Abstract
Several fMRI studies have shown that the superior cerebellum exhibits load-dependent activations during encoding of letters in a Sternberg verbal working memory (VWM) task. It has been hypothesized that the cerebellum regulates the acquisition of sensory data across all modalities, and thus, that VWM load activations may reflect high- vs low-load differences in sensory acquisition demands. Therefore, increased difficulty in sensory data acquisition should elicit greater activation in the cerebellum. The present fMRI study manipulated sensory acquisition in VWM by presenting visually degraded and non-degraded stimuli with high and low memory loads, thereby identifying load-dependent regions of interest in the cerebellum, and then testing if these regions showed greater activation for degraded stimuli. Results yielded partial support for the sensory acquisition hypothesis in a load-dependent region of the vermis, which showed significantly greater activation for degraded relative to non-degraded stimuli. Because eye movements did not differ for these stimulus types, and degradation-related activations were present after covarying eye movements, this activation appears to be related to perceptual rather than oculomotor demands. In contrast to the vermis, load-sensitive regions of the cerebellar hemispheres did not show increased activation for degraded stimuli. These findings point to an overall function of association-based prediction that may underlie general cerebellar function, with perceptual prediction of stimuli from partial representations occurring in the vermis, and articulatory prediction occurring in the hemispheres.
Keywords: cerebellum, cognition, eye tracking, working memory, sensory acquisition
Introduction
A growing body of evidence is demonstrating that the cerebellum is not only involved in motor coordination and motor learning, but also in non-motor functions such as linguistic processes (for a review see Mariën & Borgatti, 2018), affective processing and emotional regulation (e.g., Lupo et al., 2018; Jeremy D. Schmahmann, 2019; J. D. Schmahmann & Sherman, 1998), executive functions (Bellebaum & Daum, 2007; Bürk et al., 2003), working memory (e.g., Desmond et al., 1997; Hayter et al., 2007; Peterburs et al., 2010; Ravizza et al., 2006), and timing (Breska & Ivry, 2016; Coull et al., 2011; Ivry, 1997). It has been suggested that the cerebellum may subserve overarching functions irrespective of the domain, by involvement in more general processes of performance monitoring (Peterburs & Desmond, 2016), prediction and the generation of internal models (Ito, 2008; Wolpert et al., 1998), sequence detection in the context of both cognitive and motor behavior (e.g., Leggio et al., 2011; Molinari et al., 2008), and automatization (Balsters & Ramnani, 2011; Ramnani, 2014). Indeed, given that the cerebellum is characterized by a rather uniform neuroarchitecture, and given the existence of closed cerebro-cerebellar input-output loops (Middleton & Strick, 1994; Strick et al., 2009), the idea of uniform cerebellar computations and overarching functions is very enticing. However, this idea has recently been challenged by the proposal of multiple functionality, i.e. the idea that the same underlying circuits may implement functionally distinct computations that may also differ across domains (Diedrichsen et al., 2019). Of course, this idea is not necessarily incompatible with the notion of overarching cerebellar functions, because the specific role of the cerebellar circuits could potentially change during brain development, and it is also possible that a universal cerebellar transform cannot be adequately captured in the proposed functional terms (Diedrichsen et al., 2019). In any case, the debate about universal and multiple functionality illustrates that the question of how the cerebellum contributes particularly to non-motor functions warrants further investigation.
One of the most extensively studied non-motor functions of the cerebellum is verbal working memory, i.e., the ability to temporary store and manipulate verbal information. According to Baddeley’s multi-component model (Baddeley, 1992), working memory comprises a “central executive” that serves as a supervisory module controlling the flow of information to the subordinate phonological loop and visuo-spatial sketchpad which serve to temporarily rehearse and manipulate verbal and visual information, respectively. The model was later amended with an episodic buffer that links information across domains and with long-term and semantic memory (Baddeley, 2000). Previous neuroimaging studies on the role of the cerebellum for verbal working memory have revealed that specific cerebellar subregions are differentially involved in encoding and maintenance/rehearsal. Activation in a superior region localized in lobule VI and Crus I during stimulus encoding that occurs in concert with activation in posterior frontal cortex has been linked to articulatory control (Chein & Fiez, 2001; Chen & Desmond, 2005a, 2005b), while activation in an inferior region in lobules VIII and VIIB has been associated with phonological storage (Chen & Desmond, 2005a; Desmond et al., 1997).
Importantly, neuroimaging investigations of cerebellar engagement in verbal working memory have allowed scientists to empirically test different hypotheses about the precise role of the cerebellum. For instance, a study that combined eye tracking and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (Peterburs et al., 2016) clarified that cerebellar activations during encoding in a verbal working memory task cannot be attributed to oculomotor behavior, as had been proposed based on fronto-cerebellar connectivity patterns (Doron et al., 2010). Moreover, recent findings suggest that the cerebellum may contribute to phonological storage by predictive processes: maintenance-related inferior cerebellar activations were shown to be sensitive to repeating stimulus sequences (Peterburs et al., 2019). Another theory of overarching cerebellar function states that the cerebellum is involved in perceptual processes (a comprehensive overview of converging evidence for this notion is provided in a consensus paper, Baumann et al., 2015) and particularly in sensory acquisition, i.e., the extraction and primary processing of sensory information from the environment. Specifically, it has been proposed that the cerebellum ameliorates the efficiency with which other brain regions perform their respective functions by monitoring and adjusting sensory data acquisition (Bower, 1997). This notion is supported by cerebellar activation in response to cutaneous stimulation in the absence of motor output, increased cerebellar activation when there is tactile discrimination demand, and by even more pronounced cerebellar activation when tactile discrimination is combined with motor activity (L. M. Parsons et al., 1997).
The question arises if the cerebellum is also involved in sensory acquisition outside of the tactile domain. A previous meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies on auditory processing revealed that specific cerebellar regions, here most notably left lateral Crus I, were activated consistently in a range of auditory tasks (Petacchi et al., 2005), which supports a general auditory function of the cerebellum that likely relates to sensory acquisition. Findings of impaired pitch discrimination in patients with cerebellar degeneration and a strong relationship between clinical ataxia scores (reflecting the extent of the cerebellar damage) and pitch discrimination deficits also corroborate this notion (Lawrence M. Parsons et al., 2009). Moreover, activation in left Crus I was shown to scale with perceptual demands in a task that required discrimination of both visual and auditory motion signals from noise, indicating that this region may be a supramodal sensory processing region (Baumann & Mattingley, 2010).
If the cerebellum is indeed involved in sensory acquisition, taxing (visual) sensory acquisition demand in a verbal working memory task should modulate the associated cerebellar activations. More specifically, load-dependent cerebellar activations during stimulus encoding in a verbal working memory task should be sensitive to a manipulation of stimulus quality. The present study tested this hypothesis by using degraded and non-degraded letter stimuli in a variant of the Sternberg task (Sternberg, 1966). Cognitive load-dependent regions of interest (ROIs) in the cerebellum were identified and tested for greater activation for degraded relative to non-degraded stimuli. In keeping with the notion that load-dependent cerebellar activations may reflect high- vs low-load differences in sensory acquisition demand, we hypothesized that increased difficulty in sensory data acquisition should elicit greater activations.
Materials and Methods
The present study was part of a larger investigation of cerebellar contributions to verbal working memory and presents follow-up analyses of data reported in a previous study (Peterburs et al., 2016). Importantly, this earlier study investigated whether cerebellar activation during encoding could be related to eye movements and oculomotor processing, while the present work was specifically focused on elucidating sensory acquisition functions of the cerebellum. To this end, the present study presents novel analyses of the previously unanalyzed factor stimulus quality (see task description below for details).
Subjects
Fifteen healthy adult volunteers (10 women, 5 men) were recruited from the Baltimore community by public advertisement and on social media. Mean age was 31.6 ± 9.4 years, ages ranged from 22 to 54 years. All subjects were native English speakers, right-handed according to self-report, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Exclusion criteria included a history of neurological or psychiatric illness or head trauma, current medication affecting the central nervous system, and the following criteria pertaining to MRI scanning: claustrophobia, implanted electric or ferromagnetic devices, and pregnancy. Mean educational attainment was 18.2 ± 2 years (range: 15–21). All subjects gave written informed consent prior to participation and received monetary compensation for participation and travel expenses. The study conforms to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine Institutional Review Board.
Sternberg verbal working memory task
The experimental task was a variant of the Sternberg task (Sternberg, 1966). In each trial, subjects had to keep a briefly presented set of consonants in memory by rehearsal until a probe letter was presented, and to then indicate by button press whether this probe matched one of letters in the initially presented set. The Sternberg task can be divided into discrete phases of stimulus encoding, maintenance, and retrieval which differ in terms of sensory and motor demands. The present variant of the task used normal and degraded stimulus quality (see Figure 1) in order to tax sensory acquisition demand (please see below for behavioral findings from a supplementary study that confirmed increased sensory acquisition demand for the present degraded compared to the non-degraded stimuli). To tax oculomotor demand, stimulus spacing was varied in two levels (close or wide). Luminance was equated for all stimuli. In order to manipulate cognitive and articulatory load, 2- and 4-letter consonant sets were used. As shown in Figure 1, percent sign (%) placeholders were displayed in unused positions in the consonant sets with only two letters. As mentioned in the previous study (Peterburs et al., 2016), the task also included overt and covert rehearsal instructions at the beginning of each trial. However, since sensory acquisition was expected to only affect the encoding phase, trials were pooled across rehearsal conditions for the present analyses. In addition, trials were also pooled across stimulus spacing conditions.
Figure 1 illustrates the sequence and time course of stimulus presentation in one trial of the Sternberg task. Each trial started with a fixation cross that was presented for 3 to 5 seconds (which implemented jitter in trial onsets for fMRI analysis), followed by a set of 2 or 4 consonants that was presented for 2 seconds (encoding phase). A blank screen was presented in the maintenance phase for 4 or 6 seconds, while subjects were rehearsing the consonants. Last, the probe was presented for 3 seconds, and subjects decided whether it was a “match” or “no match” by pressing one of two response buttons with their right index or middle finger. Task instructions emphasized both speed and accuracy, and accordingly, response time (RT) and accuracy were recorded for each trial. Subjects completed 10 practice trials outside the scanner prior to starting the experiment. The task consisted of 3 runs with 64 trials each, amounting to 192 trials in total. Stimulus quality (degraded or normal), spacing (wide or close), and cognitive load (high or low) were balanced in each run. Trial order within each run was pseudorandomized so that presentation of identical parameters was limited to three consecutive trials. Each run contained 24 unique match and 24 unique no-match trials as well as 16 trials without a probe. The latter were included to allow the hemodynamic response to fully return to baseline following the maintenance phase. In these trials, a blank screen was presented in the retrieval phase, and no response was expected. For trials with probe, positions of the target letter in the initial sequences were counterbalanced. Task completion took approximately 55 minutes and included short breaks between runs.
Stimulus presentation was controlled with E-Prime 2 software (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, USA) running on a Hewlett Packard xw4300 workstation. The visual display was rear-projected onto a screen in the MRI scanner located behind the participant and then reflected onto a mirror fixed to the head coil within the participant’s immediate line of view. Responses were recorded using two Velcro-connected fiber optic button boxes (MRA, Inc., Washington, PA).
Behavioral test of increased sensory acquisition demand by degraded stimuli
In order to directly test whether degraded stimuli were producing increases in sensory processing, a behavioral pilot experiment was conducted with a sample of 16 healthy adult volunteers. Subjects completed a target detection task in each trial of which they received a lowercase target letter, followed immediately by one of the 4 types of visual stimuli that had been presented during the fMRI verbal working memory task described above. The subject’s task was to simply indicate as rapidly as possible whether or not the uppercase version of the target letter appeared among the 4 items of the consonant set. The increase in reaction time (RT) needed to make this decision for degraded relative to non-degraded stimuli was used to assess the increase in sensory acquisition demand. Two stimulus lists were created such that the letters that were degraded in list 1 were non-degraded in list 2 and vice versa. Half of the subjects (n = 8) received list 1 and the other half received list 2. A repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the within-subject factors load (2 letters or 4 letters) and stimulus quality (degraded or non-degraded) revealed only a significant main effect of stimulus quality (F[1, 15] = 60.269, p < .001], with mean RT values of 799 ± 39 ms for degraded stimuli and 716 ± 33 ms for non-degraded stimuli. These results support the notion that visual degradation significantly increased sensory processing.
Recording of eye movements during the task
Eye movements were recorded during scanning with an Arrington Research (Scottsdale, AZ, USA) MR-compatible infrared eye tracker and the ViewPoint® user interface at a sampling rate of 60 Hz. The camera was fixed to the scanner cot just outside the head coil with a flexible mount and pointed towards the left eye. Gaze point coordinates were recorded as relative position on the screen, [0,0] reflecting the upper left and [1,1] the lower right corner. Prior to the first task run, a 9-point auto-calibration procedure as implemented in ViewPoint® software was applied. Here, calibration points were presented at 9 equidistant locations in the stimulus display in randomized order, and subjects were instructed to foveate on each point. After all points had been presented, the recorded locations were superimposed on a 3×3 grid and visually checked for calibration quality. Calibration accuracy was determined based on the grid’s geometry: a rectilinear configuration indicated successful calibration. If the grid was distorted, single calibration points were re-presented to improve calibration.
Eye movement data were analyzed off-line using MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, Massachusetts, USA). First, data were segmented according to the onset of encoding phase of each trial. Next, these segments were scanned for blink-related and other artifacts by calculating the mean and standard deviation (SD) for pupil width and by then removing data points for which pupil width fell 2 SDs above or below the mean. This procedure was applied because we assumed that pronounced pupil dilatation changes were unlikely given that lighting was consistent during MR scanning and throughout the entire duration of experimental task. Consequently, rapid large changes in pupil width should reflect blinks (which cause the pupil to be temporarily covered by the eye lid) or scanner-related distortions which could temporarily disrupt the eye tracker’s pupil detection algorithm, resulting in incorrect gaze coordinates. Next, point-to-point absolute distances between data points were calculated and summed separately for x and y gaze point coordinates, yielding quantitative horizontal (x) and vertical (y) eye movement parameters for each trial. These parameters thus reflected the average cumulative distance the eyes “travelled” per trial/volume. Larger values reflected more eye movement, irrespective of direction or type of movement. For example, scores close to 1 for horizontal eye movement indicated that the eyes traveled a distance equivalent to the length of the stimulus display. Quantitative eye movement parameters were also determined for each volume acquired during scanning in order to generate an eye movement regressor to be included in the functional MRI analysis (see below for details).
Analysis of working memory behavioral data
Accuracy, RT, and quantitative eye movement parameters obtained during fMRI scanning were analyzed with separate 2 × 2 repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with load (high or low) and stimulus quality (non-degraded or degraded) as within-subject factors. Post hoc t tests were performed to resolve interactions. The significance level was set to p < .05.
MRI data acquisition and analysis
MRI data were acquired using a 3.0T Philips Intera scanner (Philips, Eindhoven, NL). The structural MRI protocol consisted of a T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence (TR = 6.97 ms; TE = 3.3 ms; TI = 982 ms; flip angle = 8°, in-plane resolution = 0.75 mm; slice thickness = 1 mm; 170 sagittal slices; FOV = 240 mm; 1 NEX). Functional MRI data were collected using three T2*-weighted gradient echo EPI pulse sequences (TR = 1000 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 61°; in-plane resolution = 2.75 mm; slice thickness = 6 mm, gap = 1 mm; 20 oblique-axial slices; FOV = 240 mm; 1 NEX). Images were acquired in the oblique-axial plane rotated 25° clockwise with respect to the AC-PC line in order to optimize imaging of the cerebellum and neocortex. The use of a 1000 ms TR, jittered trial onsets, variable duration in the maintenance phase, and no-probe trials has been used previously to analyze the separate encoding, maintenance and retrieval phases of the task, and simulations confirmed that voxels responding to any combination of the 3 task phases could be identified (Chen and Desmond, 2005b). The number of acquired volumes within each of the three runs ranged from 944 to 953. The start of the fMRI scan was synchronized with the start of each experimental run using E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, USA).
SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK) was used for preprocessing (and statistical computations. Standard image preprocessing steps were performed: slice timing correction (reference = middle slice), motion correction, anatomical coregistration, normalization to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) stereotaxic space, spatial smoothing (FWHM = 5 mm). Normalization was performed based on gray and white matter segmentation as implemented in SPM. This procedure involves combining registration to standardized space and tissue classification (as gray matter, white matter, or cerebrospinal fluid) in a single generative model that includes parameters for volume intensity non-uniformity (Ashburner & Friston, 2005). This procedure has been shown to yield cerebellar output that closely corresponds to that of SUIT (as opposed to the SPM default nonlinear normalization method, see Diedrichsen et al., 2009).
First level fMRI analysis.
Individual statistical maps were computed for each subject using the general linear model approach as implemented in SPM8, with high pass filtering of 128 s. Since the present investigation focused on whether sensory acquisition processes could be responsible for working memory load dependent activations, fMRI analysis was focused on the encoding phase of the task, but the other task phases (maintenance and retrieval) were nevertheless also modelled in the GLM, as were the high vs low load and degraded vs non-degraded trial types. Thus, because there were 2 load conditions, 2 stimulus quality conditions, and trials were divided into 3 phases, each scan had a total of 12 task-related regressors for first-level analysis. The duration of these regressors was 2 seconds for the encoding phase, 4 or 6 seconds for the maintenance phase, and 3 seconds for the retrieval phase. For no-probe trials, the retrieval phase was omitted. For fMRI analyses, these regressors were convolved with the SPM canonical hemodynamic response function. Only correct trials were modelled, given that overall performance accuracy was very high (94.5%). Additional regressors of no interest for each scan included motion parameters computed from motion correction and artifacts identified with Artifact Detection Tools (ART; https://www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect), a toolbox for fMRI post-processing optimized for artifact removal. Because each subject completed three scans total for the fMRI session, the GLM for first stage analysis included all 3 scans, and contrasts were pooled across these scans. Contrasts of interest that were included in the second stage (random effects) analysis were specific to the encoding phase and included working memory load (high – low), stimulus quality (degraded – nondegraded), and the load x stimulus quality interaction. These were each implemented as t-test contrasts.
Second level fMRI analysis
Second level (random effects) analyses were performed by computing (from the first level analysis) one contrast volume per subject for the working memory load, stimulus quality, and load x quality interaction contrasts, and then using these volumes to calculate one-sample t-test values at every voxel (testing the null hypothesis that the contrast value was 0). Because behavioral analyses indicated that eye movements differences during the encoding phases of degraded vs non-degraded stimulus trials approached significance, in the fMRI second level analysis of the degraded – non-degraded contrast, trial-wise eye movement parameters were included as a covariate. The analysis strategy at group level was to first identify cerebellar activations in left and right hemispheral lobule VI/Crus I and vermis, which have been shown to exhibit a significant working memory load effect (high > low) in previous studies (Chen & Desmond, 2005a, 2005b; Desmond et al., 1997). We identified these clusters in the designated regions using a voxel-wise significance level of p < .005. These clusters were used to create activation-defined regions of interest (ROIs), consisting of all contiguous voxels that reached the p value threshold. Further analyses of stimulus quality were conducted on these ROIs using a small volume correction (voxel-wise p < .005 threshold, peak FWE-corrected p < .05). In addition, we performed FWE-corrected (voxel-wise p < .005 threshold, peak FWE-corrected p < .05) whole brain analysis of the stimulus quality effect (degraded > nondegraded), and subsequently analyzed the load effect (high > low) in brain regions that showed greater activation for degraded relative to non-degraded stimuli. To further examine if additional regions of the brain were responsive to both sensory acquisition demand and working memory load, we performed an additional conjunction analysis in which voxels that were significant for both load and stimulus quality contrasts (at a voxel-wise p < .05 to maximize potential regions of overlap) were identified. We then examined peak activation within these clusters to identify all conjunction regions in which peak activation within the cluster exceeded p <.005 for both the load contrast and for the stimulus quality contrast.
MNI coordinates of activation peaks were transformed into the coordinate system of the Talairach and Tourneaux stereotaxic atlas (Talairach et al., 1988) using the MNI to Talairach transformation described by Lancaster et al. (1997) in order to make anatomical determinations of the neocortical activations (which were verified by manual inspection). However, MNI coordinates are reported in tables and figures. For the cerebellum, MNI coordinates were referenced with the SUIT atlas (Diedrichsen et al., 2009).
Results
Behavioral data
Mean percentages of correct responses according to load and stimulus quality are provided in Figure 2A. For accuracy, the ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of load (F[1, 14] = 7.492, p = .016), indicating that response accuracy was higher for trials with low (mean = 95.74%, SE = 0.57) relative to high cognitive load (mean = 93.33%, SE = 0.69). Moreover, there was a trend for a main effect of stimulus quality (F[1, 14] = 3.276, p = .092), reflecting slightly better performance for non-degraded (mean = 95.27%, SE = 0.62) compared to degraded stimuli (mean = 93.80%, SE = 0.60). The load x stimulus quality interaction failed to reach significance (p = .395).
Mean RTs for correct responses according to load and stimulus quality are provided in Figure 2B. The ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of load (F[1, 14] = 60.188, p < .001), with longer RTs when cognitive load was high (mean = 836 ms, SE = 34) compared to low (mean = 720 ms, SE = 30). No other effects reached statistical significance (all p’s > .461).
Eye movement means according to load and stimulus quality are provided in Fig 2C. Note that scores close to 1 for horizontal eye movement (as observed in the present task) suggests that the eyes traveled a distance equivalent to the length of the whole stimulus display which ranged from positional coordinate [0] for the left side to [1] for the right side. Eye movement analysis did not reveal any significant main effects or interactions (all p’s > .274), although there was a trend for a significant main effect of stimulus quality (F[1, 14] = 2.974, p = .107), pointing towards more movement for degraded (mean = 0.89, SE = 0.12) relative to non-degraded stimuli (mean = 0.86, SE = 0.11).
Imaging data
BOLD signal changes for the load effect were observed in mainly three cerebellar regions. Left hemispheral lobule VI, right hemispheral lobule VI, and vermis Crus II (see Figure 3 and Table 1) all showed greater activation for high compared to low load. Subsequent analysis of stimulus quality effects in these load-dependent activation-defined cerebellar ROIs yielded significantly increased activation for degraded relative to non-degraded stimuli only in vermis Crus II (see Figure 4), but no interaction of load and stimulus quality was observed.
Table 1:
Brain region | X | Y | Z | SPM {Z} | Size (mm3) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Significant cerebellar activations for high > low load during encoding | |||||
Left hemispheral lobule VI | −12 | −62 | −26 | 3.30 | 1006 |
Right hemispheral lobule VI | 16 | −64 | −28 | 2.95 | 2330 |
Vermis Crus II | 0 | −74 | −28 | 2.73 | 371 |
Right hemispheral lobule VIIb | 32 | −70 | −52 | 2.90 | 53 |
Significant activation for degraded > non-degraded in load-defined cerebellar ROIs | |||||
Vermis Crus II | −2 | −74 | −28 | 2.60 | 53 |
Significant extra-cerebellar activations for degraded > non-degraded during encoding | |||||
Left fusiform gyrus | −36 | −72 | −12 | 5.82 | 69295 |
Right inferior occipital gyrus | 40 | −74 | −4 | 4.94 | 80148 |
Significant activation for high > low load in stimulus quality- defined extra-cerebellar ROIs | |||||
Left fusiform gyrus | −36 | −74 | −4 | 3.62 | 4129 |
Additional analysis of extra-cerebellar effects of stimulus quality were investigated in a whole brain analysis. Consistent with the behavioral results found from our initial test of increased sensory acquisition demand by degraded stimuli (i.e., increased RT for degraded relative to non-degraded stimuli), greater activation for degraded vs non-degraded stimuli was found in a cluster in the left fusiform gyrus and a cluster in right inferior occipital gyrus, further indicating that the degradation manipulation had indeed increased sensory acquisition demand (see Figure 5 and Table 1). Using the respective activation-defined ROIs in left fusiform gyrus and right inferior occipital gyrus as ROIs, subsequent analyses of load identified only a small cluster in left fusiform gyrus that showed greater activation for high relative to low load; there were no suprathreshold clusters in the right inferior occipital region. However, fusiform activations were much more extensive for the stimulus quality effect than for the load effect.
For the conjunction analysis of working memory load and stimulus quality, the analysis revealed only 3 regions in which peak activity in the conjunction region reached p < .005 for both contrasts, the left fusiform gyrus region shown in Figure 5, the Vermis Crus II region shown in Figures 3 and 4, and a 900 mm3 region in the left middle frontal gyrus (Brodmann Area 6) located at MNI coordinates −34, 0, 46 using the load contrast peak, and at coordinates −36, 2, 46 using the stimulus quality contrast peak.
Discussion
The present study investigated whether cerebellar activations in a verbal working memory task were modulated by perceptual demands. In line with the a priori hypothesis, and in support of sensory acquisition as a general function of the cerebellum, load-dependent activations in vermis Crus II scaled with sensory acquisition demand and were increased for degraded relative to non-degraded stimuli. In contrast, activations in bilateral hemispheral lobule VI, which were also increased for high compared to low cognitive load, were unaffected by stimulus quality. In general, this result pattern suggests that the cerebellar hemispheres are involved in articulatory processing and representation, while perceptual processes related to the prediction of visual stimuli from partial representations occur in the vermis. Of note, cerebellar activations in the present study cannot be attributed to motor effects since eye movements were used as covariates in the analysis.
The remarkably uniform neuroarchitecture of the cerebellum and the identification of distinct reciprocal loops that connect different cerebellar regions to distinct motor and cognitive regions in the neocortex (Middleton & Strick, 1994; Strick et al., 2009) have inspired a search for overarching, domain-independent functions of the cerebellum. To date, a range of such functions have been proposed, e.g., prediction and internal models (Ito, 2008; Wolpert et al., 1998), general functions of performance monitoring (Peterburs & Desmond, 2016), automatization (Balsters & Ramnani, 2011), timing (Ivry, 1997), and sensory acquisition (Bower, 1997). The present study was aimed to further investigate the notion that the cerebellum is involved in perceptual processes (Baumann et al., 2015), and here specifically sensory acquisition. A Sternberg verbal working memory task was used because this type of task has been shown to reliably produce robust cerebellar activation patterns that inform about distinct functional roles of specific cerebellar subregions (e.g., Chen & Desmond, 2005a, 2005b; Desmond et al., 1997). Since sensory acquisition processes are most critical during initial stimulus encoding, data analysis in the present study focused on the encoding phase of the Sternberg task. In accordance with previous findings (Chein & Fiez, 2001; Chen & Desmond, 2005a, 2005b; Kirschen et al., 2005), cognitive load, i.e., the number of letters to be encoded into an articulatory code, modulated activations bilaterally in the cerebellar hemispheres in lobule VI. In previous studies, activations in right lobule VI occurred concordant with activations in left inferior frontal cortex, linking them to articulatory representations (Chein & Fiez, 2001; Chen & Desmond, 2005b).
Interestingly, in the present study, a smaller region in vermis Crus II was also sensitive to the load effect, with increased activation for high relative to low load. Importantly, activations in this region were also modulated by stimulus quality: in line with our a priori hypothesis, degraded stimuli that required more sensory processing than non-degraded stimuli, as confirmed by increased RTs in a separate behavioral test of the stimuli, were associated with increased activation. These effects of load and stimulus quality on activity in vermis Crus II are particularly interesting, because this area was shown to both receive projections from area 46 (middle frontal gyrus/dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) and project back to this area (rather than to M1 or oculomotor regions) in a primate neurophysiology study using neurotropic virus tracing in macaques (Kelly & Strick, 2003). Human area 46 and macaque area 46 were also shown to closely match in terms of cortical functional connectivity in a later study (Neubert et al., 2014), corroborating the notion of functional correspondence. Importantly, area 46 has been implicated in working memory for different types of stimuli in a large number of studies (for a meta-analysis of fMRI studies, see Owen et al., 2005), and the present findings of memory load and stimulus quality effects in a cerebellar region densely connected to area 46 are well in line with this.
Apart from vermis Crus II, increased activation for degraded relative to non-degraded stimuli was also found in the visual cortex in right inferior occipital gyrus and left fusiform gyrus. The left fusiform gyrus also showed load-dependent activation differences, albeit to a much lesser degree than the modulation by stimulus quality. Overall, this result pattern is consistent with increased sensory and attentional demand for degraded compared to non-degraded stimuli. Of note, adjacent vermal regions of lobule VI have been linked to processing of visual motion cues (Baumann & Mattingley, 2010) as well as biological motion based on point-light animations (e.g., Ferrari et al., 2019), while vermis lobule VII/Crus II has been associated with processing of emotionally salient stimuli (for a review, see Stoodley & Schmahmann, 2009).
Interestingly, conjunction analysis identified (in addition to vermis Crus II and left fusiform gyrus) a small region in left middle frontal gyrus (BA 6) that was also responsive to manipulations of both load and stimulus quality. Prior work has linked BA 6 to executive processes during maintenance and thus monitoring and manipulation demands (e.g., Wager & Smith, 2003; Nee et al., 2012). Previous studies from our lab (Desmond et al, 1997, Chen and Desmond 2005a, 2005b) have linked superior cerebellar activation to posterior frontal activations associated with the articulatory control system of the Baddeley model of verbal working memory (Baddeley, 1992; 2000). The present results extend these findings and suggest that a BA6-vermis crus II circuit could subserve both working memory load and sensory acquisition demands.
In general, increased activations for decreased signal strength (and hence increased sensory demand) have previously been reported in a task in which visual and auditory motion had to be distinguished from noise (Baumann & Mattingley, 2010). It has been proposed that cerebellar input is used to ameliorate processing/signal acquisition in sensory regions, e.g., by facilitating computational efficiency and providing sensory data control (Bower, 1997). This notion was based on early fMRI investigations in the tactile domain that disentangled mere finger movement control from the use of the fingers as sensory devices (Gao et al., 1996). Specifically, it was shown that activation in the dentate nucleus, the sole output node for the cerebellar hemispheres, was greater for sensory discrimination tasks with and without movement than for a task involving finger movement not associated with tactile sensory discrimination. In the auditory domain, a meta-analysis of 15 studies showed consistent cerebellar activations across a range of different simple auditory tasks that could not be attributed to attentional demand (Petacchi et al., 2005), thus supporting a role for the cerebellum in auditory sensory processing that is consistent with the sensory acquisition hypothesis. In line with this, patients with cerebellar degenerative disease relative to healthy control subjects show impaired pitch discrimination despite normal hearing thresholds and similar performance in control tasks involving sustained attention and verbal auditory working memory (Lawrence M. Parsons et al., 2009). Aside from these empirical findings, a role for the cerebellum in sensory acquisition is also supported by evolutionary considerations. Paulin (1993) provided several examples of species in which lobular expansion in the cerebellum seems to track sensory demands better than motor demands. For example, echolocating microbats and whales (which have different motor requirements, i.e., bats use distal muscles for movement whereas whales use axial muscles) show similar expansion in vermal lobule VIII that is not seen in non-echolocating bats and whales. Paulin therefore proposed that the cerebellum may serve as a tracking system that is important for the control and coordination of movements which requires that moving objects as well as own movements are tracked and that sensory consequences of movements are analyzed appropriately. Importantly, the cerebellar association with passive echolocation and passive electrolocation is analogous to that found for active sensory systems, indicating that cerebellar sensory acquisition functions are not dependent on movement (Paulin, 1993). Along these lines, in the present working memory task with visual stimuli, it is conceivable that cerebellar input may be used to improve the prediction of stimulus identity when stimuli are degraded. This would be consistent with the notion of cerebellar internal models and predictive functions (Ito, 2008; Wolpert et al., 1998).
The cerebellar role in sensory acquisition yields interesting implications for processes of learning and expertise-building. A very recent study revealed a pattern of reduced cerebellar activation that accompanied higher sensory cortical activity in experienced archery athletes. In contrast, in non-athletic control participants, visual activations were found to co-occur with extensive cerebellar activation (Lo et al., 2019). This result pattern appears to suggest that cerebellar sensory data control may be needed less when expertise is high, i.e., when sensory processing is already optimized. Future studies should investigate if this also applies to other non-motor functions.
To conclude, the present findings provide partial support for the sensory acquisition hypothesis and point to an overall function of association-based prediction that may underlie general cerebellar function, with perceptual prediction of stimuli from partial representations occurring in vermal and articulatory prediction occurring in hemispheral regions. Such a function is well in line with performance monitoring accounts of cerebellar function and with the generation of internal models for both motor and non-motor functions. On the other hand, the idea of an overarching cerebellar function has been challenged by the proposal of cerebellar multiple functionality (Diedrichsen et al., 2019). Along these lines, the regional non-uniformity of function observed in the present study (i.e., load but not sensory acquisition sensitivity of the cerebellar hemispheres) could also be viewed as evidence supporting cerebellar multiple functionality. More research is therefore needed to unravel the specific nature of the cerebellar involvement in well-defined task domains.
Funding / Acknowledgements
This work was funded by the Germany Research Society (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, DFG; grant number PE 2077/1-1 to J.P.), and by the National Institutes of Health (NIMH grant number R01MH104588 to J.E.D., and grant number K01AA020873 to D.T.C.). The MRI equipment in this study was funded by NIH grant 1S10OD021648. Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest: The authors declare that they had no conflicts of interest.
References
- Ashburner J, Friston KJ (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage, 26(3):839–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A (1992). Working memory. Science (New York, N.Y.), 255(5044), 556–559. 10.1126/science.1736359 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley A (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balsters JH, & Ramnani N [Narender] (2011). Cerebellar plasticity and the automation of first-order rules. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(6), 2305–2312. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4358-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumann O, Borra RJ, Bower J. M [James M.], Cullen KE, Habas C, Ivry RB, Leggio M, Mattingley JB, Molinari M, Moulton EA, Paulin M. G [Michael G.], Pavlova MA, Schmahmann J. D [Jeremy D.], & Sokolov AA (2015). Consensus paper: The role of the cerebellum in perceptual processes. Cerebellum (London, England), 14(2), 197–220. 10.1007/s12311-014-0627-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumann O, & Mattingley JB (2010). Scaling of neural responses to visual and auditory motion in the human cerebellum. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 30(12), 4489–4495. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5661-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellebaum C, & Daum I [Irene] (2007). Cerebellar involvement in executive control. Cerebellum (London, England), 6(3), 184–192. 10.1080/14734220601169707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. M [James M.]. (1997). Control of Sensory Data Acquisition. In International Review of Neurobiology (Vol. 41, pp. 489–513). Elsevier. 10.1016/s0074-7742(08)60367-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breska A, & Ivry RB (2016). Taxonomies of Timing: Where Does the Cerebellum Fit In? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 8, 282–288. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bürk K, Globas C, Bösch S, Klockgether T, Zühlke C, Daum I[I], & Dichgans J (2003). Cognitive deficits in spinocerebellar ataxia type 1, 2, and 3. Journal of Neurology, 250(2), 207–211. 10.1007/s00415-003-0976-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chein JM, & Fiez JA[JA] (2001). Dissociation of verbal working memory system components using a delayed serial recall task. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991), 11(11), 1003–1014. 10.1093/cercor/11.11.1003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen SHA, & Desmond JE (2005a). Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. NeuroImage, 24(2), 332–338. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen SHA, & Desmond JE (2005b). Temporal dynamics of cerebro-cerebellar network recruitment during a cognitive task. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1227–1237. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coull JT, Cheng R‑K, & Meck WH (2011). Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology : Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(1), 3–25. 10.1038/npp.2010.113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desmond JE, Gabrieli JDE, Wagner AD, Ginier BL, & Glover GH (1997). Lobular Patterns of Cerebellar Activation in Verbal Working-Memory and Finger-Tapping Tasks as Revealed by Functional MRI. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(24), 9675–9685. 10.1523/jneurosci.17-24-09675.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, Balsters JH, Flavell J, Cussans E, & Ramnani N [Narender] (2009). A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. NeuroImage, 46(1), 39–46. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diedrichsen J, King M, Hernandez-Castillo C, Sereno M, & Ivry RB (2019). Universal Transform or Multiple Functionality? Understanding the Contribution of the Human Cerebellum across Task Domains. Neuron, 102(5), 918–928. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.04.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doron KW, Funk CM, & Glickstein M (2010). Fronto-cerebellar circuits and eye movement control: A diffusion imaging tractography study of human cortico-pontine projections. Brain Research, 1307, 63–71. 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.10.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrari C, Ciricugno A, Battelli L, Grossman ED, & Cattaneo Z (2019). Distinct Cerebellar regions for Body Motion Discrimination. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. Advance online publication. 10.1093/scan/nsz088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao JH, Parsons LM[LM], Bower JM[JM], Xiong J, Li J, & Fox PT[PT] (1996). Cerebellum implicated in sensory acquisition and discrimination rather than motor control. Science (New York, N.Y.), 272(5261), 545–547. 10.1126/science.272.5261.545 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayter AL, Langdon DW, & Ramnani N[N] (2007). Cerebellar contributions to working memory. NeuroImage, 36(3), 943–954. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito M (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(4), 304–313. 10.1038/nrn2332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivry R (1997). Cerebellar Timing Systems. In International Review of Neurobiology (Vol. 41, pp. 555–573). Elsevier. 10.1016/S0074-7742(08)60370-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly RM, & Strick P. L [Peter L.] (2003). Cerebellar Loops with Motor Cortex and Prefrontal Cortex of a Nonhuman Primate. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(23), 8432–8444. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-23-08432.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschen MP, Chen SHA, Schraedley-Desmond P, & Desmond JE (2005). Load- and practice-dependent increases in cerebro-cerebellar activation in verbal working memory: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 24(2), 462–472. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster JL, Rainey LH, Summerlin JL, Freitas CS, Fox PT[PT], Evans AC, Toga AW, & Mazziotta JC (1997). Automated labeling of the human brain: A preliminary report on the development and evaluation of a forward-transform method. Human Brain Mapping, 5(4), 238–242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leggio MG, Chiricozzi FR, Clausi S, Tedesco A. M [Anna M.], & Molinari M (2011). The neuropsychological profile of cerebellar damage: The sequencing hypothesis. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 47(1), 137–144. 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lo Y‑C, Chen C‑M, Shen W‑C, & Shih Y‑L (2019). Cerebellar contributions to tactile perception in people with varying sensorimotor experiences: Examining the sensory acquisition hypothesis. Human Movement Science, 63, 45–52. 10.1016/j.humov.2018.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lupo M, Olivito G, Siciliano L, Masciullo M, Molinari M, Cercignani M, Bozzali M, & Leggio M (2018). Evidence of Cerebellar Involvement in the Onset of a Manic State. Frontiers in Neurology, 9, 774. 10.3389/fneur.2018.00774 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mariën P, & Borgatti R (2018). Language and the cerebellum. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 154, 181–202. 10.1016/B978-0-444-63956-1.00011-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Middleton FA, & Strick PL[PL] (1994). Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. Science (New York, N.Y.), 266(5184), 458–461. 10.1126/science.7939688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molinari M, Chiricozzi FR, Clausi S, Tedesco A. M [Anna Maria], Lisa M. de, & Leggio MG (2008). Cerebellum and detection of sequences, from perception to cognition. Cerebellum (London, England), 7(4), 611–615. 10.1007/s12311-008-0060-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nee DE, Brown JW, Askren MK, Berman MG, Demiralp E, Krawitz A, Jonides J (2013). A Meta-analysis of Executive Components of Working Memory. Cerebral Cortex, 23(2), 264–282. 10.1093/cercor/bhs007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neubert F‑X, Mars RB, Thomas AG, Sallet J, & Rushworth MFS (2014). Comparison of human ventral frontal cortex areas for cognitive control and language with areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron, 81(3), 700–713. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, & Bullmore E (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 46–59. 10.1002/hbm.20131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsons LM[LM], Bower JM[JM], Gao JH, Xiong J, Li J, & Fox PT[PT] (1997). Lateral cerebellar hemispheres actively support sensory acquisition and discrimination rather than motor control. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 4(1), 49–62. 10.1101/lm.4.1.49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsons L. M [Lawrence M.], Petacchi A, Schmahmann J. D [Jeremy D.], & Bower J. M [James M.] (2009). Pitch discrimination in cerebellar patients: Evidence for a sensory deficit. Brain Research, 1303, 84–96. 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulin MG[MG] (1993). The role of the cerebellum in motor control and perception. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 41(1), 39–50. 10.1159/000113822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petacchi A, Laird AR, Fox P. T [Peter T.], & Bower J. M [James M.] (2005). Cerebellum and auditory function: An ALE meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping, 25(1), 118–128. 10.1002/hbm.20137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterburs J, Bellebaum C, Koch B, Schwarz M, & Daum I [Irene] (2010). Working memory and verbal fluency deficits following cerebellar lesions: Relation to interindividual differences in patient variables. Cerebellum (London, England), 9(3), 375–383. 10.1007/s12311-010-0171-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterburs J, Blevins LC, Sheu Y‑S, & Desmond JE (2019). Cerebellar contributions to sequence prediction in verbal working memory. Brain Structure & Function, 224(1), 485–499. 10.1007/s00429-018-1784-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterburs J, Cheng DT, & Desmond JE (2016). The Association Between Eye Movements and Cerebellar Activation in a Verbal Working Memory Task. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991), 26(9), 3802–3813. 10.1093/cercor/bhv187 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterburs J, & Desmond JE (2016). The role of the human cerebellum in performance monitoring. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 38–44. 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramnani N [Narender] (2014). Automatic and controlled processing in the corticocerebellar system. Progress in Brain Research, 210, 255–285. 10.1016/B978-0-444-63356-9.00010-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravizza SM, McCormick CA, Schlerf JE, Justus T, Ivry RB, & Fiez J. A [Julie A.] (2006). Cerebellar damage produces selective deficits in verbal working memory. Brain : A Journal of Neurology, 129(Pt 2), 306–320. 10.1093/brain/awh685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD[JD], & Sherman JC (1998). The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain : A Journal of Neurology, 121 (Pt 4), 561–579. 10.1093/brain/121.4.561 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann J. D [Jeremy D.] (2019). The cerebellum and cognition. Neuroscience Letters, 688, 62–75. 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg S (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science (New York, N.Y.), 153(3736), 652–654. 10.1126/science.153.3736.652 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoodley CJ, & Schmahmann J. D [Jeremy D.] (2009). Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage, 44(2), 489–501. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strick P. L [Peter L.], Dum RP, & Fiez J. A [Julie A.] (2009). Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 413–434. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talairach J, Tournoux P, & Rayport M (1988). Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system: An approach to cerebral imaging. Thieme. http://www.loc.gov/catdir/enhancements/fy1006/2010286519-d.html [Google Scholar]
- Wager TD, Smith EE (2003). Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(4), 255–274. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.4.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert DM, Miall RC, & Kawato M (1998). Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(9), 338–347. 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01221-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]