Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jul 20.
Published in final edited form as: Public Health Nutr. 2020 Apr 14;23(11):1896–1906. doi: 10.1017/S1368980019004622

Body iron and lead status in early childhood and its effects on development and cognition: A longitudinal study from urban Vellore

Beena Koshy 1, Manikandan Srinivasan 2, Susan Mary Zachariah 1, Arun S Karthikeyan 2, Reeba Roshan 1, Anuradha Bose 3, Venkata Raghava MohanV 3, Sushil John 4, Karthikeyan Ramanujam 2, Jayaprakash Muliyil 2, Gagandeep Kang 2
PMCID: PMC7348694  NIHMSID: NIHMS1541733  PMID: 32285761

Abstract

Objective:

Early childhood factors can have persisting effects on development and cognition in children. We propose to explore the trends of iron deficiency and lead toxicity in early childhood and their association with child development at two years of age and cognition at five years

Design:

Longitudinal birth cohort study

Settings:

Urban slum, Vellore, India

Subjects:

Children enrolled at birth were followed up regularly in the first two years with developmental and cognitive assessments at two and five years of age respectively

Results:

The birth cohort enrolled 251 children with 228 children followed up at two years and 212 at five years of age. Iron deficiency (ID) was highest at 15 months of age and improved subsequently at 24 months. Blood lead levels (BLL) remained high at all age groups with an increasing trend with age; 97% at 36 months having high BLL. Persistent high mean BLL at 15 and 24 months had negative association with both cognition and expressive language raw scores of 24 months; while high mean BLL at 15, 24 and 36 months had no significant association with any of the domains of cognition at five years of age. Early childhood cumulative body iron status at 7, 15 and 24 months did not show any association with child development at two years, but was associated with verbal, performance and processing speed components of cognition at five years.

Conclusions:

Optimizing body iron status and limiting lead exposure in early childhood can augment child development and school entry cognition.

Keywords: Early Childhood, Child nutrition, Child development, cognition, iron deficiency, lead toxicity

Introduction

It is now recognized that the ‘first 1000 days of life’, from conception to the second birthday, are critical for brain development. This has resulted in an extensive exploration of early childhood factors to understand their potential impact on child development and sequentially human potential(1). There are many genetic, environmental and epigenetic factors that affect child’s development; and it is challenging to identify a single factor, the deficiency of which is common, easy to detect and treat, and amenable for community level interventions.

Micronutrient deficiencies such as that of iron and iodine, especially in early infancy can have long term effects on cognition and behavior; probably due to its impact on the rapidly developing brain during the ‘first 1000 days of life’(2; 3). Iron deficiency (ID) is considered as the most common nutritional deficiency among all age groups and is prevalent across all social strata(2; 4). Iron is vital, not just for the synthesis of hemoglobin, but also for the non-hematological effects on mitochondrial and neuro-endocrine function(4). ID therefore affects cognition and behavior in children, and these changes can be long term. Though there are studies evaluating anemia and iron status and their effects on cognition, there is a dearth of evidence, of the long term effect of ID in early childhood, especially from low and middle-income countries (LMIC).

In addition to micronutrient deficiency, lead exposure in early childhood can adversely affect child development including cognition. Even low levels of lead exposure during the early childhood brain development stage can have lasting effects on cognition and behavior (5). With increasing recognition of the adverse effects of high lead levels on young brains, there has been a revision of the cut-off for elevated blood lead levels from 10 μg/dl to 5 μg/dl by the American Academy of Pediatrics(6; 7). Background deficiency of micronutrients particularly that of iron, can modify the impact of exposure to high levels of lead through pica, and competitive inhibition of intestinal absorption (8; 9). A previous study from the same birth cohort had reported high blood lead levels (BLL) at 15 and 24 months of age in children (10). The objective of the current study was to evaluate the trends of both iron and lead status in early childhood and the association of cumulative early childhood iron and lead status on development at two years of age and cognition at five years of age.

Materials and methods

Study population and methodology

The study population was a birth cohort followed up for the Etiology, Risk Factors and Interactions of Enteric Infections and Malnutrition and the Consequences for Child Health and Development (MAL-ED) Network, a multinational, longitudinal prospective cohort study conducted in eight countries across the world(11). The study site in India is eight adjacent urban slum dwelling areas, located in Old Town, Salavanpet and neighboring areas in Vellore, South India, with a population density of 42,000 per sq km(12), as compared to 6,200 per sq km in Rio de Janeiro and 32,000 per sq km in Harlem, NYC. A population of 12,000 was covered for this study. All pregnant women in the study site were identified by door-to-door survey and invited to participate in the study immediately after the delivery, by consecutive sampling. The enumeration process carried out prior to recruitment provided the probable number of births in the study area based on recent births and the number of pregnant mothers. Using this information, it was planned to recruit more than 200 children over the 2-year period, the target number for each site as per the MAL-ED study protocol (for details, refer to John et al, 2014) (12). Mothers were screened based on the following exclusion criteria – having pre-existing plans for migration during the study period, having another child from the same family being enrolled in MAL-ED study, multiple pregnancies, having a neonate with medical complications and unavailability of mothers to provide informed consent. Babies of mothers who consented were enrolled and their demographic characteristics along with anthropometry measurements were recorded. Babies were enrolled at a mean (range) age of 10.08 (0–16) days. Trained fieldworkers visited their home twice a week for active disease surveillance for the first two years of life. The initial birth cohort recruitment was conducted between March 2010 and February 2012 and recruited 251 children; and baseline demographic data of the birth cohort was comparable to that of the study area (12). The cohort was further followed up at 36 and 60 months of age.

