphometry and metabolism: an in vivo quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography study on the effect of aging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:585–594. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830070031007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Nesvag R, Lawyer G, Varnas K, Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Frigessi A, Jonsson EG, Agartz I. Regional thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia: effects of diagnosis, age and antipsychotic medication. Schizophr Res. 2008;98:16–28. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Nunnemann S, Wohlschlager AM, Ilg R, Gaser C, Etgen T, Conrad B, Zimmer C, Muhlau M. Accelerated aging of the putamen in men but not in women. Neurobiol Aging. 2007 doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Pruessner JC, Collins DL, Pruessner M, Evans AC. Age and gender predict volume decline in the anterior and posterior hippocampus in early adulthood. J Neurosci. 2001;21:194–200. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-01-00194.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Raz N, Gunning-Dixon F, Head D, Rodrigue KM, Williamson A, Acker JD. Aging, sexual dimorphism, and hemispheric asymmetry of the cerebral cortex: replicability of regional differences in volume. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:377–396. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00118-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D, Dupuis JH, McQuain J, Briggs SD, Loken WJ, Thornton AE, Acker JD. Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7:268–282. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.3.268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Head D, Kennedy KM, Acker JD. Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe: a study of a five-year change. Neurology. 2004;62:433–438. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000106466.09835.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Haacke EM. Brain Aging and Its Modifiers: Insights from in Vivo Neuromorphometry and Susceptibility Weighted Imaging. Ann NY Acad Sci %R 101196/annals1379018 1097. 2007:84–93. doi: 10.1196/annals.1379.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Resnick SM, Goldszal AF, Davatzikos C, Golski S, Kraut MA, Metter EJ, Bryan RN, Zonderman AB. One-year age changes in MRI brain volumes in older adults. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:464–472. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.5.464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Resnick SM, Pham DL, Kraut MA, Zonderman AB, Davatzikos C. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: a shrinking brain. J Neurosci. 2003;23:3295–3301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03295.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Riello R, Sabattoli F, Beltramello A, Bonetti M, Bono G, Falini A, Magnani G, Minonzio G, Piovan E, Alaimo G, Ettori M, Galluzzi S, Locatelli E, Noiszewska M, Testa C, Frisoni GB. Brain volumes in healthy adults aged 40 years and over: a voxel-based morphometry study. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2005;17:329–336. doi: 10.1007/BF03324618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Rosas HD, Liu AK, Hersch S, Glessner M, Ferrante RJ, Salat DH, van der Kouwe A, Jenkins BG, Dale AM, Fischl B. Regional and progressive thinning of the cortical ribbon in Huntington’s disease. Neurology. 2002;58:695–701. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.5.695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Salat DH, Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Greve DN, Desikan RS, Busa E, Morris JC, Dale AM, Fischl B. Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:721–730. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Smith CD, Chebrolu H, Wekstein DR, Schmitt FA, Markesbery WR. Age and gender effects on human brain anatomy: a voxel-based morphometric study in healthy elderly. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:1075–1087. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Kan E, Woods RP, Yoshii J, Bansal R, Xu D, Zhu H, Thompson PM, Toga AW. Sex differences in cortical thickness mapped in 176 healthy individuals between 7 and 87 years of age. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1550–1560. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Reinvang I, Lundervold A, Dale AM, Eilertsen DE, Quinn BT, Salat D, Makris N, Fischl B. Effects of age on volumes of cortex, white matter and subcortical structures. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26:1261–1270. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.020. discussion 1275-1268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Reinvang I, Lundervold A, Dale AM, Quinn BT, Salat D, Makris N, Fischl B. Neuroanatomical aging: Universal but not uniform. Neurobiology of Aging. 2005;26:1279–1283. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Walhovd KB, Moe V, Slinning K, Due-Tonnessen P, Bjornerud A, Dale AM, van der Kouwe A, Quinn BT, Kosofsky B, Greve D, Fischl B. Volumetric cerebral characteristics of children exposed to opiates and other substances in utero. Neuroimage. 2007;36:1331–1344. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Xu J, Kobayashi S, Yamaguchi S, Iijima K, Okada K, Yamashita K. Gender effects on age-related changes in brain structure. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2000;21:112–118. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp1

RESOURCES