Abstract
Macaque anatomy and physiology studies have revealed multiple visual areas in posterior parietal cortex (PPC). While many response properties of PPC neurons have been probed, little is known about PPC’s large-scale functional topography — specifically related to visuotopic organization. Using high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) at 3T with a phase–encoded retinotopic mapping paradigm in the awake macaque, a large-scale visuotopic organization along lateral portions of PPC anterior to area V3a and extending into the lateral intraparietal sulcus (LIP) was found. We identify two new visual field maps anterior to V3a within caudal PPC, referred to as CIP-1 and CIP-2. The polar angle representation in CIP-1 extends from regions near the upper vertical meridian (that is the shared border with V3a and DP) to those within the lower visual field (that is the shared border with CIP-2). The polar angle representation in CIP-2 is a mirror reversal of the CIP-1 representation. CIP-1 and CIP-2 share a representation of central space on the lateral border. Anterior to CIP-2, a third polar angle representation was found within LIP, referred to as visuotopic LIP (LIPvt). The polar angle representation in LIP extends from regions near the upper vertical meridian (that is the shared border with CIP-2) to those near the lower vertical meridian. Representations of central visual space were identified within dorsal portions of LIP with peripheral representations in ventral portions. We also consider the topographic large-scale organization found within macaque PPC relative to that observed in human PPC.
Keywords: topography, fMRI, macaque, parietal, dorsal, visual, extrastriate
INTRODUCTION
Multiple visual areas within and adjacent to the intraparietal sulcus have been identified in posterior parietal cortex (PPC) of macaque monkeys (Pandya & Seltzer, 1982; Andersen et al., 1990; see Van Essen, 2004 for review). Visual areas within PPC have been distinguished based on their cyto- and myeloarchitecture as well as their connectivity patterns, including the ventral and dorsal lateral intraparietal areas (LIPv/d), and two cortical zones, dorsal prelunate (DP) and lateral occipital parietal (LOP), also referred to as caudal intraparietal (CIP). Identifying basic functional properties of these regions, such as topographic large-scale organization, may prove useful in further parcellating PPC into functional units, as it has been for other parts of the macaque visual system (Van Essen & Zeki, 1978; Maguire & Baizer, 1984; Desimone & Ungerleider, 1986; see Gattass et al., 2005 for review).
Physiology and functional brain imaging studies have provided some evidence in support of topographic large-scale organization within PPC. Specifically, dorsal portions of area DP have been shown to exhibit spatially specific representations of the visual field (Fize et al., 2003; Heider et al., 2005), though no systematic map of visual space has been demonstrated thus far. A topographic map of visual space has been reported within LIP with both physiology (Blatt et al., 1990; Ben Hamed et al., 2001) and more recently functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; Patel et al., 2010). However, there were notable differences in the pattern of topographic organization described in these studies that remain unresolved. Furthermore, previous studies of visual topography within PPC have typically focused on one particular area without investigating the relations of topographic organization to neighboring cortical areas within PPC or dorsal extrastriate cortex. Therefore, it is unclear whether macaque PPC indeed lacks organized representations of the visual field outside of LIP, as recently proposed (Patel et al., 2010). Here, we sought to investigate the visual topography across PPC and dorsal extrastriate cortex using fMRI and phase-encoded retinotopic mapping to clarify its large-scale organization.
Phase-encoded retinotopic mapping along the polar angle and eccentricity dimensions with fMRI has been widely used to reveal topographic organization within the human (Sereno et al., 1995; Engel et al., 1997; Schneider et al., 2004; Brewer et al., 2005; Hagler & Sereno, 2006; Larsson & Heeger, 2006; Swisher et al., 2007; Kastner et al., 2007; Konen & Kastner, 2008; Arcaro et al., 2009) and also the macaque visual system (Brewer et al., 2002; Kolster et al., 2009). Specifically, such topographic mapping enables the simultaneous investigation of visuotopic organization across a large region of cortex, thereby allowing the visuospatial map of an individual area to be anchored in the framework of the topographic organization across multiple surrounding areas. We investigated the representation of visual space across PPC and dorsal extrastriate cortex in awake macaque monkeys trained to maintain fixation for extended periods of time, and identified multiple topographically organized areas. Our results confirm the visuotopic organization within area V3a and the adjacent DP zone. Importantly, we report two new visuotopically organized areas within the caudal PPC which we refer to as CIP-1 and CIP-2 and clarify the visuotopic organization within LIP. Lastly, we compare the visuotopic organization within PPC between macaque monkeys and humans.
METHODS AND MATERIALS
Subjects
Two adult male macaque monkeys (Macaca fascicularis) weighing 4 – 6 kg participated in the study. All procedures were approved by the Princeton University Animal Care and Use Committee and conformed with National Institutes of Health guidelines for the humane care and use of laboratory animals.
Surgical and training procedures
Each animal was surgically implanted with a plastic head bolt for restraining the head by using ceramic screws and dental acrylic. All surgical procedures were performed under strictly aseptic conditions and under general anesthesia with isoflurane (induction 2–4%, maintenance 0.5–2%) following preanesthetic medication with atropine (0.08 mg/kg, administered intramuscularly [im]), ketamine (2–10 mg/kg, im) and acepromazine (1 mg/kg, im). The animals were treated postsurgically with antibiotics (e.g., Baytril, 2.5 mg/kg, im) and analgesics (e.g., buprenorphine, 0.01 mg/kg, im), and wound margins of skin surrounding the implant were cleaned regularly.
Monkeys were placed prone in an MR-compatible primate chair in a sphinx-like position, with their heads erect and fixed in a head-holding apparatus (Pinsk et al., 2005). The animals were acclimated to the scanner environment through the use of a mock training environment. Monkeys were trained to fixate on a small dot (0.5° diameter) at the center of a display screen by using an infrared eye tracking system sampling at a 60 Hz refresh rate (Model 504, Applied Science Laboratories, Bedford, MA). The nominal accuracy of the ASL LRO model is 0.5°. By providing the animals with regular juice rewards (PHD 2000, Harvard Apparatus Inc., Holliston, MA, USA) while they maintained fixation within a 4° × 4° (2° to each side of the fixation point) square window and systematically increasing the rate of their juice reward (2.5 to 1 s in 500-ms steps), the animals were trained to maintain fixation for several minutes. Further details regarding surgery and training procedures are given in (Pinsk et al., 2005).
Visual stimulus display and control
Visual stimuli were projected from a single-lamp 3-chip LCD projector (6500 ANSI lumens, 2000:1 contrast ratio; Christie LX650, Christie Digital Systems, Cypress, CA) outside the scanner room onto a translucent screen located at the end of the scanner bore. Monkeys viewed the projection screen directly; the total path length from eye to screen was approximately 60 cm. The screen subtended 30° of visual angle in the horizontal and vertical dimensions. Eye tracking at the scanner was performed with a 60 Hz long-range optics system located outside the bore that recorded eye position through a small aperture (0.75° diameter) in the display screen (Model LRO, Applied Science Laboratories, Bedford, MA). The stimulus presentation, eye movement tracking, and reward delivery were synchronized to the beginning of each scan using a trigger pulse from the scanner, and were controlled via a PC computer using Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Albany, CA). During all scans, monkeys performed a fixation task, as described earlier.
Visual stimuli and experimental design
Polar angle measurements
In order to obtain polar angle representations, visual stimuli consisted of a wedge that rotated either clockwise or counterclockwise around a central fixation point. The wedge spanned 1–15° in eccentricity with an arc length of 45° and moved at a rate of 9 deg/s. The wedge consisted of a colored checkerboard with each check’s chromaticity and luminance alternating at a flicker frequency of 4 Hz (see Swisher et al., 2007; Arcaro et al., 2009 for details). A transparent wedge within a dark foreground rotated around a central fixation. The underlying checkerboard was only visible through the transparent wedge, giving the appearance of a rotating checkerboard wedge. Each run consisted of four cycles of 40 s each, and runs alternated between clockwise and counterclockwise wedge rotation. 20 runs were collected for monkey M1 and 30 runs were collected for monkey M2, in 8 and 14 scanning sessions, respectively. For both monkeys, half of the runs were in the clockwise direction and the other half were in the counterclockwise direction.
Eccentricity measurements
In order to obtain eccentricity representations, visual stimuli consisted of an annulus that expanded around a central fixation point. The duty cycle of the annulus was 12.5%, that is, any given point on the screen was within the annulus for only 12.5% of the time. The annulus increased on a logarithmic scale over time in size and rate of expansion to approximately match the cortical magnification factor (Horton and Hoyt, 1991). The outer part of the ring expanded to a maximum eccentricity of 16.875°, to ensure that the whole visual display (1–15°) was stimulated for an equal amount of time, before returning to the foveal origin. The ring consisted of a colored checkerboard with each check’s chromaticity and luminance alternating at a flicker frequency of 4 Hz (see Swisher et al., 2007; Arcaro et al., 2009 for details). A transparent annulus within a dark foreground layer expanded around a central fixation point. The underlying checkerboard was only visible through the transparent annulus, giving the appearance of an expanding checkerboard ring. 20 runs were collected for monkey M1 and 30 runs were collected for monkey M2, in 5 and 14 scanning sessions, respectively.
