Abstract
In children diagnosed with pediatric bipolar disorder (PBD), disturbances in the quality of sleep and wakefulness are prominent. A novel phenotype of PBD called Fear of Harm (FOH) associated with separation anxiety and aggressive obsessions is associated with sleep onset insomnia, parasomnias (nightmares, night-terrors, enuresis), REM sleep-related problems, and morning sleep inertia. Children with FOH often experience thermal discomfort (e.g. feeling hot, excessive sweating) in neutral ambient temperature conditions, as well as no discomfort during exposure to the extreme cold, and alternate noticeably between being excessively hot in the evening and cold in the morning. We hypothesized that these sleep- and temperature-related symptoms were overt symptoms of an impaired ability to dissipate heat, particularly in the evening hours near the time of sleep onset. We measured sleep/wake variables using actigraphy, and nocturnal skin temperature variables using thermal patches and a wireless device, and compared these data between children with PBD/FOH and a control sample of healthy children. The results are suggestive of a thermoregulatory dysfunction that is associated with sleep onset difficulties. Further, they are consistent with our hypothesis that alterations in neural circuitry common to thermoregulation and emotion regulation underlie affective and behavioral symptoms of the FOH phenotype.
Keywords: pediatric bipolar disorder, Fear of Harm, phenotype, thermoregulatory deficit, distal-proximal temperature gradient, sleep disturbance, parasomnias, sleep-onset latency, sleep diary, actigraphy
Introduction
Sleep disturbance is a common feature of mood disorders. In children diagnosed with pediatric bipolar disorder (PBD), problems with the quality of both sleep and wakefulness are prominent, and include bedtime refusal, sleep onset insomnia, parasomnias, morning sleep inertia, and daily bouts of both hyperactivity and hypoactivity [1–14]. An associated observation is that children with BD alternate noticeably between being excessively hot in the evening and cold in the morning [15–16]. Despite the pervasiveness of parental complaints and frequent clinical observations of sleep-related issues, there are few studies of sleep and rest/activity patterns in PBD, and data relating to temperature regulation in PBD are virtually non-existent. It is likely that methodological complexities of obtaining these data in children with psychiatric disorders are a primary reason for the lack of such studies. In addition, the very concept of childhood-onset bipolar disorder has been controversial, and only in the last decade have empirical investigations begun to characterize symptoms and examine potential etiologies of bipolar mood disorder in children [17–18].
A majority of children with bipolar disorder exhibit a subsyndromal course of illness. This has prompted many investigative groups to explore whether such a presentation is developmental or unique. Despite the ongoing debate, there has been a rapid increase in the rate of diagnoses of bipolar disorder in children [e.g., 19]. Concurrently, breakthroughs in neurology, neuroimaging, and genetics have called into question the existing conceptually-based psychiatric constructs altogether. New dimensional versus previous categorical research approaches which reflect these advances have made progress toward identifying dimensions of symptoms, or phenotypes, that are more likely to lead to evidence-based diagnosis and treatment. Such an example is a novel phenotype of PBD called Fear of Harm (FOH) [20–22].
The FOH phenotype includes symptoms that have not previously been associated with a nosological definition of bipolar disorder or other proposed childhood phenotypes of PBD [22]. However, many of these symptoms are recognized as co-morbid with the condition. In particular, separation anxiety, sleep/arousal disorders, parasomnias (night-terrors, enuresis) and REM sleep-related problems are primary features of the FOH phenotype. In addition, children with FOH often experience thermal discomfort (e.g. feeling hot, excessive sweating) in neutral ambient temperature conditions, as well as no discomfort during exposure to the extreme cold [23]. It is conceivable that an environmental signal expected to promote cold-defensive responses and lead an individual to seek warmth, to escape the cold, and to stimulate thermogenesis is not registered or not responded to, or the sensation/perception of change in cold ambient temperature is muted in these children, and therefore there is no aversive response generated by central thermoregulatory mechanisms.
An in-depth analysis of a large sample of children at risk for, or with a community diagnosis of, bipolar disorder, indicated that the population divides into approximate thirds of no-FOH, low- FOH, and high-FOH [22]. Compared with children in the no or low FOH groups, children with high FOH have significantly higher indices of severity of mania and depression [22–23], and clearly fall within the domain of classical manic depression. Course of illness analysis has indicated that presence of the FOH trait associates with the most severe form of the illness, including early age of onset, frequent hospitalizations, significant social impairment, and school problems [22].
Children with the FOH phenotype, and bipolar disorder in general, also often experience sleep onset insomnia symptoms [1,6–9,11–13] that are typically reported in younger children as “bedtime refusal” or “difficulty settling at night”. Older children self-report an inability to fall asleep, sometimes concurrent with racing thoughts and psychomotor agitation. In addition, both children and their parents complain of the children’s severe morning lethargy and an inability to awaken spontaneously until later than similarly aged healthy children. We have hypothesized that these sleep-and temperature-related symptoms may be the overt symptoms of an impaired ability to dissipate heat, particularly in the evening hours near the time of sleep onset.
In an initial effort to confirm parental and clinical observations of these sleep and temperature problems, we examined rest/activity patterns using actigraphy, sleep parameters using parent-completed Sleep Diaries, and nocturnal skin temperature variables using a wireless temperature monitoring device, in children diagnosed with bipolar disorder, who met criteria for the FOH phenotype. These data were compared between children with FOH with a control sample of healthy children. With this approach we attempted to obtain high-quality objective data in a naturalistic setting, while circumventing some of the practical difficulties of studying sleep and temperature in children. The actigraphy and Sleep Diary data were utilized to obtain quantitative and qualitative information about sleep. Skin temperature data were utilized to examine relationships between a proxy measure of heat dissipation (i.e., the distal-proximal gradient) and its relationship to sleep parameters, particularly latency to sleep onset.
We hypothesized that relative to the controls, children with PBD/FOH would have difficulty dissipating heat at bedtime, and further that this thermoregulatory symptom would be associated with longer latencies to sleep.
2. Experimental Section
2.1 Subjects
The study was reviewed and approved by the Weill Cornell Committee on Human Subjects in Research (Weill Cornell IRB). Potential subjects were recruited primarily via the research studies portal on the website of the Juvenile Bipolar Research Foundation (jbrf.org). The aim of this initial screening methodology was to contact parents/caregivers of children who met criteria for the FOH phenotype, as described in detail in Papolos et al. (2009) [23], and controls with no psychiatric symptoms or history. Parents or caregivers completed the online Child Bipolar Questionnaire (CBQ [23]) and consented to being contacted if the responses indicated initial eligibility for the research study. Eligible subjects met the following criteria: CBQ total score >65, plus endorsed as being present almost always or always 1) at least 3 of 5 sleep-related items on the CBQ, plus the CBQ item relating to rapid, abrupt mood swings, plus the CBQ item relating to thermal discomfort Additional eligibility criteria (e.g., age 5–12 years old) were assessed via contact from the subject recruiter to the parent/caregiver respondent, and the study protocol was described. Eligible and interested adult/child teams were mailed study consent/assent forms. (Separate versions of assent forms were provided for children 5–7 years old versus 8–12 years old, as stipulated by the Weill Cornell IRB).
Following consent/assent, a diagnostic interview via telephone was conducted by a trained clinical rater using the K-SADS [24]. Diagnoses of DSM-IV Bipolar Disorder, or of no DSM-IV diagnoses for control subjects, were confirmed by expert consensus using information from the diagnostic interview and K-SADS.
Reported here are data from 16 children with PBD/FOH (9M, 7F; mean age = 8 ± 2 years, range 5–12 years). Mean age of onset of PBD was 4.25 years. All met criteria for FOH phenotype [19]. Control data are from 4 subjects (4M, mean age = 8 ± 1 years, range 7–9 years).
A study kit was provided to each adult/child subject team, and returned at the end of the protocol. The kit included a programmed Actiwatch (Respironics Minimitter, Inc., Bend, OR), a programmed Vitalsense monitor with activated wireless dermal temperature patches (Respironics Minimitter, Bend, OR), labeled Salivette tubes (Alpco Diagnostics, Inc., Wyndham, NH) for saliva collection, and an instruction binder with 14 copies of a Sleep/Medication/Activity Diary (Diary).
2.2 Protocol
2.2.1 Diary
On each one-page Diary the adult/child team cooperated to complete questions with quantitative answers about the time the child got into bed the previous night, or started to fall asleep if not in bed (diary Bedtime), estimated Sleep Onset Latency (SOL), estimated Waketime (i.e., awakening from sleep; WT), and time the child got out of bed in the morning (Risetime). Additional qualitative questions completed each morning included whether the child had experienced any long awakenings, nightmares, night terrors, or other parasomnias, got out of bed for any reason, or changed sleeping locations at any time during the night. The second portion of the Diary was completed each evening by the adult and included questions about any medications the child had taken during the day, with their administration time and dose, as well as questions to help adjudicate the Actiwatch data (e.g., periods of removal of the Actiwatch during the day, nap times). Adults/children were instructed to complete the portion of the Diary with questions about the previous night’s sleep period in the morning within 2 hours of the child’s Risetime, and the portion with questions about the day’s activities and medication regimen in the evening hours.
2.2.2 Nocturnal skin temperature
The dermal patches were to be placed on the child’s a) lower left calf (distal), and b) subclavicle region (proximal) at least 1 hour prior to anticipated bedtime on 3 nights to record overnight temperature. In order for the successful detection of the signal from dermal patches, the Vitalsense monitor needed to be within approximately 3 feet of each of the patches. While awake and out of bed, the child wore the monitor in a nylon waistpack. At bedtime, the monitor was placed near the child on a nightstand or in the bed next to the child. Although an attempt was made to record temperature for 3 consecutive nights and the intervening days, logistical and comfort considerations resulted in revised instructions for the adult to remove the sensors in the morning 1 hour after the child got out of bed. The dermal patches transmitted skin temperature (Tsk) to the Vitalsense monitor at a 1-minute sampling rate [consider mentioning resolution and accuracy?] (and transmitted ambient temperature readings when the patches were removed from the child’s skin). Skin temperature readings from the time of placement on the skin until 4 hours later (i.e., incorporating bedtime and nocturnal sleep onset) were analyzed to obtain the absolute distal temperature, absolute proximal temperature, and distal-to-proximal gradient (DPG).
2.2.3 Actigraphy
A small wrist-worn Actiwatch-L (Respironics Minimitter, Bend, OR) was programmed to obtain activity data at a 2-minute sampling rate for up to 30 days. The child was to wear the Actiwatch continuously, except for periods when the water-resistant watch would remain submerged in water for an extended time (e.g., bathing, swimming).
2.2.4 Data Analysis
Diary data were summarized for each child individually and then by group (Control, FOH). Parameters included average BT, SOL, Time Spent Asleep, WT, RT, Sleep Period Duration from Bedtime to Risetime, and Sleep Efficiency (Time Spent Asleep/Sleep Period Duration). From the continuous Actiwatch data individual sleep periods were extracted for analysis, each constrained by the Diary-reported Bedtime and Risetime of the corresponding night. Each sleep period was analyzed via Actiware 5.0, applying a medium sensitivity algorithm to distinguish sleep from wake epochs, and to thus estimate actigraphy-derived Sleep Onset Latency (minutes from the clock time of Bedtime to 5 contiguous sleep epochs) and Sleep Efficiency (SE; # of sleep epochs during sleep period divided by total # of epochs during the sleep period.
2.2.5 DPG/DPG0
Usable temperature data was operationally defined as data from nights in which Tsk from both distal and proximal sensors was recorded starting a minimum of 30 minutes prior to Bedtime and continued for at least 3 hours after Bedtime. The proximal-minus-distal Tsk difference was calculated for each minute for each night. The resulting curve was the DPG curve. DPG0° was defined for each night as the clock time at which the DPG curve first crossed 0°. The difference, or gradient, between proximal and distal skin temperature has been validated as a measure of heat dissipation, a thermoregulatory process in which heat is shunted from the body’s core to its shell [28–31]. The distal-proximal gradient (DPG) has a temporal relationship to sleep onset in normal subjects, such that when the gradient approaches 0°, sleep onset is imminent [28–30, 33–35]. This temporal relationship has been shown to be more than coincident; peripheral heat loss through skin of the extremities appears to be functionally related to, and permissive of sleep onset [28–29, 34–35]. In populations with compromised thermoregulatory function, this temporal relationship has been shown to be disrupted, with sleep onset difficulties as one result [31, 35–38]. Converging evidence for the functional role of heat dissipation in sleep onset indicates that promoting heat dissipation in those with sleep onset difficulties facilitates sleep induction [29,32,40]..
Sleep, actigraphy, and temperature parameters were compared between Control and FOH groups using t-tests, or Mann Whitney U tests. While the unit of analysis was a parameter from an individual night, degrees of freedom (when applicable) were based on the number of subjects from each group contributing to the analysis. Spearman rank-order correlations estimated the strength of relationships between variables, including between ordinal and continuous variables.
3. Results and Discussion
From the 16 FOH children, usable Diary and Actiwatch data were obtained from 92 nights versus 37 nights from the 4 Control children. Coincident, usable Diary, Actiwatch, and temperature data were obtained from 26 nights from 10 children in FOH versus 8 nights from 4 children in Control.
3.1 Sleep
Table 1 shows results of sleep-related parameters calculated from the Diary and Actiwatch. Neither Bedtime nor Risetime differed significantly between FOH and Control children, and thus sleep period duration did not differ between groups. However, sleep onset latency from both Diary and actigraphy was 2–3 times longer in FOH than Control groups. As calculated from Actiwatch data, SE did not differ between groups, but was relatively low for both groups, which may reflect a) the sensitivity of the algorithm applied to determine wakefulness versus sleep (medium sensitivity was used), b) (related to the algorithm sensitivity) the detection of restlessness during sleep in both groups of children, and/or c) relatively poor sleep, on average, in both FOH and Control children. A recalculation of sleep parameters from Actiwatch data using a less sensitive algorithm (i.e., higher threshold for determining wakefulness) increased the SE ratio equivalently in both groups.
Table 1.
Sleep parameter (source) | Control | FOH |
---|---|---|
Bedtime (d) | 21:04 ± 0:40 | 21:10 ± 1:01 |
Risetime (d) | 06:32 ± 0:50 | 07:23 ± 1:04 |
Sleep Onset Latency (d) | 9 ± 5 min | 27 ± 20 min |
Sleep Onset Latency (a) | 8 ± 4 min | 37 ± 38 min |
Total Sleep Time (a) | 7h36m ± 41m | 8h07m ± 1h17m |
Sleep Efficiency (a) | 85.7 7.1 % | 87.0 ± 6.2 % |
Sleep Period Duration (d) | 9h13m ± 44m | 10h04m ± 1h28m |
Parasomnias reported | 0 | 8 |
Source: (d) = from Diary; (a) = derived from actigraphy
3.2 Parasomnias
In the control group, there was not a single report of a parasomnia event on the Diary. In the FOH group, Diaries from 8/16 subjects indicated that the child had experienced at least one parasomnia event on a given night. More than one unique parasomnia and more than one parasomnia event on multiple nights were reported in all but 1 of these 8 children. The parasomnias reported were primarily nightmares, reported from 6 children, with 2 episodes of enuresis from the same child and 3 episodes of night terrors from 3 different children.
3.3 Skin temperature and sleep
The protocol indicated that skin temperature recording via the dermal patches and the Vitalsense monitor should begin at least 60 minutes prior to anticipated bedtime, or 1800h, whichever was earlier. However, the average start time of skin temperature recording was 2012h. This was, on a majority of nights, at least an hour prior to Bedtime.
Figure 1a–1c illustrates distal and proximal Tsk and sleep timing on individual nights from 2 FOH and 1 Control. Figures 2a–2d illustrate group mean Tsk curves for the interval from 1 hour (−60m) before until 3 hours (+180m) after Bedtime.
The average absolute distal Tsk levels for the −45m to +180m interval surrounding Bedtime for all nights for Control and FOH groups did not differ. In particular, at Bedtime, the absolute distal Tsk levels were essentially identical for both groups (control: 32.30°, FOH: 32.33°). However, proximal Tsk averaged more than 1.2°C higher in the FOH relative to Control group across the same interval. The largest magnitude of difference in absolute proximal Tsk levels occurred in the interval from 45 minutes prior to Bedtime through 15 minutes after Bedtime. In the Control group, proximal Tsk initially increased slightly at Bedtime, but as distal Tsk increased substantially and quickly after Bedtime, proximal Tsk started slowly decreasing, resulting in the DPG0 an average of 9 minutes after Bedtime.
In FOH children, a different pattern of Tsk around Bedtime was observed. On 4 nights from 3 different children in the FOH group, a DPG° did not occur in the −60m to +180m analysis interval. While distal Tsk increased around Bedtime in a manner similar to Controls, proximal Tsk remained higher before and in the 3 hours after Bedtime in FOH children.
Due primarily to the lag in proximal Tsk decrease, the time at which DPG0° occurred was significantly later in FOH children. Analyses of Tsk relative to sleep revealed that DPG0° averaged 2114h ± 40m for Control and 2212h ± 1h16m for FOH groups, respectively (t = 2.87, p < .05). Similarly, there was a delay in the DPG0° relative to Bedtime in the FOH group, even though Bedtime did not differ between the groups. The interval from Bedtime to DPG0° averaged 11m ± 15m for Control children compared with 61m ± 51m for FOH children (t = 3.21, p < .01).
This lag between Bedtime and DPG0° was associated with a longer latency to sleep onset. SOL estimated by the Diary correlated significantly with the interval from Bedtime to DPG0° (SOL/BT-to-DPG0° interval: Spearman’s rho =.48, p <.05). There was a similar trend for actigraphy-derived SOL (actigraphy-derived SOL/BT-to-DPG0° interval: Spearman’s rho = .36, p =.12).
It was the common theme of parental and clinical reports that children with FOH exhibit signs and symptoms of altered thermoregulation that led to the current study. For example, children diagnosed with BD often have unusually reddened cheeks and ears, wear few layers of clothes in cold temperatures, and frequently complain of being hot even when others are comfortable. The anecdotal evidence for this phenomenon is quite widespread, and evidence for disturbed temperature regulation, particularly in the circadian domain, has been previously described in adults with affective illness [25–27], but to our knowledge, no systematic investigations of temperature perception or thermoregulation in PBD have been conducted.
An intimate association between sleep and body temperature has long been recognized, but a renewed interest in research on this topic has revealed that temperature regulation influences sleep to a greater degree than previously known. A growing body of evidence indicates that declines in core temperature and increases in peripheral heat loss may be functionally related to sleep initiation and consolidation [28–35]. We and others have found that when the maximum rate of decline in core temperature (i.e., the steepest slope) occurs prior, and in close proximity to bedtime, sleep onset latency is reduced, and slow wave sleep is increased [28,31]. An elegant series of studies by Krauchi and colleagues determined that peripheral heat loss via distal vasodilation, which drives the nocturnal decline in core temperature, is a permissive condition for sleep initiation [28–30]. In brief, they find that sleep onset occurs when the DPG approaches 0 degrees. The DPG0° was a better predictor of sleep onset than core body temperature, its rate of change, heart rate change, melatonin levels, or subjective ratings of sleepiness [28]. A complementary body of work by van Someren and colleagues has systematically demonstrated the role of skin temperature in sleepiness [e.g., 32–33], and how manipulating the amount of heat dissipated via the skin can alter centrally-regulated vigilance levels and sleep propensity [34–35].
Some types of insomnia are associated with heat dissipation problems. Individuals with vasospastic syndrome, who have deficient vasodilation capacity, require twice as long to fall asleep as healthy controls [36]. Also, compromised capacity to lose heat from the periphery has been hypothesized to largely account for sleep maintenance insomnia in elderly individuals [37–39] and in women with menopausal hot flashes or night sweats during sleep [40]. It is conceivable that children with BD have thermoregulatory dysfunction that affects the capacity for heat dissipation and thereby interferes with the sleep initiation process. It is further possible that the neural mechanisms underlying the disruption in both thermoregulation and sleep regulation also modulate or mediate emotion dysregulation in these children.
4. Conclusions
The current data add to the emerging evidence for physiological and behavioral underpinnings of the FOH phenotype of pediatric bipolar disorder. Objective evidence of sleep disturbance, in the form of long sleep onset latencies, is in agreement with a large body of anecdotal, questionnaire, and empirical evidence for sleep problems in these children. Although neither Bedtime/Risetime, nor sleep period duration were obviously aberrant in the FOH children in this study, the average Diary-estimated sleep onset latency of greater than 30 minutes is comparable to that of sleep onset insomniacs.
It is necessary to note the numerous and varied difficulties with this in-home study, which limit interpretation of these data. As one result of these mostly logistical difficulties obtaining reliable diary, skin temperature, and sleep/wake data in children, the number of usable datasets (defined as having temperature on at least one night, and Actiwatch data for at least 7 nights) obtained were from a far smaller number of subjects than were enrolled in the study. Nonetheless, the clinical characteristics of these subjects (and Controls) are well-defined, the sample is well-characterized, and the information from the small sample is compelling.
The correlational nature of these results and the small sample size proscribe attributing directionality of effects among temperature, sleep, and emotion regulation disturbances. Nonetheless, the results are suggestive of a thermoregulatory dysfunction that is associated with sleep onset difficulties in children with a clear dysregulation of emotion as manifest in the Fear of Harm phenotype. They are consistent with our hypothesis that alterations in neural circuitry common to thermoregulation and emotion regulation, involving the orexin system, underlie affective and behavioral symptoms of the FOH phenotype [22]. At a minimum, additional studies of thermoregulation in children with this psychiatric condition are warranted.
Acknowledgments
The study was funded by NIH grant R21 MH080268 to Weill Cornell Medical College and by the Juvenile Bipolar Research Foundation.
Footnotes
Author Contributions
P.J.M. was responsible for coordinating study implementation and data collection, data management, analysis, interpretation, and initial drafts of the manuscript. D.P. was closely involved with analysis and interpretation of data as well as co-writing drafts of the manuscript. M.G.F. contributed to development of data processing and analysis strategies, interpretation, and manuscript preparation.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Contributor Information
Patricia J. Murphy, Email: patricia.jane.murphy@gmail.com.
Mark G. Frei, Email: markgfrei@gmail.com.
Demitri Papolos, Email: dpapolos@jbrf.org.
References
- 1.Baroni Argelinda, Hernandez Mariely, Grant Marie C, Faedda Gianni L. Sleep Disturbances in Pediatric Bipolar Disorder: A Comparison between Bipolar I and Bipolar NOS. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2012;3:22. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berry E, Heaton PC, Kelton CM. PMH29 National Estimates of the Inpatient Burden of Pediatric Bipolar Disorder. Value in Health. 2010;14:115–123. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chang Kiki D. Course and Impact of Bipolar Disorder in Young Patients. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2010;71:e05. doi: 10.4088/JCP.8125tx7c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dilsaver Steven C, Akiskal Hagop S. ‘Mixed Hypomania’ in Children and Adolescents: Is It a Pediatric Bipolar Phenotype with Extreme Diurnal Variation between Depression and Hypomania? Journal of Affective Disorders. 2009;116:12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Faedda Gianni L, Baldessarini Ross J, Glovinsky Ira P, Austin Nancy B. Pediatric Bipolar Disorder: Phenomenology and Course of Illness. Bipolar Disorders. 2004;6:305–313. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2004.00128.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Harvey Allison G, Mullin Benjamin C, Hinshaw Stephen P. Sleep and Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents with Bipolar Disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:1147–1168. doi: 10.1017/S095457940606055X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Harvey Allison G. The Adverse Consequences of Sleep Disturbance in Pediatric Bipolar Disorder: Implications for Intervention. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2009;18:321–338. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ivanenko Anna, Johnson Kyle. Sleep Disturbances in Children With Psychiatric Disorders. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology. 2008;15:70–78. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2008.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lofthouse Nicholas, Gilchrist Richard, Splaingard Mark. Mood-Related Sleep Problems in Children and Adolescents. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2009;18:893–916. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2009.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Luby Joan L, Navsaria Neha. Pediatric Bipolar Disorder: Evidence for Prodromal States and Early Markers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2010;51:459–471. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02210.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mehl Rochelle C, O’Brien Louise M, Jones Janet H, Dreisbach Julie K, Mervis Carolyn B, Gozal David. Correlates of Sleep and Pediatric Bipolar Disorder. Sleep. 2006;29:193–197. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Staton Dennis. The Impairment of Pediatric Bipolar Sleep: Hypotheses Regarding a Core Defect and Phenotype-Specific Sleep Disturbances. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2008;108:199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Staton Dennis, Hill Catherine M, Chen Wai. ADHD, Sleep Onset, and Pediatric Bipolar Disorder. Journal of Sleep Research. 2008;17:240. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00644.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Youngstrom Eric A, Birmaher Boris, Findling Robert L. Pediatric Bipolar Disorder: Validity, Phenomenology, and Recommendations for Diagnosis. Bipolar Disorders. 2008;10:194–214. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00563.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dienstbier Richard A, Laguardia Robert L, Wilcox Noreen S. Psychology The Relationship of Temperament to Tolerance of Cold and Heat: Beyond ‘ Cold Hands-Warm Heart ’. Motivation and Emotion. 1987;11:269–294. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Papolos Demitri F, Alissa Bronsteen A. Bipolar disorder in children: assessment in general pediatric practice. Current Opinion in Pediatrics. 2013;25(3):419–26. doi: 10.1097/MOP.0b013e3283600e2a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Geller B, Luby J. Child and adolescent bipolar disorder: a review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997;36:1168–76. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199709000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Boris Birmaher. Bipolar disorder in children and adolescents. Child and Adolescent Mental Health. 2013;18:3. doi: 10.1111/camh.12021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Moreno Carmen, Laje Gonzalo, Blanco Carlos, Jiang Huiping, Schmidt Andrew B, Olfson Mark. National Trends in the Outpatient Diagnosis and Treatment of Bipolar Disorder in Youth. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:1032–1039. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.9.1032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Papolos Demitri. Bipolar Disorder and Comorbid Disorders: The Case for a Dimensional Nosology. In: Geller B, DelBello MP, editors. Bipolar Disorder in Childhood and Early Adolescence. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. pp. 76–106. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Demitri Papolos, Hennen JJ, Cockerham Melissa S, Lachman Herbert. A Strategy for Identifying Phenotypic Subtypes: Concordance of Symptom Dimensions Between Sibling Pairs Who Met Screening Criteria for a Genetic Linkage Study of Childhood-Onset Bipolar Disorder Using the Child Bipolar Questionnaire. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2007;99:27–36. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Papolos Demitri, Mattis Steven, Golshan Shahrokh, Molay Francine. Fear of Harm, a Possible Phenotype of Pediatric Bipolar Disorder: A Dimensional Approach to Diagnosis for Genotyping Psychiatric Syndromes. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2009 Nov;118(1–3):28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Papolos Demitri, Hennen John, Cockerham Melissa S, Thode Henry C, Youngstrom Eric A. The Child Bipolar Questionnaire: A Dimensional Approach to Screening for Pediatric Bipolar Disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2006;95:149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997 Jul;36(7):980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Avery DH, Shah SH, Eder DN, Wildschiødtz G. Nocturnal Sweating and Temperature in Depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1999;100:295–301. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10864.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Avery DH, Wildshiodtz G, Rafaelsen OJ. Nocturnal Temperature in Affective Disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1982;4:61–71. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(82)90020-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Avery David H, Dahl Kitty, Savage Margaret V, Brengelmann George L, Larsen Larry H, Kenny Margaret A, Eder Derek N, Vitiello Michael V, Prinz Patricia N. Circadian Temperature and Cortisol Rhythms during a Constant Routine Are Phase-Delayed in Hypersomnic Winter Depression. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;41:1109–1123. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00210-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kräuchi Kurt, Cajochen Christian, Werth Esther, Wirz-Justice Anna. Functional Link between Distal Vasodilation and Sleep-Onset Latency? American Journal of Physiology. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 2000 Mar;278(3):R741–R748. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.278.3.R741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kräuchi Kurt, Cajochen Christian, Werth Esther, Wirz-Justice Anna. Warm Feet Promote the Rapid Onset of Sleep. 1999;5477(1991):36–37. doi: 10.1038/43366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Krauchi Kurt, Deboer Tom. The Interrelationship between Sleep Regulation and Thermoregulation. Frontiers in Bioscience : A Journal and Virtual Library. 2010;15:604–625. doi: 10.2741/3636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Murphy Patricia J, Campbell Scott S. Nighttime Drop in Body Temperature: A Physiological Trigger for Sleep Onset? Sleep. 1997;20:505–511. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.7.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Raymann Roy JEM, Swaab Dick F, Van Someren Eus JW. Skin Deep: Enhanced Sleep Depth by Cutaneous Temperature Manipulation. Brain : A Journal of Neurology. 2008;131:500–513. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Raymann Roy JEM, Swaab Dick F, Van Someren EJW. Skin Temperature and Sleep-Onset Latency: Changes with Age and Insomnia. Physiology and Behavior. 2007;90:257–266. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Van Someren Eus JW. More than a Marker: Interaction between the Circadian Regulation of Temperature and Sleep, Age-Related Changes, and Treatment Possibilities. Chronobiology International. 2000;17:313–354. doi: 10.1081/cbi-100101050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Van Someren Eus JW, Raymann Roy JEM, Scherder Eus JA, Daanen HAM, Swaab Dick F. Circadian and Age-Related Modulation of Thermoreception and Temperature Regulation: Mechanisms and Functional Implications. Ageing Research Reviews. 2002;17:313–354. doi: 10.1016/s1568-1637(02)00030-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kräuchi K1, Gasio PF, Vollenweider S, Von Arb M, Dubler B, Orgül S, Flammer J, Stutz EZ. Cold extremities and difficulties initiating sleep: evidence of co-morbidity from a random sample of a Swiss urban population. Journal of Sleep Research. 2008;17:420–426. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00678.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lack Leon C, Gradisar Michael, Van Someren EJW, Wright Helen R, Lushington Kurt. The Relationship between Insomnia and Body Temperatures. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2008;12:307–317. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kurt Lushington, Dawson Drew, Lack Leon. Core body temperature is elevated during constant wakefulness in elderly poor sleepers. Sleep. 2000;23:504–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Van Den Heuvel Cameron J, Ferguson Sally A, Gilbert Saul S, Dawson Drew. Thermoregulation in Normal Sleep and Insomnia: The Role of Peripheral Heat Loss and New Applications for Digital Thermal Infrared Imaging (DITI) Journal of Thermal Biology. 2004;29:457–461. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gilbert SS, van den Heuvel CJ, Kennaway DJ, Dawson D. Peripheral Heat Loss: A Predictor of the Hypothermic response to Melatonin Administration in Young and Older Women. Physiology & Behavior. 1999;66:365–70. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00309-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]