Abstract
The neuropeptide arginine-vasopressin (AVP) has long been implicated in the regulation of social behavior and communication, but the sources of AVP release relevant for behavior have not been precisely determined. Ablations of the sexually dimorphic AVP cells within the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), which are more numerous in males, affect social behavior differently in males and females. However, it is unknown whether these behavioral effects are caused by a reduction of AVP or of other factors associated with these cells. To test the role of AVP specifically, we used an shRNA viral construct to knock down AVP gene expression within the BNST of wild-type male and female mice, using scrambled sequence virus as a control, and evaluated subsequent changes in social behaviors (social investigation, ultrasonic vocalization (USV), scent marking, copulation, and aggression), or anxiety-like behaviors (elevated plus maze). We observed that, in males, knockdown of AVP expression in the BNST strongly reduced investigation of novel males, aggressive signaling towards other males (tail rattling, USV), and copulatory behavior, but did not alter attack initiation, other measures of social communication, or anxiety-like behaviors. In females, however, BNST AVP knockdown did not alter any of these behaviors. These results point to differential involvement of AVP derived from the BNST in social behavior.
Keywords: bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, mice, sex differences, social behavior, vasopressin
Introduction
The neuropeptide arginine-vasopressin (AVP) has been strongly implicated in the regulation of social behavior and communication across species1–5. Many studies report that vasopressin, acting via V1a receptors (V1aR), modulate these behaviors in sexually different ways2,6,7; however, the sources that drive these sex differences are not well described. In most animals, AVP is synthesized in several cell groups, each of which project to distinct brain areas8–10. Two sources likely to contribute to sexually differentiated effects of AVP are the neurons in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) and medial amygdala (MeA), which contribute to the most pronounced sex differences in AVP innervation of the brain11.
Several studies suggest a sexually differentiated role for AVP cells in the BNST in social behavior. For example, injections of AVP or AVP antagonists in BNST projection sites such as the lateral septum, lateral habenular nucleus, and ventral pallidum10 affect social behavior differently in males and females in rats and mice12–15. In addition, partial knockdown of AVP gene expression in the BNST reduces male, but not female, social interactions, while increasing male, but not female, aggression in finches4,16,17. In rats, intermale aggression correlates with AVP release in the septum and is reduced by intra-septal AVP antagonist application18. Overall, these studies indicate that BNST AVP is important for male-male interactions and for certain aspects of male prosocial communication, while playing a lesser role in female social behavior and communication.
Recently, we directly tested whether cells expressing AVP in the BNST are involved in social behaviors in mice19,20. We found that ablations of these cells reduced male-male investigation and increased male scent marking toward female stimuli19. These studies, however, left unresolved the critical question as to whether AVP or other neuroactive substances are responsible for the effects seen after removal of BNST AVP cells. Consequently, in order to specifically target AVP, we reduced AVP-expression in the BNST using viral expression of a short hairpin RNA (shRNA) targeted against AVP mRNA and tested the effects of this manipulation on social investigation, ultrasonic vocalization (USV), and urine marking, all aspects of mouse communication that show pronounced sex differences21–23.
Methods
Animals and Husbandry
All mice were maintained at 22°C on a 12:12 reverse light cycle with food and water available ad libitum and housed in individually ventilated cages (Animal Care Systems, Centennial, CO, USA) with ALPHA-dri bedding (Shepherd Specialty Papers, Watertown, TN), a nestlet square, and a housing tube. All animal procedures were performed in accordance with the Georgia State University Animal Care and Use Committee regulations and the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Subjects:
Forty-eight male and female C57BL/6J mice between 8 and 12 weeks of age were obtained from Jackson Laboratories (stock # 000664) and were singly-housed for a minimum of one week prior to testing.
Stimulus animals:
CD1(ICR) mice (Charles River Laboratories, Wilmington, MA, USA) were used as stimuli for behavioral testing and to provide male and female subjects with social experience, because strain differences between subjects and stimulus mice increase social investigation24. Mice were used at 9–16 weeks of age and were novel to the subject to which they were exposed.
Female stimulus mice were grouped-housed, ovariectomized, and implanted with an estradiol capsule (GDX+E; see below), and given two sexual experiences before testing. Two groups of stimulus males were used for behavioral testing. Mice that were used as subordinates in the home cage aggression tests and to provide aggressive experience to subjects, were grouped-housed, gonadectomized (GDX), and subjected to two aggressive encounters with a dominant male (see below). Mice in the second group, which provided sexual experience to female subjects and served as sexual partners during copulatory tests as well as stimulus animals in the three-chamber social test, were singly-housed, gonadectomized, implanted with testosterone (GDX+T; see below), and then given two sexual experiences before testing.
shRNA virus
We used an adeno-associated virus (AAV) expressing a short hairpin RNA (shRNA) to target Avp mRNA (AAV8-GFP-U6-mAVP-shRNA; titer: 1.0 × 1013 GC/mL; lot: 190930#33; sense target sequence: GGATGCTCAACACTACGCTCTCTCGAG AGAGCGTAGTGTTGAGCATCC; Vector Biolabs, Malvern, PA) and an AAV-expressing a scramble shRNA (AAV8-GFP-U6-scramble-shRNA; titer: 4 × 1013 GC/mL; lot: 19044–190820; Vector Biolabs, Malvern, PA), which does not target any known sequence as a control. The Avp-shRNA target sequence demonstrated 91% knockdown in vitro as tested by the manufacturer. BLAST searches did not reveal significant target alignment between the Avp-shRNA sequence and other coding mRNAs, including oxytocin, indicating that the shRNA targeted Avp mRNA specifically. Both scramble (control) and Avp-shRNA vectors express green fluorescent protein (GFP) to allow for visualization of the infected neurons.
Surgery
All surgeries were carried out using 1.5–2.5% isoflurane gas anesthesia in 100% oxygen; 3 mg/kg of carprofen was given subcutaneously before surgery to reduce pain.
Stereotaxic surgery
Mice were positioned in a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA, USA) with ear and incisor bars holding bregma and lambda level. After a midline scalp incision, a hand-operated drill was used to make holes in the skull, exposing the dura. 200 nl of Avp-shRNA or scramble-sequence virus was delivered bilaterally to the BNST (coordinates: AP +0.15 mm; ML ± 0.8 mm; DV 4.4 mm) at a rate of 100 nl/min using a 5 μl Hamilton syringe with a 30-gauge beveled needle mounted on a stereotaxic injector. Following virus delivery, the syringe was left in place for 10 minutes before slowly withdrawing it from the brain.
Gonadectomy and Hormone Treatment
Testes were removed after cauterizing the ductus deferens and blood supply via a midline abdominal incision. Silastic capsules (1.5 cm active length; 1.02 mm inner diameter, 2.16 mm outer diameter; Dow Corning Corporation, Midland, MI, USA) were filled with crystalline testosterone (T; Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA) and inserted subcutaneously between the scapulae after gonadectomy; this procedure leads to physiological levels of T25,26. To further reduce aggression in stimulus animals27, some males were gonadectomized, but did not receive a T implant (GDX).
The ovaries of stimulus female mice were removed after cauterizing its blood supply via an abdominal incision at the uterine horn. Silastic capsules (0.7 cm active length; 1.02 mm inner diameter, 2.16 mm outer diameter; Dow Corning Corporation, Midland, MI, USA) containing estradiol benzoate (E; diluted 1:1 with cholesterol) were implanted subcutaneously in the scapular region immediately following ovariectomy (GDX + E)28,29. To induce sexual receptivity, stimulus females were injected subcutaneously with 0.1 ml of progesterone (500 μg dissolved in sesame oil, Sigma, St. Louis, MO, USA) four hours preceding sexual experience, urine collection, and behavioral testing30.
Urine Collection
Pooled urine samples were collected from stimulus females induced into estrus and from stimulus males (5–10 mice per sample). Estrous state was verified by color, swelling, and expanded size of the vaginal opening31. To collect urine, mice were picked up by the tail base and held by dorsal neck skin; this method typically induced urination. If the mouse did not urinate, stroking its belly from an anterior to posterior direction stimulated bladder voiding. Each mouse provided 40–80 μl of urine that was pooled into a 1.5 ml Eppendorf tube. Urine samples were used fresh within one hour of collection to prevent chemosignal degradation32.
Social Experience
As opposite-sex sexual experience and attaining competitive status (“social dominance”) promote communicative behaviors32,33, mice received social experience over five consecutive days (sexual encounters on days 1 and 4, aggressive encounters on days 2 and 5; no encounters on day 3).
Sexual Experience
Subjects were given two opportunities to interact with either a stimulus female (for male subjects) or a stimulus male (for female subjects). A sexually-experienced stimulus mouse was placed in the subject’s home cage and removed the next day (first experience) or after ninety minutes (second experience). Subjects that did not ejaculate or elicit ejaculation (females) during the second sexual experience were removed from further testing.
Aggressive Experience
Male subjects were exposed to two interactions with a subordinate stimulus male treated with 50 μl of GDX+T male urine applied to its back, a manipulation which elicits offensive aggression in subjects27,34,35. Stimulus males were placed in the subject’s home cage and removed after the subject’s first offensive attack (biting) within a ten-minute period. All subject males attacked the intruder stimulus male, and all stimulus males displayed submissive behavior, defined as defensive postures (e.g. on-back postures, fleeing, and non-social exploring36). Female subjects were exposed to a female intruder; however, this did not elicit attacks from either animal.
Experimental Procedure
All testing occurred within the first five hours of the dark cycle under red light illumination (27 lux), with the exception of the elevated plus maze (EPM), which took place in bright illumination (435 lux). All tests were scored by an experimenter blind to the genotype of the subject and testing occurred across multiple cohorts of subjects. Three weeks after viral injections, subjects were habituated to the testing room and apparatus by handling and placing mice for five minutes in the three-chamber apparatus (see below) each day for three days. On experimental days, subjects were adapted to the experimental room for fifteen minutes prior to testing. First, we tested mice on an EPM to test for anxiety-related behavior37. Mice were then tested in the three-chamber apparatus over six days with a one day break on the fourth day. Lastly, copulatory and aggressive behavior were measured sequentially, with a day in between, in the subject’s home cage. Female subjects were tested irrespective of estrous cycle day, except during copulation testing, when they were in behavioral estrus. Prior research indicates minimal effects of estrous cycle on female mouse communicative behavior38–40, nevertheless the estrus cycle was monitored throughout the study. Following testing, subjects were sacrificed and their brain tissue was processed using immunohistochemistry to detect AVP, oxytocin (OT), galanin (GAL), and GFP-immunoreactive (-ir) cells and fibers in the BNST, paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN), and lateral septum (LS).
Social Behavior
USV, urine marking, and social investigation were recorded in an acrylic three-chamber apparatus (Ugo Basile, Gemonio (VA) Italy; dimensions: 60 × 40 × 22 cm)41–43. Instead of a solid floor, the apparatus was placed on absorbent paper (Nalgene Versi-dry paper, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA) to allow for accurate measurement of urine marking. One corner of the apparatus had a cylindrical cage (8 cm (D), 18 cm (H); 3 mm diameter steel bars, 7.4 mm spacing) that housed a stimulus animal, the opposite corner had a similar cage that was left empty. For testing with social odors, subjects had access to 50 μl of fresh urine from a stimulus animal or 50 μl saline pipetted onto a clean piece of filter paper (3 cm2) that was taped on the outside of cages. The location of stimulus and the “clean” cage were counterbalanced across animals. After placing the subject in the center of the middle chamber, we measured, across a 5-minute trial, close investigation of clean and stimulus cages as well as USV and urine marking, as described below. After testing, the apparatus and cages were thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol and allowed to dry before further testing. In all cases, male or female urine stimulus was presented first (day one), followed by exposure to a stimulus animal of the same sex the following day (day two); this order was then repeated one day later for the opposite sex. In this fashion, mice first experienced a weak stimulus (urine), then a stronger social stimulus (live animal). The order of male and female stimuli presentation was counterbalanced across subjects.
Investigation and Ultrasonic Vocalizations
Close investigation was defined as time spent sniffing within 2 cm of the stimulus or clean cage; climbing on the cage was not scored as investigation. USVs were detected using a condenser microphone connected to an amplifier (UltraSoundGate CM16/CMPA, 10 kHz – 200 kHz, frequency range) placed 4 cm inside the apparatus and directly above the center compartment. USVs were sampled at 200 kHz (16-bit) with target frequency set to 70 kHz (UltraSoundGate 116Hb, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany). Recordings were then analyzed using a MATLAB (MATLAB, Mathworks, RRID:SCR_001622) plug-in that automates USV analysis44. Using this program, sonograms were generated by calculating the power spectrum on Hamming-windowed data and then transformed into compact acoustic feature representations (Gammatone Filterbank). Each 200-millisecond window containing the maximum USV syllable duration was then clustered via machine learning algorithms into USV syllable types (repertoire units) based on time-frequency USV shape. Repertoire units that appeared as background noise were discarded. We collapsed and counted across all syllable types and analyzed the total number USVs produced by each subject.
Urine Marking
Following testing, the substrate sheet was allowed to dry for one hour and then sprayed with ninhydrin fixative (LC-NIN-16; Tritech Forensics Inc., Southport, NC, USA) to visualize urine marks22,45. After twenty-four hours, sheets were imaged (Sony DSC-S700 camera), binarized and analyzed using computer-aided imaging software (ImageJ, RRID:SCR_003070). Urine marking was measured as the total area (pixels) of visualized ninhydrin urine marks in the entire arena.
Copulatory and Aggressive Behavior
To measure copulatory behavior, a stimulus mouse was placed in the subject’s home cage and then removed after ninety minutes had elapsed. The number of mounts, intromissions, and ejaculations and their latencies were recorded, along with mount rejections (female kicking male off during mounting attempt) by female subjects. To measure territorial aggression, subordinate stimulus males were placed in the subject’s home cage and then removed after the subject’s first offensive attack (biting and rolling) within a ten-minute period; the latency to first bite and to first rolling-attack was recorded.
Elevated Plus Maze
The elevated plus maze (EPM) consisted of two open arms (30 × 5 × 0 cm) and two closed arms (30 × 5 × 25 cm) crossed perpendicularly and raised 60 cm above the floor. Subjects were placed at the arm intersection facing the open arm and were habituated to the apparatus for one minute; subjects were then observed for an additional five minutes. Time spent in open and closed arms and the number of risk assessment behaviors (stretch-attend posture, head-dips) were manually scored from video46. Subjects were removed from EPM data analysis if they fell off the EPM during testing.
Histology and immunohistochemistry
Mice were transcardially perfused with 50 ml of 0.1 M PBS (pH 7.4), followed by 50 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer (pH 7.2). Brains were immediately removed and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight (4 °C) and then cryoprotected for 48 h in 30% sucrose. Coronal sections (30μm) of brain tissue were sectioned on a cryostat (Leica CM3050 S, Leica Biosystems, Heidelberg, Germany) into four series and stored in cryoprotectant until immunohistochemical processing. Tissue from all subjects (n=48) was stained for GFP, and AVP, whereas sections from a subset of subjects were stained for GAL (n=10) or OT (n=8).
Sections were removed from cryoprotectant and rinsed thoroughly in PBS. For optimal AVP/OT/GAL staining, we used an antigen retrieval step in which sections were incubated in 0.05M sodium citrate in PBS at 70°C for 30 min and allowed to cool for 10 min prior to primary antibody incubation. To reduce endogenous peroxidase activity, tissue sections were incubated in 0.5% hydrogen peroxide in PBS for 15 min, followed by a blocking step for 1 hr (10% normal goat or donkey serum). Sections were incubated in the appropriate primary antibody to AVP (1:50,000, guinea pig polyclonal, lot: A17901), OT (1:100,000, guinea pig polyclonal, lot: A17698), or GAL (1:40,000, rabbit polyclonal, lot: A17602) (Peninsula Laboratories International, Inc., Switzerland) in 0.4% Triton-X 100 for 24 hr at room temperature. After incubation in primary antibody, sections were rinsed in PBS and then incubated for 1 hr in goat anti-guinea pig (lot: 151056) or goat anti-rabbit (lot: 151508) biotinylated secondary antibody (1:600 Jackson ImmunoResearch, West Grove, PA, USA) in PBS with 0.4% Triton-X 100. Sections were rinsed again in PBS and then incubated for 1 hr in avidin–biotin complex (18 μl each of A and B reagents/ml PBS with 0.4% Triton-X 100, ABC Elite Kit, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA). After rinsing in PBS and then in 0.175 M sodium acetate, sections were incubated in 3,3′-diaminobenzidine HCl (0.2 mg/ml, Sigma) and hydrogen peroxide (1 μl/ml, Sigma) in a nickel-sulfate solution (25 mg/ml, Sigma) for 15 min. The reaction was stopped by rinsing sections in sodium acetate. Stained tissue sections were mounted onto subbed glass slides and allowed to air-dry overnight. Slides were then dehydrated in alcohols, cleared in xylenes, and coverslipped using Permount (Fisher Scientific).
For GFP staining, sections were removed from cryoprotectant and rinsed thoroughly in PBS. After several rinses, sections were incubated in primary antibody against GFP (1:5,000, chicken polyclonal, lot: ab13970, Abcam, Cambridge MA) in 0.4% Triton-X 100 for 24 hr at room temperature. After incubation in primary antibody, sections were rinsed in PBS and then incubated for 2 hr in goat anti-chicken fluorescent (Alexa Fluor® 488) secondary antibody (1:600, lot: ab150169, Abcam, Cambridge MA). Stained tissue sections were mounted onto subbed glass slides and coverslipped using Prolong Gold (Fisher Scientific).
Tissue Analysis
Bilateral BNST images were taken at 10x magnification using a Zeiss Axio Imager M2 microscope (Carl Zeiss Microimaging), which transferred fluorescent images (FITC contrast reflector) to image analysis software (Stereo Investigator, MicroBrightField, RRID:SCR_002526). Imaging domains (2 mm2) were placed with reference to anatomic landmarks (ventricles, fiber tracts; Paxinos and Franklin, 2012). Only subjects with fluorescent labeled GFP cells limited to the BNST were included in the analysis (Fig. 1a); subjects with viral spread to other regions (e.g. PVN) or with only unilateral infection were removed from analysis. AVP-, OT-, and GAL-expressing cells in the BNST, AVP-expressing cells in the PVN, and AVP fiber density in the LS, a prominent target for sex-different BNST AVP-expressing cells10,47, were measured and averaged across both hemispheres and over three sections (BNST and LS) or four sections (PVN). Subjects with <50% AVP reduction in the BNST were removed from analysis. Density of AVP-ir fibers in the LS was measured by gray-level thresholding of digitally captured images using ImageJ (NIH, http://imagej.nih.gov/ij). Background measurements were taken from an adjacent area with no AVP label and averaged across all brains. Specific AVP-ir fiber density was calculated by subtracting the average non-specific background from the AVP-ir density measurement (pixels).
DeepLabCut and SimBA Analysis
Deeplabcut (DLC) 2.2b8 was used to track 8 body points (nose, ears, center, left flank, right flank, tail base, and tail end) on the subject mice. A tracking dataset was created by labeling 20 frames from 10 randomly chosen male and female subject videos for a total of 200 labeled frames. To adequately train a network that could track mice movements, the generated dataset underwent 200,000 iterations of analysis under the default neural network, resnet_5048. The resulting network was then applied to each subject’s 305-second video of their time spent in the 3-chamber apparatus. Tracking point coordinates (CSV file) were produced for each video analyzed and used for further analysis.
CSV files generated from DLC analysis and their corresponding videos were further analyzed with Simple Behavioral Analysis (SimBA 0.84.1)49, an open source program that enables the automated classification of complex social behaviors. First, pixel/millimeter ratios were established for each video, and a region of interest analysis was used to calculate features related to distance, directionality, and time spent in each chamber of the 3-chamber apparatus. The center body marker was the point of interest used to track subjects’ distance covered in the 3-chamber apparatus to detect if BNST-AVP knockdown influenced locomotor activity. Second, three rectangular regions of interest, each one outlining a different chamber in the apparatus, were generated to calculate the subject’s time spent in each chamber. Lastly, a circular region of interest was outlined around the stimulus cage to measure head orientation, and therefore attention towards a distal stimulus, similar to social vigilance measurements that indicate increased anxiety in stressed animals50. We measured this orientation behavior when the subject’s nose was oriented toward the center of the circular region of interest and was a distance of 9–30cm away from the center of the circular region of interest.
Statistical Analysis
All data were analyzed and graphed in R (3.4.4; R Core Team, 2017). Data on histology, social investigation, movement, head orientation, time spent in chambers, urine marking, copulatory behavior, and EPM met the assumptions of parametric statistical tests. Therefore, we analyzed these data using a mixed-model ANOVA with treatment (injections with Avp-shRNA, scramble control) and sex (male, female) as between-subject factors, and sex of stimulus (male, female) and stimulus location (stimulus cage, empty cage) as within-subject factors when appropriate; these were followed by planned t-tests comparing the treatment effects. For copulatory behavior, which was measured for males and females differently, we used t-tests. Differences in proportion of animals engaging in copulatory behaviors across treatments was assessed using a chi-square (χ2) test. Measures of USVs and aggressive behavior were not normally distributed and could not be transformed. Therefore, we analyzed treatment effects on these behaviors using Mann-Whitney U tests. Power analyses for each sample size were above 0.7 and post-hoc comparisons report Bonferroni-corrected p-values. Exact p values are reported, except when p exceeds a significance threshold of 0.00001. Eta-squared (η2), Cohen’s D (d), and Phi (φ) are reported for standardized effect sizes.
Results
shRNA effectively reduced AVP expression in BNST and LS
Males had more AVP-immunoreactive (-ir) cells in the BNST than females (F(1,45) = 30.34, p < 0.00001, η2 = 0.41). AVP knockdown caused an overall reduction in the number of AVP-ir cells (F(1,45) = 154.46, p < 0.00001, η2 = 0.8). There was an interaction between treatment and sex (F(1,45) = 23.01, p = 0.00002, η2 = 0.3), which may be related to the knockdown reducing the number of AVP-ir cells more in males than in females (82% in males (t(25) = 14.41, p < 0.00001, d = 5.8) and 72% in females (t(19) = 4.56, p = 0.0002, d = 2.1)) (Fig. 1c–d).
Males had a higher density of AVP-ir fibers in the LS than females (F(1,45) = 64.2, p < 0.00001, η2 = 0.61). AVP knockdown caused an overall reduction in the density of AVP-ir fibers (F(1,45) = 24.74, p = 0.00002, η2 = 0.38). There was an interaction between treatment and sex F(1,45) = 5.29, p = 0.027, η2 = 0.11 ), with the reduction being larger in males than in females (Fig.1e–f).
The number of AVP-ir cells was not altered in the PVN of male or female subjects (F(1,45) = 0.5, p = 0.47, η2 = 0.01), nor was there a reduction in the number of GAL-ir (F(1,8) = 0.51, p = 0.5, η2 = 0.05) or OT-ir cells (F(1,6) = 0.04, p = 0.85, η2 = 0.006) in the BNST in a subset of male subjects, indicating that the shRNA targeted Avp mRNA specifically and did not have off-target effects (Supplementary Fig. 1).
BNST AVP knockdown reduced social investigation of novel males in males but not in females
All subjects investigated cages with stimulus animals more than the empty cages (F(1,45) = 30.05 , p < 0.00001, η2 = 0.6). Overall, AVP knockdown decreased the time animals investigated cages with stimulus animals (F(1,45) = 8 , p = 0.007, η2 = 0.3). However, effects were limited to male animals, causing an interaction between sex and treatment (F(1,45) = 10.57 , p = 0.002, η2 = 0.2). While control males investigated male stimuli more than did control females (male stimuli: t(21) = 5.2, p = 0.0001, d = 2.27; female stimuli: t(21) = 4.22, p = 0.001, d = 1.8), BNST AVP knockdown specifically reduced the time males spent investigating cages with stimulus males (t(25) = 6.01 , p < 0.00001, d = 2.43). This eliminated a sex difference in the time that control males and females spent investigating male stimuli (t(21) = 1.06, p = 0.3, d = 0.2). AVP knockdown did not affect male subjects’ investigation of female stimulus animals (t(25) = 1.9, p = 0.14, d = 0.7) nor did it affect female subjects’ investigation of male (t(20) = 1.45, p = 0.32, d = 0.6) or female (t(20) = 1.66, p = 0.23, d = 0.7) stimulus animals (Fig. 2a–b).
All subjects investigated cages with urine samples more than the empty cages (F(1,45) = 19.67, p = 0.00006, η2 = 0.4). Overall, AVP knockdown decreased the time animals investigated cages with urine samples (F(1,45) = 14.42 , p = 0.0004, η2 = 0.3). However, as with stimulus animals, AVP knockdown effects were limited to male animals, causing an interaction between sex and treatment (F(1,45) = 6.08 , p = 0.018, η2 = 0.18). While control males investigated urine more than did control females (male urine: t(21) = 3.7, p = 0.002, d = 1.6; female urine: t(21) = 4.3, p = 0.0006, d = 1.8), BNST AVP knockdown specifically reduced the time males spent investigating both male and female urine (male urine: t(25) = 4.42, p = 0.0003, d = 1.77; female urine: t(25) = 2.99, p = 0.012, d = 1.2). This eliminated a sex difference in the time that control males and females spent investigating male urine (t(21) = 2.1, p = 0.094, d = 0.6) but not for investigating female urine (t(21) = 2.5, p = 0.04, d = 0.8). AVP knockdown did not affect female subjects’ investigation of male (t(20) = 1.13, p = 0.27, d = 0.4) or female (t(20) = 0.75, p = 0.46, d = 0.3) urine (Fig. 2c–d).
DeepLabCut and SimBA analysis revealed that subjects differed in time spent in the chamber containing stimulus animals (F(2,82) = 0.5, p = 0.44, η2 = 0.007). Overall, AVP knockdown increased the time animals spent in the chamber furthest away from the stimulus animals (F(2,82) = 4.35, p = 0.016, η2 = 0.09). There was no interaction between sex, treatment, and chamber location (F(2,82) = 1.3 , p = 0.27, η2 = 0.02). However, post hoc analysis revealed that BNST AVP knockdown specifically increased the time males spent in the furthest chamber of the apparatus with stimulus males (male stimuli: t(25) = 2.78, p = 0.01, d = 1.1; female stimuli: t(25) = 0.9, p = 0.37, d = 0.3). There was also a trend toward BNST AVP knockdown decreasing the time males spent in the chamber with the stimulus male compared to controls (male stimuli: t(25) = 2.0, p = 0.056, d = 0.8; female stimuli: t(25) = 0.4, p = 0.73, d = 0.1). AVP knockdown did not affect female subject’s time spent in the clean chambers (male stimuli: t(20) = 1.7, p = 0.11, d = 0.7; female stimuli: t(20) = 0.03, p = 0.98, d = 0.01) or the chamber with the stimulus animals compared to controls (male stimuli: t(20) = 1.3, p = 0.18, d = 0.6; female stimuli: t(20) = 0.9, p = 0.36, d = 0.3)(Supplementary Fig. 2).
BNST AVP knockdown did not affect overall locomotion within the 3-chamber apparatus ((F(1,45) = 1.55, p = 0.22, η2 = 0.018, Supplementary Fig. 3), and there was no interaction between treatment and sex (F(1,45) = 0.04, p = 0.85, η2 = 0.001). Additionally, BNST AVP knockdown did not affect the amount of time subjects spent oriented toward the stimulus cages at a 9–30cm distance (Supplementary Fig. 4).
BNST AVP knockdown reduced male-male USVs, but did not alter urine marking
Most vocalizations were produced during male-female interactions and least during female-female interactions (Fig. 3a–b). BNST AVP knockdown reduced overall USVs produced during social interactions (U = 234, p = 0.024, d = 0.55), However, this effect seems to be driven primarily by a reduction of USVs produced during male-male interactions (male subjects with male stimuli: U = 45.5, p = 0.05, d = 0.68; male subjects with female stimuli: U = 57, p = 0.11, d = 0.6). USVs produced during interactions of female subjects and stimulus animals were much lower and unaltered following BNST AVP knockdown (female subjects and female stimuli: U = 45, p = 0.35, d = 0.1; female subjects with male stimuli: U = 67, p = 0.67, d = 0.04) (Fig. 3a–b). AVP knockdown did not alter USVs produced during exposure to female urine (males: U = 77, p = 0.52, d = 0.1; females: U = 34, p = 0.09, d = 0.2) or male urine (males: U = 47, p = 0.42, d = 0.09; females: U = 77, p = 0.52 d = 0.2) (Supplementary Fig. 5).
Overall, males, but not females, produced urine marks (Fig. 3c–d). Males produced more urine marks and covered a larger area with urine in the presence of a female than of a male stimulus (number of marks: F(1,25) = 16.53, p = 0.0004, η2 = 0.1; area: (F(1,25) = 29.713, p = 0.00001, η2 = 0.19)). BNST AVP knockdown did not affect the number of urine marks nor the area covered with those marks in the presence of stimulus animals (number of marks: F(1,25) = 0.4, p = 0.56, η2 = 0.001; area: (F(1,25) = 0.1, p = 0.76, η2 = 0.01))(Fig. 3c–d) or urine samples (F (1,25) = 0.5, p = 0.51, η2 = 0.02)(Supplementary Fig. 5).
BNST AVP knockdown reduced offensive signaling, but not offensive attacks in males
Male subjects were the only ones to engage in aggressive attacks and tail rattling during the aggression tests. BNST AVP knockdown reduced tail rattles of subjects toward male intruders (U = 50, p = 0.048, d = 0.83); however, the latency to bite (U = 89, p = 0.9, d = 0.1) and attack (U = 73.5, p = 0.4, d = 0.1) the male intruder was similar between control and shRNA-injected males (Fig. 4a–c).
BNST AVP knockdown reduced copulatory behavior in males
In males, BNST AVP knockdown reduced the number of intromissions (t(25) = 3.13, p = 0.002, d = 0.9) but did not affect the number of mounts (t(25) = 0.7, p = 0.49, d = 0.2). Similarly, BNST AVP knockdown increased the latency to intromit (t(25) = 2.6, p = 0.002, d = 0.9), but did not affect the latency to mount (t(25) = 1.1, p = 0.28, d = 0.3) or ejaculate (t(25) = 1.2, p = 0.23, d = 0.2), Fig. 5a–d). Furthermore, BNST AVP knockdown caused fewer males to ejaculate with a receptive female (x2 (2) p < 0.00001, φ = 1.06, Fig. 5e–f). In females, BNST AVP knockdown did not affect the number of mounts received (t(20) = 1.3, p = 0.19, d = 0.5), intromissions received (t(20) = 1.1, p = 0.27, d = 0.5), or the latency to receive mounts (t(20) = 0.2, p = 0.82, d = 0.1), intromissions (t(20) = 0.97, p = 0.35, d = 0.4), or ejaculations (t(20) = 0.3, p = 0.78, d = 0.1)(Fig. 5a–d).
BNST AVP knockdown did not alter anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze
We did not find sex differences in time spent in the open arms (F(1,45) = 1.12, p = 0.3, η2 = 0.02), the number of stretch-attend postures (F(1,45) = 0.4, p = 0.53, η2 = 0.002) or head dips (F(1,45) = 2.05, p = 0.16, η2 = 0.02) observed in the EPM. BNST AVP knockdown did not affect any of these measures (time spent in the open arms: (F(1,45) = 0.01, p = 0.9, η2 = 0.001; stretch-attend postures: (F(1,45) = 2.8, p = 0.097, η2 = 0.01); head dips: (F(1,45) = 0.1, p = 0.75, η2 = 0.001)) (Fig. 6a–c).
Discussion
Previously, we found that removal of AVP-expressing cells in the BNST of male, but not female, mice reduced investigation of same-sex conspecifics and altered social communication, while minimally affecting female social behavior19. What was left unresolved was whether these results were due to the loss of AVP signaling from BNST cells or of other factors, such as other neuropeptides and neurotransmitters, associated with these cells. Here, we found that shRNA knockdown of AVP in BNST largely replicated the effects of BNST AVP cell ablations (e.g., on social investigation), although some effects diverged (e.g., on urine marking and copulatory behavior), which may be related to AVP cell ablation affecting more than just AVP signaling. Additionally, BNST AVP knockdown reduced communicative behavior (USV, tail rattles) toward same-sex conspecifics as well as copulatory behavior in males. Overall, this suggests that AVP from the BNST plays a more prominent role in social behavior in males than in females and that AVP expression in the BNST drives specific aspects of social behavior in a sexually differentiated way.
AVP produced by BNST cells may play a more impactful role in behavior than our results indicate. As we found a 70–80% knockdown of AVP in BNST cells, not all AVP was eliminated. In addition, since AVP cells in the MeA show similar sex differences in expression and project to overlapping areas11, it may be necessary to knockdown AVP expression in both groups of cells to identify all behaviors modulated by AVP released from these cells. Furthermore, testing started three weeks after BNST AVP knockdown was initiated. As a result, additional behavioral effects of this knockdown may have disappeared due to the system adapting to chronic depletion of AVP. Such adaptation may explain the opposite effects of acute versus chronic V1aR manipulations in the lateral septum (LS) on anxiety-like behavior51,52.
One of the largest effects we saw after Avp-shRNA knockdown of AVP in the BNST was a reduction in male investigation of novel male conspecifics. AVP knockdown also increased the time males spent in the chamber furthest away from stimulus males, suggesting active avoidance of these males. This reduction in investigation of stimulus males was not due to deficits in overall social interest, activity level, or generalized anxiety, as investigation of female stimuli and anxiety-like behaviors as measured in the EPM were unaltered. Also, head orientation toward the stimulus animals within the three-chamber apparatus was not affected by AVP knockdown, indicating a specific deficit in close social investigation and not in overall attention toward the stimulus50,53. Additionally, we found that AVP knockdown strongly reduced male investigation of urine samples, with the largest effect seen on investigation of male urine. Together, our findings suggest that, in males, AVP produced in the BNST stimulates investigation of potentially territorial competitors and their social odors, which aligns with prior work showing that vasotocin knockdown in birds reduced social contact between males17 and that AVP injections into the LS, a downstream projection site for BNST AVP cells11, increased male-male interactions in rats54.
AVP knockdown did not affect male aggressive behavior toward subordinate stimulus males. While these results match our previous finding that BNST AVP cell ablation affected investigative, but not aggressive, behavior19, they are somewhat unexpected, as knockdown of BNST AVP also reduced AVP-ir fibers in the LS, a site where AVP acts on aggressive behavior in males18,55 and females56, but see57. This inconsistency may be due to differences in testing and/or procedural conditions (i.e., exposure to different types of intruders) as well as to species and strain differences18,58. Nevertheless, our results leave open the possibility that AVP in the LS from sources other than the BNST contribute to modulation of aggressive behavior, as BNST AVP knockdown did not eliminate all LS AVP-ir fibers. Therefore, AVP inputs from sources such as the medial amygdala and PVN10 may contribute to AVP effects within the LS on aggressive behavior.
Although BNST AVP knockdown did not change offensive attack behavior, it still modestly reduced male signaling behavior in potentially antagonistic settings. Specifically, AVP knockdown reduced tail rattling, a known component of aggressive behavior by dominant males59, and USVs in the presence of other males. However, knockdown did not reduce USVs produced in the presence of receptive females. Typically, males vocalize more than females60 and primarily direct their vocalizations toward females61; however, males do produce USVs in male-male territorial contexts61,62. Therefore, our results suggest that BNST AVP is involved in some aspects of male offensive/territorial signaling (e.g., USVs and tail rattling).
In agreement with the effects of BNST AVP cell ablation19,20, we observed no changes in anxiety-like behavior following BNST AVP knockdown. This was somewhat unexpected, as central AVP has been implicated in the modulation of anxiety63–65. Moreover, pharmacological studies suggest that AVP in the LS, a target of BNST AVP cells in mice, may modulate anxiety51,57,66. However, the present results are consistent with observations that electrolytic lesions of the BNST had no effect on anxiety-like behavior in the EPM in rats67. Therefore, AVP derived from other sources may modulate anxiety action when released in the septum. We do note, however, that ablation of AVP cell groups in the PVN in males, or in the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in both sexes, increased anxiety-like behavior in the EPM68,69. Consequently, the LS may require AVP signals from both BNST and MeA to increase anxiety-like behaviors, while other sources of AVP may work to inhibit it.
While BNST AVP knockdown and BNST AVP cell ablation19 have similar effects or lack thereof on male-male social investigation, aggression, and anxiety, AVP knockdown did not replicate other behavioral effects of BNST AVP cell deletion. For example, BNST AVP cell ablation in males increased urine marking toward females, whereas BNST AVP knockdown did not19. It may be that BNST AVP cell ablations are more effective in reducing AVP levels than AVP knockdown. However, the percentage of reduction of AVP-ir cells in the BNST was high in both studies (shRNA: ~70–80% average; cell ablations: ~90%+ average). The most parsimonious explanation for this is that AVP cell ablation may have removed more signaling molecules than just AVP, each of which may have effects on behavior.
Our results demonstrate a distinct, sexually differentiated role of BNST AVP in male copulatory behavior. Knockdown of AVP within the BNST reduced intromissions and ejaculations in males, but did not alter copulatory behavior in females. These results are broadly consistent with the observation that BNST and MeA AVP cells are active during male copulatory behavior70,71. As we did not observe changes in precopulatory (male investigation, USVs, urine marking) or mounting behaviors toward receptive females after AVP knockdown, the effects on copulatory behavior are more likely to be due to deficits in bridging the appetitive and consummatory phases of sexual behavior, rather than to changes in sexual motivation72. These results do not match the effects of ablating BNST AVP cells, which reduced female, but not male, copulatory behavior19. One possible explanation for this discrepancy is that AVP ablation left the expression of galanin, a neuropeptide colocalized with AVP in the BNST73 intact in the present study. This may have increased overall inhibitory signaling to targets of BNST AVP cells, as galanin promotes neuronal inhibition74 whereas AVP promotes excitation65. Indeed, ICV injections of galanin strongly inhibited male copulatory behavior in rats75, and galanin has been shown to block AVP-induced flank marking in golden hamsters76. Future studies may help unravel the physiological and behavioral significance of co-expression of AVP and other signaling molecules.
Conclusion
In summary, our results indicate that sexually dimorphic AVP expression within the BNST contributes to sex differences in social behavior. More specifically, BNST AVP knockdown in male, but not female, mice reduced investigation and communicative behaviors directed toward same-sex conspecifics as well as male sexual behavior, without changing offensive attacks, anxiety-related behaviors, and overall activity. These results match those of other studies that have shown a sex-specific role of AVP in behavior. For example, AVP and its antagonists have different effects on aggressive play behavior in rats77, territorial aggression in hamsters78–80, and social communication in humans81–83. Together, these studies point toward a sexually differentiated role of AVP in vertebrate social behavior. By ablating specific AVP cell groups in the BNST, PVN, SCN19,68,69, or knocking down AVP specifically in the BNST (present results), we have started addressing directly the question as to which AVP cell groups contribute to sex differences in social behavior and its regulation.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (R01 MH121603) to AP and GJD, (F31 MH125659) to NR, and the Center for Behavioral Neuroscience at Georgia State University. We would like to thank Maria Olifer for her assistance with histology and immunohistochemistry.
References
- 1.Goodson JL, Bass AH. Social behavior functions and related anatomical characteristics of vasotocin/vasopressin systems in vertebrates. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2001;35:246–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Donaldson ZR, Young LJ. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science. 2008;322:900–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Insel TR. The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron. 2010;65:768–779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kelly AM, Goodson JL. Functional significance of a phylogenetically widespread sexual dimorphism in vasotocin/vasopressin production. Horm Behav. 2013;64:840–846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Caldwell HK. Oxytocin and Vasopressin: Powerful Regulators of Social Behavior. Neuroscientist. 2017;23:517–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dumais KM, Veenema AH. Vasopressin and oxytocin receptor systems in the brain: Sex differences and sex-specific regulation of social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2016;40:1–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Duque-Wilckens N, Steinman MQ, Laredo SA, et al. Inhibition of vasopressin V1a receptors in the medioventral bed nucleus of the stria terminalis has sex- and context-specific anxiogenic effects. Neuropharmacology. 2016;110:59–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.De Vries GJ, Boyle PA. Double duty for sex differences in the brain. Behav Brain Res. 1998;92:205–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rood BD, De Vries GJ. Vasopressin innervation of the mouse (Mus musculus) brain and spinal cord. J Comp Neurol. 2011;519:2434–2474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rood BD, Stott RT, You S, et al. Site of origin of and sex differences in the vasopressin innervation of the mouse (Mus musculus) brain. J Comp Neurol. 2013;521:2321–2358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.De Vries GJ, Panzica GC. Sexual differentiation of central vasopressin and vasotocin systems in vertebrates: different mechanisms, similar endpoints. Neuroscience. 2006;138:947–955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Veenema AH, Bredewold R, De Vries GJ. Vasopressin regulates social recognition in juvenile and adult rats of both sexes, but in sex- and age-specific ways. Horm Behav. 2012;61:50–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Veenema AH, Bredewold R, De Vries GJ. Sex-specific modulation of juvenile social play by vasopressin. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:2554–2561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.DiBenedictis BT, Cheung HK, Nussbaum ER, et al. Involvement of ventral pallidal vasopressin in the sex-specific regulation of sociosexual motivation in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2020;111:104462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rigney N, Beaumont R, Petrulis A. Sex differences in vasopressin 1a receptor regulation of social communication within the lateral habenula and dorsal raphe of mice. Horm Behav. 2020;104715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kelly AM, Kingsbury MA, Hoffbuhr K, et al. Vasotocin neurons and septal V1a-like receptors potently modulate songbird flocking and responses to novelty. Horm Behav. 2011;60:12–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kelly AM, Goodson JL. Behavioral relevance of species-specific vasotocin anatomy in gregarious finches. Front Neurosci. 2013;7:242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Veenema AH, Beiderbeck DI, Lukas M, et al. Distinct correlations of vasopressin release within the lateral septum and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis with the display of intermale aggression. Horm Behav. 2010;58:273–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rigney N, Whylings J, Mieda M, et al. Sexually Dimorphic Vasopressin Cells Modulate Social Investigation and Communication in Sex-Specific Ways. eNeuro;6. Epub ahead of print January 2019. DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0415-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Whylings J, Rigney N, Peters NV, et al. Sexually dimorphic role of BNST vasopressin cells in sickness and social behavior in male and female mice. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;83:68–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Crawley JN. Translational animal models of autism and neurodevelopmental disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14:293–305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lehmann ML, Geddes CE, Lee JL, et al. Urine scent marking (USM): a novel test for depressive-like behavior and a predictor of stress resiliency in mice. PLoS One. 2013;8:e69822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wöhr M Ultrasonic vocalizations in Shank mouse models for autism spectrum disorders: detailed spectrographic analyses and developmental profiles. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;43:199–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gheusi G, Bluth R-M, Goodall G, et al. Social and individual recognition in rodents” Methodological aspects and neurobiological bases. Behav Processes. 1994;33:59–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Matochik JA, Sipos ML, Nyby JG, et al. Intracranial androgenic activation of male-typical behaviors in house mice: motivation versus performance. Behav Brain Res. 1994;60:141–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Barkley MS, Goldman BD. The effects of castration and Silastic implants of testosterone on intermale aggression in the mouse. Horm Behav. 1977;9:32–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Beeman EA. The relation of the interval between castration and first encounter to the aggressive behavior of mice. Anat Rec. 1947;99:570. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bakker J, Honda S-I, Harada N, et al. The aromatase knock-out mouse provides new evidence that estradiol is required during development in the female for the expression of sociosexual behaviors in adulthood. J Neurosci. 2002;22:9104–9112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ström JO, Theodorsson A, Ingberg E, et al. Ovariectomy and 17β-estradiol replacement in rats and mice: a visual demonstration. J Vis Exp. 2012;e4013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Veyrac A, Wang G, Baum MJ, et al. The main and accessory olfactory systems of female mice are activated differentially by dominant versus subordinate male urinary odors. Brain Res. 2011;1402:20–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Caligioni CS. Assessing reproductive status/stages in mice. Curr Protoc Neurosci. 2009;Appendix 4:Appendix 4I. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Roullet FI, Wöhr M, Crawley JN. Female urine-induced male mice ultrasonic vocalizations, but not scent-marking, is modulated by social experience. Behav Brain Res. 2011;216:19–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lumley LA, Sipos ML, Charles RC, et al. Social stress effects on territorial marking and ultrasonic vocalizations in mice. Physiol Behav. 1999;67:769–775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Connor JL, Winston H. Genetic analysis of conditioned emotional responses in the mouse (Mus musculus L.). J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1972;81:37–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Van Loo PL, Mol JA, Koolhaas JM, et al. Modulation of aggression in male mice: influence of group size and cage size. Physiol Behav. 2001;72:675–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Koolhaas JM, Coppens CM, de Boer SF, et al. The resident-intruder paradigm: a standardized test for aggression, violence and social stress. J Vis Exp. 2013;e4367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology. 1987;92:180–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Maggio JC, Whitney G. Ultrasonic vocalizing by adult female mice (Mus musculus). J Comp Psychol. 1985;99:420–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Coquelin A Urine-marking by female mice throughout their reproductive cycle. Horm Behav. 1992;26:255–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Moncho-Bogani J, Lanuza E, Lorente MJ, et al. Attraction to male pheromones and sexual behaviour show different regulatory mechanisms in female mice. Physiol Behav. 2004;81:427–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Young NB, et al. Social approach in genetically engineered mouse lines relevant to autism. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8:129–142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Arakawa H, Arakawa K, Blanchard DC, et al. A new test paradigm for social recognition evidenced by urinary scent marking behavior in C57BL/6J mice. Behav Brain Res. 2008;190:97–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Crawley JN. Mouse behavioral assays relevant to the symptoms of autism. Brain Pathol. 2007;17:448–459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Van Segbroeck M, Knoll AT, Levitt P, et al. MUPET-Mouse Ultrasonic Profile ExTraction: A Signal Processing Tool for Rapid and Unsupervised Analysis of Ultrasonic Vocalizations. Neuron. 2017;94:465–485.e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Meyer H The ninhydrin reaction and its analytical applications. Biochem J. 1957;67:333–340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Cole JC, Rodgers RJ. An ethological analysis of the effects of chlordiazepoxide and bretazenil (Ro 16–6028) in the murine elevated plus-maze. Behav Pharmacol. 1993;4:573–580. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.van Leeuwen FW, Caffe AR, De Vries GJ. Vasopressin cells in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis of the rat: sex differences and the influence of androgens. Brain Res. 1985;325:391–394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mathis A, Mamidanna P, Cury KM, et al. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:1281–1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Nilsson SRO, Goodwin NL, Choong JJ, et al. Simple Behavioral Analysis (SimBA) – an open source toolkit for computer classification of complex social behaviors in experimental animals. bioRxiv. 2020;2020.04.19.049452. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Duque-Wilckens N, Steinman MQ, Busnelli M, et al. Oxytocin Receptors in the Anteromedial Bed Nucleus of the Stria Terminalis Promote Stress-Induced Social Avoidance in Female California Mice. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83:203–213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Liebsch G, Wotjak CT, Landgraf R, et al. Septal vasopressin modulates anxiety-related behaviour in rats. Neurosci Lett. 1996;217:101–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Everts HG, Koolhaas JM. Differential modulation of lateral septal vasopressin receptor blockade in spatial learning, social recognition, and anxiety-related behaviors in rats. Behav Brain Res. 1999;99:7–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Defensor EB, Pearson BL, Pobbe RLH, et al. A novel social proximity test suggests patterns of social avoidance and gaze aversion-like behavior in BTBR T+ tf/J mice. Behav Brain Res. 2011;217:302–308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Koolhaas JM, Moor E, Hiemstra Y, et al. The testosterone-dependent vasopressinergic neurons in the medial amygdala and lateral septum: involvement in social behaviour of male rats. Vasopressin. 1991;208:213–219. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Leroy F, Park J, Asok A, et al. A circuit from hippocampal CA2 to lateral septum disinhibits social aggression. Nature. 2018;564:213–218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.de Moura Oliveira VE, Lukas M, Wolf HN, et al. Oxytocin and vasopressin within the ventral and dorsal lateral septum modulate aggression in female rats. Nat Commun. 2021;12:1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Beiderbeck DI, Neumann ID, Veenema AH. Differences in intermale aggression are accompanied by opposite vasopressin release patterns within the septum in rats bred for low and high anxiety. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26:3597–3605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Chalfin L, Dayan M, Levy DR, et al. Mapping ecologically relevant social behaviours by gene knockout in wild mice. Nat Commun. 2014;5:4569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lee W, Fu J, Bouwman N, et al. Temporal microstructure of dyadic social behavior during relationship formation in mice. PLoS One. 2019;14:e0220596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Warren MR, Spurrier MS, Roth ED, et al. Sex differences in vocal communication of freely interacting adult mice depend upon behavioral context. PLoS One. 2018;13:e0204527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Matsumoto YK, Okanoya K. Mice modulate ultrasonic calling bouts according to sociosexual context. R Soc Open Sci. 2018;5:180378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Sangiamo DT, Warren MR, Neunuebel JP. Ultrasonic signals associated with different types of social behavior of mice. Nat Neurosci. 2020;23:411–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Ebner K, Wotjak CT, Landgraf R, et al. Forced swimming triggers vasopressin release within the amygdala to modulate stress-coping strategies in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;15:384–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bielsky IF, Hu S-B, Szegda KL, et al. Profound impairment in social recognition and reduction in anxiety-like behavior in vasopressin V1a receptor knockout mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:483–493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Raggenbass M Overview of cellular electrophysiological actions of vasopressin. Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;583:243–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Landgraf R, Gerstberger R, Montkowski A, et al. V1 vasopressin receptor antisense oligodeoxynucleotide into septum reduces vasopressin binding, social discrimination abilities, and anxiety-related behavior in rats. J Neurosci. 1995;15:4250–4258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Treit D, Aujla H, Menard J. Does the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis mediate fear behaviors? Behav Neurosci. 1998;112:379–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Rigney N, Whylings J, de Vries GJ, et al. Sex Differences in the Control of Social Investigation and Anxiety by Vasopressin Cells of the Paraventricular Nucleus of the Hypothalamus. Neuroendocrinology. Epub ahead of print 15 June 2020. DOI: 10.1159/000509421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Whylings J, Rigney N, de Vries GJ, et al. Reduction in vasopressin cells in the suprachiasmatic nucleus in mice increases anxiety and alters fluid intake. Horm Behav. 2021;133:104997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Ho JM, Murray JH, Demas GE, et al. Vasopressin cell groups exhibit strongly divergent responses to copulation and male-male interactions in mice. Horm Behav. 2010;58:368–377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hari Dass SA, Vyas A. Copulation or sensory cues from the female augment Fos expression in arginine vasopressin neurons of the posterodorsal medial amygdala of male rats. Front Zool. 2014;11:42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Petrulis A Chemosignals and hormones in the neural control of mammalian sexual behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2013;34:255–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Miller MA, Kolb PE, Raskind MA. Extra-hypothalamic vasopressin neurons coexpress galanin messenger RNA as shown by double in situ hybridization histochemistry. J Comp Neurol. 1993;329:378–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Kask K, Langel U, Bartfai T. Galanin--a neuropeptide with inhibitory actions. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 1995;15:653–673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Poggioli R, Rasori E, Bertolini A. Galanin inhibits sexual behavior in male rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 1992;213:87–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Ferris CF, Delville Y, Bonigut S, et al. Galanin antagonizes vasopressin-stimulated flank marking in male golden hamsters. Brain Res. 1999;832:1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Bredewold R, Smith CJW, Dumais KM, et al. Sex-specific modulation of juvenile social play behavior by vasopressin and oxytocin depends on social context. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Terranova JI, Song Z, Larkin TE. Serotonin and arginine–vasopressin mediate sex differences in the regulation of dominance and aggression by the social brain. Proceedings of the, https://www.pnas.org/content/113/46/13233.short (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Terranova JI, Ferris CF, Albers HE. Sex Differences in the Regulation of Offensive Aggression and Dominance by Arginine-Vasopressin. Front Endocrinol. 2017;8:308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Grieb ZA, Ross AP, McCann KE, et al. Sex-dependent effects of social status on the regulation of arginine-vasopressin (AVP) V1a, oxytocin (OT), and serotonin (5-HT) 1A receptor binding and aggression in Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Horm Behav. 2021;127:104878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Feng C, Hackett PD, DeMarco AC, et al. Oxytocin and vasopressin effects on the neural response to social cooperation are modulated by sex in humans. Brain Imaging Behav. 2015;9:754–764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Rilling JK, Demarco AC, Hackett PD, et al. Sex differences in the neural and behavioral response to intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin during human social interaction. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;39:237–248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Thompson RR, George K, Walton JC, et al. Sex-specific influences of vasopressin on human social communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:7889–7894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.