Data acquisition
Data were acquired in both species with a 3T head-dedicated scanner (Magnetom Allegra; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). A 12 cm transmit/receive surface coil (Model NMSC-023; Nova Medical, Wakefield, MA) was used for the scanning sessions during which functional images were acquired, and a 16 cm transmit/receive quadrature volume coil (Model NM-016; Nova Medical) was used for a scanning session, during which high-resolution anatomical images were acquired. A whole-brain structural volume was acquired with the volume coil while the animals were anesthetized with Telazol (tiletamine/zolazepam, 10 mg/kg, im) in a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence (i.e. extra-session structural: 0.5 × 0.5 × 0.5 mm resolution; field of view (FOV) = 128 mm; 256 × 256 matrix; repetition [TR] = 2,500 ms; echo time [TE] = 4.4 ms; inversion time [TI] = 1,100 ms; flip angle = 8°; 20 acquisitions). In addition, a second whole-brain structural volume was acquired with the surface coil and the animal placed in the primate chair under anesthesia (i.e. within-session structural: MPRAGE sequence; 0.5 × 0.5 × 1.0 mm resolution; FOV = 128 mm; 256 × 256 matrix; TR = 2,500 ms; TE = 4.4 ms; TI = 1,100 ms; flip angle = 8°; 1 acquisition). This second structural volume was acquired with the head in the same location as during the awake experimental sessions and served as an alignment reference for the higher-quality structural volume acquired with the volume coil. All other scan sessions, each lasting about 1.0 h, were performed with the animals awake.
For awake scanning sessions, an optimized multiecho gradient echo sequence was used (ME-EPI sequence; 1.5 × 1.5 mm in-plane resolution; 26 axial slices; 1.5 mm slice thickness, no interslice gap; FOV = 120 mm; matrix = 80 × 80; TR = 2,500 ms; TE = 26 ms; flip angle = 80°; bandwidth = 2,500 Hz per pixel). This sequence permits the acquisition of data at several echo times, under reversed gradient readouts, thereby allowing for simultaneous estimation of the magnetic field, resulting in reduced image distortions with partial recovery of susceptibility-induced signal loss (Pinsk et al., 2008; Pinsk et al., 2009). The slice prescription covered the entire brain.
Data analysis
Data were analyzed using AFNI (Cox, 1996) (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni/), SUMA (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/afni/suma/), Matlab (The Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA), and FreeSurfer (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999) (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). Scanning runs during which the animal refused to fixate or moved its body were easily discernible in the eye traces and EPI images. Deviations several orders of magnitude greater than the mean variance were apparent in EPI volumes where the animal moved. Due to the nature of the analyses, these runs were discarded. A rigid motion-correction procedure (Cox and Jesmanowicz, 1999) was performed to a reference EPI volume that was acquired during the anesthetized structural scanning session when the animal was in the primate chair. The motion-corrected data were projected onto cortical surface reconstructions created with FreeSurfer from the extra-session structural scans that were aligned to each monkeys’ within-session structural. All voxels that fell between the gray and white matter boundaries were mapped to the surface. The units of data that were projected to the surface are referred to as nodes. Given that there is not a one-to-one correspondence between nodes and voxels, the value of each node was calculated by taking a weighted average of all the voxels based on the spatial overlap within each node. All subsequent analysis procedures for the visuotopy datasets (e.g. Fourier analysis) were performed on the surface-mapped data, which favorably restricts analyses to data that are primarily within the gray matter, since white matter voxels do not get mapped onto the surface. Surface projections introduce a small degree of spatial smoothing to the data. Since the degree of smoothness from surface projections can vary across cortex, the data were spatially filtered using a Gaussian filter to a maximum smoothness of 2 mm FWHM (by estimating the FWHM prior to spatial filtering), ensuring uniformity across the surface and maintaining spatial specificity while increasing the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR; Chung et al., 2005). Surface-based spatial filtering has been shown to increase both sensitivity and spatial accuracy of BOLD signal sources (Jo et al., 2007; 2008). Surface size estimates were measured using SUMA tools that calculate the volume of all nodes that fall in between the white matter and pial surface layers.
Volumes acquired during the blank periods at the start and end of each run were discarded. A Fourier analysis was used to identify spatially selective surface nodes by the polar angle and eccentricity stimuli (Bandettini et al., 1993; Engel et al., 1994). For each node of the surface, the amplitude and phase—the temporal delay relative to the stimulus onset—of the harmonic at the stimulus frequency was determined by a Fourier transform of the mean time series of the node. To correctly match the phase delay of the time series of each node to the phase of the wedge/ring stimuli, and thereby localize the region of the visual field to which the underlying neurons responded best, the response phases were corrected for the hemodynamic lag (5 s). The counterclockwise runs were then reversed to match the clockwise runs and averaged together for each node. To quantify the reliability of phase estimates across runs, the variance of a mean phase across cycles was determined for each node. A jack-knifing method in which phase estimates were calculated from n-2 cycles (eliminating one clockwise and one counterclockwise cycle per calculation for polar angle runs) across all runs was used to determine the standard error of phase estimates (see Hansen et al., 2007; Arcaro et al., 2009 for similar application). A grand mean phase estimate was calculated from the average of each of these phase estimates along with the standard error to account for variance across estimates for each node. The standard error was then converted into seconds per cycle.
Statistical maps were thresholded at a variance of ±2 s of the 40 s cycle and overlaid on cortical surface reconstructions. The pattern and significance of activation approximately compares to a statistical threshold of p < 0.01 (uncorrected for multiple comparisons, derived from the F-ratio that was calculated from the Fourier transform). When displaying phase estimates, a 20 point color scale was assigned to the polar angle datasets with each color representing 18° visual angle, and a 10 point color scale was assigned to the eccentricity data sets with each color representing 1.5° eccentricity. While activations were found in both ventral and dorsal striate and extrastriate cortex, ventral regions are outside the scope of this report, and we will focus our analysis on dorsal extrastriate cortex, i.e., brain regions located along the lunate sulcus and in PPC. Contiguous clusters of spatially selective nodes within this anatomical region that showed a systematic representation of visual space in polar or eccentricity coordinates were defined as regions of interest (ROIs). Borders between ROIs were manually identified by the primary author based on reversals in the systematic representation of visual space, particularly with respect to polar angle. Eccentricity representations were evaluated to ensure that phase progressions were essentially orthogonal (non-parallel) to the polar angle phase progression. Prior physiology and fMRI reports were used to guide the identification of borders for previously established visuotopically organized areas. All three authors subsequently assessed the borders between ROIs independently. Surface analyses of these ROIs were performed using AFNI and MATLAB. To evaluate the progression of phase values within each visual area and across PPC, response phase was analyzed as a function of distance on the surface by drawing small line segments that run parallel to the polar angle progression and perpendicular to the eccentricity progression. Phase values within a given area were then interpolated into a common space, which allowed for inter-subject averaging. To estimate the representation of the visual field for each ROI, the polar angle phases for each surface node were binned into 20 different segments of the visual field and plotted in a polar graph as a function of percentage of the visual field coverage. To compute the representation of the visual field for each visual area as a function of polar angle, the visual field was divided into four sectors: contra-and ipsilateral, as well as upper and lower quadrants. The number of nodes within each sector was tallied and divided by the total number of nodes in each area to derive a mean representation for each subject. Data were collapsed across hemispheres and averaged across monkeys to derive a group mean average. T-tests were used to assess statistical significance.
To quantitatively compare the phase alignment between clockwise and counterclockwise polar angle datasets for each monkey, we calculated alignment indices and correlation coefficients for each visual area. The alignment index (AI) was defined as
where Δφ is the difference between the polar angle phase for the clockwise and counterclockwise runs (see Sereno & Huang, 2006 for further details). Only nodes that had phase values for both data sets thresholded at ±3 s variance were included in the analysis. AIs were calculated for both polar angle measurements within each ROI on a node-by-node basis. The distribution of AI values within an ROI were plotted for single subjects in a histogram. AI distributions peaking at or near 1 indicate that the two data sets were in good alignment (i.e. when the polar angle at a vertex is identical in the two datasets). In contrast, for two uncorrelated datasets the distribution of AIs is a shallow linear ramp starting at a count of zero at an AI of zero and ending at a small value (2v/n, where v is the number of vertices and n is the number of bins) at an alignment of 1 (see Sereno & Huang, 2006). In order to perform statistical comparisons on the AIs, single subject mean index values were derived by averaging across all index values obtained for individual nodes within an ROI. One sample t-tests were conducted for each ROI between the mean index values and an index alignment value of 0.5 (representing chance). To derive a group index value, single subject mean index values were averaged within each ROI. To further evaluate the strength of alignment between both data sets for a given ROI, the correlation between phase estimates was calculated on a node-by-node basis for each subject.
To directly compare visuotopic maps in the two monkeys, standard-mesh cortical surfaces were created. Briefly, each monkey’s surface was inflated and transformed into a sphere in a manner that minimized metric distortion (Fischl et al., 1999). The individual spheres were used to create a template sphere for each hemisphere, where the curvature pattern consisted of the average pattern across both monkeys. The individual spheres were non-rigidly aligned to the templates so that the curvature patterns of each monkey matched those of the template. To avoid interpolation of the fMRI data to match the warped spheres, the SUMA software package was used to create standard-mesh surfaces from the warped spheres using icosahedral tessellation and projection (Saad et al., 2004; Argall et al., 2006). The geometry of the resulting standard-mesh surfaces is identical to the individual monkey’s original surface geometry, but the topology is common across both monkeys. The use of standard-mesh surfaces allowed for node-to-node correspondence across surfaces of both monkeys, so that functional data mapped onto one monkey’s surface could be directly compared with data mapped on the other monkey’s surface. Datasets for polar angle and eccentricity experiments were normalized for each monkey using a Fisher’s z-score transformation. Average polar angle and eccentricity maps were derived by performing a Fourier analysis on the combined data from both monkeys. To further quantify the similarity in topography between the two monkeys, each monkey’s data were also analyzed separately on their standard mesh surfaces and AI indices were calculated for both polar angle and eccentricity between them.
Cortical surfaces containing borders of the Lewis & Van Essen macaque F99 atlas (Lewis & Van Essen, 2000a, b; Van Essen, 2002) were converted from Caret (Van Essen et al., 2001) into FreeSurfer and aligned with the standard-mesh surface of the two monkeys. Both the borders and the cortical surface areas of regions defined by the atlas were compared to the corresponding regions defined by the averaged topography data.
Eye-movement recordings
Several analyses of the eye-tracking data acquired in each monkey during the scanning sessions were performed to confirm that (1) the animals maintained fixation for the majority of the time during the scanning runs, and that (2) there were no systematic differences in the eye position, (3) in the amount of eye movement, and (4) in the frequency of saccades, while different portions of the visual field were stimulated. In each monkey, eye positions, amounts of eye movement, and frequency of saccades were compared using repeated-measures ANOVAs to test for main effects of stimulus location on these measures. Individual comparisons between the stimulus locations were performed using matched paired t-tests. Significant pairwise comparisons (p < 0.05) are reported prior to correction for multiple comparisons, and comparisons that remained significant after Bonferroni correction (alpha level of 0.01) are also reported. Effect sizes for significant pairwise comparisons are reported using Cohen’s d (see Supplementary Methods & Supplementary Fig. 1 for further details).
RESULTS
Polar angle and eccentricity maps in occipital and parietal cortex
The polar angle and eccentricity components of visuotopic maps were measured for the central 15° of the visual field using a smoothly rotating wedge stimulus and an expanding annulus stimulus, respectively, in monkeys trained to maintain fixation for several minutes (Pinsk et al., 2005).
Bilateral activations within striate and extrastriate cortex were found in both hemispheres of both monkeys for polar angle and eccentricity mapping studies. For the polar angle measurements, activations within each hemisphere were mainly confined to the contralateral hemifield. Individual activation maps of polar angle and eccentricity are shown overlaid on inflated surface reconstructions for the left (LH) and right (RH) hemispheres for monkey M1 (Fig. 1) and monkey M2 (Fig. 2). For each surface node, the variance of the phase estimates across runs was calculated using a jack-knifing method (see Methods), and the threshold was chosen to only include data with a variance of ±2 s per 40 s cycle. The color of each surface node was determined by the phase of its response and indicates the region of the visual field to which the surface node was most responsive. For the polar angle component (Figs. 1A, 2A, 4A, 6A, 8 & Supplementary Fig. 5), the upper visual field (UVF) is denoted in red-yellow, the horizontal meridian (HM) in green, and the lower visual field (LVF) in blue. Area boundaries that are formed by reversals in polar angle phase progression at either the UVF or LVF near the vertical meridian (VM) are indicated with dotted and dashed lines, respectively. For the eccentricity measurements (Figs. 1B, 2B, 6B), the central space (foveal and parafoveal) is denoted in red/orange and the periphery in blue.
We found consistent and distinct visuotopically organized cortical areas within striate and extrastriate cortices in both monkeys. Several of these areas have been previously reported in monkey fMRI studies (Brewer et al., 2002; Fize et al., 2003; Kolster et al., 2009): V1, V2, V3, V3a, V4d, V4t, MT, MST, and FST. Anterior and lateral to V3a, we identified an additional visuotopic area along the dorsal prelunate gyrus, DP (Andersen et al., 1990; Heider et al., 2005). Anterior and adjacent to V3a and DP, we identified two previously not described visuotopic areas within the caudal, lateral PPC, which we refer to as CIP-1 and CIP-2. Anterior and adjacent to CIP-2, we found another visuotopic area within LIP. We also observed significant phase activity in neighboring cortex; however, the topography was more variable and therefore no consistent visuotopic organization could be identified across the four hemispheres. Below, we provide a brief description of the previously reported areas and a more detailed one of the newly found areas.
Analysis of eye position indicated that both monkeys maintained gaze within the specified fixation window during both polar angle and eccentricity mapping, and that there were no significant shifts in gaze with the stimulus position during the experiments. Furthermore, analyses of eye movements and saccade frequency indicated that neither monkey had a tendency to make significantly more eye movements for any particular stimulus position (See Supplementary Methods & Supplementary Fig. 1 for further details).
Areas V1, V2, V3
In agreement with previous reports (Daniel & Whitteridge, 1961; Zeki, 1969; Van Essen et al., 1984; Brewer et al., 2002; Fize et al., 2003), visual areas V1, V2, and V3 were identified in each hemisphere of both monkeys by a phase progression starting within the calcarine sulcus from a HM representation to a VM representation (Figs. 1A & 2A; blue color-coded phase, dashed line) that forms the border to area V2, and then reversing back to a HM that corresponds to the border of V2 and V3 (Figs. 1A & 2A; green, boundary not drawn).
Within dorsal extrastriate cortex, a phase progression was identified from the HM border of V2d/V3d to a LVM representation (Figs. 1A & 2A, blue color-coded phase, dashed line) corresponding to the anterior border of V3d. In two hemispheres, the anterior LVM border of V3d was discontinuous within the lunate sulcus with a representation near the HM in between LVM representations, as seen in the LH of M1 (Fig. 1A; left-hand column), but continuous in the other hemispheres as seen in the RH of M1 (Fig. 1A; right-hand column). These results are in agreement with previous physiology studies that have reported two variants of the anterior V3d border, one forming a continuous representation of the LVM, and the other forming a discontinuous representation of the LVM with HM representations intermixed (see Figs. 5 and 22 in Gattass et al., 1988 for examples of the two variants).
All three areas share a representation of central space within the anterior extent of the calcarine sulcus with representations of the periphery extending both dorsally and ventrally approximately perpendicular to the polar angle phase progressions (Figs. 1B & 2B).
Areas V3a and DP
A representation of the contralateral visual field including both the LVF and UVF was identified within the lunate sulcus and parieto-occipital sulcus (POS) of each hemisphere, consistent with known topography of area V3a (Van Essen & Zeki, 1978; Gattass et al., 1988; Brewer et al. 2002; Fize et al., 2003). The posterior, lateral border of V3a was identified by a LVF representation within the dorsal, anterior bank of the lunate sulcus and along the cortex separating the lunate sulcus from the POS (Figs. 1A & 2A; blue color-coded phase, dashed line). In all hemispheres, the posterior portion of the LVF representation was continuous with the LVM border of V3d, but separated from the V3d border, as it extended in an anterior direction to the prelunate gyrus. A polar angle phase progression of contralateral space was identified extending from this LVF representation in a caudal/lateral – rostral/medial direction to an UVF representation within the POS (Figs. 1A & 2A; red color-coded phase, dotted line). A representation of the central space was found within the prelunate gyrus, anterior and lateral to portions of cortex that split the lunate sulcus and POS (Figs. 1B & 2B; asterisk). The peripheral representations of the eccentricity map extended ventrally into both the posterior and anterior POS (Figs. 1B & 2B). The anatomical extent of V3a was consistent between hemispheres and monkeys as is shown on the surface (Figs. 1C & 2C) as well as within the volume (Fig. 3 & Supplementary Figs. 2 & 3). The mean surface area of V3a was 45.6 ±3.0 mm2. The borders for V3a, in Horsley Clarke stereotaxic coordinates, extended from −6.0:−12.5 (A-P), +19.5:+25.5 (I-S), +7.0:+16.0 (M-L ) (Table 1).
Table 1.
Left Hemisphere | Right Hemisphere | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
A-P | I-S | M-L | A-P | I-S | M-L | |
V3a | ||||||
M1 | −6.0: −12.0 | +19.5: +22.5 | +7.5: +16.0 | −6.0: −12 | +20.0: +23.5 | +7.0: +15.0 |
M2 | −6.5: −12.5 | +21.0: +25.5 | +7.5: +14.5 | −6.0: −12 | +21.0: +24.5 | +7.5: +14.0 |
DP | ||||||
M1 | −7.0: −10.5 | +22.5: +25.5 | +7.5: +13.5 | −7.0: −10.5 | +23.0: +26.5 | +6.5: +13.5 |
M2 | −6.5: −10.5 | +22.0: +28.0 | +6.0: +14.0 | −7.0: −10.0 | +22.5: +26.0 | +7.0: +13.0 |
CIP-1 | ||||||
M1 | −5.5: −9.5 | +20.0: +25.5 | +6.5: +10.0 | −5.0: −8.0 | +21.5: +26.5 | +5.5: +9.5 |
M2 | −4.5: −8.5 | +20.5: +28.5 | +5.5: +9.5 | −3.5: −8.0 | +21.0: +26.5 | +5.5: +8.5 |
CIP-2 | ||||||
M1 | −3.0: −7.5 | +20.5: +26.0 | +6.0: +8.5 | −3.0: −6.0 | +22.5: +27.0 | +6.0: +8.0 |
M2 | −2.5: −5.5 | +21.5: +28.5 | +5.5: +8.5 | −1.0: −5.5 | +22.0: +27.0 | +5.5: +8.5 |
LIPvt | ||||||
M1 | +1.0: −4.5 | +22.0: +27.0 | +6.5: +12.0 | +0.5: −5.0 | +23.0: +29.0 | +6.5: +13.0 |
M2 | +1.0: −3.5 | +22.5: +29.0 | +6.0: +11.5 | +1.5: −4.0 | +22.0: +28.0 | +7.0: +12.0 |
A LVF representation of contralateral space was identified anterior to V3a along the dorsal portion of the prelunate gyrus in each hemisphere, which we tentatively label area DP (May & Andersen, 1986; Gattass et al., 1988; Fize et al., 2003; Heider et al., 2005; but also see Maguire & Baizer, 1984). A representation of the HM was identified within the POS, adjacent and medial to this LVF representation with an UVF representation extending into area V3a located further medially within the POS (Figs. 1A & 2A). This region was largely encompassed by a representation of central space abutting representations of central space within V3a, but also included cortex along the prelunate gyrus, located anterior and medial to the central space, that appeared to be distinguished from the topography of V3a (Figs. 1B & 2B). The anterior portion of V3a roughly encompassed the posterior half of the central space and the posterior portion of DP encompasses the anterior half of the central space. No clear eccentricity progression was apparent, though consistent representations of the periphery were observed in this region anterior and medial to the central space (e.g. Fig. 2B; LH of M2, left-hand column). The anatomical extent of DP was consistent between hemispheres and monkeys as is shown on the surface (Figs. 1C & 2C) as well as within the volume (Fig. 3 & Supplementary Figs. 2 & 3). The mean surface area of DP was 35.1 ±3.0 mm2. The borders for DP extended from −6.5:−10.5 (A-P), +22.0:+28.0 (I-S), +6.0:+14.0 (M-L) (Table 1).
To further investigate the topographic organization of V3a, the polar angle phase progression was quantified and evaluated. Small line segments were successively drawn parallel to the polar angle progression and perpendicular to the eccentricity progression (see Methods) from the posterior border of V3a to the shared anterior border, as indicated by the schematic lines in Figure 4A. As shown in Figure 4B, the polar angle phase values from these line segments were plotted as a function of distance (in mm) from the rostral border of V3a. The blue dots indicate the phase values for individual nodes located along the line segments, and the red line indicates the average phase values as a function of the distance on the surface. Polar angle phase progressions between area borders for individual monkeys were interpolated into a common space, which allowed for group averaging (Fig. 4C). As seen in the LH of M1 and M2 (Fig. 4B), as well as in the group polar phase plot (Fig. 4C), the posterior and anterior borders of V3a corresponded to the peaks of phase angles near the LVF and UVF, respectively, with a smooth progression of phase values in-between.
Areas CIP-1 and CIP-2
Anterior to area V3A, two polar angle phase representations of contralateral space were found in each hemisphere, spanning across the POS, posterior portions of the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and posterior portions of the inferior parietal gyrus (at the junction of the prelunate gyrus and the inferior parietal gyrus), which we refer to as CIP-1 & CIP-2. In each hemisphere, CIP-1 was defined by a polar angle phase progression extending from the UVF representation of V3a/DP (red color-coded phase/dotted line) anterior to a phase reversal in posterior IPS (Figs. 1A & 2A). In two of the four hemispheres, a small strip of LVF representation (blue color-coded phase, dashed) within a HM representation defined the phase reversal as seen in the RH of M2 (Fig. 2A). In the other two hemispheres, the phase progression reversed within a HM representation (also indicated with dashed line). In all hemispheres, CIP-2 was defined by polar angle phase progressions extending further anterior to an UVF representation (red color-coded phase, dotted line) within the posterior portion of the IPS (Figs. 1A & 2A). The UVF representation spanned across the inferior parietal gyrus medially and into the fundus of the IPS. This region of cortex within the lateral bank of caudal IPS likely corresponds to a region of cortex that has been previously referred to as cIPS (Sakata et al., 1998), LOP (Lewis & Van Essen, 2000a, b), CIP (Taira et al., 2000), CIPS (Tsao et al. 2003), and pLIP (Kagan et al., 2010). In the RH hemisphere of M1, the UVF representation of CIP-1 appeared fragmented from medial portions of the UVF representation of V3a (Fig. 1A), though the remaining polar angle phase progression was consistent with the other hemispheres. The mirror reversal in phase angle representations within CIP-1 and CIP-2 is further illustrated by the pattern of phase progressions (Fig. 4).
A representation of central space was identified anterior to that of V3a, and overlapped with lateral portions of CIP-1 and CIP-2 along the lateral IPS and onto the inferior parietal lobule (Figs. 1B & 2B; asterisk). The central space representations of CIP-1/2 appeared continuous with the central space representations of V3a in two hemispheres as seen in the RH of M1 (Fig. 1B), but were clearly separate in the LH of M2 (Fig. 2B). The eccentricity maps lacked detail in this region of cortex. In M1, representations of the periphery extended medially into the fundus of the parieto-occipital confluence and IPS (Fig. 1B). However, eccentricity representations were mostly central space and parafoveal in M2 (Fig. 2B). The anatomical extent of CIP-1 and CIP-2 was consistent between hemispheres and monkeys, as is shown on the surface (Figs. 1C & 2C) as well as within the volume (Fig. 3 & Supplementary Figs. 2 & 3). The mean surface area of CIP-1 and CIP-2 were 32.1 ±4.8 mm2 and 34.2 ±4.5 mm2 respectively. The borders for CIP-1 extended from −3.5:−9.5 (A-P), +20.0:+28.5 (I-S), +5.5:+10.0 (M-L) and for CIP-2 from −1.0:−7.5 (A-P), +20.0:+28.5 (I-S), +5.5:+8.5 (M-L) (Table 1).
Area LIPvt
A representation of the contralateral visual field was observed within the lateral portion of the IPS anterior to CIP-2 in all four hemispheres, referred to as visuotopic LIP, or LIPvt. A polar angle phase progression extended in a caudal-rostral direction from the UVF representation shared by CIP-2 to a LVF representation that spanned across the lateral bank and fundus of the IPS (Figs. 1A & 2A). In agreement with prior reports, this phase progression within lateral IPS (Blatt et al., 1990; Ben Hamed et al., 2001) encompassed both ventral and dorsal portions of LIP with the UVF located slightly more lateral to the LVF. The mirror reversal in phase angle representations within LIPvt is further illustrated by the pattern of phase progressions (Fig. 4).
In all four hemispheres, a representation of central space was identified anterior to CIP-1 and CIP-2 along the lateral bank of the IPS (Figs. 1B & 2B; asterisk). The central space appeared continuous with the representations of central space within dorsolateral portions of CIP in two hemispheres, as seen in the RH of M1 (Fig. 1B), but was separate in the other two hemispheres as seen in the LH of M1 (Fig. 1B). In agreement with prior reports (Blatt et al., 1990; Ben Hamed et al., 2001), no clear progression of eccentricity was observed, though peripheral representations were generally observed ventromedially, along the fundus of the IPS, as seen in the RH and LH of M1 (Fig. 1B). Aside from a dorsolateral-central and ventromedial-peripheral distinction, the eccentricity maps within this region of cortex were quite coarse. The anatomical extent of LIPvt was consistent between hemispheres and monkeys as is shown on the surface (Figs. 1C & 2C) as well as within the volume (Fig. 3 & Supplementary Figs. 2 & 3). The mean surface area of LIPvt was 38.6 ±4.6 mm2. The borders for LIPvt extended from +1.5:-5.0 (A-P), +22.0:+29.0 (I-S), +6.0:+13.0 (M-L) (Table 1).
To evaluate the strength of the stimulus-evoked signal relative to noise in LIPvt, as well as V3a, DP, CIP-1 and CIP-2, response amplitudes were calculated as a function of temporal frequency for the polar angle and eccentricity measurements. For each monkey, the response at the stimulus frequency (SF) of four cycles was at least 2 standard errors (SEs) greater than the mean response across all other frequencies, demonstrating a strong link between the measured neural response and stimulus location. Temporal frequency histograms were derived for each monkey and each hemisphere and then averaged to yield group data. For both the polar angle and eccentricity data, the response at the SF (4 cycles) was significantly greater than noise for V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt (Table 3; for polar angle, all t values > 4.30, p values < 0.05; for eccentricity, all t values > 3.40, p values < 0.05). In a complimentary analysis, the time series of fMRI signals within line segments from the polar angle phase progression analysis corresponding to the UVF and LVF reversal points were averaged to derive a mean time series for each border between visuotopic areas in PPC. Consistent with the power analysis, the stimulus-evoked signal in V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt was apparent in the time series, as demonstrated in monkey M1 (Supplementary Figure 4A). The phase offsets between UVF and LVF time series were estimated by calculating the correlations between the two time series across 10 timepoint offsets for each ROI and measuring the distance between the original time series and the offset time series with the highest correlation. The time series between UVF and LVF borders were out of phase with each other on average by 17 ±.5s for M1 and 17.5 ±.79s for M2. To evaluate the relation of phase between UVF and LVF borders with polar angle phase estimates, the time series were averaged between monkeys and collapsed across cycles. The modulation in fMRI signal for each area corresponded to the estimated phases from the Fourier analysis (Supplementary Figure 4B). The cycles of UVF borders (V3a/CIP-1 & CIP-2/LIPvt) were in phase with each other and 17.5s out of phase with the cycle of LVF anterior border of LIPvt. The cycle of the LVF border between CIP-1 and CIP-2 was 5s out of phase from the cycle of the anterior LVF border of LIPvt, consistent with the phase progression analysis that demonstrated the phase progression reversal point to be closer to the HM.
Table 3.
V3a | DP | CIP-1 | CIP-2 | LIPvt | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Amplitude | |||||
Polar Angle | 1.32 (.30) | 0.95 (.20) | 0.40 (.05) | 0.60 (.10) | 0.40 (.09) |
Eccentricity | 0.57 (.08) | 0.40 (.09) | 0.30 (.10) | 0.14 (.03) | 0.17 (.04) |
Visual Field Coverage | |||||
Contra vs. Ipsi | 99 (1)% | 94 (3)% | 97 (1)% | 97 (1)% | 96 (1)% |
Upper vs. Lower | 40 (4)% | 70 (4)% | 72 (4)% | 84 (6)% | 42 (7)% |
Other topographic representations in surrounding cortical areas
Additional polar angle and eccentricity phase estimates were observed across neighboring cortex, which are beyond the scope of the present report, but will be briefly described here to give a comprehensive account of the data.
V4 complex - Areas V4d and V4t
In agreement with previous studies (Zeki, 1969; Zeki, 1980; Maguire & Baizer, 1984; Desimone & Ungerleider, 1986; Gattass et al., 1988; also see Fize et al., 2003), visual areas V4d and V4t were identified in each hemisphere of both monkeys. A polar angle phase progression of contralateral space was identified from the LVM representation corresponding to the border between V3d and V4d to a HM representation corresponding to the border between V4d and V4t (Figs. 1A & 2A). The phase progression reversed further anterior into the ventral bank of the posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS) to a LVM representation corresponding to the border between V4t and MT/V5. The central space representation of V4d appears to be continuous with the central space representation shared by early visual areas V1, V2, and V3 (Figs. 1B & 2B). The central space representation of V4t is located more laterally within the fundus of posterior STS and may be distinct from the central space representations of early visual areas (see Kolster et al., 2009).
The anterior lateral portion of the LVM representation of V3a was identified as separate from the anterior border of V3d in all hemispheres (Figs. 1A & 2A). This resulted in a triangular region on the cortical surface in-between V3a, V3d, and V4d. This region consistently fell within an anterior portion of the lunate sulcus that was indented into the prelunate gyrus (Figs. 1 & 2). This region largely represented the contralateral HM and LVF and mainly represented central portions of visual space with the representations of central space shared by V3a and DP, located adjacent and medial to this region (Figs. 1 & 2). The apparent phase progression within the lower visual field suggests an organized representation of at least part of the contralateral visual field. However, this region may correspond to a transition region between dorsal V4 and DP (Lewis & Van Essen, 2000a), and not to specific visual field map(s). Further, the observed topography within this region of cortex is not easily relatable to the large-scale topographic organization observed in humans as the correspondence in topographic organization of dorsal V4 and the surrounding cortex to humans (Larsson & Heeger, 2006; Hansen et al. 2007) is still unclear and may be significantly differ between species (Wandell et al. 2007; Kolster et al. 2010). Given the region’s relatively small spatial extent and the limited spatial resolution of our methods, identifying precise topographic organization was inconclusive. A systematic investigation of this region and the surrounding cortex including V4d and V4t at higher resolution and therefore higher field strength (and possibly additional functional experiments) would be required to resolve the topography and its relation to neighboring areas, and is therefore beyond the scope of the current study.
Area 7a
Along the inferior parietal gyrus, lateral and adjacent to the central space representations of CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt, a representation of the contralateral LVF was evident in all hemispheres, though no clear topographic organization could be identified beyond these representations (Figs. 1 & 2). This region of cortex likely corresponds to area 7a (Andersen et al., 1990). This part of cortex appears to have mainly peripheral representations, but also overlaps with central space regions shared by CIP-1 and CIP-2 (Figs. 1B & 2B). Notably, Heider and colleagues (2005) have found coarse retinotopic organization within area 7a using optical imaging techniques. However, the specific details of the topographic organization appeared to vary from day-to-day. Though our results suggest the existence of some spatial topography within this region of cortex, no clearly organized map of the visual field could be identified; consistent with the interpretation that area 7a forgoes a static retinotopic organization for a spatial topography capable of adapting to alternating environments (Heider et al. 2005).
STS - Areas MT, MSTv, and FST
Polar angle phase progressions of contralateral space were observed within posterior STS, consistent with previous studies delineating MT, MSTv, and FST (Kolster et al., 2009; also see Gattass & Gross, 1981; Van Essen et al., 1981; Desimone & Ungerleider, 1986). In all hemispheres, a polar angle progression was identified from the LVM representation (Figs. 1A & 2A) corresponding to the border between V4t and MT to an UVF representation within the lower bank of posterior STS corresponding to the border between MT and MSTv. The polar angle progression reversed back towards a LVF representation further anterior and within the fundus of the STS corresponding to the border between MSTv and FST.
Within the dorsal portion of the posterior STS, superior to MT and MST, a peripheral representation of the LVF and HM was observed in all four hemispheres (Figs. 1 & 2). As suggested by Kolster and colleagues (2009), this anatomical area may correspond to MTp or other neighboring motion sensitive areas (Desimone and Ungerleider, 1986). No apparent representations central space could be identified.
Visual field representations
As seen in Table 3, areas V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt represented almost exclusively the contralateral visual field (contralateral vs ipsilateral visual field: all t(3) values > 25, p values < 0.0001). For comparison, 97 ±1% of nodes represented contralateral visual space in area V1. The strong laterality is further demonstrated in the polar plots of V1, V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt (Fig. 5). As is evident in the polar plots as well as the polar angle maps, V3a and LIPvt contained roughly equivalent representations of the upper and lower quadrants of the contralateral visual space (Table 3). The caudal IPS areas, CIP-1/2, contained a larger representation of the upper quadrant. Conversely, area DP contained a larger representation of the lower quadrant. For comparison, 47 ±1% of nodes represented the upper quadrant and 50 ±2% of nodes represented the lower quadrant in V1. Due to the limited number of samples (n = 4), more data will be necessary to conclusively assume any asymmetries in visual field for the CIP areas or DP.
Reproducibility of polar angle phase maps
To further evaluate the consistency and reproducibility of the topography, polar angle phase estimates were determined for clockwise and counterclockwise runs separately. The resulting phase maps were highly consistent between clockwise and counterclockwise runs, as indicated qualitatively by the similarities in characteristics of polar angle phase progressions described above (Supplementary Fig. 5). To quantify the similarity of phase estimates between clockwise and counterclockwise data on a node-by-node basis, alignment indices (AI) were calculated for both hemispheres in both monkeys (see Methods). The AI values range from 0, which indicates that the values obtained in the two data sets were completely out of phase by 180°, to 1, which indicates perfect phase alignment. Mean AI values were averaged across monkeys for V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt (Table 4). In comparison, AI values for V1 ranged between 0.97 and 0.92. Histograms of mean AIs are shown for V1, V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt (see Supplementary Fig. 6). Additionally, the calculated correlation coefficients of each ROI were significant for each monkey’s polar phase measurements (all r values > 0.67, all p values < 10−10 uncorrected; median r = 0.78), demonstrating that there was good alignment throughout each ROI for each monkey. These analyses provide a quantitative measure for the consistency of the topographic organization across runs regardless of the direction of stimulus rotation.
Table 4.
V3a | DP | CIP-1 | CIP-2 | LIPvt | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Alignment Indices within Monkeys | |||||
CW vs CCW RH | 0.95 (.01) | 0.95 (.01) | 0.91 (.04) | 0.92(.01) | 0.90 (.01) |
CW vs CCW LH | 0.96 (.01) | 0.95 (.01) | 0.91 (.02) | 0.93 (.01) | 0.89 (.02) |
Alignment Indices between Monkeys | |||||
Polar Angle RH | 0.91 (.002) | 0.88 (.004) | 0.84 (.007) | 0.92 (.006) | 0.94 (.002) |
Polar Angle LH | 0.96 (.001) | 0.90 (.005) | 0.96 (.002) | 0.93 (.003) | 0.89 (.004) |
Eccentricity RH | 0.85 (.004) | 0.82 (.005) | 0.70 (.005) | 0.64 (.007) | 0.84 (.006) |
Eccentricity LH | 0.84 (.003) | 0.85 (.005) | 0.80 (.007) | 0.84 (.006) | 0.75 (.004) |
Group average map
To evaluate the consistency of the large-scale topographic organization across the individual animals, polar angle and eccentricity data from both monkeys were mapped onto a standard-mesh surface (see Methods), and average topography maps were calculated for each hemisphere. The topographic organization throughout V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt was remarkably similar in visual field representation as compared to that seen in individual monkeys (Figs. 1, 2, 5 & 6A/B). Though the sample size is limited, this consistency across all 4 hemispheres of the 2 monkeys suggests that the organization is not much influenced by inter – individual variability in the higher – order areas in the macaque brain, which is in contrast to the known individual variability of human PPC (Silver et al., 2005; Schluppeck et al., 2005; Swisher et al., 2007).
To further evaluate the similarity of phase estimates between the data sets of the two monkeys on a node-by-node basis, AI values were calculated between M1 and M2. Histograms of mean polar angle AIs are shown for V1, V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt in Supplementary Fig. 7A. The mean AI values for polar angle are listed in Table 4. Additionally, the calculated correlation coefficients of each ROI were significant for each monkey’s polar phase measurements (all r values > 0.51, all p values < 10−10 uncorrected; median r = 0.64), indicating that there was good alignment throughout each ROI between monkey datasets. This analysis provides quantitative measures for the consistency of the topographic organization between individual monkeys.
The mean AI values for eccentricity are listed in Table 4. Histograms of mean eccentricity AIs are shown for V1, V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt (see Supplementary Fig. 7B). The calculated correlation coefficients of each ROI, though smaller than those for the polar angle data, were significant for each monkey’s eccentricity phase measurements (all r values > 0.28, all p values < 10−5 uncorrected; median r = 0.37). For comparison, AI values were also calculated for polar angle (RH 0.93, r = 0.72, p < 10−10uncorrected; LH 0.92, r = 0.65, p < 10−10 uncorrected) and eccentricity data (RH 0.92, r = 0.84, p < 10−10 uncorrected; LH 0.90, r = 0.74, p < 10−10 uncorrected) for V1.
Comparison to anatomical parcellation of PPC
To compare our results to an anatomical and histological parcellation of PPC, area borders (Lewis & Van Essen, 2000a, b) from the F99 macaque atlas (F99; Van Essen, 2002) were mapped onto the standard-mesh surface and compared to the boundaries determined from the average topography data of V3a, DP, CIP-1, CIP-2, and LIPvt (see Methods). Overall, there was good agreement between the atlas and topography data, particularly for early visual areas V1, V2, and V3 as well as V3a. However, there were a few notable differences between the functional mapping and the atlas data (Fig. 6C). Visuotopically - defined DP overlapped with posterior portions of the F99 - defined DP, but also included anterior portions of the F99 - defined V3a. There was no direct correspondence of CIP-1 and CIP-2 to individual areas in the atlas. Both areas fell largely within area LOP, thereby subdividing area LOP into posterior and anterior portions, though CIP-1 also overlapped with anterior portions of the F99 - defined V3a. Though CIP-2 did not overlap with F99 - defined LIP, CIP-2 may also overlap with other anatomical and functional definitions of LIP (Andersen et al., 1990; Blatt et al., 1990). Visuotopically - defined LIPvt overlapped with both LIPd and LIPv of the atlas with representations of central space falling largely within LIPd, and more peripheral representations falling within LIPv. In both hemispheres, visuotopically - defined LIPvt comprised only the posterior half of LIP defined by the atlas. Even though the comparison to anatomically and histologically-defined areas and zones of an atlas relative to functional topography in individual animals can only reveal approximate correspondences, the comparison of our data to the F99 parcellation schema may prove useful for investigators performing recordings in these regions, taking the atlas as a guide for electrode placement.
Comparison to functional organization of human PPC
Recent fMRI studies including from our laboratory and others have identified several visuotopically - organized areas within dorsal visual cortex extending into the posterior parietal cortex in humans using a variety of different mapping methods including phase – encoded retinotopic mapping similar to that used here (Sereno et al., 2001; Silver et al., 2005; Schluppeck et al., 2005; Swisher et al., 2007; Konen & Kastner, 2008; see Silver & Kastner, 2009 for review). In Figure 7, we compare the topographic organization of PPC in the macaque and human based on polar angle (Konen & Kastner, 2008) and representations of central space (Konen & Kastner 2008, unpublished observations; also see Swisher et al., 2007). In both species, polar angle and eccentricity phase progressions were apparent throughout the lunate sulcus and into PPC. As has been noted previously, the topographic organization of V3a in the macaque is similar to area V3a in the human (Van Essen et al., 2001; Brewer et al., 2002; Fize et al., 2003; Tsao et al., 2003). In both species, there is a posterior - anterior polar angle phase progression from LVM to UVM representations with a representation of central space on its lateral border. The topographic organization observed for DP appears similar to the topography of human area V3b. In both species, there is a posterior - anterior polar angle phase progression from LVM to UVM representations with a representation of the central space on its medial border shared with V3a. The topographic organization and location in relation to surrounding topographic regions of CIP-1 and CIP-2 in the macaque is similar to areas V7 (IPS-0) and IPS-1 in the human. In both species, there is a posterior - anterior polar angle phase progression starting and ending with an UVF representation. However, in the human, the border of IPS-0 and IPS-1 is defined by a reversal in polar phase in the LVF, whereas in the monkey, the border between CIP-1 and CIP-2 is largely within the HM (though a sparse LVF representation was identified along the border in two of four hemispheres). For both monkey and human topographies, the representations of central space are located on the lateral border, with more peripheral representations located medially. LIPvt in the macaque appears similar to IPS-2 in the human. In both species, there is a posterior - anterior polar angle phase progression from UVM to LVM representations with a representation of central space on the lateral border.
Topographic organization within the IPS in humans consists of additional areas, IPS-3/4/5, extending further anterior. The IPS also extends anterior from LIPvt in the macaque. However, consistent topographic organization was not identified beyond LIPvt in all hemispheres. It is important to note that our comparison of the topographic organization of the dorsal pathway in humans and monkeys is not aimed at establishing homology between areas, but instead, it is aimed at revealing similarities and differences in large-scale brain topography that may aid future investigations with a simple starting point to find regions that have analogous functions or regions that show similar functional organization.
DISCUSSION
We investigated topographic organization of dorsal extrastriate cortex and PPC using fMRI and phase-encoded retinotopic mapping in monkeys trained to maintain fixation. By considering both the polar angle and eccentricity phase estimates, we identified four visuotopically-organized areas representing contralateral visual space within PPC. A representation of the visual field adjacent and lateral to area V3a was found within dorsal portions of the prelunate gyrus, referred to as DP. Adjacent and anterior to V3a within the caudal, lateral portions of the IPS, two previously not described visuotopic areas were found, referred to as CIP-1 and CIP-2. Adjacent and anterior to CIP-2, we identified a representation of the visual field within the lateral bank of the IPS, referred to as LIPvt. The visuotopic organization within dorsal extrastriate cortex and PPC and the presented framework for outlining area borders was highly consistent in all four hemispheres.
Macaque dorsal extrastriate cortex and PPC have been subdivided based on results from anatomy and physiology studies, and several different parcellation schemes have been derived (Pandya & Seltzer, 1982; Seltzer & Pandya, 1986; Colby et al., 1998; Andersen et al., 1990; Felleman & Van Essen, 1991; Preuss & Goldman-Rakic, 1991; Lewis & Van Essen, 2000a, b). We compared our data to the areas delineated in the F99 macaque atlas (Lewis & Van Essen, 2000a, b; Van Essen, 2002) since it proposes subregions within caudal IPS, dorsal prelunate, and the lateral IPS, and has been applied to both physiology (Stoet & Snyder, 2004; Huk & Shadlen, 2005; Janssen et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2010) and fMRI studies (Tsao et al., 2003; Denys et al., 2004; Durand et al., 2007; Kagan et al., 2010; Patel et al., 2010).
Within dorsal portions of the prelunate gyrus, we identified a visual field map, DP, that predominantly contained representations of the LVF with adjacent cortex within POS representing the horizontal and upper vertical meridians. Our results are consistent with physiology recordings that have found the receptive fields within DP to be concentrated within the contralateral LVF (Andersen et al., 1990), and with fMRI studies that have found representations of the LVF within the dorsal prelunate gyrus (Fize et al., 2003). Using optical imaging, Heider and colleagues (2005) found representations of both the UVF and LVF within DP, though the topography varied between monkeys with no apparent systematic progression of visual field locations. Thus, our results extend these prior findings by identifying a systematic visual field map within the dorsal prelunate gyrus and its relation to neighboring topographic cortex.
Within the caudal PPC, we found, to our knowledge for the first time, spatial topography suggesting that this region of cortex may be functionally subdivided into two separate areas, referred to as CIP-1 and CIP-2. CIP-1/2 were located within caudal PPC, including anterior portions of the POS and lateral IPS. CIP-2 extended into lateral portions of posterior IPS that may correspond to cortex defined as part of LIP (Blatt et al., 1990; Kagan et al., 2010). The posterior border of CIP-1 abutted the borders of V3a and DP with the anterior border of CIP-2 extending into the lateral bank of the IPS. This region of cortex has been shown to represent 3-dimensional (3D) shape and texture information (see Katsuyama et al., 2010 for review). Future investigations are required to define a functional role of topographic organization in the processing of 3D stimulus features, and to characterize whether the response properties of CIP-2 more closely reflect previously defined CIP (Sakata et al., 1998; Taira et al., 2000) relative to LIP (Bushnell et al., 1981; Gnadt & Andersen, 1988; Shadlen & Newsome, 2001).
Within the lateral IPS, we identified a visual field map, LIPvt, that shared its posterior border with the anterior border of CIP-2 and encompassed both dorsal and ventral portions of lateral IPS. Based on results from previous physiology studies, the topographic organization within LIP has been rather unclear (Blatt et al., 1990; Platt & Glimcher, 1998; Ben Hamed et al., 2001). Blatt and colleagues (1990) reported a posterior-anterior gradient from LVF to UVF representations of the contralateral visual field, whereas Ben Hamed and colleagues (2001) reported the opposite pattern, a posterior-anterior gradient from UVF to LVF representations. Both studies agreed regarding a central space representation within LIPd. However, a third report was unable to find any systematic organization of contralateral space within LIP (Platt & Glimcher, 1998). The polar angle phase progression of LIPvt appears in agreement with the topographic organization suggested by Ben Hamed and colleagues (2001), as shown in Figure 8, where we adapted the schema of LIP topography based on their single-unit data (Fig. 8C in Ben Hamed et al., 2001) and projected it onto the LH of M1. However, the polar angle phase progression of CIP-2 just posterior to LIPvt is in good agreement with the topographic organization and anatomical coordinates reported by Blatt and colleagues (1990), suggesting that the discrepancies between these two physiology studies are due to differences in the recording site location and the existence of multiple organized visuotopic maps within lateral IPS. Thus, our data is in good agreement with physiology and offers an interpretation regarding discrepancies observed in previous studies.
Our results of visuotopic organization within LIP partially confirm, but more importantly extend recent findings of topographic organization within PPC using fMRI (Patel et al., 2010; see also Fize et al., 2003). Using a covert attention task on visual stimuli presented at only few different polar angle and eccentricity locations within the visual field, a topographic representation of the contralateral hemifield was found that appeared to be confined to LIPv (Patel et al., 2010). It was concluded that PPC contains a single topographic representation that can only be revealed in a complex cognitive task, suggesting a specific role of LIPv in higher cognition. While our results concur with the posterior-anterior progression of an UVF to LVF representation, there are several notable differences between our findings and those of Patel and colleagues (2010). First and foremost, we demonstrate a systematic and continuous progression of phase angle representations within LIP, which is an important criterion in establishing a visual field map (see Wandell et al., 2007). This requires identifying the most effective visual field position for each location within a map and can be best achieved with a continuous mapping approach such as phase-encoded mapping, rather than by stimulating a few discrete locations within the visual field. It is also noteworthy that the visuotopic map within LIP was revealed using standard mapping procedures under passive viewing conditions without requirement of a cognitive task. Second, the visuotopic map within LIP is not isolated, but is embedded in a rich topographic organization of PPC with several visuotopically organized areas adjacent to LIPvt, which were differentiated on the basis of their systematic representation of visual space and reversals in polar angle phase progression at or near the vertical meridians. Third, given the posterior-anterior direction of polar angle phase progression, the dorsolateral representation of central space that was identified in our and prior physiology studies appears to be a better candidate for LIPvt than an anterior fovea reported within LIPv (Patel et al., 2010). Throughout the visual system, the progressions of polar angle and eccentricity are typically represented orthogonal (and not parallel) to each other. It is possible that the anterior representation of central space that was also found in the present study corresponds to additional topographic representations within anterior LIPvt.
A spatial map of saccade trajectories was recently found within anatomically - defined LIPv (Savaki et al., 2010). It is not entirely clear how this map of oculomotor space relates to the visuotopic map identified in our data and prior studies due to the differences in criteria used for parcellating LIPd and LIPv (Medalla & Barbas, 2006). However, the apparent close proximity of the oculomotor map to the anterior intraparietal area (AIP; see Figs. 3b, 9 & 10a in Savaki et al., 2010) suggests that the oculomotor map lies anterior to the visuotopic map observed within the posterior half of the anatomical extent of LIP. Consistent with this interpretation, Kagan and colleagues (2010) reported stronger BOLD modulation for saccades towards the contralateral visual field within an anterior portion of LIP (aLIP), which appears to be anterior to LIPvt (see Fig. 1 & Supplementary Fig. 3 in Kagan et al. 2010). Interestingly, Savaki and colleagues (2010) reported representations for small saccade trajectories within LIPv, which may correspond to the representations of central visual space observed in the current data anterior to LIPvt, but within the anatomical extent of LIP. The findings of oculomotor and visuotopic maps, possibly in different parts of LIP, suggest the existence of topographic organization along multiple functional reference frames in macaque PPC.
Several visuotopically-organized areas within dorsal extrastriate cortex and PPC have recently been reported using fMRI in humans (Sereno et al., 2001; Silver et al., 2005; Schluppeck et al., 2005; Swisher et al., 2007; Konen & Kastner, 2008; reviewed in Silver & Kastner, 2009). In our comparison of macaque and human large-scale PPC topography, several parallels in polar angle and eccentricity representations were identified based on the topography within individual areas and their relation to the surrounding large-scale topography. In addition to the previously noted correspondence of V3a topography, macaque CIP-1 and human IPS-0, CIP-2 and IPS-1, as well as LIPvt and IPS-2 showed striking similarities in topographic organization. Similarities in large-scale topographic organization between species do not necessitate or imply homology, nor do they preclude functional dissociations between species. Rather, large-scale cortical organization can be used to systematically investigate functional properties between species. Our results provide a framework for detailed comparisons of parietal cortical areas between humans and monkeys that should prove useful in relating the functional similarities and dissociations between species.
Supplementary Material
Table 2.
V1 | V3a | DP | CIP-1 | CIP-2 | LIPvt | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Surface Volume | 1092.5 | 45.6 | 35.1 | 32.1 | 34.3 | 38.6 |
SEM | 45.6 | 2.5 | 3.1 | 4.8 | 4.5 | 4.6 |
% of V1 | 100.0 | 4.2 | 3.2 | 3.0 | 3.1 | 3.5 |
Acknowledgments
M.J.A. and M.A.P. contributed equally to this study. We thank B. Singer for providing code for fourier analyses, U. Hasson, Y. Saalmann, & N. Turk-Browne for helpful comments. Supported by P50MH062196, R01-EY017699, R01MH064043, BCS-0633281 & T90-DA022763.
References
- Andersen RA, Asanuma C, Essick G, Siegel RM. Corticocortical connections of anatomically and physiologically defined subdivisions within the inferior parietal lobule. J Comp Neurol. 1990;296:65–113. doi: 10.1002/cne.902960106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arcaro MJ, McMains SA, Singer BD, Kastner S. Retinotopic organization of human ventral visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2009;29:10638–10652. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2807-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Argall BD, Saad ZS, Beauchamp MS. Simplified intersubject averaging on the cortical surface using SUMA. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006;27:14–27. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandettini PA, Jesmanowicz A, Wong EC, Hyde JS. Processing strategies for time-course data sets in functional MRI of the human brain. Magn Reson Med. 1993;30:161–173. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910300204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben Hamed S, Duhamel JR, Bremmer F, Graf W. Representation of the visual field in the lateral intraparietal area of macaque monkeys: a quantitative receptive field analysis. Exp Brain Res. 2001;140:127–144. doi: 10.1007/s002210100785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blatt GJ, Andersen RA, Stoner GR. Visual receptive field organization and cortico-cortical connections of the lateral intraparietal area (area LIP) in the macaque. J Comp Neurol. 1990;299:421–445. doi: 10.1002/cne.902990404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewer AA, Press WA, Logothetis NK, Wandell BA. Visual areas in macaque cortex measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2002;22:10416–10426. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-23-10416.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewer AA, Liu J, Wade AR, Wandell BA. Visual field maps and stimulus selectivity in human ventral occipital cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:1102–1109. doi: 10.1038/nn1507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bushnell MC, Goldberg ME, Robinson DL. Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. J Neurophysiol. 1981;46:755–772. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.46.4.755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen A, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Macaque parieto-insular vestibular cortex: responses to self-motion and optic flow. J Neurosci. 2010;30:3022–3042. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4029-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung MK, Robbins SM, Dalton KM, Davidson RJ, Alexander AL, Evans AC. Cortical thickness analysis in autism with heat kernel smoothing. Neuroimage. 2005;25:1256–1265. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colby CL, Gattass R, Olson CR, Gross CG. Topographical organization of cortical afferents to extrastriate visual area PO in the macaque: a dual tracer study. J Comp Neurol. 1988;269:392–413. doi: 10.1002/cne.902690307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW, Jesmanowicz A. Real-time 3D image registration for functional MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1999;42:1014–1018. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1522-2594(199912)42:6<1014::aid-mrm4>3.0.co;2-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniel PM, Whitteridge D. The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys. J Physiol. 1961;159:203–221. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1961.sp006803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denys K, Vanduffel W, Fize D, Nelissen K, Peuskens H, Van Essen D, Orban GA. The processing of visual shape in the cerebral cortex of human and nonhuman primates: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2004;24:2551–2565. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3569-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Multiple visual areas in the caudal superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. J Comp Neurol. 1986;248:164–189. doi: 10.1002/cne.902480203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durand JB, Nelissen K, Joly O, Wardak C, Todd JT, Norman JF, Janssen P, Vanduffel W, Orban GA. Anterior regions of monkey parietal cortex process visual 3D shape. Neuron. 2007;55:493–505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel SA, Rumelhart DE, Wandell BA, Lee AT, Glover GH, Chichilnisky EJ, Shadlen MN. fMRI of human visual cortex. Nature. 1994;369:525. doi: 10.1038/369525a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel SA, Glover GH, Wandell BA. Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cereb Cortex. 1997;7:181–192. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.2.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1991;1:1–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage. 1999;9:195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fize D, Vanduffel W, Nelissen K, Denys K, Chef d’Hotel C, Faugeras O, Orban GA. The retinotopic organization of primate dorsal V4 and surrounding areas: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in awake monkeys. J Neurosci. 2003;23:7395–7406. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-19-07395.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gattass R, Gross CG. Visual topography of striate projection zone (MT) in posterior superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. J Neurophysiol. 1981;46:621–638. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.46.3.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gattass R, Sousa AP, Gross CG. Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaque. J Neurosci. 1988;8:1831–1845. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-06-01831.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gattass R, Nascimento-Silva S, Soares JG, Lima B, Jansen AK, Diogo AC, Farias MF, Botelho MM, Mariani OS, Azzi J, Fiorani M. Cortical visual areas in monkeys: location, topography, connections, columns, plasticity and cortical dynamics. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005;360:709–731. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gnadt JW, Andersen RA. Memory related motor planning activity in posterior parietal cortex of macaque. Exp Brain Res. 1988;70:216–220. doi: 10.1007/BF00271862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagler DJ, Jr, Sereno MI. Spatial maps in frontal and prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2006;29:567–577. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen KA, Kay KN, Gallant JL. Topographic organization in and near human visual area V4. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11896–11911. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2991-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heider B, Jando G, Siegel RM. Functional architecture of retinotopy in visual association cortex of behaving monkey. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:460–478. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horton JC, Hoyt WF. The representation of the visual field in human striate cortex. A revision of the classic Holmes map. Arch Ophthalmol. 1991;109:816–824. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1991.01080060080030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huk AC, Shadlen MN. Neural activity in macaque parietal cortex reflects temporal integration of visual motion signals during perceptual decision making. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10420–10436. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4684-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janssen P, Srivastava S, Ombelet S, Orban GA. Coding of shape and position in macaque lateral intraparietal area. J Neurosci. 2008;28:6679–6690. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0499-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jo HJ, Lee JM, Kim JH, Shin YW, Kim IY, Kwon JS, Kim SI. Spatial accuracy of fMRI activation influenced by volume- and surface-based spatial smoothing techniques. Neuroimage. 2007;34:550–564. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jo HJ, Lee JM, Kim JH, Choi CH, Gu BM, Kang DH, Ku J, Kwon JS, Kim SI. Artificial shifting of fMRI activation localized by volume- and surface-based analyses. Neuroimage. 2008;40:1077–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kagan I, Iyer A, Lindner A, Andersen RA. Space representation for eye movements is more contralateral in monkeys than in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:7933–7938. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002825107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kastner S, DeSimone K, Konen CS, Szczepanski SM, Weiner KS, Schneider KA. Topographic maps in human frontal cortex revealed in memory-guided saccade and spatial working-memory tasks. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:3494–3507. doi: 10.1152/jn.00010.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katsuyama N, Yamashita A, Sawada K, Naganuma T, Sakata H, Taira M. Functional and histological properties of caudal intraparietal area of macaque monkey. Neuroscience. 2010;167:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.01.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolster H, Mandeville JB, Arsenault JT, Ekstrom LB, Wald LL, Vanduffel W. Visual field map clusters in macaque extrastriate visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2009;29:7031–7039. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0518-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolster H, Peeters R, Orban G. The Retinotopic Organization of the Human Middle Temporal Area MT/V5 and Its Cortical Neighbors. J Neurosci. 2010;30:9801–9820. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2069-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konen CS, Kastner S. Representation of eye movements and stimulus motion in topographically organized areas of human posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:8361–8375. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1930-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsson J, Heeger DJ. Two retinotopic visual areas in human lateral occipital cortex. J Neurosci. 2006;26:13128–13142. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1657-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis JW, Van Essen DC. Mapping of architectonic subdivisions in the macaque monkey, with emphasis on parieto-occipital cortex. J Comp Neurol. 2000a;428:79–111. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001204)428:1<79::aid-cne7>3.0.co;2-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis JW, Van Essen DC. Corticocortical connections of visual, sensorimotor, and multimodal processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 2000b;428:112–137. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001204)428:1<112::aid-cne8>3.0.co;2-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire WM, Baizer JS. Visuotopic organization of the prelunate gyrus in rhesus monkey. J Neurosci. 1984;4:1690–1704. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-07-01690.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May JG, Andersen RA. Different patterns of corticopontine projections from separate cortical fields within the inferior parietal lobule and dorsal prelunate gyrus of the macaque. Exp Brain Res. 1986;63:265–278. doi: 10.1007/BF00236844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Medalla M, Barbas H. Diversity of laminar connections linking periarcuate and lateral intraparietal areas depends on cortical structure. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23:161–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04522.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pandya DN, Seltzer B. Intrinsic connections and architectonics of posterior parietal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1982;204:196–210. doi: 10.1002/cne.902040208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patel GH, Shulman GL, Baker JT, Akbudak E, Snyder AZ, Snyder LH, Corbetta M. Topographic organization of macaque area LIP. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:4728–4733. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908092107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinsk MA, Moore T, Richter MC, Gross CG, Kastner S. Methods for functional magnetic resonance imaging in normal and lesioned behaving monkeys. J Neurosci Methods. 2005;143:179–195. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinsk MA, Arcaro M, Konen CS, Li X, Kastner S, Inati SJ. Improved functional MRI of the macaque ventral visual pathway at 3 T using multi-echo EPI and dynamic, field map corrected image reconstruction. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2008;34:260. [Google Scholar]
- Pinsk MA, Arcaro M, Weiner KS, Kalkus JF, Inati SJ, Gross CG, Kastner S. Neural representations of faces and body parts in macaque and human cortex: a comparative FMRI study. J Neurophysiol. 2009;101:2581–2600. doi: 10.1152/jn.91198.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Platt ML, Glimcher PW. Response fields of intraparietal neurons quantified with multiple saccadic targets. Exp Brain Res. 1998;121:65–75. doi: 10.1007/s002210050438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Architectonics of the parietal and temporal association cortex in the strepsirhine primate Galago compared to the anthropoid primate Macaca. J Comp Neurol. 1991;310:475–506. doi: 10.1002/cne.903100403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saad Z, Reynolds R, Argall B, Japee S, Cox R. SUMA: an interface for surface-based intra- and inter-subject analysis with AFNI. Proceedings of the 2004 IEEE International Symposium on Biomedical Imaging: From Nano to Macro.2004. [Google Scholar]
- Sakata H, Taira M, Kusunoki M, Murata A, Tanaka Y, Tsutsui K. Neural coding of 3D features of objects for hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1998;353:1363–1373. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savaki HE, Gregoriou GG, Bakola S, Raos V, Moschovakis AK. The place code of saccade metrics in the lateral bank of the intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci. 30:1118–1127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2268-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluppeck D, Glimcher P, Heeger DJ. Topographic organization for delayed saccades in human posterior parietal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1372–1384. doi: 10.1152/jn.01290.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider KA, Richter MC, Kastner S. Retinotopic organization and functional subdivisions of the human lateral geniculate nucleus: a high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2004;24:8975–8985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2413-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seltzer B, Pandya DN. Posterior parietal projections to the intraparietal sulcus of the rhesus monkey. Exp Brain Res. 1986;62:459–469. doi: 10.1007/BF00236024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno MI, Huang RS. A human parietal face area contains aligned head-centered visual and tactile maps. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:1337–1343. doi: 10.1038/nn1777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB. Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science. 1995;268:889–893. doi: 10.1126/science.7754376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno MI, Pitzalis S, Martinez A. Mapping of contralateral space in retinotopic coordinates by a parietal cortical area in humans. Science. 2001;294:1350–1354. doi: 10.1126/science.1063695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:1916–1936. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.4.1916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver MA, Kastner S. Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:488–495. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver MA, Ress D, Heeger DJ. Topographic maps of visual spatial attention in human parietal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1358–1371. doi: 10.1152/jn.01316.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoet G, Snyder LH. Single neurons in posterior parietal cortex of monkeys encode cognitive set. Neuron. 2004;42:1003–1012. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swisher JD, Halko MA, Merabet LB, McMains SA, Somers DC. Visual topography of human intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci. 2007;27:5326–5337. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0991-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taira M, Tsutsui KI, Jiang M, Yara K, Sakata H. Parietal neurons represent surface orientation from the gradient of binocular disparity. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:3140–3146. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.5.3140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsao DY, Vanduffel W, Sasaki Y, Fize D, Knutsen TA, Mandeville JB, Wald LL, Dale AM, Rosen BR, Van Essen DC, Livingstone MS, Orban GA, Tootell RB. Stereopsis activates V3A and caudal intraparietal areas in macaques and humans. Neuron. 2003;39:555–568. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00459-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC. Surface-based atlases of cerebellar cortex in the human, macaque, and mouse. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002;978:468–479. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07588.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC. Organization of visual areas in macaque and human cerebral cortex. In: Chalupa L, Werner JS, editors. The Visual Neurosciences. MIT Press; 2004. pp. 507–521. [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Zeki SM. The topographic organization of rhesus monkey prestriate cortex. J Physiol. 1978;277:193–226. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Maunsell JH, Bixby JL. The middle temporal visual area in the macaque: myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization. J Comp Neurol. 1981;199:293–326. doi: 10.1002/cne.901990302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Newsome WT, Maunsell JH. The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability. Vision Res. 1984;24:429–448. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90041-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Lewis JW, Drury HA, Hadjikhani N, Tootell RB, Bakircioglu M, Miller MI. Mapping visual cortex in monkeys and humans using surface-based atlases. Vision Res. 2001;41:1359–1378. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00045-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA. Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron. 2007;56:366–383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeki SM. Representation of central visual fields in prestriate cortex of monkey. Brain Res. 1969;14:271–291. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(69)90110-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeki S. The representation of colours in the cerebral cortex. Nature. 1980;284:412–418. doi: 10.1038/284412a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.