Abstract
The dorsomedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (dmPFC, dlPFC) together support cognitive control, with dmPFC responsible for monitoring performance and dlPFC responsible for adjusting behavior. The dlPFC contains a topographic organization that reflects complexity of control demands, with more anterior regions guiding increasingly abstract processing. Recent evidence for a similar gradient within dmPFC suggests the possibility of parallel, hierarchical organization. Here, we measured connectivity between functional nodes of dmPFC and dlPFCusing resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans. We found a posterior-to-anterior connectivity gradient: posterior dmPFC maximally connected to posterior dlPFC while anterior dmPFC maximally connected to anterior dlPFC. This parallel topographic pattern replicated across three independent datasets collected on different scanners, within individual participants, and through both point-to-point and voxelwise analyses. We posit a model of cognitive control characterized by hierarchical interactions – whose level depends on current environmental demands – between functional subdivisions of medial and lateral PFC.
Introduction
A hallmark of the cerebral cortex is its topographic organization. Topographieshave been long-recognized within sensory and motor systems, whose spatial and somatotopic maps were elucidated in early animal and subsequent human research (Udin and Fawcett, 1988; Reep et al., 1996; Swindale, 1996; Kaas, 1997; Schneider et al., 2004; Silver and Kastner, 2009). Other cortical regions, however, have less obvious large-scale structure. While early conceptions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) were shaped by the lack of clear functional deficits associated with specific lesions (Ferrier, 1886; Lashley, 1929, 1950; Tizard, 1959), recent theoretical models have argued that all of PFC conjointly supports the adaptive control of behavior (Fuster et al., 2000; Miller and Cohen, 2001).
Within the lastdecade, functional neuroimaging has generated models of dlPFC that contain a topographic organization (Christoff and Gabrieli, 2000; Koechlin et al., 2000; Koechlin et al., 2003; Christoff and Keramatian, 2007; Koechlin and Summerfield, 2007; Badre et al., 2009). Although the specific mapping of regions to functions differs across models, all share some familial properties: posterior dlPFC controls relatively simple mappings of stimuli to actions, anterior dlPFC shapes complex conditional relationships among behavioral rules, and information flows between regions in a largely hierarchical fashion from anterior to posterior regions.
Considerably less is known about the organization of dorsomedialPFC (dmPFC). Itcontributes to a welter of cognitive processes, including monitoring performance, selecting actions based on goals, anticipating rewards, and signaling errors and adverse outcomes (Bush et al., 2000; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004a; Ridderinkhof et al., 2004b). One popular view is that this functional diversity parallels apparent structural heterogeneity, with the above processes considered to be intercalated throughout this region (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004a). Yet, recent evidence from neuroimaging studies suggests that dmPFC has functional, and potentially topographic, subdivisions. Direct comparison of three distinct types of cognitive control demands – response-related, decision-related, and strategy-related – revealed a posterior-to-anterior gradient reflecting the transition from simple to higher-order rules for control (Venkatraman et al., 2009a). Moreover, recent structural analyses indicate that subdivisions of cingulate cortex have distinct white-matter connectivity profiles (Beckmann et al., 2009), particularly with respect to the lateral PFC.
We hypothesized that the dlPFC and dmPFC share a common topographic pattern of functional connectivity, consistent both with the known anatomical connections between these regions (Petrides and Pandya, 1999; Petrides, 2005) and their putative joint contributions to cognitive control (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004b; Egner, 2009; Kouneiher et al., 2009). Here, we mapped functional connectivity within prefrontal cortex using resting-state fMRI (Fransson, 2005; Damoiseaux et al., 2006; De Luca et al., 2006; Margulies et al., 2007), drawing data from a large primary dataset and from two independent replications collected on different scanners. Our results demonstrated a clear posterior-to-anterior gradient in connectivity: posterior dmPFC regions were maximally connected to posterior dlPFC regions, whereas anterior dmPFC regions were maximally connected to anterior dlPFC regions. This parallel topography supports an integrative and hierarchical view of PFC, such that its medial and lateral aspects jointly contribute to the adaptive control of behavior.
Methods
Participants and Data Collection: Primary Dataset
Sixty-four young adults (32 female, mean age = 24, range 18-43) participated in the primary experiment. All participants received monetary compensation for their participation in the study. Twelve participants were excluded before dataanalysis due to excessive motion (>2 mm), leaving a total of 52 participants(26 female) in the final analyses. All participants gavewritten informed consent as part of protocols approved by theInstitutional Review Board of Duke UniversityMedical Center.
We report data from a 6-minute resting-state scan, which was obtained at the end of a 90-minute experimental session. During this scan, participants were instructed to keep their eyes open, to focus on the fixation cross that was presented in the center of the screen, to remain alert, and to refrain from directing their thoughts toward anything specific.
Data were acquired on a 4T GE scanner using an inverse-spiral pulse sequence (Guo and Song, 2003) with the following parameters: TR = 2000 ms; TE = 27 ms; 34 axial slices parallel to the AC-PC plane, with voxel size of 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.8 mm. High-resolution 3D full-brain SPGR anatomical images were acquired and used for normalizing individual participants' data.
Participants and Data Collection: Replication Datasets
We replicated all analyses in resting-state data from two additional datasets collected on different scanners, with different pulse sequences, and with different groups of participants. Twenty-two young adults (11 female, mean age = 23, range 19-32) participated in Replication 1. Fifteen young adults (5 female, mean age = 22, range 20-24) participated in Replication 2. All participants gave written informed consent as part of protocols approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Duke University Medical Center or of the Duke-NUS Graduate Medical School, Singapore.
Replication 1 data were acquired on a 3T GE scanner using a SENSE spiral pulse sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2000 ms; TE = 27 ms; 34 axial slices parallel to the AC-PC plane, with voxel size of 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.8 mm. High-resolution 3D full-brain SPGR anatomical images were acquired and used for normalizing individual subjects' data. Replication 2 data were acquired on a 3T Siemens scanner using an inverse spiral pulse sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2000 ms; TE = 30 ms; 34 axial slices parallel to the AC-PC plane, with voxel size of 3.75 × 3.75 × 3.8 mm. High-resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE anatomical images were acquired and used for normalizing individual subjects' data.
Data Analyses
We used FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool) Version 5.92, part of FSL (FMRIB's Software Library; www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) package (Smith et al., 2004). Preprocessing consisted of 1) motion correction using MCFLIRT (Motion Correction using fMRIB's Linear Registration Tool), 2) slice timing correction, 3) removal of non-brain voxels using BET (Brain Extraction Tool), 4) spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel with full-width at half maximum 6mm, and 5) high pass temporal filtering (cutoff = 100s). Normalization to MNI standard space was carried out using FLIRT.
Functional Connectivity: Selection of Seed Regions
We identified 5 seed regions in the dmPFC, hereafter described using the nomenclature DM1 through DM5 (i.e., posterior to anterior). The peak coordinates of these seeds were taken from previous work indicating distinct foci for response-, decision-, and strategy-related control (Venkatraman et al., 2009a), which became DM1, DM3, and DM5, respectively. The remaining two seeds, DM2 and DM4, were defined by linear interpolation between the above pairs of neighboring coordinates (Table 1).
TABLE 1. MNI coordinates for the dmPFC and dlPFC seed regions.
MNI Coordinates | Functional Contrast | Source | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | |||
DM1 | -4 | 10 | 50 | Response conflict | (Venkatraman et al., 2009a) |
DM2 | -4 | 16 | 45 | - | Interpolated (DM1 and DM3) |
DM3 | -6 | 23 | 39 | Decision conflict | (Venkatraman et al., 2009a) |
DM4 | -4 | 30 | 37 | - | Interpolated (DM3 and DM5) |
DM5 | -6 | 35 | 34 | Strategy conflict | (Venkatraman et al., 2009a) |
DL1 | -32 | -11 | 60 | Stimulus effect | (Koechlin et al., 2003) |
DL2 | -51 | -2 | 39 | - | Interpolated (DL1 and DL3) |
DL3 | -44 | 7 | 22 | Context effect | (Koechlin et al., 2003) |
DL4 | -46 | 22 | 14 | - | Interpolated (DL3 and DL5) |
DL5 | -42 | 45 | 7 | Episode effect | (Koechlin et al., 2003) |
DL6 | -30 | 56 | -2 | Branching effect | (Koechlin et al., 1999) |
We also identified 6 seed regions in the dlPFC, hereafter labeled as DL1 through DL6 (i.e., posterior to anterior). We identified three of the seed regions' peak coordinates based on prior work delineating a dlPFC topography based on stimulus, context and integrative demands (Koechlin et al., 2003); these became seeds DL1, DL3, and DL5 (Table 1). The intermediate seeds DL2 and DL4 were defined by linear interpolation between neighboring coordinates and coordinates for DL6 were determined based on prior work associating activation in the anterior prefrontal cortex with cognitive branching (Koechlin et al., 1999). Each seed consisted of 33 2mm × 2mm × 2mm voxels (defined using a sphere of radius 2 voxels around the peak coordinate described in Table 1). Time-series signal for all subsequent analyses was obtained by averaging dataacross all voxels within each seed.
Functional Connectivity: Data Analysis
To map the pattern of functional connectivity between medial and lateral PFC, we began with a dmPFC mask that included the anterior cingulate gyrus and the medial superior frontal gyrus. Following normalization of each participant's data to the standard MNI space, we extracted the time-series from each dmPFC voxel and measured the functional connectivity with each of the six dlPFC seed regions. This provided a voxel-by-voxel map, for each participant, of the correlation between each point in dmPFC with each of the seed regions in the dlPFC. The corresponding analysis was also performed to obtain a voxel-by-voxel map of the correlation between each point in dlPFC with each of the seed regions in the dmPFC. We used a dlPFC mask, extracted the time-series from each dlPFC voxel, and measured the functional connectivity with each of the five dmPFC seed regions.
The above analysis provides a visual representation of topography in dlPFC based on the seeds in dmPFC, and vice versa. To next evaluate the parallel connectivity between hierarchical regions in the dmPFC and dlPFC, we extracted the preprocessed mean time-series from each of the five dmPFC seed regions and six dlPFC seed regions defined above for every participant. We demeaned all dmPFC and dlPFC time series by subtracting the mean whole-brain time series (extracted from a whole-brain mask for every participant). To obtain a measure of point-to-point connectivity, we computed Pearson correlations between the five dmPFC time series and each of the six dlPFC time series, which were then combined across all participants (significance tests: Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient, p < 0.05, two-tailed). All analyses were repeated independently for the left and right hemispheres.
Functional Connectivity: Specificity Analysis
For each dmPFC seed, we calculated the standard deviation of the correlation values to each of the six dlPFC seeds. A lower standard deviation would indicate broad, similar connectivity of voxels within that seed to different regions in the dlPFC, while a higher standard deviation would indicate greater specificity such that connectivity strength varies across seeds. To statistically test the presence of an anterior-posterior gradient in specificity, we ran an ANOVA of the standard deviation values for each of the five different dmPFC seeds across subjects, followed by post hoc tests of specific seed pairs.
Regression Analyses
Each dlPFC/dmPFC correlation for each participant was entered as an observation (i.e., 30 pairwise correlations per participant) into an OLS regression analysis (JMP Version 7; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Each dlPFC/dmPFC correlation was served as a dependent variable, and explanatory variables included dmPFC region (normalized posterior to anterior as -1 to 1), dlPFC region (-1 to 1), a dmPFC*dlPFC interaction term, and a categorical variable coding for the data set from which each observation originated (primary, Replication 1, or Replication 2). Using spatial locations as predictors allowed us to answer the question of whether interaction between spatial locations predicts dmPFC-dlPFC correlation (i.e. our measure of connectivity). This analysis was run using the aggregate observations from all three data sets, and was replicated using only the primary data set.
Results
We measured the unique, non-directional functional connectivity between locations within dmPFCand dlPFC using two independent methods, each controlling for the common fMRI signal covariationbetween the regions as a whole. First, we examined the connectivity between each of the six dlPFC seed regions and every voxel in dmPFC, defined anatomically. The results demonstrate a posterior-to-anterior gradient in connectivity, with posterior dlPFC having maximal connectivity to posterior dmPFC and anterior dlPFC having maximal connectivity to anterior dmPFC (Fig. 2). The corresponding analysis of connectivity between each of the five dmPFC seed regions and every voxel in dlPFC produces a similar gradient. Second, we examined point-to-point connectivity between the pre-selected seed regions in dlPFC and dmPFC. We found a similar topographic pattern when focusing on just the connectivity between seed regions, independently for both the left (Fig. 3a, Table 2) and right hemispheres (Fig. 3b, Table 2). This topography was replicated in two additional data sets collected on different scanners, with different participants, and using different imaging parameters.
TABLE 2. Pairwise correlations between each dmPFC and dlPFC seed regions, for the left and right hemispheres.
Primary Dataset | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Left Hemisphere | Right Hemisphere | |||||||||
DM1 | DM2 | DM3 | DM4 | DM5 | DM1 | DM2 | DM3 | DM4 | DM5 | |
DL1 | 0.45 | 0.24 | 0.19 | 0.10 | 0.08 | 0.26 | 0.19 | 0.11 | 0.04 | -0.00 |
DL2 | 0.34 | 0.31 | 0.26 | 0.15 | 0.13 | 0.47 | 0.39 | 0.20 | 0.18 | 0.12 |
DL3 | 0.24 | 0.37 | 0.32 | 0.25 | 0.21 | 0.30 | 0.29 | 0.15 | 0.18 | 0.15 |
DL4 | 0.11 | 0.25 | 0.21 | 0.24 | 0.25 | 0.17 | 0.12 | 0.14 | 0.17 | 0.16 |
DL5 | 0.04 | 0.26 | 0.30 | 0.28 | 0.24 | 0.17 | 0.21 | 0.20 | 0.28 | 0.28 |
DL6 | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.07 | 0.16 | 0.17 | -0.04 | -0.01 | 0.12 | 0.17 | 0.22 |
We next evaluated the spatial pattern of functional connectivity using a regression analysis. Point-to-point connectivity values, for all pairs of dmPFC and dlPFC regions and for all subjects, were included as dependent variables in the model. Predictor variables were dmPFCspatial location (i.e., 5 levels), dlPFCspatial location (i.e., 6 levels), and their interaction. Note that this analysis controls for any overall effects of increased connectivity in either posterior or anterior regions; of primary interest would be a significant interaction, such that connectivity is maximal between regions matched spatially. We found that thedmPFC*dlPFC interaction term was highly significant in both the left and right hemispheres (all ps < 10-10; Table 3). This effect held both for the primary data set (when considered alone) and for the combination of all three data sets, when including data set as an additional categorical predictor variable (all ps < 10-10 in similar analyses to those of Table 3).
TABLE 3.
Left Hemisphere | Right Hemisphere | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
β | p | β | P | |
Intercept | 0.21 | <10-20 | 0.18 | <10-20 |
dmPFC | -0.02 | 0.01 | -0.04 | <10-7 |
dlPFC | -0.02 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.04 |
dmPFC*dlPFC | 0.18 | <10-20 | 0.19 | <10-20 |
Finally, we evaluated whether these findings held for individual participants. We determined, for each participant and each dmPFC seed, the seed in dlPFC to which there was maximal functional connectivity. This provides a conservative measure of point-to-point connectivity, in that it collapses the entire distribution of connectivity values to a single maximum. As hypothesized,these maxima clustered along the diagonal, consistent with a parallel topography at the individual level in both hemispheres (Fig. 4). A similar pattern was seen in our two replications.
We additionally observed that the specificity of the functional connectivity decreased when moving along the posterior-to-anterior gradient (cf. Figure 4) within the dmPFC. We computed the standard deviation of connectivity values from each dmPFC seed to the set of the six dlPFC seeds. There was a significant decrease from posterior to anterior dmPFC seeds (F(4,255) = 2.77, p = 0.02). Voxels in posterior dmPFC exhibited greater specificity (mean = 0.23) in connectivity to dlPFC seeds than did voxels in anterior dmPFC (mean = 0.18, t(51) = 3.20, p = 0.002).
Discussion
Several lines of empirical evidence have suggested a hierarchical rostral-to-caudal organization within the dlPFC, with neurons in the more rostral regions processing more abstract goals and neurons in the more posterior regions processing more concrete information related to corresponding motor actions (Fuster et al., 2000; Fuster, 2001, 2004; Koechlin and Summerfield, 2007). More recent evidence also suggests a similar anterior-to-posterior topography within the dmPFC, with the more anterior regions being associated with more complex and abstract levels of cognitive control (Venkatraman et al., 2009b; Venkatraman et al., 2009a). Given the strong anatomical evidence for reciprocal connections between these regions based on studies in non-human primates (Bates and Goldman-Rakic, 1993; Petrides and Pandya, 1999), a natural hypothesis is that monitoring-related activity in dmPFC engages regulatory processes in the dlPFCthat shape behavior (Botvinick et al., 2001; Kerns et al., 2004; Botvinick, 2008). Yet, despite the striking similarity in their hierarchical organization, it has been unclear whether a similar gradient also exists in the functional connectivity between these regions.
In one recent study, Kouneiher and colleagues demonstrate that the medial prefrontal cortex regulates processing resources in the lateral PFC according to motivational incentives. Specifically, there is increased effective connectivity between posterior dmPFC (pre-SMA) and posterior dlPFCfor contextual incentives and increased connectivity between middle dmPFC and mid-dlPFC for episodic incentives. However, there was no increased connectivity during control conditions that lacked such incentives. Therefore, the authors argue for a posterior-to-anterior hierarchical system of executive processes in the medial and lateral PFC with the functional interactions between them conveying motivational incentives rather than simple control demands (Kouneiher et al., 2009).
In the current study, we used spontaneous fluctuations in BOLD activity to characterize further the functional connectivity between the medial and lateral prefrontal regions in the absence of task-associated cognitive processes. This task-free approach using resting-state data relies on correlations among low-frequency BOLD changes to identify regions that function in tandem. Using such an approach, we demonstrated a posterior-to-anterior gradient in connectivity between the medial and lateral prefrontal regions, with posterior dlPFC having maximal connectivity to posterior dmPFC and anterior dlPFC to anterior dmPFC. This pattern replicated in three independent datasets collected using three different MR scanners and was evident even in individual participants.
This pattern of resting-state functional connectivity is consistent with prior mappings of structural connectivity between these regions in both non-human primates (Petrides and Pandya, 1999) and humans (Beckmann et al., 2009). Petrides and Pandya used florescent tracers placed in selected areas of the dorsolateral frontal cortex of monkeys to study their connectivity to the rest of the brain (Petrides and Pandya, 1999). They found that injections placed in posterior dlPFC(similar to DL1 in our study) revealed labeled neurons in posterior dmPFC regions of the SMA, pre-SMA, and cingulate sulcus (similar to DM1 in our study). On the other hand, injections placed in mid-dlPFC(corresponding roughly to DL3 seed in our study) revealed labeled cells in a more anterior part of cingulate cortex (here, roughly DM3). Similar connection patterns in the medial regions were also observed for injections placed in rostrolateraldlPFC(corresponding to DL6 seed in our study). These findings suggest strong intrinsic structural connectivity between lateral and medial prefrontal regions in the macaque cortex.
In another study involving magnetic resonance tractography-based parcellation of the human cingulate cortex (Beckmann et al., 2009), the dorsolateral prefrontal regions (consistent with seeds DL3 and DL4 in our study) showed greatest connectivity with anterior cingulate sulcus (similar to DM4 in our study), while the premotor regions (overlapping with our seeds DL1 and DL2) showed greatest connectivity with the pre-SMA and more posterior regions within the cingulate (overlapping with seeds DM1 and DM2). Therefore, these studies provide independent evidence across different modalities about the robustness of the functional connections between these regions. Though these methods are largely agnostic about the directionality of these interactions, the findings of Kouneiher and colleagues suggests that the medial frontal regions may regulate the cognitive control resources in the lateral regions according to motivational incentives (Kouneiher et al., 2009). Therefore, we speculate that dmPFC exerts a regulatory or modulatory influence on dlPFC (Wood and Grafman, 2003; Egner, 2009; Kouneiher et al., 2009).
We contend that different regions of dmPFC and dlPFC, according to the computational demands of the task, interact to guide the adaptive control of behavior (Fig. 5). The proposed model extends the hierarchical organization postulated for dlPFC to include a parallel organization in dmPFC, whose subregions shape processing in their lateral counterparts. That is, the anterior dmPFC regulates activity in the anterior dlPFC when the control demands are associated with high levels of abstraction (e.g., implementing strategic planning in a decision-making task), while the posterior dmPFC works in concert with posterior dlPFC and premotor cortices when the control demands are limited to choosing between two competing responses. While we here emphasize potential medial-to-lateral effects, bidirectional influences are likely: dmPFC activity may be biased by dlPFC inputs, consistent with the former's role in monitoring reward-value of action sets according to outcomes (Daw et al., 2006; Kennerley et al., 2006). Moreover, whether these regions interact in a hierarchical fashion remains a provocative question. Anterior regions of dlPFCinfluence processing in posterior regions of dlPFCin a task-dependent manner (Koechlin et al., 2003; Kouneiher et al., 2009), and damage to more anterior portions of prefrontal cortex leads to deficits on a wider array of tasks, including abstract processing (Badre et al., 2009). Our finding that the more anterior regions in the dmPFC demonstrate more diffuse projections to the lateral PFC, relative to the posterior regions, provides converging evidence toward a hierarchical perspective.
The new framework proposed here provides empirical evidence for a longstanding conjecture: that the PFC contributes to the temporal integration of behavior by simultaneously maintaining context and goal-related information at several levels (Fuster, 2001, 2004). Fuster argued that automatic behaviors only require integration at lower levels – sensory areas in the posterior perceptual hierarchy and motor areas in the frontal executive hierarchy – while more complex behaviors require integration at higher levels of perceptual and executive hierarchies. This influential conjecture has recently gained significant supporting evidence in the lateral PFC (Koechlin and Summerfield, 2007; Badre and D'Esposito, 2009). We show that this topography extends to the functional interactions between the medial and lateral PFC. Such a functional gradient in connectivity could reflect a dynamic mechanism for identifying and responding adaptively to contextual changes in behavior.
Acknowledgments
We thank McKell Carter, John Clithero, and Justin Meyer for assistance with data collection and analysis, and for their manuscript comments. We thank Michael Chee for contribution of data for replication analyses. This project was supported by NINDS P01-41328 and by an Incubator Award from the Duke Institute for Brain Sciences.
Footnotes
Author Contributions All authors contributed to the design of the experiment. A.A.T. and V.V. performed the data analyses, under the supervision of S.A.H. All authors wrote the paper and approved the final version for publication.
We declare no conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise.
References
- Badre D. Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2008;12:193–200. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badre D, D'Esposito M. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19:2082–2099. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badre D, D'Esposito M. Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical. Nature reviews. 2009;10:659–669. doi: 10.1038/nrn2667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badre D, Hoffman J, Cooney JW, D'Esposito M. Hierarchical cognitive control deficits following damage to the human frontal lobe. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:515–522. doi: 10.1038/nn.2277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbalat G, Chambon V, Franck N, K E, Farrer C. Organization of cognitive control within the lateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:377–386. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates JF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Prefrontal connections of medial motor areas in the rhesus monkey. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1993;336:211–228. doi: 10.1002/cne.903360205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000;123:2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann M, Johansen-Berg H, Rushworth MFS. Connectivity-based parcelation of human cingulate cortex and its relation to functional specialization. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:1175–1190. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3328-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM. Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function. Trends in Cognitive Science! 2008;2:201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev. 2001;108:624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2000;4:215–222. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christoff K, Gabrieli JDE. The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology. 2000;28:168–186. [Google Scholar]
- Christoff K, Keramatian K. Abstraction of mental representations: Theoretical considerations and neuroscientific evidence 2007 [Google Scholar]
- Damasio H, Grabowski T, Frank R, Galaburda AM, Damasio AR. The return of Phineas Gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science. 1994;264:1102–1105. doi: 10.1126/science.8178168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, Beckmann CF. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2006;103:13848–13853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, O'Doherty JP, Dayan P, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature. 2006;441:876–879. doi: 10.1038/nature04766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Luca M, Beckmann CF, De Stefano N, Matthews PM, Smith SM. fMRI resting state networks define distinct modes of long-distance interactions in the human brain. NeuroImage. 2006;29:1359–1367. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egner T. Prefrontal cortex and cognitive control: motivating functional hierarchies. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:821–822. doi: 10.1038/nn0709-821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrier D. The functions of the brain. 2nd. London: Smith Elder; 1886. [Google Scholar]
- Fransson P. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctations: An fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;26:15–29. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex - An update: Time is of the essence. Neuron. 2001;30:319–333. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00285-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM. Upper processing stages of the perception-action cycle. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2004;8:143–145. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM, Bodner M, Kroger JK. Cross-model and cross-temporal association in neurons of frontal cortex. Nature. 2000;405:347–351. doi: 10.1038/35012613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo H, Song AW. Spiral-in-and-out functional image acquisition with embedded z-shimming for susceptibility signal recovery. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 2003;18:389–395. doi: 10.1002/jmri.10355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaas JH. Topographic Maps are Fundamental to Sensory Processing. Brain Research Bulletin. 1997;44:107–112. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(97)00094-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennerley SW, Walton ME, Behrens TE, Buckley MJ, Rushworth MF. Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:940–947. doi: 10.1038/nn1724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, III, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303:1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koechlin E, Summerfield C. An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koechlin E, Ody C, Kouneiher F. The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science. 2003;302 doi: 10.1126/science.1088545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koechlin E, Corrado G, Pietrini P, Grafman J. Dissociating the role of the medial and lateral anterior prefrontal cortex in human planning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2000;97:7651–7656. doi: 10.1073/pnas.130177397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koechlin E, Basso G, Pietrini P, Panzer S, Grafman J. The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature. 1999;399:148–151. doi: 10.1038/20178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kouneiher F, Charron S, Koechlin E. Motivation and cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:939–947. doi: 10.1038/nn.2321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lashley KS. Brain mechanisms and intelligence: A quantitative study of injuries to the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago press; 1929. [Google Scholar]
- Lashley KS. In search of engram. Society of experimental biology. 1950:454–482. [Google Scholar]
- Margulies DS, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex. NeuroImage. 2007;37:579–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2005;360:781–795. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M, Pandya DN. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;11:1011–1036. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reep RL, Corwin JV, King V. Neuronal connections of orbital cortex in rats: topography of cortical and thalamic afferents. Experimental Brain Research. 1996;111:215–232. doi: 10.1007/BF00227299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004a;306:443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof KR, van den Wildenberg WPM, Segalowitz SJ, Carter CS. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain and Cognition. 2004b;56:129–140. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider KA, Richter MC, Kastner S. Retinotopic organization and functional subdivisions of the human lateral geniculate nucleus: A high-resolution functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:8975–8985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2413-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver M, Kastner S. Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2009;13:488–495. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23 1:S208–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swindale NV. The development of topography in the visual cortex: a review of models. Network: Computation in Neural Systems. 1996;7:161–247. doi: 10.1088/0954-898X/7/2/002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tizard B. Theories of brain localization from Flourens to Lashley. Medical History. 1959;3:132–145. doi: 10.1017/s0025727300024418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Udin S, Fawcett J. Formation of Topographic Maps. Ann Rev Neurosci. 1988;11:289–327. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkatraman V, Rosati AG, T AA, Huettel SA. Resolving response, decision, and strategic control: Evidence for a functional topography in dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009a;29:13158–13164. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2708-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venkatraman V, Payne JW, Bettman JR, Luce MF, Huettel SA. Separate neural mechanisms underlie choices and strategic preferences in risky decision making. Neuron. 2009b;62:593–602. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood JN, Grafman J. Human prefrontal cortex: Processing and representational perspectives. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;4:139–147. doi: 10.1038/nrn1033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]