Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 May 22.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2015 Jun;15(2):276–286. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0324-5

Diminished neural responses predict enhanced intrinsic motivation and sensitivity to external incentive

Karen E Marsden a,1, Wei Ji Ma b,1, Edward L Deci c, Richard M Ryan c, Pearl H Chiu d,e,*
PMCID: PMC4412758  NIHMSID: NIHMS637280  PMID: 25348668

Abstract

The duration and quality of human performance depend on both intrinsic motivation and external incentives. However, little is known about the neuroscientific basis of this interplay between internal and external motivators. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the neural substrates of intrinsic motivation, operationalized as free-choice time spent on a task when not required, and tested the neural and behavioral effects of external reward on intrinsic motivation. We found that increased duration of free-choice time was predicted by generally diminished neural response in regions associated with cognitive and affective regulation. By comparison, the possibility of additional reward improved task accuracy and specifically increased neural and behavioral responses following errors. Those individuals with the smallest neural responses associated with intrinsic motivation exhibited the greatest error-related neural enhancement under the external contingency of possible reward. Together, these data suggest a ‘tonic’ and ‘phasic’ relationship between the neural substrates of intrinsic motivation (tonic) and the impact of external incentives (phasic) that guides human performance.

Keywords: intrinsic motivation, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), internal and external incentives, neural substrates, behavioral performance

Introduction

The nature of human motivation has long intrigued scientists and practitioners in the areas of decision-making (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), educational psychology (Dweck, 1986), artificial intelligence (Barto, Singh, & Chentanez, 2004; Kaplan & Oudeyer, 2007), economic behavior (Bénabou & Tirole, 2003; Camerer & Hogarth, 1999), and the pathophysiology and therapy of psychiatric illnesses (Der-Avakian & Markou, 2012; Miller & Rollnick, 2002). Data from these areas emphasize that human motivation derives from internal and external sources that operate on differing time-scales, and that can complement or compete with one another to influence both the duration and quality of behavioral performance (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Dweck, 1986). However, understanding the neuroscientific basis of the interplay between internal and external motivators has been challenging, in part due to the use of varied, often subjective, measures of intrinsic motivation. Here, we operationalize intrinsic motivation as free-choice time spent on a task when not required (Deci et al., 1999); this metric guides our investigation of the neural substrates of intrinsic motivation and the neurobehavioral effects of external rewards.

A robust literature indicates that actions are motivated by external rewards through a neural learning system in which decisions are reinforced by outcomes that are better (or worse) than expected (Pessiglione, Seymour, Flandin, Dolan, & Frith, 2006). These neural ‘prediction errors’ guide learning and maximize an agent’s reward over time (Dayan & Balleine, 2002; Montague, King-Casas, & Cohen, 2006; Sutton & Barto, 1998), and anomalies in value-guided decisions have been implicated in psychiatric conditions including depression and substance dependence (Chiu & Deldin, 2007; Chiu, Lohrenz, & Montague, 2008). In contrast, very little is known about the neural substrates supporting intrinsic motivation, despite the central role of internal factors for guiding behavior (for a few studies that report neural responses to stimuli consistent with participants’ self-described internal motivation, see (Bengtsson, Lau, & Passingham, 2009; Linke et al., 2010; Mizuno et al., 2008)).

In general terms, actions are considered intrinsically motivated if an agent engages in behavior for its own sake, without attempting to attain external consequences (Barto et al., 2004; Kaplan & Oudeyer, 2007). In behavioral studies, intrinsic motivation has been linked to more positive self-reported experience (Leary, 2007), and from an evolutionary perspective, intrinsic motivation is thought to facilitate exploration and lead to knowledge or skills that confer a fitness advantage (Berlyne, 1966; Hebb, 1955). When tasks are not inherently interesting, external incentives may be offered to evoke performance (Camerer & Hogarth, 1999), and moderate external incentives can improve behavioral output when rewards are operative (Lazear, 2000; Murayama, Matsumoto, Izuma, & Matsumoto, 2010), though performance decrements have been observed when incentives are large (Ariely, Gneezy, Loewenstein, & Mazar, 2009; Chib, De Martino, Shimojo, & O’Doherty, 2012). This phenomenon was explored in a provocative study showing that activity in neural regions involved in extrinsic reward valuation was excessively diminished when rewards were removed, and that the diminished neural activity paralleled decreases in intrinsic motivation (Murayama et al., 2010). Together, these data highlight that motivated behavior may be characterized on at least the axes of duration and quality of output, neither of which alone is sufficient for evolutionary fitness, but each of which may be influenced by both internal and external factors.

To examine the neural underpinnings of intrinsic motivation and the interplay with external incentives, we measured intrinsic motivation as free-choice time spent on a word-problem task beyond the expectations of the experimenter and informed a subgroup of participants that performance on the task would be rewarded (after Ryan & Deci, 2000). Using this paradigm and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we report a network of brain regions in which diminished task-related activity predicts increased subsequent intrinsic motivation and show that external contingencies enhance behavioral performance and neural responses in these regions for task-critical events, particularly in the most intrinsically motivated participants.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty-three right-handed, MRI-compatible male and female participants were recruited to participate in a neuroimaging study involving word problem solving. Participants were limited to native speakers of English due to the nature of the task, which required knowledge of common English phrases. All participants gave written informed consent and were paid for their participation. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Baylor College of Medicine and Virginia Tech. Three participants were excluded from the fMRI data analysis: one who moved excessively during the scan, and two whose data were contaminated by scanner artifact. Thus 40 individuals were included in the imaging analysis, and 43 were included in the behavioral analysis.

Experimental session: Overview

The experimental session was divided into three sections: a practice period, a performance period, and a “free-choice period” (see Figure 1a for schematic representation). Each section of the task was performed in the scanner, thus allowing consistency of procedures and timing between the sections across participants. In the scanner, bimanual two-button optical response boxes allowed participants to navigate a cursor and submit responses on an onscreen keyboard (developed with Psychophysics Toolbox Version 3). All participants began with the practice period during which they acclimated to unambiguous, single-solution, remote associates-type word problems (Mednick & Mednick, 1967) and typing on the onscreen keyboard (Figure 1b).

Figure 1. Schematic depiction of experimental flow and remote associates word problems task.

Figure 1

(a) Following a 9-minute practice period in which participants practiced completing remote associates word problems, participants were assigned to a “No Reward” or “Reward” group. During the performance period, all participants completed additional remote associates word problems. The session ended with a 5-minute free-choice period during which participants were given the option to complete more problems, turn off the screen, or read an archived digital newspaper. “Intrinsic motivation” was operationalized as percent of free-choice time spent completing word problems during this free-choice period. (b) For each word problem, participants were presented with two words and asked to complete the blank with another word that forms a common phrase or compound word with each of the two onscreen words. Participants typed their responses with an onscreen keyboard. Upon submitting an answer, participants were shown onscreen feedback indicating “The most common answer is [‘ship’].” Word problem trial onsets were the events of interest in the present analysis.

Remote associates word problems

Remote associate-type word problems (Mednick and Mednick, 1967; Bowden and Jung-Beeman, 2003) were used in each phase of the study. For each word problem, participants were presented with two words separated by a blank and asked to complete the blank with another word that forms a common phrase or compound word when it follows the first word and when it precedes the second word. For example, a solution to the problem “SUN _____ HOUSE” would be “LIGHT” because “sunlight” and “lighthouse” are both common words. At the end of each trial, the most common solution was displayed (e.g., “The most common answer is ‘light’.”) Participants were instructed to try to complete the puzzle with the most common solution, and all responses were typed with the onscreen keyboard. Each remote associates problem was displayed until one of three criteria was met: the participant submitted an answer, the participant chose to pass on the trial, or a 60 second time limit for the trial was reached. Feedback display time was jittered between 4 and 6 seconds. The items were presented in a random sequence, and no participant ever saw the same item more than once throughout the entire laboratory visit. Additional details about the remote associates word problems are provided in the Supplementary Online Materials.

Measuring intrinsic motivation and providing external incentive

Following the practice, and just prior to the performance period, participants were randomly assigned to one of two external incentive groups: “Reward” or “No Reward.” The “Reward” participants (N = 21, 8 males and 13 females, mean age 24.6 years; N = 19 included in imaging analysis) were instructed that upon task completion they would receive a monetary bonus based on their performance during the study, and the “No Reward” participants (N = 22, 8 males and 14 females, mean age 25.0 years; N = 21 included in imaging analysis) were not offered any bonus. Participants then moved on to the nine-minute performance period, which involved further remote associates problems (mean per puzzle solution time 20.2 ± 5.3 s).

After the performance period, the study concluded with a five-minute free-choice period in which the following options were presented onscreen: (i) read a recently archived digital news site, (ii) clear the screen and wait for the experimenter, or (iii) continue with more remote associates problems. During the free-choice period, participants could alternate freely among these three free-choice options. Upon task completion, participants’ intrinsic motivation to perform remote associates problems was measured as the percentage of time they chose to spend on word problems during the free-choice period. For categorical analyses, participants who spent at least 50 percent of this period on remote associates problems were considered the “High Intrinsic” group. Prior to departure, participants completed a post-task questionnaire that contained questions about participants’ interest, perceived control, and perceived competence (items from the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory; Ryan & Deci, 2000) in completing the puzzles.

Behavioral analyses

The behavioral analyses focused on intrinsic motivation and task performance under the external contingency of possible reward (or not). As described above, intrinsic motivation was quantified as the percentage of time each participant spent completing remote associates problems during the free-choice period, and the incentive treatment was categorical, with a Reward group and a No Reward group. Measures of performance included cumulative accuracy and response time, as well as accuracy and response time coded by the accuracy of current and immediately subsequent trials (e.g., accuracy and response time after correct trials, accuracy and response time after incorrect trials).

Image acquisition and analyses

General linear model analyses of the imaging data were first conducted with the trial onset events to identify neural responses associated with intrinsic motivation, defined as free-choice time spent on the word puzzles when no longer required by the experimenter. The identified regions were then used in region-of-interest (ROI) analyses to examine the effects of possible reward (external incentive) on the neural substrates and associated behavior of intrinsic motivation. Given the high inter-correlation (discussed below, Results) among neural activity in the identified ROIs, we performed principal components analyses (PCA) to identify a single metric for the shared variability in response across the network of regions associated with intrinsic motivation. The first components derived from the PCAs were used in subsequent analyses relating neural activity to behavior. Additional details are below.

Continuous whole-brain imaging was implemented with a Siemens 3.0 Tesla Trio scanner. Headphones reduced the noise from the scanner and allowed participants to hear instructions. Participants were provided MRI-compatible glasses as needed. Initial high-resolution T1-weighted scans were acquired using an MP-RAGE sequence (Siemens). Functional run acquisition parameters were as follows: echo-planar imaging, gradient recalled echo, repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, 64 × 64 matrix, slice thickness = 4mm, FoV = 220 mm, 34 slices acquired hyperangulated 30° from the anteroposterior commissural line. Scanning yielded functional 3.4 mm × 3.4 mm × 4.0 mm voxels.

Data reduction and analysis of images were performed using SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK). Before analysis, the images were realigned, normalized, coregistered, segmented, and smoothed (6 × 6 × 6 mm). The imaging data for each participant was then fit to a general linear model which included six event regressors: trial onset and answer reveal following correct and incorrect trials, and the first button press following trial onset for correct and incorrect trials. “Trial onset” was defined as the time at which the remote associates problem was presented, and the “answer reveal” event was the time at which the solution to the problem was displayed. The onset events during the performance period were the primary focus of the present analyses. Events were modeled separately and convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. Two participants had no errors and were not included in the imaging analyses specific to errors. For first and second-level whole brain analyses, statistical significance was defined as cluster size ≥ 5, p ≤ 0.001 uncorrected, and p ≤ 0.05, FDR corrected for multiple comparisons.

The results of the Low Intrinsic > High Intrinsic contrast identified six key regions that predicted intrinsic motivation for word problems, regardless of extrinsic Reward group; these regions were used in subsequent region-of-interest analyses as associated with intrinsic motivation. As a conservative estimate of ROI values, average beta coefficients from the ROIs were extracted from the smaller of (1) the total cluster of significant activation in the anatomical region or (2) the 5 mm radius centered at the peak significant voxel in that region from the Low Intrinsic > High Intrinsic contrast. Neural structures were defined anatomically using Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL), and ROIs were extracted from the total cluster of significant activation in bilateral amygdala and caudate, and from anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), and anterior and posterior insula as bilateral spheres around peak voxels, all from the Low Intrinsic > High Intrinsic contrast. The PHG ROI was adjusted to exclude any parts of the amygdala that may have been included in the spheres. Average beta coefficients for these ROIs were extracted and analyzed.

Principal components analysis (MATLAB R2010b) was conducted on the matrix comprising the beta coefficients for trial onsets for each subject and each ROI with columns z-standardized to have zero mean and unit variance. An analogous PCA was conducted on the matrix comprising the neural error reactivity values for each subject and each ROI where neural error reactivity was defined for each subject as the difference in beta coefficients for trial onsets immediately following errors and trial onsets for the current trial on which errors occurred.

Results

Behavioral metrics of intrinsic motivation

Intrinsic motivation was quantified following the experimental session as the percentage of free-choice time participants spent on remote associates word problems, with greater percent time working on problems indicating greater intrinsic motivation toward the word problems (for review, see (Deci et al., 1999)). The distribution of free-choice time was bimodal rather than normal across all participants (N = 43), with roughly half of the participants spending at least 50% free-choice time on remote associates problems and half spending less than 50% (Figure 2), consistent with previous work (Wiechman & Gurland, 2009). This 50% criterion defined our “High” (N = 20) and “Low” intrinsic motivation (N = 23) groups. Thus, participants in the High Intrinsic group, relative to the Low Intrinsic group, allocated more time to attempting to complete word problems during the free-choice period (90.0% and 11.2%, respectively). The High and Low Intrinsic groups did not differ in number of problems completed, response time, age, gender composition, baseline mood assessed with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, or verbal IQ assessed with the Wechsler Test for Adult Reading (all two-tailed between-group comparisons p > 0.05). The two groups also performed equally well on the remote associates problems during the performance period (p = 0.9) and were equally represented in the Reward and No Reward conditions (9 of 21 Reward participants and 11 of 22 No Reward participants were High Intrinsic; χ2 = 0.22, ns). Post-task self-report questions that assessed participants’ interest, perceived competence, and perceived control in completing the puzzles revealed that the two groups perceived the task as equally enjoyable and interesting and themselves as similarly competent (all between group comparisons p > 0.1), and that the high intrinsic group showed a trend toward greater perceived autonomy (i.e., “I did these word puzzles because I wanted to;” p = 0.054, two-tailed, uncorrected). Free-choice time was not related to behavioral accuracy (Spearman rho = −0.08, p = 0.6; Figure S1).

Figure 2. Distribution of participants’ intrinsic motivation to complete remote associates word problems (N = 43).

Figure 2

Each participant’s intrinsic motivation to complete word problems was quantified as the percentage of the total free-choice period spent on the problems. Participants exhibited a bimodal distribution of free-choice time. For categorical analyses, those who spent at least 50% of the free-choice period completing word problems were defined as intrinsically motivated (“High Intrinsic”), and those who spent less than 50% of the free-choice period completing puzzles were considered low in intrinsic motivation to complete word problems (“Low Intrinsic”).

Neural predictors of intrinsic motivation

To identify neural regions associated with intrinsic motivation, we first compared blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) activity to trial onsets during the performance period between participants high and low in intrinsic motivation, regardless of extrinsic Reward group. Specifically, a two-sample t-test was conducted between all participants who spent at least 50 percent (High Intrinsic motivation) of the free-choice period on word problems and those who spent less than 50 percent (Low Intrinsic motivation) of the free-choice period on the word problems. This whole-brain contrast identified a distinct pattern comprising six major brain structures: amygdala, caudate, ACC, PHG, anterior insula, and posterior insula (t(38) = 3.4; p ≤ 0.0008 uncorrected; p ≤ 0.05, FDR-corrected; Cohen’s d = 1.1; Figure 3). In each of these regions, the High Intrinsic group exhibited significantly diminished neural responses relative to the Low Intrinsic group (Figure 3). The six primary regions identified in this contrast were used in subsequent region-of-interest (ROI) analyses as regions associated with diminished intrinsic motivation.

Figure 3. High intrinsic motivation participants show diminished neural responses.

Figure 3

SPM8 t-maps of neural responses to puzzle onset during the puzzle performance period, across the Reward and No Reward groups; two-sample t-test of low intrinsic motivation > high intrinsic motivation groups (t(38) = 3.4; p = 0.0008 uncorrected; p = 0.05, FDR-corrected). The six key regions identified in this contrast (anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala, anterior and posterior insula, PHG, and caudate) were used in subsequent region-of-interest analyses as regions associated with intrinsic motivation.

The BOLD response to trial onsets was highly correlated among the six regions of interest (range r = 0.47 to 0.85), suggesting that responses in these regions were collectively related to intrinsic motivation for word-problems. Thus, to identify a single metric accounting for the shared variability in response across neural regions, we performed a principal component analysis on the average trial-onset beta coefficients in each of these regions for each participant. The first principal component accounted for a majority (72.4%) of the total variance in the six ROIs’ response to trial onsets; each of the six ROIs’ beta coefficients is highly correlated with the PCA first component scores (r from .78 to .89), suggesting that no single region’s response dominated the component. We thus used the first component scores from participants’ trial onset events as a composite measure of each subject’s neural response related to intrinsic motivation. No relationship was observed between participants’ behavioral accuracy and trial-onset component scores (r = −0.08, p = 0.63) or between accuracy and beta coefficients in any of the six regions (r range −0.01 to −0.26, all p > 0.05).

Effects of external incentive on neural substrates of intrinsic motivation

To examine the impact of external contingencies on task performance and neural substrates of intrinsic motivation, we assigned half of the participants to a “Reward” condition in which they were instructed that they could receive a monetary bonus based on their final task performance. A univariate ANOVA with two between-group factors (Intrinsic, Reward) revealed that the potential for reward did not affect participants’ subsequent free-choice time on remote associates problems (F(1,42) = 0.25, p = 0.62; Figure 4a). In contrast, a significant effect of reward on accuracy was observed, such that participants who were offered reward for performance (“Reward”) showed significantly greater accuracy than those who were not (“No Reward”), regardless of intrinsic motivation status (F(1,39) = 11.33; p = 0.002; partial η2 = 0.27; Figure 4b).

Figure 4. External reward incentive does not affect intrinsic motivation but enhances accuracy, particularly following incorrect responses.

Figure 4

Participants in the “Reward” group were instructed that they would receive a monetary bonus based on final task performance. “Intrinsic” participants are those who spent at least half of the free-choice time on remote associates problems. This allowed us to identify four distinct subgroups who were: (i) low in intrinsic motivation and not offered reward (“No Motivation,” red bar), (ii) low in intrinsic motivation and offered an external reward for performance (“Reward Only,” yellow bar), (iii) intrinsically motivated without additional reward (“Intrinsic Only,” blue bar), or (iv) both intrinsically motivated and offered the additional reward for performance (“Intrinsic & Reward,” green bar). (a) External reward incentive did not impact the amount of free-choice time that participants allocated to completing puzzles during the free-choice period (F(1, 21) = 0.0930, p = 0.48 and F(1, 18) = 0.009, p = 0.92, respectively for Low and High Intrinsic participants). (b) In contrast, Reward group participants showed greater accuracy during the puzzle performance period than did No Reward participants, regardless of intrinsic motivation status (F(1,36) = 13.6, p = .001). (c) The Reward group exhibited enhanced accuracy following incorrect, relative to correct, responses (F(1, 20) = 19.44, p < .0001), regardless of intrinsic motivation, whereas the No Reward group showed no performance facilitation (F(1,21) = .70, p = .41). Error bars show standard error of the mean.

The rich literature in reinforcement learning suggested the hypothesis that performance improvement occurs via prediction error signaling of outcomes as “better or worse than expected,” and led us to examine behavioral performance following correct and error trials. This analysis indicated that externally incentivized participants (Reward) exhibited improved accuracy following error trials relative to correct trials (F(1,20) = 19.44, p = 0.0003; partial η2 = 0.49), while those who did not receive external incentive (No Reward) showed no performance boost (F(1,21) = 0.70, p = 0.41; partial η2 = 0.03; Figure 4c). To assess the nature of this behavioral enhancement, for each of the six ROIs identified to be related to intrinsic motivation, we computed a neural “error reactivity” measure defined as the difference between general linear model (GLM) beta coefficients for trial onsets immediately following errors and beta coefficients for trial onsets on which errors occurred (post-error trial onset beta minus error trial onset beta; schematically depicted in Figure 6b). An analogous measure of “correct reactivity” was similarly defined as the difference between post-correct trial onset beta values and correct trial onset neural beta values. The BOLD responses in the six ROIs were highly correlated within the error reactivity and correct reactivity trials, respectively (range, r = 0.49 to 0.81), and we again performed PCA on the matrices comprising each participant’s neural error and correct reactivity to yield composite metrics of neural error reactivity and correct reactivity, respectively, for each participant. Again, responses in each ROI were significantly correlated with the first component scores (range, r = 0.77 to 0.92), indicating no single region likely dominates the components.

Figure 6. Neural responses associated with intrinsic motivation are negatively related with neural error reactivity.

Figure 6

(a) Those who exhibit smaller BOLD responses to general trial onsets (i.e., those with higher intrinsic motivation) demonstrate greater BOLD responses in reacting to trials following errors, and vice versa (r = −0.39, p = 0.016, two-tailed; between component scores from the two PCAs). (b) A schematic depiction illustrates the hypothesis that high intrinsic motivation and its associated diminished general neural responses facilitate enhanced neural responses during critical task periods (here, post-error responding). The blue line illustrates a low general neural response to trial onsets, which allows a higher incremental neural response following errors under external incentives, as posited for those with high intrinsic motivation. In comparison, the orange line illustrates a greater response to general trial onsets, which limits the neural reactivity to errors, as suggested for those with low intrinsic motivation. As detailed throughout, the Reward participants show enhanced neural error reactivity, and the data together suggest a ‘tonic’ and ‘phasic’ relationship between the neural substrates of intrinsic motivation (tonic) and the impact of external incentives (phasic). Behavioral response events were modeled in the neural GLM analyses but for simplicity are not illustrated here.

A 2 × 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA on the first neural PCA components with two between group factors (Intrinsic: High, Low; Reward: No Reward, Reward) and the within-group factor of trial type (error, correct) revealed a significant interaction (F(1, 33) = 4.29, p = .046; partial η2 = 0.12) that was driven by differences in the Reward and No Reward groups, specifically for error trials (F(1, 36) = 6.26, p = .017; partial η2 = 0.15; Figure 5). No effects of Reward or intrinsic motivation were observed for neural correct reactivity (see Figure S2 for Reward group effects by ROI for correct and error reactivity). This increased neural reactivity in Reward participants specifically to errors parallels the improved behavioral accuracy this group showed following incorrect responses. Participants’ neural error reactivity scores regressed against behavioral accuracy following errors revealed that increased neural error reactivity significantly predicted improved accuracy following errors (r = 0.40, p = 0.013, two-tailed), suggesting that external contingencies selectively enhance effortful performance when needed, for example following errors when improved performance is necessary to attain the reward. Post-hoc analyses suggest that the increased neural error reactivity conferred by external reward was evident in participants with High Intrinsic motivation and not in the Low Intrinsic group; see Supplemental Materials and Figure S3).

Figure 5. External incentive enhances neural responses following errors.

Figure 5

For each participant, neural error (or correct) reactivity was quantified as the difference in neural response (beta coefficients) for trial onsets immediately following errors (or correct responses) and trial onsets for the current trial on which errors (or correct responses) subsequently occurred. (a) A repeated measures ANOVA on the first neural PCA components for error and correct reactivity with two between group factors (Intrinsic: High, Low; Reward: No Reward, Reward) and the within-group factor of trial type (error, correct) revealed a significant interaction (F(1, 33) = 4.29, p = .046; partial η2 = 0.12) that was driven by enhanced neural error reactivity in the Reward group (F(1, 36) = 6.26, p = .017; partial η2 = 0.15). No effects of Reward or intrinsic motivation were observed for neural correct reactivity. Error bars show standard error of the mean.

Finally, to assess the interplay between neural substrates of intrinsic motivation and neural error reactivity conferred by external incentive, we correlated participants’ first principal component scores of neural activity associated with general trial onsets (predictive of less intrinsic motivation) with the first principal component scores of neural error reactivity. This revealed a significant negative relationship (r = −0.39, p = 0.016, two-tailed; Figure 6a), such that those who exhibited smaller responses to general trial onsets (greater intrinsic motivation) demonstrated greater neural responses to trials following errors. Moreover, those with smaller neural responses to general trial onsets (more intrinsic motivation) also showed shorter response time and increased accuracy following errors (r = 0.41, p = 0.009 and r = −0.35, p = 0.027, respectively). These data support the hypothesis that intrinsic motivation and its associated diminished general neural responses facilitate enhanced neural responses and behavioral performance during critical task periods (here, post-error responding). This possibility is schematically illustrated in Figure 6b wherein the blue line illustrates a low general neural response to trial onsets, which allows a higher incremental neural response following errors, and the orange line illustrates a greater response to general trial onsets, which limits the neural reactivity to errors.

As the regions’ variance that loads onto the first PCA component of the intrinsic motivation analysis may differ from the regions loading onto the first component of the neural error reactivity scores, we regressed neural error reactivity responses in each of the 6 ROIs against the intrinsic motivation PCA first component scores to clarify the regions that may facilitate the negative relationship. This analysis indicated that the association between intrinsic motivation and the effects of external incentive are particularly prominent in the neural error reactivity responses in caudate (r = −.44, p = .006) and anterior cingulate (r = −.48, p = .002). This pattern is intriguing as large literatures highlight the role of caudate and anterior cingulate cortex in error signaling, particularly when task performance is incentivized (e.g., Kerns et al., 2004; Montague et al., 2006); this is further discussed below.

Exploratory Analyses

While we have focused this manuscript on the neural substrates involved in intrinsic motivation and how external incentives affect processing in these regions, we also conducted several post-hoc exploratory analyses to address how differences in neural responding to external rewards may affect the relationship between external incentives and intrinsic motivation (detailed in Supplemental Materials, Supplementary Results). In brief summary, these analyses revealed greater activity specifically in right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) in the No Reward > Reward comparison at trial onsets (p < .001, uncorrected). We next compared participants who (i) ranked above the median on both intrinsic motivation and Reward group neural responses (first component of the intrinsic motivation PCA and beta coefficients extracted from the Reward-related IFG ROI, respectively) and (ii) those who ranked below the median on both neural measures. On both free-choice time and behavioral accuracy after errors, the ‘Low’ neural response group showed optimal performance (greater free-choice time, and greater behavioral accuracy following incorrect responses). IFG is typically activated during response inhibition (for review, see Aron et al., 2014), and these data support the hypothesis that during task performance, reduced neural responses in regions associated with neurocognitive/affective regulation may be a biomarker for a subset of individuals who are both intrinsically motivated and sensitive to the performance boosts that external incentives confer following errors.

Discussion

Taken together, these data identify neural predictors of intrinsic motivation and begin to clarify the dynamics between intrinsic motivation and external incentives. The regions in which less general task-related activity is associated with subsequent greater intrinsic motivation—amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus, anterior and posterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and caudate—have separately and collectively been implicated in affective-regulation, cognitive control and learning (Craig, 2009; Critchley, Wiens, Rotshtein, Ohman, & Dolan, 2004; Kerns et al., 2004; LeDoux, 2012; Leotti & Delgado, 2011; Montague et al., 2006; Sheth et al., 2012). While our present paradigm is unable to parse the specific contributions of these respective neural functions to intrinsic motivation, the consistent pattern of diminished neural activity in those with high intrinsic motivation fits with other data demonstrating that activating affective and cognitive control tends to diminish subsequent self-regulation and intrinsic motivation (Baumeister, Muraven, & Tice, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Following from this, in our case, decreased activation of neural affective/cognitive control may facilitate increased intrinsic motivation, here measured as task persistence.

In concert, the addition of performance-based incentive enhanced participants’ neural and behavioral sensitivity to errors. Specifically, the Reward participants did not show enhanced free-choice time, but did show enhanced neural reactivity to errors in the network of regions implicated in intrinsic motivation, and this reactivity for Reward participants in turn predicted behavioral improvement on trials subsequent to errors (see Laming, 1979 for a classic report of post-error behavioral improvement). These neural and behavioral data fit with models of reinforcement learning, which highlight the role of striatal regions, including caudate, in neural error signaling of outcomes that are better or worse than expected in learning to maximize rewards over time (for review, see Dayan & Balleine, 2002; Montague et al., 2006). Extensive literatures also implicate the anterior cingulate cortex in post-error processing and behavioral adjustments (Kerns et al., 2004; Sheth et al., 2012), and the amygdala and insular cortices in regulating affective responses to and interoceptive awareness of errors (Craig, 2009; Critchley et al., 2004). Of note, the enhanced neural error reactivity evoked by external incentive was most prominent in those with the smallest neural responses associated with intrinsic motivation (i.e., the High Intrinsic group (Figure 6a; Figure S3). Behaviorally though, Reward participants showed greater accuracy following errors (mean over 90% correct), regardless of intrinsic motivation status. This overall high accuracy in Reward participants suggests the possibility that a task with a higher ‘ceiling’ may allow an even greater behavioral facilitation effect in High Intrinsic participants that matches the increased neural reactivity seen in this group, yielding individuals who perform with both duration and increased quality when appropriate extrinsic incentives are provided. This remains an intriguing topic for future investigation.

The current data suggest complementary neurobehavioral processes: greater intrinsic motivation is associated with generally diminished neural responses that extend task engagement, while external contingencies enhance neural sensitivity and behavioral adjustments specifically to failure events. In particular, an overall state of dampened neurocognitive regulation, as suggested by the lower general task-related neural responses in our High Intrinsic group, may facilitate enhanced neural and behavioral responses when needed, as in the case of errors, and when evoked by positive external contingencies for doing well. That is, in line with a “resource depletion” framework (Baumeister et al., 2000; Muraven, Shmueli, & Burkley, 2006; Vohs & Heatherton, 2000), it may be the case that individuals high in intrinsic motivation for an activity tend to “conserve resources,” which renders these neurocognitive resources available when needed during critical task periods. Consonant with this hypothesis, we observed a significant negative relationship between general neural responses to trial onsets and specific neural reactivity following errors, such that those who exhibit diminished responses to general trial onsets also demonstrate enhanced neural responses on trials following errors. The apparently distinct functions of the neural substrates of intrinsic motivation (associated with general task processing) and the neural impact of external contingencies (associated with task-related error events) are also notable for their consistency with molecular and computational model-based accounts of ‘tonic’ and ‘phasic’ neuromodulatory functions that subserve motivation and learning (including serotonergic, dopaminergic, and norepinephrine systems (Aston-Jones & Cohen, 2005; Barto et al., 2004; Daw, Kakade, & Dayan, 2002; Howe, Tierney, Sandberg, Phillips, & Graybiel, 2013; Montague et al., 2006).

Future considerations

Human motivation derives from internal and external sources that can complement one another to influence both the duration and quality of behavioral performance, although under some conditions they may be antagonistic (Deci et al., 1999; Murayama et al., 2010). Indeed, external rewards have been related to impaired error processing in mood, anxiety, and substance-related disorders (Chiu & Deldin, 2007; Chiu et al., 2008; Paulus, Feinstein, Simmons, & Stein, 2004) and shown to undermine intrinsic motivation in some cases (Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973; Murayama et al., 2010), but not in others (Cooke et al., 2011; Kremer, Miguel, & Thornton, 2009) see (Deci et al., 1999; Wiechman & Gurland, 2009) for a thorough discussion of these issues), thus emphasizing the need to consider both the benefits and costs of manipulating external incentives. Also, we caution that the present data are unable to establish a causal relationship between neural responses and intrinsic motivation; that is, it may be the case that the highly intrinsically motivated conserve neural resources for use during critical events or that activating neural control during general task performance diminishes intrinsic motivation. Additional studies are needed to differentiate these possibilities. Finally, our findings of increased neural signaling and behavioral accuracy following error trials particularly in High Intrinsic + Reward participants may be relevant to computational models of learning and theories of motivation used in computer intelligence (e.g., Dayan & Balleine, 2002; Kaplan & Oudeyer, 2007; Sutton & Barto, 1998). Future investigations that incorporate learning of stimulus-outcome contingencies and valuation of objective rewards with measures of intrinsic motivation will facilitate the use of formal learning models to test these theories.

Conclusions

Our data point to the potential importance of considering intrinsic motivation in error signaling under external incentives. The results indicate that greater intrinsic motivation, quantified here as free-choice time on a task, is predicted by lower general task-related neural activity, suggesting less tonic expenditure of neural resources in individuals high in intrinsic motivation. In comparison, external incentive improves task performance (here, accuracy) and yields increased phasic neural sensitivity to errors. This dissociation points to an interesting subset of individuals in the present data—those who evidenced high intrinsic motivation (low general task-related neural activity) and at the same time exhibited behavioral and neural benefits from the offer of external reward for good performance, suggesting the possibility of a biomarker for individuals who perform with an optimal combination of duration and quality when appropriate extrinsic incentives are provided. Future clarification of the neurocomputational mechanisms that underlie intrinsic motivation and the impact of external contingencies will inform our understanding of the dynamics between internal and external factors on performance in the laboratory, schoolroom, corporate office, and clinic.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Materials

Acknowledgments

Funding

This work was partially supported by the National Institutes of Health (MH091872 and MH087692 to PC).

The authors gratefully acknowledge discussions with Brooks King-Casas, George Christopoulos, Jenny Lu, Stephen LaConte, Read Montague, and the technical assistance of LaRaun Lindsey, Nina Lauharatanahirun, and Katie McCurry.

Footnotes

Competing Financial Interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Ariely D, Gneezy U, Loewenstein G, Mazar N. Large stakes and big mistakes. The Review of Economic Studies. 2009;76:451–469. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aron A, Robbins TW, Poldrack R. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2014;18:177–85. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2005;28:403–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barto AG, Singh S, Chentanez N. Intrinsically motivated learning of hierarchical collections of skills. International Conference on Developmental Learning; LaJolla, CA. 2004. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baumeister RF, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: A resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled processing. Social Cognition. 2000;18:130–150. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bénabou R, Tirole J. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Review of Economic Studies. 2003;70:489–520. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bengtsson SL, Lau HC, Passingham RE. Motivation to do well enhances responses to errors and self-monitoring. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:797–804. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berlyne DE. Curiosity and exploration. Science. 1966;153:25–33. doi: 10.1126/science.153.3731.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Camerer CF, Hogarth RM. The effects of financial incentives in experiments: A review and capital-labor-production framework. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. 1999;19:7–42. [Google Scholar]
  10. Chib VS, De Martino B, Shimojo S, O’Doherty JP. Neural mechanisms underlying paradoxical performance for monetary incentives are driven by loss aversion. Neuron. 2012;74(3):582–594. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chiu PH, Deldin PJ. Neural evidence for enhanced error detection in major depressive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164(4):608–616. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chiu PH, Lohrenz TM, Montague PR. Smokers’ brains compute, but ignore, a fictive error signal in a sequential investment task. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11(4):514–520. doi: 10.1038/nn2067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cooke LJ, Chambers LC, Anez EV, Croker HA, Boniface D, Yeomans MR, Wardle J. Eating for pleasure or profit: The effect of incentives on children’s enjoyment of vegetables. Psychological Science. 2011;22(2):190–196. doi: 10.1177/0956797610394662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Craig AD. How do you feel--now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10(1):59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7(2):189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Daw ND, Kakade S, Dayan P. Opponent interactions between serotonin and dopamine. Neural Networks. 2002;15(4–6):603–616. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00052-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dayan P, Balleine BW. Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning. Neuron. 2002;36(2):285–298. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00963-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Deci EL, Koestner R, Ryan RM. A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125(6):627–668. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Der-Avakian A, Markou A. The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits. Trends in Neurosciences. 2012;35(1):68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dweck CS. Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist. 1986;41(10):1040–1048. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hebb DO. Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system) Psychological Review. 1955;62(4):243–254. doi: 10.1037/h0041823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Howe MW, Tierney PL, Sandberg SG, Phillips PE, Graybiel AM. Prolonged dopamine signaling in striatum signals proximity and value of distant rewards. Nature. 2013;500(7464):575–579. doi: 10.1038/nature12475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979;47:263–291. [Google Scholar]
  24. Kaplan F, Oudeyer PY. In search of the neural circuits of intrinsic motivation. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2007;1(1):225–236. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.017.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303(5660):1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kremer M, Miguel E, Thornton R. Incentives to learn. The Review of Economics and Statistics. 2009;91(3):437–456. [Google Scholar]
  27. Laming D. Choice reaction performance following an error. Acta Psychologica. 1979;43(3):199–224. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(79)90032-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lazear E. Performance pay and productivity. The American Econonomic Review. 2000;90(5):1346–1361. [Google Scholar]
  29. Leary MR. Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:317–344. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. LeDoux J. Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron. 2012;73(4):653–676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Leotti LA, Delgado MR. The inherent reward of choice. Psychological Science. 2011;22(10):1310–1318. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lepper MR, Greene D, Nisbett RE. Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of the “overjustification” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1973;28(1):129–137. [Google Scholar]
  33. Linke J, Kirsch P, King AV, Gass A, Hennerici MG, Bongers A, Wessa M. Motivational orientation modulates the neural response to reward. NeuroImage. 2010;49(3) doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mednick SA, Mednick MT. Examiner’s manual: Remote associates test. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  35. Miller WR, Rollnick S. Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change. 2. New York: The Guilford Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  36. Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Ishii A, Tanabe HC, Onoe H, Sadato N, Watanabe Y. The neural basis of academic achievement motivation. NeuroImage. 2008;42(1):369–378. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Montague PR, King-Casas B, Cohen JD. Imaging valuation models in human choice. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2006;29:417–448. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Muraven M, Shmueli D, Burkley E. Conserving self-control strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91(3):524–537. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Murayama K, Matsumoto M, Izuma K, Matsumoto K. Neural basis of the undermining effect of monetary reward on intrinsic motivation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107(49):20911–20916. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1013305107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Paulus MP, Feinstein JS, Simmons A, Stein MB. Anterior cingulate activation in high trait anxious subjects is related to altered error processing during decision making. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;55(12):1179–1187. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature. 2006;442(7106):1042–1045. doi: 10.1038/nature05051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ryan RM, Deci EL. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Psychology. 2000;25:54–67. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Sheth SA, Mian MK, Patel SR, Asaad WF, Williams ZM, Dougherty DD, Eskandar EN. Human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex neurons mediate ongoing behavioural adaptation. Nature. 2012;488(7410):218–221. doi: 10.1038/nature11239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sutton RS, Barto AG. Reinforcement learning. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  45. Vohs KD, Heatherton TF. Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science. 2000;11(3):249–254. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wiechman BM, Gurland ST. What happens during the free-choice period? evidence of a polarizing effect of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43:716–719. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials

RESOURCES