Blood samples were collected at 7, 15, 24, 36 and 60 months. Samples were tested for hemoglobin (at 7, 15, 24, 36 and 60 months); iron and ferritin assays (at 7, 15 and 24 months) and for blood lead levels (at 15, 24 and 36 months). All children underwent a developmental assessment at 7, 15, 24 and 36 months of age using Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-3rd edition (BSID-III) and cognitive assessment at five years of age, using Wechsler Preschool Primary Scales of Intelligence – third edition (WPPSI-III). Child development is defined as the ability of the child in domains of motor, language, cognition, play, social and adaptive skills (13). Cognition or intelligence is defined as a combination of multiple abilities in a child (14). The original study and further follow-up studies were approved by the institutional review board (IRB). This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects namely growth monitoring, cognition assessments and blood collection were approved by the IRB, Christian Medical College, Vellore, India. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents/caregivers of all the subjects.

Study measures

Blood collection

Venous blood was collected from children to a maximum quantity of five ml at each visit. Hemoglobin was analyzed using a point of care screening tool, Hemocue® Hb 301 kit (azidemethemoglobin method), Angelholm, Sweden at the study clinic immediately after blood collection using a drop of blood. One ml of the collected venous blood was used for blood lead level analysis and preserved with Dipotassium Ethylene-diamine-tetra-acetic acid (K2 EDTA) and the remaining blood was stored in the biochemistry tube for analysis of serum ferritin and transferrin levels. The samples were refrigerated and transported using cold packs to the research lab within two hours of blood collection for further analysis. Serum ferritin, transferrin receptors and lead levels were tested in the Biochemistry department, Christian Medical College, Vellore. Chemiluminescence immunoassay was done for Serum Ferritin analysis using Siemens Advia Centaur, Camberley, United Kingdom (Model Siemens Advia Centaur Xpi Chemiluminescence immune assay) with 0.5 microgram as limit of detection. For estimating transferrin levels, Immunoturbidimetry was performed using Roche Coba C system, Indianapolis, Germany (Model Roche Cobas C 702 Immuno turbidimetry assay) with 10 microgram as limit of detection. Using Graphite Furnace Atomic Absorption Spectroscopy (GFAAS) method in M-series Atomic Absorption Spectro-photometer (Model Thermo Scientific X series 2.ICP-MS), blood lead levels were estimated with 0.5 microgram as limit of detection.

Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development-III (BSID-III)

The BSID-III assesses development in the domains of gross motor, fine motor, expressive language, receptive language and cognition between 1 and 42 months of age (13). The measure was culturally adapted, translated and piloted prior to administration. A single trained psychologist conducted the testing in a standardized child friendly setting in the community clinic for each age group. Around 5–6% of the tests were video-recorded and centrally reviewed for quality control purposes (15). For the purpose of current analysis, the 24 month assessment was used, as two years denotes the completion of infancy and the ‘1000-day window’ period (1). Receptive and expressive language as well as cognition domain raw scores were derived for each child from the 24 month assessment. Receptive and expressive language skills measure receptive and expressive abilities of children in primarily language and communication areas; while cognition measures sensorimotor development, exploration and manipulation, object relatedness, concept formation and memory (13).

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence-third edition (WPPSI-III)

The WPPSI-III can be administered to children aged between 2 years 6 months and 7 years 3 months and assesses cognition in the domains of verbal, performance and processing speed. This measure was adapted centrally for cultural appropriateness, and was translated and piloted in local settings before being administered by a single trained psychologist. Verbal, performance and processing speed raw scores were obtained after administration for each child. Ten percent of these assessments were recorded and reviewed centrally for quality control.

Maternal intelligence was assessed at 6–8 months of child age by Raven’s progressive matrices(16). This time of assessment was chosen in the protocol for mothers’ convenience as they would be able to leave their babies with another caregiver around this time, to complete cognitive assessment in the study clinic. To assess the socioeconomic status (SES), a socioeconomic index was created using Water and sanitation, Assets, Maternal education and household Income (WAMI) and SES of the families were assessed at recruitment and six-monthly intervals as per the study protocol (17). The WAMI index was validated against prevalence of stunting in all the eight countries involved in the MAL-ED cohort study (17). The broader aim of the main study was to assess the predictors of early child development, amongst which SES influences child development through both biological and physical home environment (11; 18).

As serum ferritin levels can be influenced by inflammation, total body iron levels were calculated from ferritin (F) and transferrin (R) levels as below(19):

Body Iron (mg/kg)=((log(RFratio)2.8229)0.1207)

Positive values indicated iron surplus in stores while negative values indicated deficiency of iron in tissues.

Data entry and analysis

The Data Coordinating Center (DCC) developed a double data entry database application. Forms were checked for completeness and accuracy of the data by data supervisors before being entered into the application. Discrepancies between the first and second entry were cleared by data supervisors after rechecking forms and this process of cross validation was done for quality control of the study. The data was analyzed using STATA 13. (StataCorp, College station,TX,USA). Missing data values were limited as this was a prospective study with rigorous quality checks. If a child did not have information at one time point, that child was excluded from corresponding analysis and no missing value corrections were applied.

Mean and standard deviation (SD) were used to summarize hemoglobin, lead, ferritin and transferrin levels measured at each time point. A birth weight less than 2.5 kg was considered as low birth weight category. We reported children who were anemic (hemoglobin <11gm/dl) (20), iron deficient (blood iron < 0 mg/kg) and with elevated blood lead levels (> 0.24 μmol/L) using proportions. Characteristics of the study children such as sex and socioeconomic status at 2nd and 5th year of follow-up were compared with baseline data using ‘Chi-square for trends’. Mean values of hemoglobin across the time points (7, 15, 24, 36 and 60 months) were compared using repeated measures ANOVA. Bonferroni test was used for post-hoc analysis, to compare the hemoglobin levels between any of the two time points measured. Estimates of median serum ferritin, transferrin and blood lead levels across each time point were compared using Friedman’s test and post-hoc analysis between any of the two points was done using Wilcoxon Sign rank test. Proportions of children with anemia, iron deficiency and elevated lead levels across each time point were compared using ‘Chi-square for trends’. The proportion with ID in anemic children was evaluated to understand causes of anemia in each age group setting. The outcome variables namely child development (BSID-III) measured at 2 years was expressed as raw scores of cognition, expressive language, and receptive language domains, and cognition (WPPSI-III) measured at 5 years was expressed as raw scores of verbal, performance, and processing speed domains. Median (IQR - Interquartile range) was used to summarize the scores under each domain of the outcome variables. Cronbach’s alpha was used to assess the internal consistency within items under each domain of outcome measures to evaluate if the responses provided by the subject remained consistent. Since iron and lead levels were analyzed at different time points, a summary statistic approach was used and the repeatedly measured information was condensed to a single number per subject (21; 22). Mean body iron was derived by averaging the body iron levels at 7, 15 and 24 months and this measure was considered for analysis at both 2 and 5 years. Mean blood lead levels was derived by averaging the blood lead levels at 15 and 24 months for analysis at 2 years, and those at 15, 24 and 36 months for analysis at 5 years.

Linear regression modeling was done to find the predictors of the outcome variables - BSID-III and WPPSI-III scores using separate models. The predictors that were included in the model were –sex of the child, maternal intelligence raw scores, socioeconomic status, mean body iron levels and mean blood lead levels. In the bivariate analysis, unadjusted beta co-efficient along with 95% confidence interval was reported for each predictor. A multivariable linear regression was performed using the same set of predictors listed above. Following assumptions of linear regression were tested - normality of the outcome variable using Shapiro-Wilk test and multicollinearity of predictors using correlation matrix were tested. Only those independent variables with the correlation co-efficient (r value) of less than 0.5 in the multicollinearity analysis, were retained in the model. The standardized beta co-efficient along with 95% confidence interval of the predictor variables was noted. In the post-estimation analysis, homoscedasticity of the residuals was assessed using Breusch-Pagan test. The level of significance for the test of association was considered at p < 0.05.

Results

After visiting 301 pregnant mothers, 251 children were enrolled at birth after parental consent, as 10 mothers refused to participate and 40 mothers did not fulfill the screening criteria (screen failures). Reasons for screen failures included pre-existing plans for migration during the study period (n=5), another child of the same family being part of the MAL-ED study (n=8), multiple pregnancy (n=1), presence of pre-existing morbidities in children (n=7) and mothers not available to give informed consent (n=9) and a combination of two or more of the above reasons (n=10). On follow-up, 228 children participated at two years of age and 212 children at five years of age (Fig. 1) and, there was no significant difference in the socio demographic characteristics between participants at baseline and at 2 or 5 years of age follow-ups (Table 1). Loss to follow-up was mainly due to migration of the families (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1:

Fig. 1:

Flowchart depicting the follow-up of the birth cohort

WAMI – Water and sanitation, Assets, Maternal education and Income score for socioeconomic status; RPM – Raven’s progressive matrices; BSID III – Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development III; WPPSI III – Wechsler Preschool Primary Scales of Intelligence

Table 1:

Characteristics of study children at enrolment, two and five years of age

Characteristics Enrolment (N=251) At 2 years (N=228) At 5 years (N=212) p*
1. Sex 0.960
Male (%) 113(45.0) 105(46.0) 98 (46.2)
Female (%) 138(55.0) 123(54.0) 114 (53.8)
2. SES (WAMI) 0.955
 Low (<33rd centile) 71(30.2) 71 (31.1) 65 (30.7)
High (≥ 33rd centile) 164(69.8) 157 (68.9) 147 (69.3)
3. Maternal IQ - raw score [Median (range)] 46.5 (8–68) 45 (8–68) -

SES – Socio-economic status; WAMI - Water and sanitation, Assets, Maternal education and Income score; IQ – Intelligence quotient

*

- Using chi-square for trends

Baseline SES data was available only for 235 children.

The birth cohort had a mean (range) family size of 5.7 (3–13) and the mean (range) educational years of the head of the family was 6.8 (0–18) (12). The mean (SD) body mass index (BMI) of mothers was 22 (3.9) kg/m2 with 20.3% and 21.1% of mothers being underweight and overweight/obese respectively. The cohort participants were predominantly girls (56%) and the mean birth weight (SD) was 2.8 (0.44) kg. 17 % of babies were low birth weight and weighed less than 2.5kg at birth. The mean (SD) exclusive breastfeeding duration was 76.9(43.8) days.

There was a reduction in hemoglobin levels between 7 and 15 months, however this reduction was not significant (p =1.000) (Table. 2). Subsequently, there was a significant increase in the hemoglobin levels at 15, 24 and 36 months (15 mo vs. 24 mo, p = 0.012; 24 mo vs. 36 mo, p = 0.0001). Though, ferritin levels significantly dropped between 7 months and 24 months (p < 0.0001), there was no change at 36 months (15 mo vs. 36 mo, p = 0.3745). There was a significant increase in mean blood lead levels (BLL) across the three time points (15 mo vs. 24 mo, p < 0.0001; 24 mo vs. 36 mo, p = 0.0001) (Table 2).

Table 2:

Trend of hemoglobin, ferritin, transferrin and lead levels at different time points of the cohort follow up

Indicator 7 months 15 months 24 months 36 months 60 months p
Hemoglobin (gm/dl) N 233 228 225 112 159 <0.0001
Mean (SD) 10.8 (1.2) 10.6 (1.3) 10.9 (1.4) 11.5 (1.2) 11.8 (0.9)
Ferritin (pmol/L) N 230 221 222 NA NA <0.0001
Mean (SD) 91.2 (123.4) 33.9 (45.6) 41.6 (60.4) NA NA
Transferrin(μmol/L) N 218 226 225 NA NA 0.001
Mean (SD) 16.8 (112) 27.1 (20.7) 27.6 (20.7) NA NA
Lead(μmol/L) N NA 228 223 113 NA 0.002
Mean (SD) NA 0.5 (0.3) 0.6 (0.4) 0.6 (0.3) NA

N refers to the total number of children whose blood samples were available for analysis at respective time points; NA - Values are not available.

More than half the children were anemic at seven and 15 months of age, with subsequent improvements in hemoglobin levels at 24, 36 and 60 months. Almost half the study group (46.1%) had low iron status at 15 months of age (Table 3). Across the time points, there was an increase in proportion of anemia attributable to ID from 19% at 7 months to 68% at 24 months (Figure 2).

Table 3:

Percentage of children with anemia (Hb < 11 gm/dl), low body iron stores and high blood lead level (> 0.24 μmol/L)

Indicator 7 months 15 months 24 months 36 months 60 months p
Hemoglobin status Percentage of children with anemia (N) 51.5 (120) 56.6 (129) 42.7 (96) 31.3 (35) 15.1 (24) <0.0001
Percentage of children without anemia(N) 48.5 (113) 43.4 (99) 57.3 (129) 68.7 (77) 84.9 (135)
Body iron status Percentage of children with iron deficiency(N) 11.1 (24) 46.1 (102) 42.9 (96) NA NA <0.0001
Percentage of children without iron deficiency(N) 88.9 (192) 53.9 (119) 57.1 (128) NA NA
Blood level status Percentage of children with elevated blood lead level(N) NA 89.9 (228) 95.9 (214) 97.3 (110) NA 0.004
Percentage of children without elevated blood lead level (N) NA 10.1 (23) 4.1 (9) 2.7 (3) NA

N refers to the total number of children whose blood samples were available for analysis at respective time points; NA - Values are not available.

Fig. 2:

Fig. 2:

Proportion of iron deficiency in anemic children at 7, 15 and 24 months of age

The Cronbach’s alpha for internal consistency was 0.927, 0.712 and 0.727 for expressive language, receptive language and cognition domains of BSID-III at two years of age respectively; and 0.789, 0.705 and 0.802 for verbal, performance and processing speed domains of WPPSI-III at five years of age respectively. Median raw scores (IQR) for expressive language, receptive language and cognition were 27 (16–33), 29 (15–36) and 59 (47–69) respectively at two years of age; while the median raw scores (IQR) at five years of age for verbal, performance and processing speed domains were 39 (13–69), 48 (22–75) and 35 (1–86) respectively (Table 4).

Table 4:

Outcome raw scores of development and cognition using Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development III (BSID III) and Wechsler Preschool Primary Scales of Intelligence IV (WPPSI IV) measured at 2 and 5 years respectively

Mean (SD) Median (IQR)
Development scores at 2 years (N=227*)
Cognition 59.5 (3.4) 59(57–62)
Expressive language 27.8 (4.7) 29(25–31)
Receptive language 26.9 (2.5) 27 (25–29)
Cognition scores at 5 years (N=212)
Verbal 37.9 (9.2) 39(32–44)
Performance 48.2(9.8) 48(42–54)
Processing speed 35.8(18.4) 35 (22–50)

IQR – Interquartile range

*

Of the 228 children who completed 2 years of follow up, one subject did not have complete developmental raw scores.

The multivariate models built for each domain of the child development measured at two years of age (Table 5) was statistically significant (p < 0.01) and had an adjusted R square value ranging between 5–7% (Adjusted R square values of cognition, expressive language and receptive language domains were 4.6%, 7.2% and 4.7% respectively). Considering the collinearity between the body iron and hemoglobin (correlation co-efficient, r =0.63), the hemoglobin was excluded from the final model. High mean BLL of 15 and 24 months was associated with lower cognition and expressive language raw scores at two years of age, which remained significant in the multivariate analysis with sex, mean body iron (of 7, 15 and 24 months), maternal intelligence and socioeconomic status (Table 5). Mean body iron did not have any association with either cognition, expressive or receptive language scores at two years of age. Other factors associated with the two year development in the multivariate analysis were the socioeconomic status associated with both cognition and expressive language; girls had better expressive language than boys and higher maternal intelligence was associated with better receptive language.

Table 5:

Factors associated with cognition, expressive and receptive language components of Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development III (BSID III) raw scores assessed at 2 years (N=226*)

Unadjusted Beta coefficient (95% CI) p Adjusted Beta coefficient (95% CI) p
Cognition
Sex −0.1 (−0.9 – 0.9) 0.977 −0.3(−1.2 – 0.6) 0.529
Mean body iron 0.1 (−0.1 – 0.2) 0.131 0.1 (−0.1 – 0.2) 0.306
Mean blood lead§ −0.1 (−0.2 – −0.02) 0.009 −0.2 (−0.2 – −0.03) 0.009
Mother’s IQ scores 0.1 (−0.01 – 0.06) 0.203 0.01 (−0.03 – 0.05) 0.712
WAMI 1.5 (0.5 – 2.4) 0.002 1.4 (0.4 – 2.4) 0.006
Expressive language
Sex 1.7 (0.5 – 2.3) 0.006 1.4 (0.2 – 2.7) 0.020
Mean body iron 0.1 (−0.02 – 0.3) 0.111 0.04 (−0.1– 0.2) 0.548
Mean blood lead§ −0.2(−0.3 – −0.06) 0.003 −0.2 (−0.3 – −0.1) 0.006
Mother’s IQ scores 0.04 (−0.02 – 0.09) 0.64 0.1 (−0.5 – 0.07) 0.673
WAMI 1.6 (0.3 – 2.9) 0.015 1.5 (0.09 – 2.8) 0.036
Receptive language
Sex 0.4 (−0.2 – 1.1) 0.188 0.2 (−0.4 – 0.9) 0.473
Mean body iron 0.07 (−0.02 – 0.2) 0.125 0.03 (0.05 – 0.1) 0.408
Mean blood lead§ −0.04(−0.1 – 0.02) 0.185 −0.04 (−0.1 – 0.02) 0.205
Mother’s IQ scores 0.05(0.02 – 0.08) 0.001 0.04 (0.01 – 0.07) 0.007
WAMI 0.8 (0.6 – 1.5) 0.034 0.4(−0.3 – 1.1) 0.294

IQ – intelligence quotient; WAMI – Water and sanitation, Assets, Maternal education and Income score for socio-economic status.

*

Of the 228 children who completed 2 years of follow up, 2 subjects had missing data either in blood lead levels or in development scores, hence the linear model includes data for only 226 children.

Significant associations in unadjusted and adjusted analyses are presented in bold letters.

Mean cumulative body iron of 7, 15 and 24 months.

§

Mean cumulative blood lead levels of 15 and 24 months.

Linear models constructed for each domain of cognition assessed at five years of age (Table 6) were statistically significant (p < 0.0001) and had an adjusted R square value ranging between 10–13% (Adjusted R square values of verbal, performance and processing speed domains were 9.8%, 12.3% and 12.7% respectively). Body iron status of early childhood (cumulative values of 7, 15 and 24 months) had a significant association with verbal, performance and processing speed at five years of age, which remained significant in the multivariate analysis (Table 6). Mean BLL did not have a significant association with any of the domains of cognition assessed at five years of age. Other factors associated with the five year cognition in the multivariate analysis were the maternal intelligence affecting verbal, performance and processing speed components; and socioeconomic status the performance domain.

Table 6:

Factors associated with verbal, performance and processing speed components of Wechsler Preschool Primary Scales of Intelligence IV (WPPSI IV) raw scores assessed at 5 years (N=212)

Unadjusted Beta coefficient (95% CI) p* Adjusted Beta coefficient (95% CI) p*
Verbal
Sex 1.1 (−1.4 – 3.6) 0.379 −0.1 (−2.5 – 2.4) 0.978
Mean body iron 0.5 (0.2 – 0.9) 0.001 0.4 (0.1 – 0.7) 0.011
Mean blood lead −0.1 (−0.3– 0.1) 0.423 −0.1 (−0.3 – 0.1) 0.462
Mother’s IQ scores 0.3 (0.1–0.4) < 0.0001 0.2 (0.1 – 0.3) < 0.0001
WAMI category 3.1 (0.4 – 5.7) 0.025 0.9 (−1.9 – 3.6) 0.524
Performance
Sex −0.3(−2.9 – 2.3) 0.813 −1.73 ( −4.2- .8) 0.182
Mean blood iron 0.6 (0.3 to 0.96) <0.0001 0.5 (0.2–0.8) 0.003
Mean blood lead −0.2 (−0.5 – 0.02) 0.071 −0.2 (−0.5 - −0.1) 0.056
Mother’s IQ scores 0.2(0.1–0.3) <0.0001 0.2 (0.04–0.3) 0.009
WAMI category 4.9 (2.1–7.8) <0.0001 3.1(0.2 – 5.9) 0.037
Processing speed
Sex 6.5(1.6–11.4) 0.010 4.3 (−0.4– 9.1) 0.076
Mean body iron 1.3 (0.6–1.9) <0.0001 0.9 (0.2–1.5) 0.007
Mean blood lead −0.16 (−0.7–0.4) 0.493 −0.1 (−0.5 – 0.3) 0.682
Mother’s IQ scores 0.5 (0.2 – 0.7) <0.0001 0.3 (0.1–0.6) 0.005
WAMI category 8.7 (3.3 – 13.9) 0.002 4.7(−0.7– 10) 0.090

IQ – intelligence quotient; WAMI – Water and sanitation, Assets, Maternal education and Income score for socio-economic status.

*

Significant associations in unadjusted and adjusted analyses are presented in bold letters.

Mean cumulative body iron of 7, 15 and 24 months.

Mean cumulative lead levels of 15, 24 and 36 months.

Discussion

This prospective birth cohort follow-up study from urban Vellore provided the trends of hemoglobin, iron and lead levels during the early childhood. Further, a negative association of high BLL with cognition and expressive language domains of child development was demonstrated. This study also showed a negative association of ID with verbal, performance and processing domains of cognition. Retention in this study was good at all planned time points. There was minimal drop out and 212 children presented at five years of age (Table 1). Our finding of high prevalence of anemia in more than half the study children at both seven and fifteen months of age has been reported from other studies and reviews (23; 24; 25; 26). The proportion of anemia attributable to ID, was only 18.7% at 7 months. Iron deficiency anemia became the commonest cause of anemia at 15 months of age and remained so at 24 months, despite a drop in overall prevalence of anemia and improvement in iron stores in the later age group. Prevalence of anemia in Indian children aged 6–59 months was estimated as 58.5% and 69.4% in National Family Health Survey (NFHS) conducted during 2015–16 (NFHS-4) and 2005–06 (NFHS-3) respectively. Though the prevalence of anemia had lowered to 58.5% in NFHS-4, from its previous estimate of 69.4% in NFHS-3, anemia continues to be a public health problem in India(27). Other studies from India also had reported that anemia was common in the older age group with a prevalence ranging between 27 and 63 percent(28; 29). Other causes of anemia in children from LMICs are hookworm infestation with resultant blood loss, vitamin B12 deficiency manifesting as megaloblastic anemia, hemoglobinopathy disorders and environmental enteropathy (23; 30; 31). However, the association between above-mentioned causes of anemia and cognitive development was not investigated in the current analysis.

Young children showed the lowest body iron status at 15 months of age probably due to a combination of factors such as increased iron demand to support the expanding blood volume requirements, depletion of antenatal iron stores and sub-optimal iron intake(23). The estimated daily requirement of absorbed iron in the first year of life ranges between 0.55mg and 0.75mg and cannot be fully met by breast milk, which has a mean iron level of 0.4 mg/L (32). Typical weaning foods used in South India are rice and milk based and contain low iron. Introduction of animal source foods, which can be a good source of dietary iron, into a child’s diet is usually later in this community; unlike other communities where meat is added earlier on (26). This might explain the lowest body iron status shown at 15 months of age and is corroborated with other LMIC studies (32; 33). Iron status improved in the second year in this study, probably due to the child eating more variety of food from the family pot with a decrease in consumption of milk and milk products (26).

Most of the iron in our body is bound in heme proteins and storage proteins and around three percent is bound with critical enzyme systems such as cytochromes and peroxidases. ID manifesting during the early childhood, when there is biologically an increased demand for iron, can be detrimental especially to critical iron-dependent enzymes(34). It is possible therefore that ID can cause deficiencies in mitochondrial function, neurotransmitter synthesis, and neuronal maturation in the brain, which can have long-lasting effects on cognition(4; 35; 36; 37).

High blood lead levels (BLL) as in the current study have been reported from India (38; 39; 40). Previous studies had used BLL of 10 μg/dl (0.48 μmol/L) as the cut off for high level; but the revised cut off of 5 μg/dl (0.24 μmol/L) has been used for the current analysis(6; 7; 41). This current community based prospective study shows increasing lead levels from 15 to 36 months of age, with high lead levels present in 97% of children [>5 μg/dl (0.24 μmol/L)] at 36 months of age. This has been reported also from Nepal, in a hospital based study of 6–36 months old children, where 64.4% had high BLL of > 5 μg/dl (0.24 μmol/L)(42). Risk factors associated with high BLL include low socioeconomic status, industrial area exposure, playing with dirt and mud, exposure to enamel paints, piped water supply and mud/clay floors of the house; the last two factors being shown as predictors for high BLL in this birth cohort in an evaluation published earlier(10; 38; 39; 40; 41; 42; 43; 44). There is evidence suggesting transplacental transfer of lead, with 16 (84.2%) cord blood samples obtained from 19 mothers, who had high BLL during their antenatal period, had elevated lead levels (45). This highlights the possibility of lead exposure even as early as the in-utero phase of life. In the current study, the continued environmental exposure to lead during early childhood, could have caused increasing BLL trends.

There can be interactions between lead and iron. ID can cause pica, which can exaggerate lead exposure through mouthing of inanimate objects (8; 9). Heavy metals including lead share intestinal iron absorption pathways, where there can be competitive inhibition between lead and iron (8). The classic clinical manifestation of lead poisoning in children is anemia; more specifically hypochromic microcytic anemia (41). The prevalence and severity of anemia correlates directly with BLL. Younger and iron deficient children are at higher risk of developing lead-induced anemia. Lead toxicity results in anemia through two mechanisms – by causing direct damage to the cell membrane of red blood cells resulting in hemolysis and through inhibition of the enzyme delta-aminolevulinic acid dehydratase (ALAD) in the iron-dependent heme synthesis pathway (41; 46). Inhibition of ALAD enzyme, not only results in anemia by affecting heme synthesis, but also results in the accumulation of heme precursors such as aminolevulinic acid, which can cause harmful effects to the nervous system (46; 47).

In the current study, high mean BLL over 15 and 24 months had negative association with both cognition and expressive language raw scores of 24 months;. Literature shows that early childhood lead exposure can adversely affect later age cognitive capability – a doubling of BLL or tooth lead levels at 2 years of age can cause a mean deficit of 1–2 intelligence quotient (IQ) points at five years of age(48; 49). Lead even in smaller dose exposure is toxic to the nervous system and can impair cognition and behavior; thus the recommendation that there is no safe BLL (6; 41). Lead interferes with channels in the brain by its function as a calcium analogue (6; 46). In addition, lead increases oxidative stress by generation of reactive oxygen species (ROS) and inactivates anti-oxidants such as glutathione by binding to them (50; 51). Lead can accumulate over time as shown in the current study can have a negative association with cognition and learning such as impaired visual-motor integration, longer reaction time and reading disability in school age years; which persist into young(40; 49).

The finding of this study is that early childhood cumulative body iron status had no association with development at two years of age, but was associated with verbal, performance and processing speed components of cognition at five years. This may be due to the fact that cognition is a stable construct at five years as frontal lobes are better myelinated and mature at this age (1; 52). Early onset ID through its impact on the activity of iron containing enzymes can affect neuronal myelination, mitochondrial function and neurotransmitter metabolism of developing brain, which can have long term effects on and behavior of children (3; 4; 34; 53). Subsequent iron supplementation beyond the early can optimize body iron stores, but cannot neutralize the impact of early onset ID on neurodevelopment, cognition and behavior (2; 53; 54). ID in school age years can also affect cognition, as per other Indian studies (24; 28; 30; 55). Among cognitive functions, ID impairs executive function the which includes but not limited to attention, inhibitory control, planning ability and working (3; 4; 36; 54). ID also can cause concentration and attention problems, tiredness and low mood in school-age years; which can improve on appropriate iron supplementation (3; 54). ID without anemia in women of reproductive age group was shown to affect their executive function, indicating the need to evaluate ID on its own without the ramification of presence or absence of anemia (56). Antenatal or childhood iron supplementation shows inconsistent effects on both early and long term cognition in children (37; 57; 58).

Other factors that have association with development and cognition in this study are maternal cognition, SES factors and gender. Maternal intelligence can affect child cognition not just by genetic(59), but also by providing optimal macro and micro-environments including appropriate play and stimulating experiences (60; 61; 62). Improvement in SES factors such as access to safe water and sanitation, better maternal education status and improvement of family economy, can offer experiences for children to grow and develop(17). Repeated diarrheal illnesses predisposed by lack of safe water and sanitation facilities in the early childhood, can have direct negative impact on child development and cognition. Indirect impact of diarrheal illnesses on cognition is through manifestation of nutritional deficiencies following these illnesses, which in turn affects cognitive development. In addition to these infections, exposure of toxins such as lead, can further affect the cognition(61; 62). Girls tend to have better speech and expressive language skills than boys at two years of age, as shown in the current analysis, probably due to better environmental interactions (63; 64).

The current study has strengths such as longitudinal birth cohort follow-up, standardized developmental and cognitive assessments by a single trained psychologist, rigorous quality control with internal and external reviews, standardized laboratory assessments and adjustment for socioeconomic status. However, regression models could explain only up to 13% of the variance associated with the predicted outcomes, highlighting the presence of other factors that could have influenced the outcome. A comparatively smaller sample size prevented a detailed analysis of other factors within the model. Inclusion of factors such as years of preschool education could have made the model more explanatory. Due to the collinearity between serum iron and hemoglobin levels, the latter was not included in the regression; which can be also a limitation to this study. Despite these limitations, this prospective birth cohort study showed that high blood lead levels in early childhood was negatively associated with development at two years of age, and the association of early childhood iron deficiency with cognition at the age of five years. Future studies can explore the contribution of other early childhood micronutrient deficiency and environmental toxicity to development and cognition, and the ideal timing of nutritional interventions such as iron supplementation and fortification.

Significance.

This longitudinal birth cohort shows maximum iron deficiency (ID) at 15 months of age and increasing blood lead levels (BLL) with age in a low-middle income country (LMIC) setting.

Early childhood ID shows a negative association with verbal, performance and processing speed domains of cognition at five years of age; while high BLL was negatively associated with cognition and expressive language at two years of age.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the participants, their families and staff of the MAL-ED Network project

Financial support

The Etiology, Risk Factors and Interactions of Enteric Infections and Malnutrition and the Consequence for Child Health and Development Project (MAL-ED) is carried out as a collaborative project supported by the Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation, the Foundation for the NIH and the National Institutes of Health/Fogarty International Center.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

None

Ethical Standards disclosure

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the Institutional Review Board, Christian Medical College, Vellore. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants’ parents.

Bibliography

  • 1.Biesalski HK (2016) The 1,000-Day Window and Cognitive Development. World Rev Nutr Diet. 115, 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Yehuda S, Rabinovitz S, Mostofsky DI (2006) Nutritional deficiencies in learning and cognition. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 43 Suppl 3, S22–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Benton D (2008) Micronutrient status, cognition and behavioral problems in childhood. Eur J Nutr 47, 38–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Jảuregui-Lobera I (2014) Iron deficiency and cognitive functions. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 10, 2087–2095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.World Health Organization (2018) Lead poisoning and Health. Geneva:WHO: (accessed 4th October 2019) [Google Scholar]
  • 6.American Academy of Pediatrics Council on Environmental Health (2016) Prevention of Childhood Lead Toxicity. Pediatrics 138e 20161493, 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Environmental Health (2005) Lead exposure in children: prevention, detection, and management. Pediatrics 116, 1036–1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hegazy AA, Zaher MM, Abd El-Hafez MA et al. (2010) Relation between anemia and blood levels of lead, copper, zinc and iron among children. BMC Res Notes 3, 133–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Wigg NR (2001) Low-level lead exposure and children. J Paediatr Child Health 37, 423–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mohan VR, Sharma S, Ramanujam K et al. (2014) Effects of elevated blood lead levels in preschool children in urban Vellore. Indian Pediatr 51, 621–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.MAL-ED Network Investigators (2014) The MAL-ED study: a multinational and multidisciplinary approach to understand the relationship between enteric pathogens, malnutrition, gut physiology, physical growth, cognitive development, and immune responses in infants and children up to 2 years of age in resource-poor environments. Clin Infect Dis : an official publication of the Infectious Diseases Society of America 59 Suppl 4, S193–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.John SM, Thomas RJ, Kaki S et al. (2014) Establishment of the MAL-ED birth cohort study site in Vellore, Southern India. Clin Infect Dis : an official publication of the Infectious Diseases Society of America 59 Suppl 4, S295–299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bayley N (2005) Bayley scales of infant and toddler development III. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gardner H (1993) The Theory of Multiple Intelligence (10th Anniversary Edition). 10th Edition ed New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Murray-Kolb LE, Rasmussen ZA, Scharf RJ et al. (2014) The MAL-ED cohort study: methods and lessons learned when assessing early child development and caregiving mediators in infants and young children in 8 low- and middle-income countries. Clin Infect Dis: an official publication of the Infectious Diseases Society of America 59 Suppl 4, S261–272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH (2003) Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Psaki SR, Seidman JC, Miller M et al. (2014) Measuring socioeconomic status in multicountry studies: results from the eight-country MAL-ED study. Popul Health Metr 12, 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mal-Ed Network Investigators (2017) Childhood stunting in relation to the pre- and postnatal environment during the first 2 years of life: The MAL-ED longitudinal birth cohort study. PLoS Med 14, e1002408–e1002408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cook JD, Flowers CH, Skikne BS (2003) The quantitative assessment of body iron. Blood 101, 3359–3364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.World Health Organization (2011). Haemoglobin concentrations for the diagnosis of anaemia and assessment of severity. Geneva:WHO: https://www.who.int/vmnis/indicators/haemoglobin.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Schober P, Vetter TR (2018) Repeated Measures Designs and Analysis of Longitudinal Data: If at First You Do Not Succeed—Try, Try Again. Anesth Analg. 127, 569–575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Senn S, Stevens L, Chaturvedi N (2000) Repeated measures in clinical trials: simple strategies for analysis using summary measures. Stat Med. 19, 861–877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kotecha PV (2011) Nutritional Anemia in Young Children with Focus on Asia and India. Indian J Community Med 36, 8–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Subramaniam G, Girish M (2015) Iron deficiency anemia in children. Indian J Pediatr 82, 558–564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dos Santos RF, Gonzalez ES, de Albuquerque EC et al. (2011) Prevalence of anemia in under five-year-old children in a children’s hospital in Recife, Brazil. Rev Bras Hematol Hemoter 33, 100–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Reinbott A, Jordan I, Herrmann J et al. (2016) Role of Breastfeeding and Complementary Food on Hemoglobin and Ferritin Levels in a Cambodian Cross-Sectional Sample of Children Aged 3 to 24 Months. PloS one 11, e0150750–e0150750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ministry of Health and Family Welfare (2016) National Family Health Survey 4: NFHS 4. Mumbai: International Institute for Population Studies. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Thalanjeri P, Karanth H, Shankar VMS, Kutty K (2016) Impact of iron deficiency anemia on cognition of school children of South India Indian J Clin Anat Physiol 3, 135–138. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.More S, Shivkumar VB, Gangane N et al. (2013) Effects of Iron Deficiency on Cognitive Function in School Going Adolescent Females in Rural Area of Central India. Anemia 2013, 5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Batra J, Sood A (2005) Iron deficiency anaemia: Effect on congnitive development in children: A review. Indian J Clin Biochem 20, 119–125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Darnton-Hill I, Mkparu UC (2015) Micronutrients in pregnancy in low- and middle-income countries. Nutrients 7, 1744–1768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Joo EY, Kim KY, Kim DH et al. (2016) Iron deficiency anemia in infants and toddlers. Blood Res. 51, 268–273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Meinzen-Derr JK, Guerrero ML, Altaye M et al. (2006) Risk of Infant Anemia Is Associated with Exclusive Breast-Feeding and Maternal Anemia in a Mexican Cohort. J Nutr 136, 452–458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Brittenham GM (2005) Disorders of iron metabolism: Iron deficiency and overload In Hematology Basic Principles and Practice 4th ed, pp. 481[Hoffman R, Benz EJ Jr, Shattil SJ, editor]. New York: Churchill Livingstone. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.van der Merwe LF, Eussen SR (2017) Iron status of young children in Europe. Am J Clin Nutr 106, 1663s–1671s. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wenger MJ, DellaValle DM (2017) Effect of iron deficiency on simultaneous measures of behavior, brain activity, and energy expenditure in the performance of a cognitive task. Nutr Neurosci. 7, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Larson LM, Phiri KS, Pasricha SR (2017) Iron and Cognitive Development: What Is the Evidence? Ann Nutr Metab.71 Suppl 3, 25–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kalra V, Chitralekha KT, Dua T et al. (2003) Blood lead levels and risk factors for lead toxicity in children from schools and an urban slum in Delhi. J Trop Pediatr 49, 121–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kalra V, Sahu JK, Bedi P et al. (2013) Blood lead levels among school children after phasing-out of leaded petrol in Delhi, India. Indian J Pediatr. 80, 636–640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Palaniappan K, Roy A, Balakrishnan K et al. (2011) Lead exposure and visual-motor abilities in children from Chennai, India. Neurotoxicology 32, 465–470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.World Health Organization (2010). Childhood Lead Poisoning. Geneva: WHO: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/lead-poisoning-and-health [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Dhimal M, Karki KB, Aryal KK et al. (2017) High blood levels of lead in children aged 6–36 months in Kathmandu Valley, Nepal: A cross-sectional study of associated factors. PloS one 12, e0179233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Roy A, Hu H, Bellinger DC et al. (2009) Predictors of blood lead in children in Chennai, India (2005–2006). Int J Occup Environ Health 15, 351–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Chaudhary S, Firdaus U, Ali SM et al. (2018) Factors Associated With Elevated Blood Lead Levels in Children. Indian Pediatr 55, 38–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sindhu KN, Bondu JD, Ganesan SK et al. (2018) Blood Lead Levels in Mother-Infant Pairs. Ind J Ped. 85, 1143–1144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wani AL, Ara A, Usmani JA (2015) Lead toxicity: a review. Interdisciplinary toxicology 8, 55–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Bandeen-Roche K, Glass TA, Bolla KI et al. (2009) Cumulative lead dose and cognitive function in older adults. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass) 20, 831–839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Pocock SJ, Smith M, Baghurst P (1994) Environmental lead and children’s intelligence: a systematic review of the epidemiological evidence. BMJ (Clinical research ed) 309, 1189–1197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Needleman HL, Schell A, Bellinger D et al. (1990) The long-term effects of exposure to low doses of lead in childhood. An 11-year follow-up report. New Eng J Med. 322, 83–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Lopes AC, Peixe TS, Mesas AE et al. (2016) Lead Exposure and Oxidative Stress: A Systematic Review. Rev Environ Contam Toxicol 236, 193–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Mah V, Jalilehvand F (2012) Lead(II) complex formation with glutathione. Inorg Chem 51, 6285–6298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lenroot RK, Giedd JN (2006) Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 30, 718–729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Dallman PR (1993) Iron deficiency anemia: A synthesis of current scientific knowledge and U.S. recommendations for prevention and treatment In Iron Deficiency Anemia: Recommended Guidelines for the Prevention, Detection, and Management Among US Children and Women of Childbearing Age [Institute of Medicine, editor]. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Beard JL, Connor JR (2003) Iron status and neural functioning. Ann Rev Nutr 23, 31–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Chauhan U, Golhar S, Dahake P (2016) Correlation between Iron Deficiency Anemia and Cognitive Achievement in School Aged Children. . Ann Int Med Den Res 2, 178–180. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Scott SP, Murray-Kolb LE (2016) Iron Status Is Associated with Performance on Executive Functioning Tasks in Nonanemic Young Women. J Nutr. 146, 30–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Guo XM, Liu H, Qian J (2015) Daily iron supplementation on cognitive performance in primary-school-aged children with and without anemia: a meta-analysis. Int J Clin Exp Med 8, 16107–16111. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hermoso M, Vucic V, Vollhardt C et al. (2011) The effect of iron on cognitive development and function in infants, children and adolescents: a systematic review. Ann Nutr Metab 59, 154–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gecz J, Mulley J (2000) Genes for cognitive function: developments on the X. Genome research 10, 157–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Giustardi A, Stablum M, De Martino A (2011) Mother infant relationship and bonding myths and facts. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 24, 59–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Murray-Kolb LE, Rasmussen ZA, Scharf RJ et al. (2014) The MAL-ED Cohort Study: Methods and Lessons Learned When Assessing Early Child Development and Caregiving Mediators in Infants and Young Children in 8 Low- and Middle-Income Countries. Clin Infect Dis:: An Official Publication of the Infectious Diseases Society of America 59, S261–S272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM et al. (2007) Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet (London, England) 369, 145–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Huttenlocher J, Haight W, Bryk A, Seltzer M, & Lyons T (1991) Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–248. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Stolarova M, Brielmann AA, Wolf C et al. (2016) Early Vocabulary in Relation to Gender, Bilingualism, Type, and Duration of Childcare. Adv Cogn Psychol 12, 130–144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES