Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Hum Brain Mapp. 2013 May 14;35(4):1390–1403. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22261

Functional MRI of cerebellar activity during eyeblink classical conditioning in children and adults

Dominic T Cheng 1, Ernesta M Meintjes 2, Mark E Stanton 3, John E Desmond 1, Mariska Pienaar 2, Neil C Dodge 4, John M Power 5, Christopher D Molteno 2, John F Disterhoft 6, Joseph L Jacobson 2,4, Sandra W Jacobson 2,4
PMCID: PMC3823743  NIHMSID: NIHMS501914  PMID: 23674498

Abstract

This study characterized human cerebellar activity during eyeblink classical conditioning (EBC) in children and adults using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). During fMRI, participants were administered delay conditioning trials, in which the conditioned stimulus (a tone) precedes, overlaps, and coterminates with the unconditioned stimulus (a corneal airpuff). Behavioral eyeblink responses and brain activation were measured concurrently during two phases: pseudoconditioning, involving presentations of tone alone and airpuff alone, and conditioning, during which the tone and airpuff were paired. Although all participants demonstrated significant conditioning, the adults produced more conditioned responses (CRs) than the children. When brain activations during pseudoconditioning were subtracted from those elicited during conditioning, significant activity was distributed throughout the cerebellar cortex (Crus I– II, lateral lobules IV–IX, and vermis IV–VI) in all participants, suggesting multiple sites of associative learning-related plasticity. Despite their less optimal behavioral performance, the children showed greater responding in the pons, lateral lobules VIII, IX, and Crus I, and vermis VI, suggesting that they may require greater activation and/or the recruitment of supplementary structures to achieve successful conditioning. Correlation analyses relating brain activations to behavioral CRs showed a positive association of activity in cerebellar deep nuclei (including dentate, fastigial, and interposed nuclei) and vermis VI with CRs in the children. This is the first study to compare cerebellar cortical and deep nuclei activations in children versus adults during eyeblink classical conditioning.

Keywords: cerebellum, development, learning, memory, neuroimaging

Introduction

Eyeblink classical conditioning (EBC) is a widely used model of learning, in which a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., a tone) and an unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g., a corneal airpuff) are temporally paired. One version of this procedure is delay conditioning, in which the CS precedes, overlaps, and coterminates with the US. Following repeated CS-US presentations, the CS alone reliably elicits a conditioned response (CR; e.g., an eyeblink) in anticipation of the US presentation, indicating that an association between the CS and US has been learned.

This well-characterized model system provides a powerful tool for studying the neural correlates of learning and memory. Through extensive laboratory animal investigations, the neural circuitry supporting EBC has been mapped in great detail (for review, see Christian and Thompson, 2003). This line of research has produced overwhelming evidence that the cerebellum and associated structures are critically important for eyeblink conditioning. Specifically, contributions from the cerebellar cortex (Yeo and Hardiman, 1992; Yeo, et al., 1984; Yeo, et al., 1985), particularly in lateral lobule VI, and cerebellar deep nuclei (Lavond, et al., 1985; Lavond, et al., 1984; McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; McCormick and Thompson, 1984b) have been well-documented.

EBC has also been used to investigate cerebellar development and the ontogeny of learning. Developmental studies of EBC have shown that older (24 days) rat pups conditioned at a faster rate compared to younger (17 days) rat pups (Stanton, et al., 1998; Stanton, et al., 1992), reflecting specific ontogenetic changes in cerebellar circuitry (Freeman, 2010; Freeman, et al., 1995a).

Developmental EBC studies in humans ranging from infants to older adults have focused mainly on behavioral measures of conditioning (Cheng, et al., 2010; Claflin, et al., 2002; Herbert, et al., 2003; Jacobson, et al., 2011; Jacobson, et al., 2008; Knuttinen, et al., 2001; Stanton, et al., 2010; Woodruff-Pak and Thompson, 1988). Although these studies offer significant insight on how development affects behavioral performance during eyeblink conditioning, little is known about how development affects cerebellar activity mediating human EBC. To date, investigations of cerebellar function in humans during EBC have been limited to studies of adult patients with cerebellar lesions (Gerwig, et al., 2003; Gerwig, et al., 2010) and adult neuroimaging studies (Blaxton, et al., 1996; Cheng, et al., 2008; Knuttinen, et al., 2002; Logan and Grafton, 1995; Molchan, et al., 1994; Parker, et al., 2012; Ramnani, et al., 2000; Schreurs, et al., 2001; Schreurs, et al., 1997). Findings from these studies support the premise that the adult cerebellum is critically important for EBC. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in adults report activations in cerebellar lobule VI during EBC (Cheng, et al., 2008; Ramnani, et al., 2000). In a PET study of older and younger adults (Schreurs, et al., 2001), older adults showed decreased regional cerebral blood flow, but both populations demonstrated learning-related changes in the cerebellum. However, to date, no study has directly examined cerebellar activation patterns in children during EBC.

The lack of information relating to cerebellar function in children during EBC represents a significant gap in knowledge. By comparing activity patterns between the developing and adult brain, we can begin to address important questions concerning how and when this fundamental form of learning occurs. This study investigated how development affects cerebellar cortical and deep nuclear activity during delay eyeblink conditioning in children and adults using fMRI.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fourteen children and 16 adults participated in this study, which was conducted at the Cape Universities Brain Imaging Centre in Cape Town, South Africa. Two children were excluded for failure to respond to the US, and 11 of the remaining 12 (91.7%) met criterion for conditioning. Three adults were excluded for failure to respond to the US and one for excessive spontaneous blinks during baseline. Of the remaining 12, nine (75.0%) met criterion for conditioning. The criteria for conditioning were one or more conditioning sessions with at least 25% CRs and 15% CRs above the level of blinking during pseudoconditioning. These exclusion criteria were modified from those we have used previously (Jacobson, et al., 2008) to reflect the general reduction in % CRs observed in the scanner environment and were defined based on visual inspection of the behavioral data before any imaging analyses were performed. Exclusion rates did not differ for the children vs. the adults (χ2 = 1.20, p > 0.20).

Data from the 11 children (7 male, 4 female; mean age = 11.5 years; range = 9.3–13.8) and nine adults (5 male, 4 female; mean age = 24.9 years; range = 19.0–29.7) who met criteria for conditioning were included in the analyses. Neuroimaging data from one child's last two conditioning sessions were lost due to technical difficulties. None of the children or adults had previously participated in any eyeblink conditioning studies. All procedures were approved by the Human Investigation Committee at Wayne State University School of Medicine and Faculty of Health Sciences Human Research Ethics Committee at the University of Cape Town.

Procedure

Behavioral apparatus

Stimulus presentation was controlled and behavioral data were recorded using a laptop computer interfaced to an NI USB-6218 data acquisition module running custom software developed under LabView version 7.1 (National Instruments, Austin, TX). Auditory stimuli were presented through the standard Siemens MR scanner headphones. A video (The Adventures of Milo and Otis) was projected without its soundtrack through a waveguide in-line with the bore of the magnet onto a rear projection screen positioned behind the bore of the magnet and viewed using an adjustable mirror attached to the single channel head coil. Standard laboratory safety goggles were modified by attaching the end of a polyethylene tube (Nalgene, Rochester, NY), which delivered an airpuff, and an MRI-compatible infrared sensor, which recorded eyeblinks. Airpuff delivery was controlled by a solenoid valve (Asco, Florham Park, NJ), and a fiber-optic probe (RoMack Inc., Williamsburg, VA) measured the reflectance of infrared light from the left eye (Cheng, et al., 2008; Miller, et al., 2005).

Imaging apparatus

The MR scans were acquired on a 3T Allegra (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) MRI scanner at the Cape Universities Brain Imaging Centre (CUBIC). All children were prepared for scanning in a mock scanner where they listened to a recording of the scanner noises.

High-resolution T1-weighted structural MR images were acquired using a 3D echo planar imaging (EPI) -navigated (Tisdall, et al., 2012) multiecho MPRAGE (van der Kouwe, et al., 2008) sequence that had been optimized for morphometric analyses using FreeSurfer software. Imaging parameters were: FOV: 256 × 256 mm; 128 sagittal slices, TR: 2530 ms; TE: 1.53/3.21/4.89/6.57 ms; TI: 1100 ms; Flip angle: 7°; voxel size: 1.3 × 1.0 × 1.3 mm3. The 3D EPI navigator provided real-time motion tracking and correction (Tisdall, et al., 2012), which served to substantially reduce the presence of any motion artifacts in the structural imaging data.

A T2*-weighted gradient echo, echo planar imaging (EPI) pulse sequence was used to collect 205 whole brain functional volumes (6:36 minutes) sensitive to blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) contrast (TR 2000 ms, TE 30 ms, 34 interleaved slices, slice thickness 3 mm, gap 1.5 mm, FOV 200 mm × 200 mm, in-plane resolution 3.125 × 3.125 mm2) during the EBC acquisitions.

Presentation of stimuli

We used a conditioning procedure that produced significant learning in young adults (Cheng, et al., 2008; Cheng, et al., 2010). The CS tone was a binaural 1000 Hz tone (95 dB), lasting 750 ms, that co-terminated with a 100 ms corneal airpuff to the left eye (10 psi measured at the delivery site). Trials were grouped into blocks: 9 trials/block, 2 s/trial, 4 s inter-trial interval (ITI), such that each block lasted 34 s. Each session consisted of eight 9-trial blocks. Pseudoconditioning consisted of four sets of alternating tone alone and airpuff alone blocks. Conditioning blocks consisted of eight paired CS-US trials, plus a ninth CS-alone test trial. Blocks were separated by 16 s rest periods. See Figure 1A. The ITI in the current paradigm is similar to the ITI we used previously (Cheng, et al., 2008) and was selected to ensure a sufficient number of trials to permit analysis of the fMRI data. These temporal parameters were also selected to provide enough trials to permit conditioning within a limited time period and to ensure participant comfort. For children, we performed five scans, one pseudoconditioning and four conditioning sessions. For adults, we performed three scans, one pseudoconditioning and 2 conditioning sessions. Children were administered two additional conditioning sessions to determine whether, with additional training, their conditioning performance would reach the level of the adults.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Study design/analysis and sample topography of eyeblink responses measured in the MRI scanner. A) Pseudoconditioning consisted of alternating four delay tone alone and four airpuff alone blocks. Conditioning sessions consisted of eight blocks of paired CS-US trials. Adults received three separate sessions (1 pseudoconditioning + 2 conditioning) and children received five separate sessions (1 pseudoconditioning + 4 conditioning). B) Typical response profiles are shown for non-CR and CR trials. Peak amplitude responses indicate maximal eye closure. C) Dotted lines indicate the time window from which eyeblink responses were sampled.

Once participants were fitted with the conditioning goggles and positioned in the magnet, they were asked to lie still and watch the video. They were instructed to pay attention to the video as best they could while distracting tones and airpuffs were presented. The children were given a 2-hr lunch break between the second and third conditioning sessions.

Data Analysis

The topography of typical behavioral eyeblink responses collected inside the magnet is shown in Figure 1B. The 300-ms time period prior to presentation of the tone was used as a baseline. To determine trials during which CRs occurred, blink amplitudes during this baseline period were compared with the maximum blink amplitude during the 350 ms preceding the airpuff (Fig. 1C). The time window of 350-ms pre-US presentation was selected, in accordance with common conventions in the literature (Finkbiner and Woodruff-Pak, 1991; Jacobson, et al., 2011; Jacobson, et al., 2008), to exclude voluntary and alpha blink responses as CRs (Gormezano, 1966; Spence and Ross, 1959). To qualify as a CR, the difference between the maximum blink amplitude during this 350-ms time window and the mean response amplitude during the baseline had to exceed three times the standard deviation of the mean during the 300-ms baseline period. Performance was expressed as the percentage of trials with valid CRs (% CR). This measure of learning was examined in a session (pseudoconditioning/session 1/session 2) by age (child/adult) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Latency of the peak amplitude of the blink response (relative to onset of the 350 ms CR time window) was averaged across the CS tone alone trials (9th trial in each conditioning block; to avoid UR contamination) to examine age-related differences in the timing of eyeblink responses. For pseudoconditioning, all nine tone alone trials from the four tone alone blocks were used in the peak latency analysis.

Structural and functional imaging preprocessing and statistical analyses were performed with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM2 and SPM8) software (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK). Pre-processing included motion correction, structural data coregistration, normalization, and smoothing. EPI functional images were realigned and resliced correcting for minor motion artifacts, and structural images were co-registered to the mean motion-corrected functional image for each participant. Cerebellar structural and functional data were isolated and normalized into standard stereotaxic space using the spatially unbiased atlas template (SUIT) of the human cerebellum and brainstem (Diedrichsen, 2006), and the functional images were smoothed with a Gaussian filter (full-width half-maximum 5 mm). Given the children's age range, all structural and functional data were carefully inspected to ensure proper normalization and every child was found to be acceptable. Following SUIT transformation, voxel dimension was 2 mm3. The general linear model was used to estimate individual subject activations and a random-effects analysis was conducted on all subjects.

Contrasts were designed to investigate brain activity changes as a function of associative learning (conditioning versus pseudoconditioning) as well as age-related differences (children versus adults). To control for Type I error, Monte Carlo simulations (Forman, et al., 1995) were performed, which indicated that activation clusters of at least 10 voxels were significant at a p < 0.01 (corrected) level. Activation clusters within the cerebellum surviving this threshold and also previously reported to be involved during EBC were used in ROI analyses. These included a region in lateral lobule VI (Cheng, et al., 2008; Ramnani, et al., 2000; Yeo, et al., 1984) and vermis VI. The latter was selected due to its involvement during event timing (Spencer, et al., 2007) and the role of timing during CR expression (Gerwig, et al., 2005). Exploratory analyses were performed to examine the relations of additional large clusters of activity in left Crus II and right lobule VIII to learning (Gerwig, et al., 2003; Plakke, et al., 2007). Coordinates for the peak response within each region of interest (ROI) were identified at the group level and served as the center of a three-dimensional sphere (4 mm radius) that was created to sample individual subject mean brain activity in that ROI. This radius was chosen to restrict functional activity to within the anatomical boundaries of these regions. In addition to this functional ROI approach, anatomically-derived ROIs were generated for the deep nuclei (which were subsequently isolated into the dentate, fastigial, and interposed nuclei) and the hippocampus using probabilistic maps (Amunts, et al., 2005; Diedrichsen, et al., 2011; Dimitrova, et al., 2006), in light of the extensive evidence of the involvement of these regions in eyeblink conditioning (Berger, et al., 1976; Christian and Thompson, 2003; McCormick and Thompson, 1984a). Estimated mean and peak beta weights generated from ROI analyses were used as indices of brain activation. Mean and peak responses within the ROIs were examined in relation to behavioral performance to assess the relation between neural activation and behavioral CRs.

Results

Behavioral Findings

Significant across session differences in eyeblink conditioned responses (% CRs) were observed in both children and adults (Fig. 2). A repeated-measures ANOVA showed main effects of session, F(1,18) = 42.63, p < 4.0 × 10−6 and age, F(1,18) = 7.94, p < 0.02. The session × age interaction fell short of statistical significance F(1,18) = 3.52, p < 0.08. Post-hoc comparisons showed that child (M ± SD = 12.88 ± 12.00 % CR) and adult (18.16 ± 6.31) performance did not differ during pseudoconditioning, t(18) = 1.19, p > 0.20, but the adults produced a significantly greater percentage of CRs than the children during both conditioning session 1 (49.77 ± 19.37 and 27.78 ± 13.00, respectively), t(18) = 3.03, p < 0.008, and session 2 (55.81 ± 24.52 and 33.71 ± 17.15, respectively), t(18) = 2.37, p < 0.03.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Behavioral findings in children and adults. A significantly greater percentage of conditioned responses (CRs) was exhibited during conditioning relative to pseudoconditioning, indicating that both groups learned the CS-US association. Between session contrasts: % CRs in each session labeled “b” was significantly greater (at p < 0.05) than during pseudoconditioning, which is labeled “a”. Between group contrasts: adults produced significantly more CRs than children during sessions 1 and 2 (** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05).

Within-group ANOVAs showed main effects of session for both the children, F(1,10) = 6.19, p < 0.04, and the adults, F(1,8) = 17.97, p < 0.003. Post-hoc comparisons showed that both the children (p's < 0.05) and the adults (p's < 0.004) produced significantly more CRs during conditioning relative to pseudoconditioning. Importantly, there were no significant differences in percent unconditioned responses across sessions (pseudoconditioning: M ± SD = 87.44 ± 4.75, conditioning 1: 87.74 ± 2.01, conditioning 2: 84.71 ± 2.42), F(1,18) = 0.24, p > 0.20, or between children (87.55 ± 2.16) and adults (85.52 ± 4.70), F(1,18) = 0.18, p > 0.20. Thus, these behavioral findings indicate that, although all participants met criteria for conditioning and produced similar rates of unconditioned responses, the adults produced more conditioned responses than the children.

Repeated-measures ANOVA on latency of peak responses on CS alone trials showed a main effect of session, F(1,17) = 54.83, p < .001 but not age, F(1,17) = 1.31, p > 0.20. The session × age interaction fell short of statistical significance, F(1,17) = 3.87 p < 0.07. Post-hoc comparisons showed that child and adult performance did not differ during pseudoconditioning, t(17) = 1.36, p > 0.19, but the children's peak latency (715.91 ± 81.63 ms) was significantly longer than the adults (458.56 ± 69.27 ms) during conditioning session 1, t(18) = 2.34, p < 0.04, but not during session 2, t(18) = 0.86, p > 0.20.

Neuroimaging Findings

Initial imaging analyses focused primarily on differences in brain activity between pseudoconditioning and conditioning session 1, because that was when initial learning occurred (Fig. 2). Whole cerebellar analyses of children and adults revealed structures that demonstrated significantly (p < 0.01, corrected) greater responses during conditioning session 1 relative to pseudoconditioning (Table I). Significant cerebellar areas of activation in children (Fig. 3) included lateral lobules IV, V, VI, VIII, IX, Crus I and II, and vermis IV, V, and VI. Although the adults activated fewer cerebellar areas, most of those overlapped with areas activated in the children (e.g., lobule VIII, Crus I).

Table I.

Significant activations during Session 1 relative to Pseudoconditioning

a) Children
Hemisphere X Y Z SPM {Z} N Vox Brain region
Right 26 −48 −48 4.8 572 Lobule VIII
26 −52 −58 3.7 Lobule VIII
18 −42 −54 3.5 Lobule IX
6 −64 −30 3.3 33 Lobule VIII
20 −86 −24 3.4 29 Crus I
18 −68 −42 3.2 26 Lobule VIII
44 −58 −30 2.9 24 Crus I
26 −56 −30 3.3 21 Lobule VI
18 −58 −48 2.9 10 Lobule VIII
Left −42 −58 −46 3.7 65 Crus II
−30 −38 −34 4.1 50 Lobule VI
−24 −68 −28 3.9 42 Lobule VI
−2 −48 −16 3.1 28 Vermis IV,V
−18 −42 −50 3.2 24 Lobule IX
−22 −30 −28 3 21 Lobule IV, V
−44 −60 −24 3.3 20 Crus I
−16 −44 −46 2.8 13 Lobule IX
2 −66 −18 3 12 Vermis VI
−12 −50 −38 3 10 Lobule IX
−6 −44 −54 2.9 10 Lobule IX
b) Adults
Hemisphere X Y Z SPM {Z} N Vox Brain region
Right 44 −44 −54 3.1 45 Lobule VIIB
30 −40 −54 2.9 17 Lobule VIII
52 −56 −28 2.7 16 Crus I
16 −76 −38 3.3 13 Crus II
46 −58 −42 3.1 12 Crus II
Left −16 −34 −18 2.9 21 Lobule IV, V

MNI coordinates of activation maxima in the cerebellum (Schmahmann et al., 2000) in children (a) and adults (b). Regions listed were thresholded at a minimum cluster size of 10 voxels and z-scores of p < 0.01 (corrected). Indented entries represent local maxima within the main cluster.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Whole cerebellum analyses showing regions with significantly greater responses during session 1 of conditioning relative to pseudoconditioning in children.

Between-group comparisons between children and adults also showed significant age-related differences in cerebellar activations (Fig. 4). Although the adults produced more conditioned responses, the children showed significantly (p < 0.01, corrected) greater activation in the pons, cerebellar lateral lobule VIII, lateral lobule IX, Crus I, and vermis VI compared to adults (Table II). By contrast, the adults did not show any areas of activation that were significantly greater than the children's during conditioning session 1.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Whole cerebellum analyses showing regions with significantly greater response in children relative to adults during session 1 of conditioning (minus pseudoconditioning).

Table II.

Greater activations in children relative to adults during Session 1

Hemisphere X Y Z SPM {Z} N Vox Brain region
Right 8 −26 −30 3.9 166 Pons
32 −46 −48 3.2 27 Lobule VIII
34 −76 −36 3.3 25 Crus I
16 −42 −54 3.4 19 Lobule IX
2 −66 −18 3.1 13 Vermis VI
Left −50 −52 −40 3.8 62 Crus I
−22 −68 −32 3.7 41 Crus I
−4 −48 −50 3.3 25 Lobule IX
−8 −64 −36 3.2 23 Lobule VIII
−6 −52 −32 2.7 12 Lobule IX

MNI coordinates of activation maxima in the cerebellum (Schmahmann et al., 2000) in children relative to adults. Regions listed were thresholded at a minimum cluster size of 10 voxels and z-scores of p <0.01 (corrected).

Relation of Neuroimaging Data to Behavior

Correlation analyses relating brain activity to behavior were performed to identify regions that may contribute to the expression of conditioned responses. Mean activation within two functionally (lateral lobule VI and vermis VI) and five anatomically (cerebellar deep nuclei, dentate nucleus, fastigial nucleus, interposed nuclei, and hippocampus) defined ROIs was examined in relation to % CRs within each session (Table III), and mean activity within these ROIs during session 1 was also examined in relation to % CRs during each of the subsequent conditioning sessions (Table IV).

Table III.

Relation of mean brain activity to percent conditioned responses within each session

a) Children                          Structure
Session Left Deep Nuclei Right Deep Nuclei Left Lobule VI Right Lobule VI Vermis VI Left Hippocampus Right Hippocampus
1 0.78 ** 0.65 * 0.38 0.08 0.88 *** 0.37 0.20
2 0.48 0.47 −0.04 0.11 0.27 0.30 0.34
3 0.37 0.36 0.41 0.49 0.40 0.57 0.35
4 −0.17 0.08 −0.02 0.10 0.03 −0.15 −0.07
Session Left Dentate Left Fastigial Left Interposed Right Dentate Right Fastigial Right Interposed

1 0.73 * 0.77 ** 0.67 * 0.65 * 0.67 * 0.79 **
b) Adults                          Structure
Session Left Deep Nuclei Right Deep Nuclei Left Lobule VI Right Lobule VI Vermis VI Left Hippocampus Right Hippocampus
1 0.53 0.02 0.08 0.53 0.31 0.51 −0.20
2 −0.03 0.05 0.07 0.36 0.32 0.40 0.18
Session Left Dentate Left Fastigial Left Interposed Right Dentate Right Fastigial Right Interposed

1 0.73 * −0.13 0.13 0.09 0.06 0.18

Pearson correlation values between mean brain activity and behavioral performance within each session. Mean activity in the deep nuclei and vermis VI significantly correlated with conditioned responses during session 1 in children. Activity in the sub-nuclei (dentate, fastigial, and interposed) were also signficantly correlated during session 1 in children.

p < 0.10,

*

p < 0.05,

**

p < 0.01,

***

p < 0.001

Table IV.

Relation of mean brain activity in session 1 to percent conditioned responses in each of the sessions

a) Children                          Structure
Session Left Deep Nuclei Right Deep Nuclei Left Lobule VI Right Lobule VI Vermis VI Left Hippocampus Right Hippocampus
1 0.78 ** 0.65 * 0.38 0.08 0.88 *** 0.37 0.20
2 0.67 * 0.57 0.48 0.37 0.66 * 0.45 0.32
3 0.40 0.42 0.70 * 0.13 0.29 0.42 0.22
4 0.05 0.09 0.21 −0.11 0.03 −0.01 −0.07
Session Left Dentate Left Fastigial Left Interposed Right Dentate Right Fastigial Right Interposed

1 0.73 * 0.77 ** 0.67 * 0.65 * 0.67 * 0.79 **
2 0.65 * 0.26 0.54 0.58 0.36 0.65 *
b) Adults                          Structure
Session Left Deep Nuclei Right Deep Nuclei Left Lobule VI Right Lobule VI Vermis VI Left Hippocampus Right Hippocampus
1 0.53 0.02 0.08 0.53 0.31 0.51 −0.20
2 0.31 −0.11 0.51 0.50 0.38 0.53 −0.03
Session Left Dentate Left Fastigial Left Interposed Right Dentate Right Fastigial Right Interposed

1 0.73 * −0.13 0.13 0.09 0.06 0.18
2 0.53 −0.37 −0.22 −0.03 −0.13 −0.11

Pearson correlation values between mean brain activity during session 1 and behavioral performance in each of the sessions. Session 1 activity in the left deep nuclei and vermis VI signficantly correlated with conditioned responses during sessions 1 and 2 in children.

p < 0.10,

*

p < 0.05,

**

p < 0.01,

***

p < 0.001

Mean activity in left and right deep nuclei and vermis VI in session 1 was significantly correlated with conditioned responses during conditioning session 1 in the children (Table IIIa, Figs. 56). Mean activity within each of the cerebellar sub-nuclei (dentate, fastigial, and interposed) was also significantly correlated with conditioned responses during session 1 in children. Scatter plots with regression lines show the strong, positive relation of mean activity in left cerebellar deep nuclei and each of the sub-nuclei to % CRs during session 1 (Fig. 5). When session 1 brain activity was examined in relation to subsequent behavioral performance (sessions 2–4), activity in the left deep nuclei and vermis VI was also significantly correlated with conditioned responses during session 2 in the children (Table IVa). Significant correlations between conditioned responses during session 2 and cerebellar sub-nuclei activity in session 1 were only detected in the left dentate and right interposed nuclei while correlations with the left interposed and right dentate nuclei approached significance.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Relation of mean activation in left cerebellar deep nuclei to conditioned responses in children. Left panel shows that activity in the left cerebellar deep nuclei was positively correlated with conditioned responses in session 1. Smaller graphs on the right show this region separated into dentate, fastigial, and interposed nuclei and show that session 1 activity within these sub-nuclei also correlated with conditioned responses.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

Relation of mean activation in vermis to conditioned responses in children. Activity in the vermis was positively correlated with conditioned responses in sessions 1 and 2.

In adults, mean activity in the left dentate nucleus was significantly correlated with conditioned responses during session 1 but the correlation for all other regions fell short of statistical significance (Table IIIb). Finally, age of children and adults was not significantly correlated with either behavioral conditioned responses or brain beta weights (r's ranged from −0.06 to +0.25 all p's > 0.20).

Discussion

This is the first fMRI study to examine the neural substrates underlying eyeblink classical conditioning in children, compared with adults. Both children and adults demonstrated successful conditioning in the MRI environment. Although both groups produced similar rates of unconditioned responses, the adults produced more conditioned responses than the children. Significant learning-related activity was distributed throughout the cerebellar cortex in all participants, suggesting multiple sites of plasticity. Despite higher levels of behavioral conditioning in the adults, the children showed greater activity in multiple areas within the cerebellar cortex, suggesting that they may require greater activation and/or the recruitment of supplementary structures to achieve successful conditioning. The strong correlations between the imaging data and the learned behavioral responses further emphasized the importance of the cerebellar deep nuclei and vermis VI in the expression of conditioned eyeblink responses.

Behavior

Behavioral EBC studies have demonstrated that humans ranging from infants as young as 5 months to older adults show successful conditioning (Cheng, et al., 2010; Claflin, et al., 2002; Herbert, et al., 2003; Jacobson, et al., 2011; Jacobson, et al., 2008; Knuttinen, et al., 2001; Stanton, et al., 2010; Woodruff-Pak and Thompson, 1988). In the current study, children and adults demonstrated significantly greater % CRs during conditioning compared to pseudoconditioning, suggesting both groups learned the CS-US association. Adults showed higher levels of conditioning than children, even though the children received twice as many trials, providing evidence of continued developmental change from child to adulthood in this domain. Importantly, although their learning was not as robust as adults, children showed significant conditioning inside the MRI environment.

The current behavioral findings, coupled with results from other studies on how aging affects conditioning (Cheng, et al., 2010; Claflin, et al., 2002; Herbert, et al., 2003; Jacobson, et al., 2011; Jacobson, et al., 2008; Knuttinen, et al., 2001; Stanton, et al., 2010; Woodruff-Pak and Thompson, 1988), suggest that EBC performance is optimal during young adulthood and less robust during childhood and in older populations. The present findings in children extend our knowledge on EBC performance over the human lifespan.

Learning-related Changes in Brain Activity

Laboratory animal work has identified the cerebellar cortex as one important site of plasticity for EBC, particularly lateral lobule VI (Harvey, et al., 1993; Lavond and Steinmetz, 1989; Nolan and Freeman, 2006; Yeo and Hardiman, 1992; Yeo, et al., 1984; Yeo, et al., 1985). Rats with damage to Purkinje cells in the cerebellar cortex were impaired in the acquisition and extinction of eyeblink conditioning (Nolan and Freeman, 2006), and aspirations of lateral lobule VI were found to impair normal acquisition of eyeblink conditioning in rabbits (Yeo, et al., 1984; Yeo, et al., 1985). In humans, conditioning levels in patients with damage to lateral lobule VI were reduced compared to controls (Gerwig, et al., 2003), and fMRI findings showed that this area of cortex responds during delay EBC (Cheng, et al., 2008; Ramnani, et al., 2000). The present data revealed two distinct foci of activation within left lateral lobule VI for the children (Table I), suggesting multiple regions of plasticity within this lobule. This finding is consistent with evidence that partial lesions to this structure may not be sufficient to eliminate conditioning completely (Harvey, et al., 1993; Yeo and Hardiman, 1992).

Differential responding in the adult cerebellar cortex has been reported in several neuroimaging studies on human EBC (Blaxton, et al., 1996; Cheng, et al., 2008; Knuttinen, et al., 2002; Logan and Grafton, 1995; Molchan, et al., 1994; Parker, et al., 2012; Ramnani, et al., 2000; Schreurs, et al., 1997), although PET studies have produced varying results - decreases in one study (Molchan, et al., 1994) and increases in another (Logan and Grafton, 1995). Using fMRI and the cerebellar SUIT template (Diedrichsen, 2006), the present study was able to improve localization and report the spatial extent of activation within the cerebellar cortex (Tables I and II). Although Ramnani and colleagues (2000) showed increased fMRI activation in lateral lobule VI in adults, the adults in the current study did not show learning-related activity in this area. This lack of differential activity may reflect a more rapid acquisition due to the relatively simple conditioning protocol. The present study used a single-cue conditioning protocol and reinforced 8/9 trials within each block, whereas Ramnani and colleagues (2000) used a differential conditioning protocol and a 50% reinforcement rate. The greater activation levels in children, who do not learn as well as adults, may reflect an active learning process and are consistent with previous fMRI studies using more complex designs (Cheng, et al., 2008; Ramnani, et al., 2000).

Gerwig et al. (2010) reported that patients with focal and degenerative lesions to lateral lobule VI showed deficits in CR acquisition even after multiple training sessions, indicating that this region is involved in the learning of the CS-US association. The rapid acquisition by the adults in the current study makes it difficult to detect changes in the fMRI signal related to an active learning process. The lack of lateral lobule VI activation in the adults, therefore, does not rule out an important role for this area during acquisition but rather suggests that CR expression, in well-trained participants, does not critically rely on this area. It is possible that if the adult's acquisition rate were reduced (similar to that of the children), acquisition-related activity would have been detected in this area.

Large, significant learning-related activations in right lobule VIII were found in all participants, and age-related comparisons showed that children activated this region bilaterally more than adults. Gerwig et al. (2003) found that patients with lesions in the inferior cerebellar cortex (e.g. lobule VIII) were not as impaired during EBC as patients with lesions to the superior cerebellar cortex (e.g. lobule VI). Thus, lobule VIII may play a supportive, but not critical role during EBC. These authors also found that relative to unilateral cerebellar cortical lesions, bilateral lesions produced modest (but not statistically significant) impairments during EBC, and animal studies suggest that contralateral cerebellar cortical lesions do not affect eyeblink conditioning (Freeman, et al., 1995b). In the present study, bilateral cerebellar cortical activations in children were detected in lobules VI, IX, and Crus I (Table I). In conjunction with lesion data (Freeman, et al., 1995b; Gerwig, et al., 2003), bilateral cerebellar activation in PET work (Blaxton, et al., 1996; Logan and Grafton, 1995), and the presence of bilateral eyeblink CRs (Campolattaro and Freeman, 2009; Disterhoft, et al., 1977), the present neuroimaging findings support the idea that contralateral regions of the cerebellar cortex may play a modulatory but not necessary role during EBC. The widespread fMRI activations in the current study suggest that delay EBC engaged multiple structures throughout the cerebellar cortex (Gerwig, et al., 2010; Plakke, et al., 2007).

Differential activation within the cerebellar deep nuclei was not seen in any conditioning vs. pseudoconditioning comparisons in the present study. This is consistent with the majority of neuroimaging investigations of human eyeblink conditioning (Blaxton, et al., 1996; Cheng, et al., 2008; Knuttinen, et al., 2002; Molchan, et al., 1994; Parker, et al., 2012; Ramnani, et al., 2000; Schreurs, et al., 2001; Schreurs, et al., 1997). Given that the only study to report cerebellar deep nuclear activation during human EBC used PET (Logan and Grafton, 1995), it is possible that current fMRI techniques and analytic approaches may not be optimal for characterizing activity in these regions. The lack of differential activity in the present study may also be due to significant elevated responding in the deep nuclei elicited during pseudoconditioning. Unpaired CS-US presentations appear to be sufficient to activate the deep nuclei due to stimulation of the afferent mossy fiber (pons) and climbing fiber (inferior olive) pathways that transmit the CS and US, respectively (Mauk, et al., 1986; Steinmetz, et al., 1986). Alternatively, although the deep nuclei have been identified as a critical site of plasticity in EBC in laboratory animals (Christian and Thompson, 2003; Lavond, et al., 1985; Lavond, et al., 1984; McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; McCormick and Thompson, 1984b), it is possible that the relative role of the deep nuclei in the circuitry subserving this form of learning differs between species.

Age-Related Changes in Brain Activity

As indicated earlier, despite adults' producing more CRs, children showed greater activation in several regions, including the pons and cerebellar cortical structures (Fig. 4 and Table II). Conversely, adults did not show greater activation in any cerebellar region. The pons are part of the essential neural circuitry underlying EBC and serve as a pathway for auditory CS afferents (Christian and Thompson, 2003). The increased cerebellar activation exhibited by children may represent a more active learning process given their lower conditioning levels as compared to adults. The children's more distributed activation maps compared to adults (Tables I and II) support the idea that children may recruit supplementary structures in order to achieve successful conditioning, which is consistent with studies showing that children exhibit more extensive and diffuse activations to achieve the same level of performance as adults in other neuroimaging tasks as well (De Guio, et al., 2012; Meintjes, et al., 2010; Rivera, et al., 2005).

Behavioral analyses of the latency of peak responses showed that relative to children, adults' peak responses occurred earlier; that is, closer to when the US would have been presented on the non-reinforced CS tone alone trials during conditioning session 1. Despite their poorer timing, children showed greater activation in vermis VI (Table II), which has been linked to cerebellar-mediated timing in finger tapping studies (Spencer, et al., 2007; Stoodley, et al., 2010). Timing of the blink response is critically important in EBC, as poorly timed responses do not fully protect the eye from the airpuff. The significant correlations between activity in vermis VI and CRs during sessions 1 and 2 further confirm the importance of the cerebellar cortex in event timing (Gerwig, et al., 2005; Ivry, et al., 2002; Perrett, et al., 1993; Spencer, et al., 2007) in EBC (Fig. 6). Left anterior lobules IV and V were also more active in all participants. The anterior cerebellar cortex in adult rabbits has been implicated in CR timing (Perrett et al., 1993), which becomes more accurate between adolescence and adulthood in the rat (Brown, et al., 2006).

Neurodevelopmental changes presumably mediate the age-related changes in brain activation during EBC seen in this study. Whole brain volume is not likely to account for these changes because children (7–11 years) have approximately 95% of the volume of the adult brain (Caviness, et al., 1996). However, nonlinear changes in cerebral gray and white matter volume continue throughout the lifespan (Sowell, et al., 2003). Although the greatest changes in myelination in the cerebellar peduncles occur during the first 36 months of human development, a gradual increase in myelination continues through age 11 years (Saksena, et al., 2008). The mechanisms mediating the developmental changes seen here require further investigation.

Behavior and Neuroimaging

The analysis examining the relation between neuroimaging data and behavioral CRs highlighted several regions. Activity in the lateral cerebellar cortex was related to learning rates in children and adults, but these correlations fell short of statistical significance in this small sample. For example, moderate correlations were seen between activation in left lobule VI and session 1 performance in children (Table IIIa) and between right lobule VI activation and session 1 performance in adults (Table IIIb). Other large clusters of activity that were examined in relation to children's CR performance but did not reach statistical significance in this small sample were left Crus II (r = +0.24, p = 0.49) and right lobule VIII (r = +0.35, p = 0.29).

Children's mean activity in the cerebellar deep nuclei in session 1 was significantly correlated with CRs in sessions 1 and 2 (Table IVa). The relation between brain activity during session 1 and behavioral CRs during session 2 suggests that brain processes associated with initial CS-US learning may be predictive of future performance. Deep nuclear activity is critically involved in the acquisition and expression of the behavioral conditioned eyeblink response in laboratory animals (McCormick and Thompson, 1984a; McCormick and Thompson, 1984b). Although the interposed nuclei have been identified as a critical site of plasticity in animal EBC studies (Lavond, et al., 1985), activity within all three sub-nuclei (dentate, fastigial, and interposed) was significantly correlated with conditioned responding in children in this study (Fig. 5). Significant learning-related activation was not detected in the interposed nuclei in adults in this and previous fMRI eyeblink conditioning studies (Cheng, et al., 2008; Knuttinen, et al., 2002; Ramnani, et al., 2000), possibly due to iron accumulation in this region associated with normal development. The MRI signal within the deep cerebellar nuclei is susceptible to iron deposits, and the variability of mean signal intensities in this region also significantly increases with age (Maschke, et al., 2004). An fMRI study in rabbits showed increased activation in the anterior interposed nuclei during eyeblink conditioning (Miller, et al., 2003), and human cerebellar deep nuclei activity, as assessed with PET, was correlated with conditioned eyeblink responses (Logan and Grafton, 1995). This is the first fMRI study to show that activity in the interposed nuclei was significantly correlated with human eyeblink conditioned responding.

Activity in only the left dentate nucleus was significantly correlated with CRs in adults. A secondary analysis revealed that if peak responses within the ROI are used as the index of brain activity, strong positive correlations with behavior are found in the left (r = +0.86, p < 0.003), but not right (r = +0.20, p > 0.20), deep nuclei. These correlations seem to be largely driven by peak activity within the left dentate nucleus (r = +0.73, p < 0.03). It was somewhat unexpected that the dentate and not interposed nuclei showed activity correlated with CRs. Although this specific nucleus is not thought to be crucial for EBC, animal recordings from and lesions to the dentate-interposed nuclear complex (Bracha, et al., 1994; McCormick and Thompson, 1984a) suggest that the dentate activity detected in the present study may contribute to, but is probably not necessary for, EBC.

Limitations

Limitations of the present study include the relatively small sample. Certain correlations reported in Tables III and IV might have reached statistical significance given more power. Another limitation is that our children's age range (9.3–13.8 years), although narrow, may have encompassed more than one stage of neurodevelopment. Finally, the MRI environment, including loud noise and discomfort associated with lying in the bore of the scanner during learning, may have contributed to the relatively low levels of conditioning in this study.

Summary

This is the first fMRI investigation of the neural substrates underlying eyeblink classical conditioning to compare responses in children and adults. Thus, it is the first study to begin to address important questions regarding the ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning, including how the developing brain processes this fundamental learning task. Additional studies are needed to determine when during development brain activity and behavioral responses begin to resemble more mature brain function. These questions may further aid in the diagnoses and treatment of neurodevelopmental disorders known to affect EBC, such as fetal alcohol syndrome (Jacobson, et al., 2011; Jacobson, et al., 2008).

Acknowledgments

We thank Maggie September and Moira Raatz from the University of Cape Town Child Development Research Laboratory and the staff at the Cape Universities Brain Imaging Centre for their contributions to the study.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism at the National Institutes of Health (R01 AA016781 to S.W.J. and K01 AA020873 to D.T.C.) and the South African National Research Chair Initiative.

References

  1. Amunts K, Kedo O, Kindler M, Pieperhoff P, Mohlberg H, Shah NJ, Habel U, Schneider F, Zilles K. Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps. Anat Embryol (Berl) 2005;210:343–52. doi: 10.1007/s00429-005-0025-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berger TW, Alger B, Thompson RF. Neuronal substrate of classical conditioning in the hippocampus. Science. 1976;192:483–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1257783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blaxton TA, Zeffiro TA, Gabrieli JD, Bookheimer SY, Carrillo MC, Theodore WH, Disterhoft JF. Functional mapping of human learning: a positron emission tomography activation study of eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci. 1996;16:4032–40. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-12-04032.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bracha V, Webster ML, Winters NK, Irwin KB, Bloedel JR. Effects of muscimol inactivation of the cerebellar interposed-dentate nuclear complex on the performance of the nictitating membrane response in the rabbit. Exp Brain Res. 1994;100:453–68. doi: 10.1007/BF02738405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brown KL, Pagani JH, Stanton ME. The ontogeny of interstimulus interval (ISI) discrimination of the conditioned eyeblink response in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:1057–70. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.5.1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Campolattaro MM, Freeman JH. Examination of bilateral eyeblink conditioning in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123:1346–52. doi: 10.1037/a0017314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Caviness VS, Jr., Kennedy DN, Richelme C, Rademacher J, Filipek PA. The human brain age 7–11 years: a volumetric analysis based on magnetic resonance images. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:726–36. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.5.726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cheng DT, Disterhoft JF, Power JM, Ellis DA, Desmond JE. Neural substrates underlying human delay and trace eyeblink conditioning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:8108–13. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800374105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cheng DT, Faulkner ML, Disterhoft JF, Desmond JE. The effects of aging in delay and trace human eyeblink conditioning. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:684–90. doi: 10.1037/a0017978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Christian KM, Thompson RF. Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. Learn Mem. 2003;10:427–55. doi: 10.1101/lm.59603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Claflin DI, Stanton ME, Herbert J, Greer J, Eckerman CO. Effect of delay interval on classical eyeblink conditioning in 5-month-old human infants. Dev Psychobiol. 2002;41:329–40. doi: 10.1002/dev.10050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. De Guio F, Jacobson SW, Molteno CD, Jacobson JL, Meintjes EM. Functional magnetic resonance imaging study comparing rhythmic finger tapping in children and adults. Pediatr Neurol. 2012;46:94–100. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2011.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Diedrichsen J. A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage. 2006;33:127–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Diedrichsen J, Maderwald S, Kuper M, Thurling M, Rabe K, Gizewski ER, Ladd ME, Timmann D. Imaging the deep cerebellar nuclei: a probabilistic atlas and normalization procedure. Neuroimage. 2011;54:1786–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dimitrova A, Zeljko D, Schwarze F, Maschke M, Gerwig M, Frings M, Beck A, Aurich V, Forsting M, Timmann D. Probabilistic 3D MRI atlas of the human cerebellar dentate/interposed nuclei. Neuroimage. 2006;30:12–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Disterhoft JF, Kwan HH, Lo WD. Nictitating membrane conditioning to tone in the immobilized albino rabbit. Brain Res. 1977;137:127–43. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)91016-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Finkbiner RG, Woodruff-Pak DS. Classical eyeblink conditioning in adulthood: effects of age and interstimulus interval on acquisition in the trace paradigm. Psychol Aging. 1991;6:109–17. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.6.1.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Forman SD, Cohen JD, Fitzgerald M, Eddy WF, Mintun MA, Noll DC. Improved assessment of significant activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): use of a cluster-size threshold. Magn Reson Med. 1995;33:636–47. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910330508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Freeman JH. Developmental Neurobiology of Cerebellar Learning. In: Blumberg MS, Freeman JH, Robinson SR, editors. Oxford Handbook of Developmental Behavioral Neuroscience. Oxford University Press; New York: 2010. pp. 546–572. [Google Scholar]
  20. Freeman JH, Jr., Barone S, Jr., Stanton ME. Disruption of cerebellar maturation by an antimitotic agent impairs the ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning in rats. J Neurosci. 1995a;15:7301–14. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-11-07301.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Freeman JH, Jr., Carter CS, Stanton ME. Early cerebellar lesions impair eyeblink conditioning in developing rats: differential effects of unilateral lesions on postnatal day 10 or 20. Behav Neurosci. 1995b;109:893–902. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.109.5.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gerwig M, Dimitrova A, Kolb FP, Maschke M, Brol B, Kunnel A, Boring D, Thilmann AF, Forsting M, Diener HC, Timmann D. Comparison of eyeblink conditioning in patients with superior and posterior inferior cerebellar lesions. Brain. 2003;126:71–94. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gerwig M, Guberina H, Esser AC, Siebler M, Schoch B, Frings M, Kolb FP, Aurich V, Beck A, Forsting M, Timmann D. Evaluation of multiple-session delay eyeblink conditioning comparing patients with focal cerebellar lesions and cerebellar degeneration. Behav Brain Res. 2010;212:143–51. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gerwig M, Hajjar K, Dimitrova A, Maschke M, Kolb FP, Frings M, Thilmann AF, Forsting M, Diener HC, Timmann D. Timing of conditioned eyeblink responses is impaired in cerebellar patients. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3919–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0266-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gormezano I. Classical Conditioning. In: Sidowski JB, editor. Experimental Methods and Instrumentation in Psychology. McGraw-Hill Book Co; New York: 1966. pp. 385–420. [Google Scholar]
  26. Harvey JA, Welsh JP, Yeo CH, Romano AG. Recoverable and nonrecoverable deficits in conditioned responses after cerebellar cortical lesions. J Neurosci. 1993;13:1624–35. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-04-01624.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Herbert JS, Eckerman CO, Stanton ME. The ontogeny of human learning in delay, long-delay, and trace eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:1196–210. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ivry RB, Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J. The cerebellum and event timing. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002;978:302–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07576.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jacobson SW, Stanton ME, Dodge NC, Pienaar M, Fuller DS, Molteno CD, Meintjes EM, Hoyme HE, Robinson LK, Khaole N, Jacobson JL. Impaired delay and trace eyeblink conditioning in school-age children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:250–264. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01341.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jacobson SW, Stanton ME, Molteno CD, Burden MJ, Fuller DS, Hoyme HE, Robinson LK, Khaole N, Jacobson JL. Impaired eyeblink conditioning in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:365–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00585.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Knuttinen MG, Parrish TB, Weiss C, LaBar KS, Gitelman DR, Power JM, Mesulam MM, Disterhoft JF. Electromyography as a recording system for eyeblink conditioning with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage. 2002;17:977–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Knuttinen MG, Power JM, Preston AR, Disterhoft JF. Awareness in classical differential eyeblink conditioning in young and aging humans. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115:747–57. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.115.4.747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lavond DG, Hembree TL, Thompson RF. Effect of kainic acid lesions of the cerebellar interpositus nucleus on eyelid conditioning in the rabbit. Brain Res. 1985;326:179–82. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)91400-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lavond DG, Lincoln JS, McCormick DA, Thompson RF. Effect of bilateral lesions of the dentate and interpositus cerebellar nuclei on conditioning of heart-rate and nictitating membrane/eyelid responses in the rabbit. Brain Res. 1984;305:323–30. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(84)90438-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lavond DG, Steinmetz JE. Acquisition of classical conditioning without cerebellar cortex. Behav Brain Res. 1989;33:113–64. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(89)80047-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Logan CG, Grafton ST. Functional anatomy of human eyeblink conditioning determined with regional cerebral glucose metabolism and positron-emission tomography. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92:7500–4. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.16.7500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Maschke M, Weber J, Dimitrova A, Bonnet U, Bohrenkamper J, Sturm S, Kindsvater K, Muller BW, Gastpar M, Diener HC, Forsting M, Timmann D. Age-related changes of the dentate nuclei in normal adults as revealed by 3D fast low angle shot (FLASH) echo sequence magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurol. 2004;251:740–6. doi: 10.1007/s00415-004-0420-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mauk MD, Steinmetz JE, Thompson RF. Classical conditioning using stimulation of the inferior olive as the unconditioned stimulus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1986;83:5349–53. doi: 10.1073/pnas.83.14.5349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McCormick DA, Thompson RF. Cerebellum: essential involvement in the classically conditioned eyelid response. Science. 1984a;223:296–9. doi: 10.1126/science.6701513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McCormick DA, Thompson RF. Neuronal responses of the rabbit cerebellum during acquisition and performance of a classically conditioned nictitating membrane-eyelid response. J Neurosci. 1984b;4:2811–22. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-11-02811.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Meintjes EM, Jacobson JL, Molteno CD, Gatenby JC, Warton C, Cannistraci CJ, Hoyme HE, Robinson LK, Khaole N, Gore JC, Jacobson SW. An FMRI study of number processing in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34:1450–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01230.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Miller MJ, Chen NK, Li L, Tom B, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF, Wyrwicz AM. fMRI of the conscious rabbit during unilateral classical eyeblink conditioning reveals bilateral cerebellar activation. J Neurosci. 2003;23:11753–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-37-11753.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Miller MJ, Li L, Weiss C, Disterhoft JF, Wyrwicz AM. A fiber optic-based system for behavioral eyeblink measurement in a MRI environment. J Neurosci Methods. 2005;141:83–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2004.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Molchan SE, Sunderland T, McIntosh AR, Herscovitch P, Schreurs BG. A functional anatomical study of associative learning in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1994;91:8122–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.17.8122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nolan BC, Freeman JH. Purkinje cell loss by OX7-saporin impairs acquisition and extinction of eyeblink conditioning. Learn Mem. 2006;13:359–65. doi: 10.1101/lm.168506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Parker KL, Andreasen NC, Liu D, Freeman JH, Ponto LL, O'Leary DS. Eyeblink Conditioning in Healthy Adults: A Positron Emission Tomography Study. Cerebellum. 2012 doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0377-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Perrett SP, Ruiz BP, Mauk MD. Cerebellar cortex lesions disrupt learning-dependent timing of conditioned eyelid responses. J Neurosci. 1993;13:1708–18. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-04-01708.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Plakke B, Freeman JH, Poremba A. Metabolic mapping of the rat cerebellum during delay and trace eyeblink conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;88:11–8. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ramnani N, Toni I, Josephs O, Ashburner J, Passingham RE. Learning- and expectation-related changes in the human brain during motor learning. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:3026–35. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.6.3026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Rivera SM, Reiss AL, Eckert MA, Menon V. Developmental changes in mental arithmetic: evidence for increased functional specialization in the left inferior parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1779–90. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Saksena S, Husain N, Malik GK, Trivedi R, Sarma M, Rathore RS, Pandey CM, Gupta RK. Comparative evaluation of the cerebral and cerebellar white matter development in pediatric age group using quantitative diffusion tensor imaging. Cerebellum. 2008;7:392–400. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0041-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Schreurs BG, Bahro M, Molchan SE, Sunderland T, McIntosh AR. Interactions of prefrontal cortex during eyeblink conditioning as a function of age. Neurobiol Aging. 2001;22:237–46. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00224-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Schreurs BG, McIntosh AR, Bahro M, Herscovitch P, Sunderland T, Molchan SE. Lateralization and behavioral correlation of changes in regional cerebral blood flow with classical conditioning of the human eyeblink response. J Neurophysiol. 1997;77:2153–63. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.4.2153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:309–15. doi: 10.1038/nn1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Spence KW, Ross LE. A methodological study of the form and latency of eyelid responses in conditioning. J Exp Psychol. 1959;58:376–81. doi: 10.1037/h0045837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Spencer RM, Verstynen T, Brett M, Ivry R. Cerebellar activation during discrete and not continuous timed movements: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2007;36:378–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Stanton ME, Claflin DI, Herbert J. Ontogeny of Multiple Memory Systems: Eyeblink Conditioning in Rodents and Humans. In: Blumberg MS, Freeman JH, Robinson SR, editors. Oxford Handbook of Developmental Behavioral Neuroscience. Oxford University Press; New York: 2010. pp. 501–526. [Google Scholar]
  58. Stanton ME, Fox GD, Carter CS. Ontogeny of the conditioned eyeblink response in rats: acquisition or expression? Neuropharmacology. 1998;37:623–32. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00072-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Stanton ME, Freeman JH, Jr., Skelton RW. Eyeblink conditioning in the developing rat. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106:657–65. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.4.657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Steinmetz JE, Rosen DJ, Chapman PF, Lavond DG, Thompson RF. Classical conditioning of the rabbit eyelid response with a mossy-fiber stimulation CS: I. Pontine nuclei and middle cerebellar peduncle stimulation. Behav Neurosci. 1986;100:878–87. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.100.6.878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Stoodley CJ, Valera EM, Schmahmann JD. An fMRI study of intra-individual functional topography in the human cerebellum. Behav Neurol. 2010;23:65–79. doi: 10.3233/BEN-2010-0268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Tisdall MD, Hess AT, Reuter M, Meintjes EM, Fischl B, van der Kouwe AJ. Volumetric navigators for prospective motion correction and selective reacquisition in neuroanatomical MRI. Magn Reson Med. 2012;68:389–99. doi: 10.1002/mrm.23228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. van der Kouwe AJ, Benner T, Salat DH, Fischl B. Brain morphometry with multiecho MPRAGE. Neuroimage. 2008;40:559–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Woodruff-Pak DS, Thompson RF. Classical conditioning of the eyeblink response in the delay paradigm in adults aged 18–83 years. Psychol Aging. 1988;3:219–29. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.3.3.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Yeo CH, Hardiman MJ. Cerebellar cortex and eyeblink conditioning: a reexamination. Exp Brain Res. 1992;88:623–38. doi: 10.1007/BF00228191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Yeo CH, Hardiman MJ, Glickstein M. Discrete lesions of the cerebellar cortex abolish the classically conditioned nictitating membrane response of the rabbit. Behav Brain Res. 1984;13:261–6. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(84)90168-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Yeo CH, Hardiman MJ, Glickstein M. Classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit. II. Lesions of the cerebellar cortex. Exp Brain Res. 1985;60:99–113. doi: 10.1007/BF00237023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES