Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 May 14.
Published in final edited form as: J Neurosci. 2012 Nov 14;32(46):16095–16105. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1712-12.2012

In vivo functional and myeloarchitectonic mapping of human primary auditory areas

Frederic Dick 1,2, Adam Taylor Tierney 4, Antoine Lutti 5, Oliver Josephs 1,5,*, Martin I Sereno 1,2,3,*, Nikolaus Weiskopf 5
PMCID: PMC3531973  NIHMSID: NIHMS421754  PMID: 23152594

Abstract

In contrast to vision, where retinotopic mapping alone can define areal borders, primary auditory areas such as A1 are best delineated by combining in vivo tonotopic mapping with post mortem cyto- or myelo-architectonics from the same individual. We combined high-resolution (800 μm) quantitative T1 mapping with phase-encoded tonotopic methods to map primary auditory areas (A1 and R) within the ‘auditory core’ of human volunteers. We first quantitatively characterize the highly myelinated auditory core in terms of shape, area, cortical depth profile, and position, with our data showing considerable correspondence to post-mortem myeloarchitectonic studies, both in cross-participant averages and in individuals. The core region contains two ‘mirror-image‘ tonotopic maps oriented along the same axis as observed in macaque and owl monkey. We suggest that thee two maps within the core are the human analogues of primate auditory areas A1 and R. The core occupies a much smaller portion of tonotopically organized cortex on the superior temporal plane and gyrus than is generally supposed. The multi-modal approach to defining the auditory core will facilitate investigations of structure-function relationships, comparative neuroanatomical studies, and promises new biomarkers for diagnosis and clinical studies.


The parcellation of cortex into distinct areas is a long-standing program in neuroscience (Zilles and Amunts, 2010), but one more advanced in vision than in audition. While some early visual areas can be defined solely on the basis of architectonics (e.g., stria in V1) or non-invasive retinotopy (Engel et al., 1994), the borders of primary auditory cortex (A1) cannot easily be defined by anatomy or function alone. Areas A1 and R are two major fields of the ‘auditory core’ (Hackett, 2007), a narrow, keyhole-shaped region on the temporal plane. In ex vivo preparations, auditory core shows heavy, layer IIIb/IV-specific staining for cell bodies, myelin, acetylcholinesterase, cytochrome oxidase, and parvalbumin (Hackett, 2011). Although greater myelination in caudal core may correspond to A1 (Hackett et al., 2001), the boundary with R is difficult to define using myeloarchitectonic criteria (Morel et al., 1993).

A1 and R are challenging to delineate using tonotopic mapping since tonotopy provides only one spatial axis, with no agreed-upon means to define borders perpendicular to isofrequency bands, which span multiple auditory areas (Hackett, 2011). In a small number of invasive experiments in animals, physiological measures (tonotopy, response properties) have been combined with ex-vivo histological mapping to define borders (Merzenich and Brugge, 1973; Imig et al., 1977; Pfingst and O’Connor, 1981; Morel and Kaas, 1992; Morel et al., 1993).

Studies of anesthetized macaques show best frequency (BF) progressions across and around auditory core. As seen in Figure 1 (most complete map from Morel et al., 1993, redrawn and contoured), A1 - in the highly myelinated caudomedial core - is characterized by a high-to-low BF progression, from a medial high BF corona descending to a rostrolateral low BF trough, which marks the A1/R border. In the narrower and more lightly myelinated area R in the lateral aspect of core, there is a gentler low-to-mid BF progression moving rostrolaterally.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Log-frequency isocontours within and around myelo- and cytoarchitectonically-defined auditory core (thick black lines), reconstructed from electrophysiological recording data reported in Figure 2A of Morel et al., 1993. (See Methods for details on contour rendering). Thin dotted lines show shape of underlying coronal sections of exposed temporal plane and superior temporal gyrus; thick dashed line is estimate of A1/R border. Figure 2A from Morel et al. was chosen for having the most extensive set of recording data over A1 and R, and for being representative of other datasets in Morel et al. and in other combined physiology/cytoarchitectonic experiments.

Tonotopic progressions in macaque can generally be described as follows: wrapping around the caudal cap of the core is a corona of high best-frequency (BF) neurons, with highest BF neurons near or slightly outside the core proper. This high-BF region tends to extend more than halfway along the medial edge of the core (moving rostrally), with BFs then dropping to mid- or low frequencies. Continuing along the medial edge toward the rostralmost tip of core, there is often a moderate increase from low to medium BFs. Progressing posteromedially to anterolaterally across core, there is a steep drop from higher to lower BFs ending in a low BF rostrolateral trough. Along the lateral edge of core, moving posterior to anterior, there is a steeper descent in BF that joins the low-BF trough. Finally, in some cases there is an increase from low-to-medium/high BFs moving out from the anterolateral edge of the less densely myelinated aspect of core (for an almost complete low-to-mid BF gradient in R, see Figure 2B of Morel et al., 1993).

There are multiple tonotopic maps across the temporal plane, as shown with fMRI in humans (Da Costa et al., 2011; Formisano et al., 2003; Talavage et al., 2004) and macaques (Petkov et al., 2006; Baumann et al., 2010). Whereas the A1/R boundary can be localized using tonotopic gradients (Formisano et al., 2003), the relation between frequency progressions and the position of core is disputed (Humphries et al., 2010). Current probabilistic post-mortem cytoarchitectonic maps of primary auditory areas (Morosan et al., 2001) are too blurry to localize core boundaries.

Anatomical MRI results (Walters et al., 2003; Bridge & Clare, 2006; Sigalovsky et al., 2006; Trampel et al., 2011; Glasser and Van Essen, 2011; Sereno et al., 2012) suggest that areal boundaries can be defined by measuring proxies for myelination. Here, we characterize the strongly myelinated auditory core using a high-resolution bias-free quantitative T1 mapping protocol which provides estimates of myelination directly comparable across cortical regions or subjects. We combine these myelin maps with detailed tonotopic mapping in the same participants to characterize frequency progressions within and around auditory core.

Materials and Methods

Participants

6 adults (ages 22–55, 3 female) with normal hearing and vision participated in all parts of the study. 3 additional adults (ages 22–30, all male) participated only in the functional imaging part of the study. Experimental protocols were approved by local ethics committees, and all participants gave informed and signed written consent.

Structural imaging

Structural images were acquired on a whole-body Tim Trio system (2.89T, Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany), with radio-frequency (RF) body transmit and 32-channel receive head coil at the Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging. 3T scanners such as the Trio will not have as high signal-to-noise as higher field strength (e..g, 7T), but do benefit from greater uniformity of signal (less B1 field inhomogeneities), smaller susceptibility artifacts (B0 field inhomogeneities), lower acoustic noise, and less dropout in parts of the temporal lobe due primarily to B1 field inhomogeneities. As part of the quantitative R1 mapping protocol, proton density-weighted (PDw) and T1-weighted (T1w) images were acquired using an in-house multi-echo 3D FLASH pulse sequence (Weiskopf et al., 2011): voxel size: 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.81 mm3, FOV = 256 × 216 × 194 mm, matrix = 320 × 270 × 240, TR = 23.7 ms, excitation flip angle: 6° (PDw) or 28° (T1w). Acquisition was speeded up by 2× GRAPPA parallel imaging in the phase encoding and 6/8 Partial Fourier in the partition direction. To improve image quality (maximize SNR and minimize geometric distortion at the same time), 4 gradient echoes were acquired (TE=2.20, 4.75, 7.30, 9.85 ms) with high readout bandwidth after each excitation pulse. Each session consisted of four 10 min 31 sec acquisitions (two PDw and two T1w) and two shorter scans to estimate B0 and B1 inhomogeneities (see below). Quantitative R1 (=1/T1) maps were estimated from the PDw and T1w images according to the model developed by Helms et al., (2008a) including a correction for RF transmit field inhomogeneities (Lutti et al., 2010) and imperfect spoiling (Preibisch and Deichmann, 2009). Recent applications of this method have demonstrated its robustness and accuracy (Helms et al., 2008b; 2009; Weiskopf et al., 2011; Draganski et al., 2011). To correct for the effect of RF transmit inhomogeneities on R1 maps, maps of the transmit field B1+ were acquired using a 3D EPI spin-echo (SE)/stimulated echo (STE) method (Lutti et al., 2012; FOV = 256 × 192 × 192 mm3, matrix = 64 × 48 × 48, TESE/TESTE = 39.38/72.62 ms, TR = 500 ms, α varying from 270° to 130° by steps of 10°, acquisition time 3 min 48 s), which was corrected for off-resonance effects using a standard B0 fieldmap (double gradient echo FLASH, 3 mm isotropic resolution, whole-brain coverage (Lutti et al., 2010)).

Functional imaging

Functional images were acquired on a 1.5T whole-body Tim Avanto System (Siemens Healthcare), at the Birkbeck/UCL Centre for NeuroImaging, with RF body transmit and a 32-channel receive head coil. (Supplementary data for three participants was also acquired using a specialized single-loop coil positioned over the superior temporal gyrus). 1.5T has evolved dramatically over the last years with much more stable hardware and 32-channel head coils leading to 200–300% improvement in BOLD sensitivity in auditory regions (Wiggins et al., 2006); echo-planar imaging at this field strength also has less warping and distortion in auditory regions than higher-field magnets. Echo-planar images (EPI) were acquired with the following parameters: 24 slices, voxel resolution 3.2 × 3.2 × 3.2 mm (matrix size: 64 × 64), flip angle = 90°, bandwidth = 1474 Hz/pixel, TR = 2000ms, TE = 39ms, data acquired with prospective motion correction (Thesen et al., 2000). The voxel resolution is similar to that (~3×3×3mm) used in many recent retinotopy studies of small areas, such as V6, VIP, FEF, IPS1&2, V8, and LOC, e.g. Sereno & Huang (2006); Hagler et al., (2007); Larsson & Heeger (2006), Rajimehr & Tootell (2009); Konen et al., (2011). Individual scans had 262 volumes; to allow longitudinal relaxation to reach equilibrium, 6 initial volumes were discarded from each run (initial 2 not saved by scanner). For each imaging session, a short (3 min) T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE (88 partitions, voxel resolution 1 × 1 × 2 mm3, flip angle = 7°, TE = 4 ms, TI = 1000 ms, TR = 8.2 ms, mSENSE acceleration = 2×, slab-selective excitation) was acquired with the same orientation and slice block center as the functional data for initial alignment with the high-resolution scans used to reconstruct the subject’s cortical surface. For the 3 additional participants who did not take part in the quantitative imaging, we acquired a single high-resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE for cortical surface reconstruction (1×1×1mm, 176 slices, TR=2730ms, TE=3.57ms, flip angle = 7°).

Auditory stimuli

Both retinotopic (Saygin and Sereno, 2008) and tonotopic (Woods et al., 2009) mapping studies have suggested that targeted attentional demands and increased stimulus complexity can significantly modulate fMRI activation in cortical maps. Thus, we used bandpass-swept complex and engaging non-linguistic vocalizations to map tonotopic regions. Base stimuli were adapted from the Montreal Affective Voices (Belin et al., 2008), a series of recordings of emotional non-verbal vocalizations elicited by actors producing sounds corresponding to a set of eight emotions (a ninth “neutral” emotion was not used in the current study, and only male voices were included). Tokens from this set were randomly selected such that no two recordings were repeated until the entire set had been presented. These tokens were then spliced together with no intervening pause to form 8-minute, 32-second long passages. Next, these stimuli were amplitude-compressed in Adobe Audition 1.5 with additional manual editing; a bandpass filter was then cycled with a period of 64 seconds over the entire passage with center frequency logarithmically ascending from 150 Hz to 9600 Hz (Q=2, expanding to Q=3 at tails, where Q is the ratio of center frequency over bandwidth). Descending stimuli were created by reversing the entire waveform before filtering, then re-reversing at the final step. This initial filtering was cleaned using similarly ascending low- and high-pass filters positioned an octave above and below the center frequency. Finally, a dynamic amplitude envelope was imposed over the frequency sweeps to equilibrate perceived loudness in the scanner environment. For presentation with the Sensimetrics headphones, stimuli were passed through a final earbud-specific filter that compensated for the slight frequency peaks and phase offsets induced by the acoustic transfer functions of the earbuds. During scanning, subjects were asked to monitor the stimuli and press a button whenever they heard laughter (laughter stimuli were sparsely and non-periodically distributed through the stimulus train). For an example of a single stimulus sweep, see Audiofile 1.

Additional control stimuli were created to test the generality of the tonotopic maps across different types of sound. Similar bandpass-sweep filtering procedures were used on carefully edited musical excerpts (ABBA or Beatles songs), repetitive male speech samples, or amplitude modulated (16Hz) white noise (similar to Talavage et al., 2004).

Stimulus setups

Each subject was scanned using two different stimulus delivery setups in two to five different sessions. In the first setup, stimuli were delivered by an in-house manufactured electrodynamic, magnetless headphone (Josephs et al., 2009) using an isodynamically driven lightweight membrane either coupled to an ear insert that also acted as a passive attenuator of acoustical scanner noise, or presented directly at the ear. The headphone gives a high amplitude, smooth, and minimally distorted response over a wide bandwidth (approximately 14 kHz). Stimuli were delivered to only the left ear, as the size of the ear defender did not allow for subjects’ heads to fit into the compact 32-channel head coil with two headphones. The number of 8 min 32 sec long runs collected per participant using this setup were 4, 8, 8, 8, 8, 8, 12, 12, and 16. Three participants also took part in additional control stimulus conditions (see below), each with four 8′32” ″ runs.

In the second setup, stimuli were delivered binaurally using in-house-safety-enhanced Sensimetrics (Malden, MA, USA) S14 earbuds, with the head centered within the 32-channel coil. NoMoCo (NoMoCo Inc, San Diego, CA, USA), cushions were placed around the head to provide additional passive scanner acoustical noise attenuation and to stabilize head position. All volunteers participated in four 8′32″ long runs in this setup. Finally, a subset of 7 participants was scanned on an additional single run of a 8min 32sec block design ‘auditory localizer’ experiment where 16-second blocks of the tonotopy stimulus (with stimuli across blocks balanced for frequency range) were alternated with 16-second blocks of no stimulation.

Cortical surface reconstruction and sampling of R1 values within cortical ribbon

Cortical surfaces were reconstructed with FreeSurfer (v5.0.0, Dale et al., 1999) from the aligned (AFNI 3dAllineate, Cox, 2011), hand-inspected average of the two high-resolution T1-weighted scans acquired for R1 mapping. We initially attempted using quantitative R1 scans for surface reconstruction but experienced localized segmentation failures because some boundaries between the pial surface, the CSF, and the skull have different contrast from FreeSurfer’s priors in the segmentation algorithms. (Cortical surfaces for the three participants who were not scanned using quantitative protocols were reconstructed using the single T1-weighted MPRAGE acquired at 1.5T). Each subject’s reconstructed cortical surface was inflated to a sphere and registered to an average spherical surface atlas in FreeSurfer using a best-fit sulcal alignment (Fischl et al., 1999).

R1 data sets were sampled along the normal to each gray/white matter surface vertex in steps of 10% of cortical thickness (thickness estimated in FreeSurfer, Fischl and Dale, 2000) and then smoothed tangentially at each depth with a 4 mm FWHM 2D kernel. The human cortex has deep sulci, but also a complex pattern of concavity and convexity, including many convex regions buried within major sulci. Postmortem studies in humans show that myeloarchitecture varies significantly with local cortical convexity (Annese et al., 2004), where more convex regions are thicker and more myelinated, especially in middle and upper layers. Recently we found that there is a moderately strong correlation between local curvature and R1 at middle cortical depth fractions as well as a lesser but still significant correlation between cortical thickness and R1 (Sereno et al., 2012). tThus, for each participant, we used the FreeSurfer estimate of local curvature (smoothed with a < 1 mm FWHM 2D kernel) and cortical thickness as hemisphere-wise linear predictors of R1 values at each cortical depth to adjustfor curvature-dependent changes in myelination as well as potential sampling artifacts due to local imperfections in reconstructed white or pial surfaces. Vertex-wise residuals from this regression were used as ‘de-curved’ and ‘de-thickened’ estimates of R1 values whose units are directly comparable to raw demeaned R1 values. Cross-subject cortical-surface-based averages of R1-derived values were calculated by projecting the values from each subject onto the unit sphere after 1 step (<1mm FWHM) of surface-based smoothing (Hagler et al., 2006), and averaging the values at each vertex. For visualization purposes, resulting average values were back-projected onto a single representative subject’s entire inflated hemisphere or a resected temporal lobe showing the pial surface, with the latter graphically edited to remove the medial temporal lobe and aspects of the insula that were difficult to remove cleanly in FreeSurfer.

Creation of isofrequency contours from data of Figure 2a from Morel, Garraghty, & Kaas (1993)

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(a, left panel) Relaxation rate (R1 sec−1) as function of cortical depth, averaged within probabilistically defined subdivisions of Brodmann’s area 41 (TE1.0, TE1.1, & TE1.2 according to Morosan et al., 2001). Average R1 within TE1.0 (putative auditory core) decreases steeply from the gray/white boundary (depth fraction 0.0) to a tilted plateau at middle depths (0.3 to 0.6), then again drops steeply at superficial depths (0.7 to 1.0) (error bars: +/−1 SEM over subjects). R1 within lateral (TE1.1) and medial (TE1.2) subdivisions shows a more gentle monotonic decrease from deep to superficial cortex. For comparison, the left inset is a myelin-stained section of human auditory core and belt cortex (from Wallace et al., 2002, contrast reversed) with a similar profile of myelination. (b, right panel): Group spherical average R1 values sampled at 50% of cortical depth and projected onto a single subject’s left and right inflated hemispheric surfaces. The auditory core is visible in both hemispheres as a keyhole-shaped hyperintensity maximum running posteromedially to anterolaterally over the medial half of Heschl’s gyrus. Hyperintensity maxima can also be observed within the densely myelinated pre- and post-central gyri.

These data were chosen for being (to our knowledge) the most complete and representative published tonotopic physiological recordings in a single macaque where auditory core was also characterized cyto- and myeloarchitectonically. Each best frequency (BF) datapoint along the physiological tracks in Figure 2A was entered into a 2D grid using the Draw Dataset plugin in AFNI (Cox, 2011) with a high-resolution jpeg of the original figure as an underlay. The sparse data were trilinearly interpolated onto a regular high-resolution grid using the ‘TriScatteredInterp’ function in Matlab R2010a (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA); ‘contourf’ was used to calculate and draw logarithmically-spaced isofrequency contours over the entire recording area (see Recanzone et al., 1999, for a similar approach). Coronal slice profiles and areal borders were traced directly over the jpeg in Pages ’09 (Apple, Cupertino, CA).

Analysis of R1 regional differences and depth profiles using morphed probabilistic maps

In order to provide an initial assessment of R1-based myelination maps, and to examine R1 cortical depth profiles using an independent set of regions-of-interest (ROIs), we created ROIs based on the 3D cytoarchitectonic probabilistic maps of subdivisions of Brodmann’s area 41 (TE1.0, TE1.1, TE1.2) with TE1.0 corresponding to the auditory core (Morosan et al., 2001, pg 695; Hackett et al., 2001). The Morosan et al., (2001) raw probability maps provided in the current AFNI distribution (Cox, 2011) were projected to a FreeSurfer ‘fsaverage’ brain registered to the Talairach target brain, resampled onto the cortical surface, and thresholded at p > 0.60 to create ROI labels. The labels were ~2mm FWHM (5 steps) surface smoothed with manual removal of isolated marked vertices (due to ‘spillover’ from the 3D to 2D projection within the lateral fissure), then spherically morphed to each subject. The morphed (unthresholded) probability maps are illustrated in Figure 3e.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Group average R1 values from 50% of cortical depth, projected onto the pial surface of the digitally resected temporal lobes of a single subject; (a) local increases in R1 values along medial Heschl’s gyrus, averaged across both scans, same data as Figure 2b; (b, c) single-scan R1 averages show excellent scan-rescan reproducibility; (d) maps of the change in R1 after removing R1 variance accounted for by local curvature and thickness show a very similar topology to raw R1 maps; (e) probability maps of cytoarchitectonically defined TE1.0 (‘core’), TE1.1, and TE1.2. derived from Morosan et al. (2001) Note that the overlap between probability distributions for TE1.0/TE1.1 and TE1.0/TE1.2 causes some probability maxima for TE1.1 (medial) and TE1.2 (lateral) to be darker colored, as shown in the overlapping probability ovoids at right.

For each subject, average R1 values for each ROI (TE1.0, TE1.1, TE1.2) were calculated in 10% steps of cortical thickness fraction (see above); these values were used to create R1 depth profiles. Planned matched pairs t-tests comparing R1 over the three ROIs were calculated from the data sampled at 50% of cortical thickness.

Functional analyses

Functional images were motion-corrected (AFNI 3dvolreg, heptic interpolation), and hand-registered (4×4 affine) with the high-resolution average T1-weighted volume used to create the cortical surface. Initial registration was carried out with the ‘align’ T1-weighted scan (same block center, slice plane direction as EPI scans); this initial registration was applied to the EPI volume, where final registration was finely adjusted by manual blink comparison (with contrast-reversed EPI images) to achieve a more exact overlay. Mapping data were analyzed using Fourier methods with individual and group analysis methods as previously described (Sereno et al., 1995; Sereno and Huang, 2006; Hagler et al., 2007), where voxels preferentially responding to a particular point in the stimulus cycle will show a higher amplitude at the frequency of stimulus cycling than at any other frequency. The phase of the signal, which corresponds to a particular point of the stimulus ramp, is then mapped to the color wheel, while the amplitude of the signal is mapped to the voxel’s color saturation. Runs with downward frequency sweeps were time-reversed and averaged with upward-swept scans in order to compensate for unspecified delays in the BOLD response.

Averaging of phase-encoded mapping data was performed using the methodology developed by Hagler and Sereno (2006) in which the real and imaginary components of the signal with respect to the stimulus ramp are averaged across subjects, preserving any phase information consistent between subjects. This was performed by projecting each participant’s phase-encoded map to the FreeSurfer spherical atlas, performing 1 step of surface-based smoothing (< 1 mm FWHM in 2D), averaging across subjects at each vertex, then painting back onto a single subject’s surface for viewing. Four cross-subject phase-encoded tonotopic averages were computed per hemisphere. Three of the four included only those subjects (N=6) with both functional and R1 data, including separate averages for each auditory stimulus setup (binaural, left monoaural) as well as a grand average over these two setups. The fourth average drew from independent data collected using different coil, headphone, and stimulus arrangements, and served to test map replicability. The left hemisphere average (N=5) included data from three participants scanned with a 7 cm ring coil (Siemens Double Loop Array) over the left superior temporal gyrus, with monaural right-ear stimulation using bandpass-swept musical and vocalization stimuli, and two participants scanned with the 32-channel head coil and binaural stimulation using bandpass-swept vocalization stimuli. All data for the right hemisphere average (N=4) were collected using binaural stimulation and the 32-channel coil and bandpass-swept musical, noise, or verbal stimuli. The three subjects without R1 data contributed to both left and right hemisphere averages; two subjects included in the first three averages contributed different data to the left (S6) and left and right hemisphere (S1) averages.

To show uninterrupted phase data in the same cortical locations in all participants (da Costa et al., 2011), the cross-subject-average block design ‘auditory localizer’ data were used to create an independent functional mask for the tonotopic map data (N=7, 3.2mm 2D FWHM of surface smoothing for sampling each subject’s data to the icosahedral sphere, 4.5mm 2D FWHM when rendering to a representative subject). A single contiguous patch of suprathreshold vertices on the superior temporal plane was extracted for each hemisphere (vertexwise p <0.01, with 3054 (right) and 3431 (left) connected vertices), then projected via spherical registration to each subject. Within this mask, group average and individual subject tonotopic phase-encoded data are displayed using a pseudocolor scale representing the systematic phase offsets in hemodynamic response corresponding to the phase of the auditory sweeps (2.2mm 2D FWHM surface smoothing was applied to tonotopic phase maps to gently even out voxel edges).

Auditory frequency gradient directions were drawn by imposing isofrequency contours over the color phase map (20 steps per complete cycle), then overlaying arrows perpendicular to each contour line in and around core, with arrow length spanning 1/40th of a complete phase cycle. The arrow direction thus represents the gradient direction while its length is proportional to the (logarithmic) tonotopic magnification factor (see below).

RESULTS

R1 as a function of cortical depth in probabilistically defined core and adjacent regions

As noted above, primate auditory core can be differentiated from surrounding belt regions by the presence of heavy myelination in deep and middle cortical depths with particularly heavy deep and middle layer staining within medial core (Hackett et al., 2001). An increase in overall myelination as well as expansion of layers 5 and 6 along the crown of human Heschl’s gyrus has also been noted (Wallace et al., 2002). Our in vivo human MRI samples much more coarsely than histology (800 μm versus ~20 μm), although we benefit from a relative super-resolution effect, in that at each vertex (whose position on the reconstructed cortical surface is at sub-voxel resolution), multiple voxels are sampled using linear interpolation. Nevertheless, we expected R1, which is proportional to the myelination level, to start high in deeper layers, drop to a moderate plateau at middle cortical depths, and then drop more steeply at superficial depth fractions. Given the continuously heavy myelination across middle layers of core (“astriate” profile, Hackett et al., 2001), the greatest R1 difference between core and adjacent regions should be observed at middle cortical depths.

In order to test these predictions, we first created depth profiles of average R1 in auditory ROIs likely to correspond to auditory core proper (TE1.0) and to adjacent regions lateral and medial to core (TE1.1 and TE1.2) in the published postmortem probabilistic maps of Brodmann’s area 41 (Morosan et al., 2001). The cross-subject cross-hemisphere average R1 depth profiles for TE1.0 in Figure 2a show the predicted steep drop in R1 at the gray matter/white matter border, followed by a tilted plateau in R1 at middle cortical depths and a second steep drop approaching the gray matter/pial boundary. The R1 profiles for the medial (TE1.1) and lateral (TE1.2) regions are smoother and almost entirely overlapping, and clearly undershoot the TE1.0 ‘core’ ROI profile at middle sampling depths as expected.

We verified these inter-ROI differences with hemisphere-wise paired t-tests comparing R1 values sampled halfway through cortex (0.5). Here, R1 in TE1.0 was greater than in corresponding TE1.1 (left hemisphere (lh),t(5)=5.90, p<0.002; right hemisphere (rh), t(5)=5.60, p<0.0025) and than in TE1.2 (lh, t(5)=4.14, p<0.0090, rh, t(5)=−12.51, p< 0.0001), with no significant difference between TE1.1 and TE1.2 (p > 0.4).

Shape, position, and size of auditory core

Myleoarchitectonic studies (see Introduction) show that middle layer myelination of auditory core has a characteristic keyhole shape, with its long axis oriented diagonally on the superior temporal plane. In humans, it tends to lie along Heschl’s gyrus (with darkest staining on the gyral crown - Wallace et al., 2002) but sometimes moves into the adjacent sulcus, with the broader rounder medial segment tapering to a thinner anterolateral finger (Hackett et al., 2001).

As an initial means of visualizing potential R1 differences expected to relate the topography of increased myelin content, we inspected the spherical-surface-based cross-subject average maps of R1 values sampled from voxels at middle cortical depths (depth fraction 0.5). When these average maps were thresholded at the mean R1 value from the mid-probability TE1.0 ROI (0.66 sec−1), a clear hyperintensity appeared in both hemispheres, spanning a thin strip of cortex (~1.9 cm × ~0.7 cm) running roughly along the posteromedial two thirds of Heschl’s gyrus (see Figures 2b and 3a). The only other hyperintensities visible at this threshold were along the pre- and post-central gyri, corresponding to the strongly myelinated primary motor and somatosensory cortices (Glasser and Van Essen, 2011; Sereno et al., 2012). R1 values within this presumptive core region (see Figure 4 & R1 contours in Figure 6) were highest within a semicircular patch at its posteromedial aspect, with decreasing R1 moving anterolaterally. A second smaller disconnected hyperintensity appeared further laterally along the superior temporal gyrus.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Montage of individual participants’ R1 maps, with values sampled halfway through cortex and projected onto the inflated surface of temporal and frontal lobes. As shown in the colorbars, the range of projected R1 values is constant over participants, with the colorscale slope adjusted slightly to show individual patterns of myelination. Keyhole-shaped regional increases in R1 oriented posteromedially to anterolaterally across Heschl’s gyrus were observed in all hemispheres (medialmost aspect indicated by arrow head) except the left hemisphere of participant S4 (marked with black diamond shape). R1 maxima are also observed along the pre- and post-central gyri within strongly myelinated presumptive primary motor and somatosensory regions. Abbreviations: S. Circular S => superior circular sulcus of the insula; Cent S => central sulcus; Heschl’s G => Heschl’s gyrus.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Montage of individual participants’ tonotopic maps and R1 contours, sampled halfway through cortex and detrended for effects of local curvature and thickness (see Methods, and Figure 3d). Tonotopic colormaps are as in Figure 5. For visual clarity, contours indicate stepwise progression of R1 values in probable auditory core only; see heatscale images in Figure 4 for raw R1 data for each individual.

As noted above, local R1 values, cortical myelin density, and laminar profile are known to vary with cortical thickness and local cortical curvature (Annese et al., 2004; Sereno et al., 2012), and thus may affect the R1-based localization of auditory core. As a validation measure, we compared the raw average R1 maps to a cross-subject average of thickness-and R1 values after regressing out the effects of curvature (see Methods and Figure 3d). This resulted in a picture of the keyhole-shaped maximum almost indistinguishable from that found with raw R1 values. As an additional validation measure, comparison of cross-subject R1 averages based on the two separate scan-rescan datasets also showed very close correspondence of auditory core localization (Figure 3b,c). A hyperintense R1 strip over posteromedial Heschl’s gyrus -- the presumptive auditory core -- could be easily identified in each participant, in 11 of 12 hemispheres (see Figure 4, and R1 contours in Figure 6). As seen in Figure 4, there are also additional regions of moderately high R1 values in individual participants more laterally and posteriorly to core; we speculate that these may correspond to area STA and potentially PA as identified in Wallace et al. (2002), but a conclusive homology will require directly comparable post mortem data.

Tonotopic maps and frequency gradients within and around the core region

Combined average human R1 and tonotopic maps and gradients (Figure 5)

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Combined tonotopic maps, gradients and R1 contours from group averages. Colormap shows fMRI results for characteristic frequency with logarithmic scaling in Hz around the color wheel. The angle of the overlaid white arrows shows the tonotopic gradient direction from low-to-high frequency preference at approximately 1/3 octave steps within tonotopic maps (perpendicular to isofrequency contour), with arrow length reflecting the approximate magnification factor (reciprocal of gradient vector). Dots indicate points where the gradient direction was unclear or interrupted. Dashed lines show R1 values in grayscale-coded steps of 0.005 sec−1 (same data as Figure 4); curved yellow line is suggested A1/R border based on presence of low-frequency reversal and R1 contour. Top and middle inset figures show tonotopic averages with left monaural and binaural stimulation from the same subjects as in the combined average. Bottom inset shows comparison tonotopic averages from different sets of participants and experimental setups; all tonotopic maps were identically masked using the independent auditory localizer (see Methods).

The relationship between average tonotopic maps and average R1 maps (sampled at 0.5 cortical depth fraction) from both hemispheres was similar to that seen in individual experiments in non-human primates (see inset, Figure 5). In both hemispheres, the posterior-medial end of presumptive core lies at the middle of an arch-shaped high frequency region (red). Moving anterolaterally along the long axis of the core, there is a steep high-to-low frequency progression (red->blue->green). Beyond the anterolateral end of the core, there is a large low frequency region (green), which progresses back to high frequencies (red). On the opposite anterior-medial edge of the core, selectivity progresses steeply back to high frequencies (red). Finally, at the anterior-most and posterior-most positions along the superior temporal gyrus, the characteristic frequencies descend once again (red->blue->green).

The arrangement of tonotopic maps can also be visualized by looking at the local frequency gradient direction (steepest uphill direction in frequency preference at a point; that is, ∇f with respect to cortical position). At each point, the gradient direction is perpendicular to the local isofrequency contour. The gradient direction has been drawn at a series of points along each isofrequency contour (iso-response-phase contour). The length of each vector is proportional to the scalar cortical magnification for logarithmic frequency (the reciprocal of the gradient vector amplitude). Thus, regions of slowest change in frequency sensitivity (smallest gradient amplitude) will have the longest arrows.

As noted above, the anterolateral part of the core lies within an elongated low-frequency trough in both hemispheres. Also in both hemispheres, the medial end of that low-frequency trough is located within region with the highest myelination. From this low-frequency trough, characteristic frequencies (CFs) rise in all directions. Along the anterolateral edge, there is a low-to-medium frequency progression, with higher CFs more lateral and anterior, as well as posterior. There is another progression from the low CF trough rising anteromedially, reaching a high CF maximum just anteromedial to the core. There is a third progression moving posteriorly from the middle of core that is interrupted by a possible discontinuity, where there is an extremely rapid progression through middle frequencies that ends in a high frequency maximum slightly posterior to core. Finally, there is some indication of a posteromedial progression within core in a direction parallel to the long edge that extends into higher frequency CFs, reaching a CF maximum posteromedially. It is interesting that CFs continue to rise for a short distance beyond the posteromedial end of the core (about 3 mm - see results for individual subjects below).

The pattern of tonotopic fields and progressions within and around core was consistent over the two different experimental setups (Figure 5 top and middle insets), although the stimulation (monaural vs. binaural) and head position in the head coil (shifted to right vs. centered) were different. In the right hemisphere, where auditory input was more closely matched, the average pattern of tonotopic maps over core is highly conserved over both scan types. In the left hemisphere (ipsilateral stimulation), we also see similar tonotopic mapping, but with a shallower transition to higher characteristic frequencies in and around the posteromedial cap of core. These tonotopic maps were also similar to cross-subject averages from data collected using different surface coil setups as well as individuals for whom R1 data were not acquired (Figure 5, bottom insets). There was some divergence across maps (particularly medially), whereas the tonotopic progressions within and around the presumptive core region were very similar over all averages. The general layout of the tonotopic maps - where high CFs predominate along the medial superior temporal plane, with two to three high-CF ‘fingers’ extending laterally, and interdigitate with more lateral lower-CF regions extending medially - particularly paralleled the results of Formisano et al (2003, Figure 5), and Humphries et al (2010, Figures 5 & 6).

Individual R1 and tonotopic maps

Since group average tonotopic map could potentially obscure individual differences, we also assessed individual maps relative to the position of auditory core (Fig. 6) and describe here four qualitative features of the average map (and of macaque monkey maps) that can be found in most individual subjects. A fifth feature involving correlations between tonotopy and R1 is actually more obvious in individuals. For each feature, we list the number of occurrences by hemisphere (N.B.: left hemisphere results are less robust because of fewer runs and less optimal receive coil positioning; also, S4 was excluded on myeloarchitectonic grounds for showing no clear core). The five features are: (1) a corona of higher characteristic frequencies around the medial cap of core (rh N=5/6, lh N=4/5), (2) an elongated high-frequency patch adjoining the anteromedial edge of core, transitioning to lower frequencies moving anteriorly (rh N=6/6, lh N=5/5), (3) a mid-core trough of lowest characteristic frequencies (rh N=6/6, lh N=5/5), (4) a high-to-medium-to-low progression lateral to the posterior two thirds of the core (rh N=6/6, lh N=4/5), (5) a transition from ~2k-1k-~0.5k characteristic frequency running from the anteromedial edge posterolaterally, with the ~0.5k characteristic frequency region in the highest R1 region (highest 2–3 contour lines in each participant) (rh N=5/6, lh N=4/5).

It is also notable that, unlike the group average tonotopic map, the posteromedial aspect of core reaches into regions with the highest CF in individual maps; in addition, several participants showed a more extensive low-to-higher-frequency progressing anterolaterally to the tip of core (as has been reported for some macaques, e.g., case 91–13 of Morel et al., 1993). In order to assess the stability of an individual’s tonotopic maps, we compared repeated sessions (4 runs each) on one individual (see Figure 7), and found very similar patterns of results for the three sessions using bandpass-swept vocalizations and one session with bandpass-swept music; the session using the same experimental setup but with bandpass-swept amplitude-modulated white noise showed similar but much weaker maps. These results were in keeping with the robust maps shown in previous human tonotopy studies (e.g., Formisano et al., 2003; Talavage et al., 2004; Woods et al., 2009, 2011), in particular the consistency over individual runs and sessions (Humphries et al., 2010).

Figure 7.

Figure 7

Functional tonotopic maps of an individual subject with focus on the right hemisphere of the inflated superior temporal lobe. Top row shows results from three different sessions of tonotopy using bandpass-swept vocalization stimuli (4 runs each); bottom left inset shows one session using bandpass-filtered music (4 runs), bottom right shows one session using bandpass-filtered, amplitude-modulated white noise (4 runs).

Summary of Results

Using quantitative anatomical R1 (=1/T1) maps as myelin markers, we found a small keyhole-shaped area running over the medial two thirds of Heschl’s gyrus that corresponded to non-human primate and human post mortem descriptions of auditory core (Hopf, 1951; Jones et al., 1995; Hutsler and Gazzaniga, 1996; Kosaki et al., 1997; Hackett et al., 1998; Morosan et al., 2001; Hackett et al., 2001; Wallace et al., 2002; Fullerton and Pandya, 2007; Hackett and de la Mothe, 2009) in terms of its laminar profile, orientation, shape, and intensity differences. This was true both in the surface-based group average as well as in most individual participants. This pattern was consistent across scans and subjects, and was essentially unchanged when the influence of local thickness and curvature on myelination and R1 measures was taken into account (Sereno et al., 2012).

On both a group and individual level, tonotopic mapping of this core region showed a pattern consistent with previous electrophysiological and architectonic studies in macaques and owl monkeys, with a progression from high to low frequency preference moving medially to laterally within the most densely myelinated part of core. We suggest that this region corresponds to area A1. Within the rostrolateral slightly less densely myelinated finger of the core, there is a low-to-medium frequency progression. Based on similarities with studies in non-human primates (Merzenich and Brugge, 1973; Imig et al., 1977; Morel et al., 1993) we suggest this region is the homologue of area R. It is also notable that the gradient field map position associated with A1 and R (Formisano et al, 2003, Figure 4) appears to be similar to the layout suggested by the R1-defined core and tonotopic maps. The small low-to-medium-to-high frequency gradient within R is more robust in individual participants than in the group average, probably a result of blurring due to anatomical inter-subject variation in this small area. Nonetheless, further high-resolution tonotopic imaging combined with quantitative R1 mapping will be useful in clarifying the group average result, particularly as it is not entirely as expected given some current models of tonotopic mapping in macaques and humans (Baumann et al., 2010; Woods et al., 2009).

DISCUSSION

These results show that non-invasive in vivo MRI techniques in humans can be used to characterize basic auditory functional regionalization originally revealed in non-human primates via a labor-intensive combination of physiological recording followed by postmortem cortical flattening and histology. Since post-recording histology is often partly compromised (Sincich & Horton, 2005), the definition of auditory core boundaries has also relied on other physiological criteria such as tuning bandwidth or firing rate/latency (Recanzone et al., 2000). The current study demonstrates for the first time concordance between tonotopy and myelination on the surface of the human cortex comparable with earlier studies in macaque and owl monkeys (Merzenich and Brugge, 1973; Imig et al., 1977; Pfingst and O’Connor, 1981; Morel and Kaas, 1992; Morel et al., 1993), a result which should help to narrow down the location of the auditory core in humans (Humphries et al., 2010). The current results were consistent across subjects, sessions, auditory stimulus setups and scanner field strength.. Given that the combined functional and structural protocol can be acquired using standard scanner hardware within approximately one hour, these techniques should be useful for defining primary auditory fields for further study (for a similar approach, see Talkington et al., 2012) as well as investigations of clinical populations with atypical auditory processes, such as congenital deafness (Karns et al., 2012) or in patients with schizophrenia or tinnitus.

One notable finding is the small size of core and its constituent areas A1 and R, compared to that of primary visual and somatosensory cortices (noted also in macaque by Merzenich and Brugge, 1973). Even when borders are liberally defined, the surface area of core itself rarely exceeds ~2 cm2 across individuals (see Figure 4), with A1 occupying somewhat more than half of that area. These dimensions are comparable to those reported for auditory core in human post-mortem studies (~2 cm × ~0.9 cm, as in Fig. 6a of Wallace et al., 2002), after corrections for shrinkage, distortion, and unfolding method.

One open question for identifying auditory core with either R1 maps or post-mortem histology is how to set intensity thresholds, particularly at the medial and lateral extents. Borders based on post-mortem myelin staining methods tend to be defined by eye, with a sudden change in stain density corresponding to areal edges (after correcting for section-to-section intensity differences), which can then be correlated with borders derived from other measures such as laminar-specific patterns of Nissl concentrations. However, there are large differences in myelination within core (high medially, decreasing smoothly moving laterally), making a fixed threshold difficult to apply. For instance, as can be seen in Hackett et al., 1998 (their figures 2c&d), the grayscale level in the thin, lateral-most part of core is very similar to that in the belt region surrounding medial core.

In the present MRI-based in vivo study, an analogous case of equivalent R1 values at the lateral tip of core and the region surrounding medial core can be observed in participant S5 (see Figure 4). We have addressed this issue by presenting both the graded and unmasked heatscale data (Figures 24), along with the R1 contour plots (Figures 5&6), where low R1 values well outside the probable contiguous boundaries of core are not shown. Gradient-based approaches potentially avoid fixed threshold problems, but the gradient is a local measure that is noisy with unsmoothed data yet highly sensitive to spatial smoothing parameters.

The current findings confirm several results and predictions of the pioneering study of Sigalovsky et al. (2006), who used a multi-scan whole-brain FLASH protocol (with different flip angles for each scan) to map R1 variation across auditory cortex (averaged over cortical depth), finding consistent patterns of higher R1 values in medial Heschl’s gyrus in individual participants, along with hyperintensities along the superior temporal gyrus and planum temporale. As predicted by Sigalovsky et al., higher resolution (0.512 mm3 voxel volume in the present study versus 1.69 mm3 in Sigalovsky) and increased SNR (due to 32-channel coil, multi-echo readout, 3T field strength, more data points) made it possible to see laminar differences in R1 between probabilistically defined core and adjacent regions in the present study (Figure 2a). The increased resolution and SNR also enabled us to resample and render R1 values at specific cortical depths where core and non-core regions are most different. This sharpened the resulting maps and revealed a correlation between cortical curvature and myelination (Annese et al., 2004) at middle cortical depths (Sereno et al., 2012). Note that the average R1 values of ~0.75 sec−1 measured by Sigalovsky et al. (2006) at 1.5T cannot be directly compared to the average value of 0.69 sec−1 within TE1.0 measured in this study at 3T, since R1 decreases with field strength (Oros-Peusquens et al., 2008). However, the rather small R1 measured at 1.5T suggests that it might have been affected by partial volume effects, particularly contamination with cerebral spinal fluid, whereas the R1 measured in this study is in line with typical R1 values of gray matter at 3T (Oros-Peusquens et al., 2008).

The present study showed that the size and shape of the high-R1 core varies subtly between hemispheres within one individual (e.g., S1 and S6 in Fig. 4) and more markedly among individuals. In addition, the absolute R1 values in and around core varies. We think that most of these differences reflect real anatomical intra- and inter-individual differences. However, some may be due to technical limitations. For example, image artifacts due to head motion most likely caused misestimates of R1 in participant S4, obscuring the hyperintensity in the auditory core of the left hemisphere and causing anomalies in the temporal lobe, and around highly myelinated areas such as primary motor and somatosensory areas. Quantitative mapping combining data from multiple image acquisitions and with higher-than-usual resolution requires extra vigilance to reduce head motion artifacts. Shortening of the acquisition time would help to reduce motion artifacts within and across the runs, but sufficient SNR must be preserved in order to preserve R1 map precision.

Inaccuracies in the reconstruction of the white or pial surfaces originating in local misclassification of tissue types (and thus local misestimation of cortical thickness and depth) is another potential source of artifacts.. Since myelination (and R1) vary as much with depth as they do tangentially, this can obscure areal boundaries. Surface imperfections can occur near large pial vessels, where the pial surface closely approaches itself, or where the gray/white matter surface closely approaches itself along thin strands of white matter under small gyri (although it should be noted that these comprise only a tiny fraction of temporal cortex).

Another unexpected finding was that the medial, high-frequency edge of A1 appeared to extend slightly medially (~3mm) beyond the boundary of highest R1 values in the group average, and in some individuals. This possible over-estimated extent in the group average result may be due in part to blurring over somewhat variable functional anatomy, but may also be due to inherent difficulties in mapping of the BOLD response at the boundary between medial Heschl’s gyrus and the deep, narrow circular sulcus. This region is highly vascularized, which may blur the BOLD response where primary cortex adjoins the caudal (CM) auditory field. This could be resolved by tonotopic mapping at higher field strengths and higher resolution (e.g., da Costa et al., 2011; Formisano et al., 2003, Humphries et al., 2010), or by using acquisition methods thought to suppress contributions from larger vessels (e.g., spin-echo EPI at 7T, Yacoub et al., 2005). However, this finding may also reflect true physiological and anatomical properties. CM, a very small auditory area which shares the medial high-frequency border with A1, shows much broader frequency tuning, and a more discontinuous tonotopic map (Kajikawa et al., 2005). CM is also highly myelinated, making it difficult to distinguish the A1/CM border based on myeloarchitectonics alone, even in post-mortem assays, where changes in cell type are a more unambiguous marker of the A1/CM border.

Our new quantitative, high-resolution, multi-modal definition of the auditory core will make it possible to investigate how musical expertise or auditory acuity are related to differences in myelination of A1 and R (see Duncan and Boynton, 2003; Schwarzkopf et al., 2011; Song et al., 2011; and Duncan and Boynton, 2007, for related studies in the visual, auditory, and somatosensory systems). Our methods can also be extended to non-human primates to improve our understanding of homologies as well as potential differences. Studies of auditory and language function in individuals whose core and primary auditory fields are precisely mapped will improve our understanding of the contribution of these early areas to perceptual and cognitive processes. Finally, the sensitive quantitative indices of cortical microstructure developed here may help with early diagnosis of disease and serve as a biomarker to assess the effectiveness of new therapies.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Troy Hackett, Lori Holt, Jen Linden, and Sam Schwarzkopf for very useful comments and suggestions, and to Holly Bridge and Stuart Clare for generous support. This research was funded by the Medical Research Council NIA G0400341 and G0700399, The Royal Society RG081218, Royal Society Wolfson Research Merit Award, and NIH RO1 MH 081990. The Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from the Wellcome Trust 091593/Z/10/Z

References

  1. Annese J, Pitiot A, Dinov ID, Toga AW. A myelo-architectonic method for the structural classification of cortical areas. NeuroImage. 2004;21:15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baumann S, Griffiths TD, Rees A, Hunter D, Sun L, Thiele A. Characterisation of the BOLD response time course at different levels of the auditory pathway in non-human primates. NeuroImage. 2010;50:1099–1108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Belin P, Fillion-Bilodeau S, Gosselin F. The Montreal Affective Voices: a validated set of nonverbal affect bursts for research on auditory affective processing. Behavior research methods. 2008;40:531–539. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.2.531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bridge H, Clare S. High-resolution MRI: in vivo histology? Philos Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol Sci. 2006;361:137–146. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cox RW. AFNI: What a long strange trip it’s been. NeuroImage advance online publication. 2011 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Da Costa S, van der Zwaag W, Marques JP, Frackowiak RSJ, Clarke S, Saenz M. Human Primary Auditory Cortex Follows the Shape of Heschl’s Gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:14067–14075. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2000-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. DeYoe EA, Carman GJ, Bandettini P, Glickman S, Wieser J, Cox R, Miller D, Neitz J. Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:2382–2386. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.6.2382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Draganski B, Ashburner J, Hutton C, Kherif F, Frackowiak RSJ, Helms G, Weiskopf N. Regional specificity of MRI contrast parameter changes in normal ageing revealed by voxel-based quantification (VBQ) NeuroImage. 2011;55:1423–1434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Duncan RO, Boynton GM. Cortical magnification within human primary visual cortex correlates with acuity thresholds. Neuron. 2003;38:659–671. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00265-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Duncan RO, Boynton GM. Tactile hyperacuity thresholds correlate with finger maps in primary somatosensory cortex (S1) Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:2878–2891. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Engel SA, Rumelhart DE, Wandell BA, Lee AT, Glover GH, Chichilnisky EJ, Shadlen MN. fMRI of human visual cortex. Nature. 1994;369:525. doi: 10.1038/369525a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM. High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;8:272–284. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)8:4&#x0003c;272::AID-HBM10&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Formisano E, Kim DS, Di Salle F, van de Moortele PF, Ugurbil K, Goebel R. Mirror-symmetric tonotopic maps in human primary auditory cortex. Neuron. 2003;40:859–869. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00669-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fullerton BC, Pandya DN. Architectonic analysis of the auditory-related areas of the superior temporal region in human brain. J Comp Neurol. 2007;504:470–498. doi: 10.1002/cne.21432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Glasser MF, Van Essen DC. Mapping human cortical areas in vivo based on myelin content as revealed by t1- and t2-weighted MRI. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:11597–11616. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2180-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hackett TA. Organization and Correspondence of the Auditory Cortex of Humans and Nonhuman Primates. In: Kaas JH, editor. Evoluntion of the Nervous System. 2007. pp. 109–119. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hackett TA. Information flow in the auditory cortical network. Hear Res. 2011;271:133–146. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hackett TA, Stepniewska I, Kaas JH. Subdivisions of auditory cortex and ipsilateral cortical connections of the parabelt auditory cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 1998;394:475–495. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19980518)394:4<475::aid-cne6>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hackett TA, Preuss TM, Kaas JH. Architectonic identification of the core region in auditory cortex of macaques, chimpanzees, and humans. J Comp Neurol. 2001;441:197–222. doi: 10.1002/cne.1407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hackett TA, la Mothe de LA. Regional and laminar distribution of the vesicular glutamate transporter, VGluT2, in the macaque monkey auditory cortex. J Chem Neuroanat. 2009;38:106–116. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2009.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hagler D, Saygin A, Sereno M. Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data. NeuroImage. 2006;33:1093–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hagler D, Riecke L, Sereno M. Parietal and superior frontal visuospatial maps activated by pointing and saccades. NeuroImage. 2007;35:1562–1577. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Helms G, Dathe H, Dechent P. Quantitative FLASH MRI at 3T using a rational approximation of the Ernst equation. Magn Reson Med. 2008a;59:667–672. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Helms G, Finsterbusch J, Weiskopf N, Dechent P. Rapid radiofrequency field mapping in vivo using single-shot STEAM MRI. Magn Reson Med. 2008b;60:739–743. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Helms G, Draganski B, Frackowiak R, Ashburner J, Weiskopf N. Improved segmentation of deep brain grey matter structures using magnetization transfer (MT) parameter maps. NeuroImage. 2009;47:194–198. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hopf A. Die Myeloarchitektonik des Isocortex temporalis beim Menschen. In: Vogt O, Vogt C, editors. Journal für Hirnforschung. Vol. 2. 1951. pp. 208–279. [Google Scholar]
  29. Humphries C, Liebenthal E, Binder JR. Tonotopic organization of human auditory cortex. NeuroImage. 2010;50:1202–1211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hutsler JJ, Gazzaniga MS. Acetylcholinesterase staining in human auditory and language cortices: regional variation of structural features. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:260–270. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Imig TJ, Ruggero MA, Kitzes LM, Javel E, Brugge JF. Organization of auditory cortex in the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus) J Comp Neurol. 1977;171:111–128. doi: 10.1002/cne.901710108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jones EG, Dell’Anna ME, Molinari M, Rausell E, Hashikawa T. Subdivisions of macaque monkey auditory cortex revealed by calcium-binding protein immunoreactivity. J Comp Neurol. 1995;362:153–170. doi: 10.1002/cne.903620202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Josephs O, Dick F, Sereno M, Weiskopf N. An Isodynamic High Fidelity MRI Compatible Headphone. NeuroImage. 2009;47:S186. [Google Scholar]
  34. Kajikawa Y, la Mothe de L, Blumell S, Hackett TA. A comparison of neuron response properties in areas A1 and CM of the marmoset monkey auditory cortex: tones and broadband noise. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:22–34. doi: 10.1152/jn.00248.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Karns CM, Dow MW, Neville HJ. Altered cross-modal processing in the primary auditory cortex of congenitally deaf adults: a visual-somatosensory FMRI study with a double-flash illusion. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:9626–9638. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6488-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kay KN, Naselaris T, Prenger RJ, Gallant JL. Identifying natural images from human brain activity. Nature. 2008;452:352–355. doi: 10.1038/nature06713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Konen CS, Behrmann M, Nishimura M, Kastner S. The functional neuroanatomy of object agnosia: a case study. Neuron. 2011;71:49–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kosaki H, Hashikawa T, He J. Tonotopic organization of auditory cortical fields delineated by parvalbumin immunoreactivity in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 1997;386:304–316. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Langers DRM, Van Dijk P. Mapping the Tonotopic Organization in Human Auditory Cortex with Minimally Salient Acoustic Stimulation. Cerebral Cortex advance online publication. 2011 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Larsson J, Heeger DJ. Two retinotopic visual areas in human lateral occipital cortex. J Neurosci. 2006;26:13128–13142. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1657-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lutti A, Hutton C, Finsterbusch J, Helms G, Weiskopf N. Optimization and validation of methods for mapping of the radiofrequency transmit field at 3T. Magn Reson Med. 2010;64:229–238. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lutti A, Stadler J, Josephs O, Windischberger C, Speck O, Bernarding J, Hutton C, Weiskopf N. Robust and Fast Whole Brain Mapping of the RF Transmit Field B1 at 7T. In: Zhan W, editor. PLoS ONE. Vol. 7. 2012. p. e32379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Merzenich MM, Brugge JF. Representation of the cochlear partition of the superior temporal plane of the macaque monkey. Brain Res. 1973;50:275–296. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90731-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Morel A, Garraghty PE, Kaas JH. Tonotopic organization, architectonic fields, and connections of auditory cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 1993;335:437–459. doi: 10.1002/cne.903350312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Morel A, Kaas JH. Subdivisions and connections of auditory cortex in owl monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 1992;318:27–63. doi: 10.1002/cne.903180104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Morosan P, Rademacher J, Schleicher A, Amunts K, Schormann T, Zilles K. Human primary auditory cortex: cytoarchitectonic subdivisions and mapping into a spatial reference system. NeuroImage. 2001;13:684–701. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Oros-Peusquens AM, Laurila M, Shah NJ. Magnetic field dependence of the distribution of NMR relaxation times in the living human brain. Magn Reson Mater Phy. 2008;21:131–147. doi: 10.1007/s10334-008-0107-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Petkov CI, Kayser C, Augath M, Logothetis NK. Functional imaging reveals numerous fields in the monkey auditory cortex. PLoS Biol. 2006;4:e215. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Pfingst BE, O’Connor TA. Characteristics of neurons in auditory cortex of monkeys performing a simple auditory task. J Neurophysiol. 1981;45:16–34. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.45.1.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Preibisch C, Deichmann R. Influence of RF spoiling on the stability and accuracy of T1 mapping based on spoiled FLASH with varying flip angles. Magn Reson Med. 2009;61:125–135. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rajimehr R, Tootell RBH. Does retinotopy influence cortical folding in primate visual cortex? Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:11149–11152. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1835-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Recanzone GH, Schreiner CE, Sutter ML, Beitel RE, Merzenich MM. Functional organization of spectral receptive fields in the primary auditory cortex of the owl monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1999;415:460–481. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19991227)415:4<460::aid-cne4>3.0.co;2-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Recanzone GH, Guard DC, Phan ML. Frequency and intensity response properties of single neurons in the auditory cortex of the behaving macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:2315–2331. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.4.2315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Saygin AP, Sereno MI. Retinotopy and attention in human occipital, temporal, parietal, and frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:2158–2168. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Schwarzkopf DS, Song C, Rees G. The surface area of human V1 predicts the subjective experience of object size. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:28–30. doi: 10.1038/nn.2706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB. Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science. 1995;268:889–893. doi: 10.1126/science.7754376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sereno MI, Lutti A, Weiskopf N, Dick F. Mapping the human cortical surface by combining quantitative T1 with retinotopy. Cerebral Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs213. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sereno MI, Huang R-S. A human parietal face area contains aligned head-centered visual and tactile maps. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:1337–1343. doi: 10.1038/nn1777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sigalovsky IS, Fischl B, Melcher JR. Mapping an intrinsic MR property of gray matter in auditory cortex of living humans: a possible marker for primary cortex and hemispheric differences. NeuroImage. 2006;32:1524–1537. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sincich LC, Horton JC. The circuitry of V1 and V2: integration of color, form, and motion. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:303–326. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Song C, Schwarzkopf DS, Kanai R, Rees G. Reciprocal Anatomical Relationship between Primary Sensory and Prefrontal Cortices in the Human Brain. J Neurosci. 2011;31:9472–9480. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0308-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Talavage TM, Sereno MI, Melcher JR, Ledden PJ, Rosen BR, Dale AM. Tonotopic organization in human auditory cortex revealed by progressions of frequency sensitivity. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:1282–1296. doi: 10.1152/jn.01125.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Talkington WJ, Rapuano KM, Hitt LA, Frum CA, Lewis JW. Humans Mimicking Animals: A Cortical Hierarchy for Human Vocal Communication Sounds. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:8084–8093. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1118-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Thesen S, Heid O, Mueller E, Schad LR. Prospective acquisition correction for head motion with image-based tracking for real-time fMRI. Magn Reson Med. 2000;44:457–465. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200009)44:3<457::aid-mrm17>3.0.co;2-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Trampel R, Ott DVM, Turner R. Do the Congenitally Blind Have a Stria of Gennari? First Intracortical Insights In Vivo. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21:2075–2081. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wallace M, Johnston P, Palmer A. Histochemical identification of cortical areas in the auditory region of the human brain. Exp Brain Res. 2002;143:499–508. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1014-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Walters NB, Egan GF, Kril JJ, Kean M, Waley P, Jenkinson M, Watson JDG. In vivo identification of human cortical areas using high-resolution MRI: an approach to cerebral structure-function correlation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:2981–2986. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0437896100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Weiskopf N, Lutti A, Helms G, Novak M, Ashburner J, Hutton C. Unified segmentation based correction of R1 brain maps for RF transmit field inhomogeneities (UNICORT) NeuroImage. 2011;54:2116–2124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wiggins GC, Triantafyllou C, Potthast A, Reykowski A, Nittka M, Wald LL. 32-channel 3 Tesla receive-only phased-array head coil with soccer-ball element geometry. Magn Reson Med. 2006;56:216–223. doi: 10.1002/mrm.20925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Woods DL, Herron TJ, Cate AD, Kang X, Yund EW. Phonological processing in human auditory cortical fields. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:42. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Woods DL, Stecker GC, Rinne T, Herron TJ, Cate AD, Yund EW, Liao I, Kang X, García AV. Functional Maps of Human Auditory Cortex: Effects of Acoustic Features and Attention. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e5183. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Yacoub E, van de Moortele PF, Shmuel A, Ugurbil K. Signal and noise characteristics of Hahn SE and GE BOLD fMRI at 7 T in humans. NeuroImage. 2005;24:738–750. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Zilles K, Amunts K. Centenary of Brodmann’s map--conception and fate. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:139–145. doi: 10.1038/nrn2776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES