Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Feb 11.
Published in final edited form as: Obesity (Silver Spring). 2015 Mar 25;23(5):1105–1111. doi: 10.1002/oby.21052

Obesity, perceived weight discrimination, and psychological wellbeing in older adults in England

Sarah E Jackson 1, Rebecca J Beeken 1, Jane Wardle 1
PMCID: PMC4414736  NIHMSID: NIHMS660493  PMID: 25809860

Abstract

Objective

To examine whether the adverse effect of obesity on psychological wellbeing can be explained by weight discrimination.

Methods

The study sample included 5056 older (≥50y) men and women living in England participating in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Participants reported experiences of weight discrimination in everyday life, and completed measures of quality of life (CASP-19 scale), life satisfaction (Satisfaction with Life Scale), and depressive symptoms (8-item CES-D scale). Height and weight were objectively measured, with obesity defined as BMI ≥30kg/m2. Mediation analyses were used to test the role of perceived weight discrimination in the relationship between obesity and each psychological factor.

Results

Obesity, weight discrimination, and psychological wellbeing were all significantly inter-related. Mediation models revealed significant indirect effects of obesity through perceived weight discrimination on quality of life (β=−0.072, SE=0.008), life satisfaction (β=−0.038, SE=0.008), and depressive symptoms (β=0.057, SE=0.008), with perceived weight discrimination explaining approximately 40% (range: 39.5%–44.1%) of the total association between obesity and psychological wellbeing.

Conclusion

Perceived weight discrimination explains a substantial proportion of the association between obesity and psychological wellbeing in English older adults. Efforts to reduce weight stigma in society could help to reduce the psychological burden of obesity.

Keywords: discrimination, stigma, obesity, psychological wellbeing, mediation

Introduction

In addition to the well-documented physical health risks associated with obesity (1), adverse effects on psychological wellbeing have long been recognised. In 1985, the National Institutes of Health drew attention to the “enormous psychological burden” created by obesity (2). This may to some extent overstate the case, but there is certainly evidence that obese individuals experience poorer quality of life (3), body image disturbance (4), and lower self-esteem (5), and are at increased risk of depression and other psychiatric disorders (6,7). Effects are typically strongest among people who are very obese, with one study finding that many patients awaiting bariatric surgery would prefer to be normal-weight with a major handicap (deaf/blind/one leg amputated) than stay morbidly obese, and the majority saying they would rather be normal-weight than a morbidly obese multi-millionaire (8).

Weight stigma is often cited as a potential mechanism leading from obesity to poorer psychological wellbeing (4,5,7,9). Prejudice against obese individuals is pervasive and rarely challenged in Western society (10). As a result, many obese individuals, and particularly those with severe obesity, report being discriminated against because of their weight in their everyday lives (11,12). Given that weight stigma and discrimination have both been shown to have a negative impact on psychological health outcomes, including wellbeing (10), depression (13,14), self-esteem and self-acceptance (13,15), and body image dissatisfaction (13,16), this might explain why obese people suffer psychologically.

Only one study to our knowledge has tested the mediating effect of weight-related discrimination, showing a significant reduction in the association between obesity and self-acceptance after adjusting for perceived weight discrimination (15). None have examined the role of discrimination in relation to more global indices of psychological wellbeing, such as quality of life or depression. The aim of the present study was therefore to investigate the extent to which perceived weight discrimination mediates associations between obesity and three markers of wellbeing: quality of life, life satisfaction, and depressive symptoms.

Method

Study population

Data were from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA); a cohort study of older adults (≥50y) living in England (17). ELSA participants were recruited from an annual cross-sectional survey of households, and comparisons of their socio-demographic characteristics against the national census indicate that the sample is broadly representative of the older English population (17). Six waves of ELSA data have been collected to date, starting in 2002 and repeated every other year since. At each assessment, participants complete an interview and questionnaires, and in alternate (even) waves nurse visits are conducted to obtain objective measures of health status, including body weight. Discrimination was assessed in wave 5 (2010–2011). The present analyses used anthropometric data from wave 4 (2008–2009) and data on discrimination and psychological wellbeing from wave 5. Our sample included participants with complete data on discrimination, BMI, and at least one psychological outcome (n=5056).1 Participants gave full informed consent and ethical approval was obtained from the London Multi-Centre Research Ethics Committee.

Measures

Obesity

Weight was measured by nurses to the nearest 0.1kg using portable electronic scales, and height was measured to the nearest millimetre using a portable stadiometer. Nurses recorded any factors that might have compromised the reliability of the measurements (e.g. participant was stooped/unwilling to remove shoes) and these cases were excluded. Underweight was defined as a BMI <18.5, normal-weight as BMI 18.5–25, overweight as BMI 25–29.9, and obesity as BMI ≥30.

Weight discrimination

Questions on perceived discrimination were based on items developed and used widely in other longitudinal studies in the USA (12,15,19). Participants were asked about the frequency of five discriminatory experiences: ‘In your day-to-day life, how often have any of the following things happened to you: 1) you are treated with less respect or courtesy; 2) you receive poorer service than other people in restaurants and stores; 3) people act as if they think you are not clever; 4) you are threatened or harassed; 5) you receive poorer service or treatment than other people from doctors or hospitals (almost every day/at least once a week/a few times a month/a few times a year/less than once a year/never).’ Because data were skewed, with most participants reporting never experiencing discrimination, we dichotomised responses to indicate whether or not respondents had ever experienced discrimination (never vs. all other options). A follow-up question asked participants to indicate the reason(s) for discrimination from a list including weight, age, gender, race, and physical disability. For the purpose of these analyses, perceived weight discrimination was defined as experiencing discrimination and attributing it to weight.

Psychological wellbeing

We included three measures of psychological wellbeing in our analyses: quality of life, life satisfaction, and depressive symptoms. Our rationale for including these distinct constructs was to have one broad measure of wellbeing, one of positive affect, and one of negative affect.

Quality of life was assessed with the CASP-19 (20), a scale designed to measure quality of life in older people. Items cover four domains: control (e.g. ‘I feel that what happens to me is out of my control’), autonomy (e.g. ‘My health stops me from doing things I want to do’), self-realisation (e.g. ‘I feel that life is full of opportunities’), and pleasure (e.g. ‘I enjoy being in the company of others’). Respondents were asked how often each statement applies to them (often=0, sometimes=1, not often=2, never=3). Positively-worded items were reverse scored so that higher scores indicated higher quality of life. The Cronbach α in the present sample was 0.86.

Life satisfaction was assessed with the Satisfaction With Life Scale (SWLS) (21), which asks the extent to which participants agree with five statements (e.g. ‘In most ways my life is close to my ideal’). Responses were on a Likert scale from 0 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The Cronbach α was 0.91.

Depressive symptoms were assessed with an eight-item version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (22). This asks about feelings over the last week (e.g. ‘Over the last week have you felt sad’), with binary response options (1=yes/0=no). Positively-framed items were reverse scored. The eight-item version has comparable validity and reliability to the original 20-item CES-D (23). The Cronbach α was 0.77.

For each of these three scales, we computed mean scores for participants with data on at least 75% of items to maximise the number of participants that could be included in the analyses. Standardised scores (z-scores) were calculated for each scale for ease of comparison across the three scales.

Demographic information

Interviewers collected information on age, sex, ethnicity, and household non-pension wealth. Due to the small number of participants from non-white ethnic groups, we categorised ethnicity as white vs. non-white. Wealth was categorised into five equal groups of net total non-pension wealth measured at benefit unit level (a benefit unit is a couple or single person along with any dependent children they might have) across all ELSA participants who took part in wave 5. Wealth has been identified as a particularly appropriate indicator of SES in this age group (24).

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using STATA version 13.1 (STATA Corporation, TX, USA). Age, sex, and wealth (as a proxy for SES) were entered as covariates for all analyses because of their known associations with obesity (25) and psychological wellbeing (e.g. 26,27). Ethnicity was not adjusted for because participants were almost exclusively white (98%). We used logistic regression to test the association between obesity and perceived weight discrimination, and analyses of covariance to test for differences in psychological wellbeing by obesity and perceived weight discrimination.

Mediation analyses were used to test the hypothesis that perceived weight discrimination mediated the relationship between obesity and psychological wellbeing (Figure 1). We calculated total, direct, and indirect effects, and tested the significance of the indirect effect using the Sobel test (28,29). The total effect (path c) of an independent variable (IV) on a dependent variable (DV) consists of a direct effect (path c′) of the IV on the DV and an indirect effect (path a × b) of the IV on the DV via a proposed mediator. Path a represents the effect of the IV on the mediator, and path b is the effect of the mediator on the DV. In these analyses, obesity was the IV, psychological variables were the DVs, and perceived weight discrimination was the mediator. Standardised scores were used for indices of psychological wellbeing for ease of comparison across the three psychological variables. We used bootstrapping with 5000 sampling replications to estimate the 95% confidence interval (CI) (30). Bootstrap tests of mediation are considered a better method of testing the significance of indirect effects than the Sobel test because they do not assume a normal distribution and therefore reduce the likelihood of type 2 error (30,31). If the 95% CI does not include 0 the indirect effect is considered significant (30). We also calculated effect ratios that reflect the proportion of the total effect of the IV on the DV that is explained by the mediator; in this case, the proportion of the total effect of obesity on psychological wellbeing that is explained by perceived weight discrimination. For example, an effect ratio of 0.5 would indicate that half of the total effect is explained by the mediator (31). In addition to comparing all obese individuals with those who were not obese, we repeated these mediation models separately for those with class I obesity (BMI 30–34.9) and class II/III obesity (BMI ≥35), to examine whether the ‘average’ effects were driven by participants in the more severely obese group. Previous research has indicated that the prevalence of weight discrimination increases substantially above a BMI of 35 (11), and psychological impairment is greater with more severe obesity (18).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mediation model of associations between obesity and psychological wellbeing via perceived weight discrimination

We performed sensitivity analyses testing for mediation of the association between obesity and psychological wellbeing by two other types of perceived discrimination (age and sex discrimination; the most prevalent forms of perceived discrimination reported by the sample, at 40% and 11% respectively) in order to establish whether effects were – as predicted – specific to weight discrimination or applied to experiences of discrimination in general. Additionally, because previous research has indicated that there may also be causal effects in the other direction (lower psychological wellbeing is associated with greater likelihood of perceiving discrimination (32)), we also tested this model. We used multiple mediation analysis (33) with obesity as the IV, weight discrimination as the DV, and quality of life, life satisfaction, and depressive symptoms as mediators (Figure 2). We followed the product-of-coefficients method using seemingly unrelated regression and bootstrapping with 5000 replications.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Mediation model of associations between obesity and perceived weight discrimination via psychological wellbeing

Results

Characteristics of the study sample are shown in Table 1. Participants were on average 67.5 years old, 55.9% were women, and 97.9% were white. Mean BMI was 28.2 and 31.2% of participants were obese. Obese individuals were on average younger (66.8 vs. 67.8 years, p<.001) and less wealthy (p<.001) than non-obese individuals, and a greater proportion were female (60.4% vs. 53.8%, p<.001). Ethnicity did not differ by obesity status (p=.327). Weight discrimination was reported by 4.6%, but was strongly related to weight status with 12.9% of obese individuals reporting weight discrimination (6.7% of class I obese, 26.8% of class II/III obese) and only 0.9% of non-obese individuals (2.6% of underweight, 0.7% of normal-weight, 0.9% of overweight; adjusted OR [obese vs. non-obese]=15.18, 95% CI=10.26–22.48, p<.001).

Table 1.

Characteristics of the study sample (n = 5056) – mean ± SD or % (n)

Whole sample
(n = 5056)
Non-obese
(n = 3480)
Obese
(n = 1576)
p
Age (years) 67.46 ± 8.85 67.80 ± 9.02 66.76 ± 8.43 <.001
Sex
  Male
44.1 (2231) 46.2 (1607) 39.6 (624) <.001
  Female 55.9 (2825) 53.8 (1873) 60.4 (952) -
Ethnicity
  White
97.9 (4949) 98.0 (3411) 97.6 (1538) .327
  Non-white 2.1 (107) 2.0 (69) 2.4 (38) -
Wealth quintile*
  1 (lowest)
15.8 (798) 13.4 (468) 20.9 (330) <.001
  2 19.4 (979) 17.9 (624) 22.5 (355) -
  3 19.9 (1005) 19.1 (664) 21.6 (341) -
  4 21.8 (1103) 23.1 (804) 19.0 (299) -
  5 (highest) 23.2 (1171) 26.4 (920) 15.9 (251) -
Weight (kg) 77.68 ± 15.77 70.98 ± 11.53 92.48 ± 13.66 <.001
BMI (kg/m2) 28.19 ± 5.05 25.52 ± 2.73 34.10 ± 3.84 <.001
Weight status
  Underweight
0.8 (38) 1.1 (38) - -
  Normal-weight 26.6 (1344) 38.6 (1344) - -
  Overweight 41.5 (2098) 60.3 (2098) - -
  Obese 31.2 (1576) - 100 (1576) -
    Class I obese 21.6 (1091) - 69.2 (1091) -
    Class II/III obese 9.6 (485) - 30.8 (485) -
Perceived weight discrimination 4.6 (233) 0.9 (30) 12.9 (203) <.001
Quality of life
  Mean score
2.16 ± 0.46 2.20 ± 0.45 2.08 ± 0.47 <.001
<.001
  Z-score 0.00 ± 1.00 0.08 ± 0.98 −0.18 ± 1.02 -
Life satisfaction
  Mean score
4.14 ± 1.27 4.20 ± 1.23 4.00 ± 1.33 <.001
<.001
  Z-score 0.00 ± 1.00 0.05 ± 0.97 −0.11 ± 1.05 -
Depressive symptoms
  Mean score
0.18 ± 0.24 0.16 ± 0.23 0.21 ± 0.25 <.001
<.001
  Z-score 0.00 ± 1.00 −0.07 ± 0.96 0.03 ± 1.06 -
*

Weight quintiles were derived from the whole ELSA sample. p values are for the difference between non-obese and obese individuals.

Obesity was significantly related to psychological wellbeing, although effect sizes were modest. Obese individuals reported lower quality of life (p<.001), lower life satisfaction (p=.005), and more depressive symptoms (p<.001) than those who were not obese (Table 2). Among the obese group, psychological impairment was greater in those with class II/III obesity than the class I obese (quality of life: p<.001; life satisfaction: p=.004, depressive symptoms: p<.001). Individuals who reported experiences of weight discrimination also had poorer psychological wellbeing in all three domains than those who did not report weight discrimination (p<.001) (Table 2). Associations between weight discrimination and psychological wellbeing were similar in participants who were excluded for missing weight data to those in the included cases.

Table 2.

Mean ± SE psychological wellbeing by obesity status and perceived weight discrimination

Obesity Perceived weight discrimination
No Yes F p No Yes F p
Quality of life
  Mean score
2.19 ± 0.01 2.11 ± 0.11 38.95 <.001 2.18 ± 0.01 1.88 ± 0.03 90.42 <.001
  Z-score 0.06 ± 0.02 −0.13 ± 0.02 - - 0.03 ± 0.01 −0.61 ± 0.07 - -
Life satisfaction
  Mean score
4.17 ± 0.02 4.06 ± 0.03 8.07 .005 4.16 ± 0.02 3.70 ± 0.09 26.04 <.001
  Z-score 0.03 ± 0.02 −0.06 ± 0.03 - - 0.02 ± 0.01 −0.34 ± 0.07 - -
Depressive symptoms
  Mean score
0.17 ± 0.004 0.20 ± 0.006 21.88 <.001 0.17 ± 0.003 0.29 ± 0.02 49.57 <.001
  Z-score −0.04 ± 0.02 0.10 ± 0.02 - - −0.02 ± 0.01 0.46 ± 0.07 - -

Values are adjusted for BMI, age, sex, and wealth.

SE = standard error.

Table 3 summarises the results of the mediation analyses (path c, path c’, and indirect effects in Figure 1). We observed significant indirect effects of obesity through weight discrimination on all three measures of psychological wellbeing (quality of life: β=−0.072, SE=0.008, 95% CI=−0.091 to −0.054; life satisfaction: β=−0.038, SE=0.008, 95% CI=−0.058 to −0.019; depressive symptoms: β=0.057, SE=0.008, 95% CI=0.036 to 0.078). Effect ratios indicated that weight discrimination explained just over 40% of the total effect of obesity on psychological wellbeing (range: 39.5–44.1%). There were also direct effects of obesity on quality of life (β=−0.110, SE=0.030) and depressive symptoms (β=0.081, SE=0.031), but the direct effect on life satisfaction was not significant. Analysis of associations between obesity and the four domains of quality of life revealed consistent evidence of mediation by weight discrimination, with effect ratios ranging from 31.0% (autonomy) to 46.4% (pleasure) (Supplementary Table 1). Despite higher prevalence of perceived weight discrimination and greater psychological impairment among individuals with more severe (class II/III) obesity than those with class I obesity, we observed no notable differences in the mediating effect of perceived weight discrimination when we ran mediation analyses separately for the two obese groups (data not shown).

Table 3.

Models testing mediation of associations between obesity and psychological wellbeing by perceived weight discrimination (see Figure 1)

Coeff. SE p* Bootstrap 95% CI Effect ratio
Obesity and quality of life
  Total effect (path c)
−0.182 0.029 <.001 - -
  Direct effect (path c’) −0.110 0.030 <.001 - -
  Indirect effect (via mediator) −0.072 0.008 <.001 [−0.091; −0.054] 0.395
Obesity and life satisfaction
  Total effect (path c)
−0.086 0.030 .004 - -
  Direct effect (path c’) −0.048 0.031 .123 - -
  Indirect effect (via mediator) −0.038 0.008 <.001 [−0.058; −0.019] 0.441
Obesity and depressive symptoms
  Total effect (path c)
0.137 0.030 <.001 - -
  Direct effect (path c’) 0.081 0.031 .009 - -
  Indirect effect (via mediator) 0.057 0.008 <.001 [0.036; 0.078] 0.412

Models use z-scores for all psychological wellbeing variables.

All models are adjusted for age, sex, and wealth.

Coeff. = coefficient; SE = standard error; CI = confidence interval.

*

p values shown for indirect effects are derived from the Sobel test for consistency with total and direct effects, however bootstrap 95% confidence intervals provide a more robust indication of significant mediation (see Method for more details).

We repeated the mediation analyses substituting age discrimination and sex discrimination in turn for weight discrimination to investigate whether mediation was specific to weight discrimination (Supplementary Table 2). Although perceived discrimination on the basis of age or sex was significantly associated with poorer psychological wellbeing, we observed no evidence of mediation of the effect of obesity.

We also tested the reverse model (Figure 2) to investigate whether obese individuals’ reports of weight discrimination could be explained by their lower wellbeing (Table 4). We observed both a direct effect of obesity on perceived weight discrimination (β=0.106, SE=0.006) and a small indirect effect of psychological wellbeing (β for total indirect effect=0.006, SE=0.001, 95% CI=0.004 to 0.009), driven predominantly by a mediating effect of quality of life (β=0.006, SE=0.001, 95% CI=0.004 to 0.009). Psychological variables explained 5.6% of the association between obesity and perceived weight discrimination.

Table 4.

Model testing mediation of the association between obesity and perceived weight discrimination by psychological wellbeing (see Figure 2)

Coeff. SE p* Bootstrap 95% CI Effect ratio
Total effect (path c) 0.112 0.006 <.001 - -
Direct effect (path c’) 0.106 0.006 <.001 - -
Indirect effect (via mediators) 0.0063 0.001 <.001 [0.004; 0.009] 0.056
  Indirect effect (via quality of life) 0.0059 0.001 <.001 [0.004; 0.009] 0.053
  Indirect effect (via life satisfaction) −0.0009 0.0005 .072 [−0.002; −0.0001] −0.008
  Indirect effect (via depressive symptoms) 0.0013 0.0007 .067 [0.0001; 0.003] 0.012

Model uses z-scores for all psychological wellbeing variables.

Model is adjusted for age, sex, and wealth.

Coeff. = coefficient; SE = standard error; CI = confidence interval.

*

p values shown for indirect effects are derived from the Sobel test for consistency with total and direct effects, however bootstrap 95% confidence intervals provide a more robust indication of significant mediation (see Method for more details).

Discussion

In this study we examined associations between obesity, perceived weight discrimination, and three markers of psychological wellbeing: quality of life, life satisfaction, and depressive symptoms. Obese individuals showed poorer wellbeing in all three domains, although effect sizes were modest. They were also substantially more likely to report weight discrimination. We used mediation models with bootstrapping to test the proposition that associations between obesity and wellbeing are mediated by weight discrimination and found that approximately 40% of the total effect of obesity on psychological wellbeing could be explained by perceptions of weight discrimination.

In order to rule out the possibility that any discrimination would have the same effect – i.e. it was nothing to do with weight per se, we carried out sensitivity analyses using other types of discrimination. Although age and sex discrimination were commonly reported, and had negative effects on wellbeing, they did not explain the lower levels of wellbeing among obese than non-obese participants. This finding is consistent with a previous study that showed that the relationship between obesity and self-acceptance was not attenuated when general experiences of discrimination were adjusted for, but became non-significant in analyses controlling for appearance-related discrimination (15).

The fact that perceived weight discrimination explained such a significant proportion of the unique variance in the association between obesity and psychological wellbeing emphasises the need to combat weight stigmatisation in society. Public health campaigns designed to tackle obesity may inadvertently stigmatise obese individuals, with messages that emphasise volitional control of body weight and minimise the importance of non-volitional factors that contribute to obesity (34). There have been calls for such interventions to focus on facilitating behavioural change and to endorse health rather than ‘ideal weight’ as the primary desired outcome (34). Other suggested strategies to reduce weight bias in the public health context include training health professionals about stigma and stereotyping, involving obese people in finding solutions to stigmatising programmes and policies, and ensuring consistent implementation of non-stigmatising messages and approaches (35). Promoting self-acceptance for obese individuals could also help to minimise the impact of perceived discrimination and improve wellbeing. In obese individuals who had recently completed a weight loss programme, a brief acceptance-based intervention that focused on weight-related stigmatising thoughts was associated with significant improvements in psychological distress and quality of life (36). However, directly addressing the issue of weight discrimination, rather than simply teaching obese people how best to cope with it, will inevitably have a greater impact on wellbeing.

The occurrence of discrimination is difficult to determine objectively because it relies on interpretation of the intentions of others. As such, discrimination can occur without being perceived by the individual who is discriminated against, and equally, it can be perceived in cases where it did not occur. In the latter situation, a person’s psychological state may influence the way they interpret others’ behaviour and hence whether discrimination is perceived. A study exploring perceptions of race discrimination in minority adolescents in the US showed that those with higher levels of depression or anxiety were more likely to perceive discrimination (32). In the present study, we tested for this ‘reverse effect’ and observed small but significant indirect effects of quality of life, life satisfaction, and depressive symptoms on perceived weight discrimination, suggesting that differences in these psychological factors contribute to obese individuals being more likely than those who are not obese to perceive weight discrimination. However, the total indirect effect of psychological wellbeing explained only 6% of the association between obesity and perceived weight discrimination, indicating that there are other important factors that account for this relationship.

This study had a number of strengths. It used a large sample drawn from a nationally-representative cohort, in which the prevalence of perceived weight discrimination was comparable to previous estimates in the equivalent age group in the US population (37). Many studies on weight discrimination or psychological wellbeing have been limited to smaller, treatment-seeking obese samples for whom weight discrimination may have been part of the motivation to seek treatment. These groups may not be representative of obese individuals in the general population who tend to suffer less psychological disturbance and are typically less obese, so may be less likely to perceive weight discrimination (5,38,39). The question on discrimination was phrased generally at first and then weight was included among a list of other possible attributions for discrimination, limiting reporting bias among obese respondents. The availability of objective measurements of height and weight in ELSA is also an advantage because many large longitudinal studies rely on self-reported data.

However, there were also limitations. Weight was not measured in the same data collection wave as discrimination, and participants may have changed weight status prior to reporting discrimination. We did not have complete data on weight and it is possible that people most troubled by their weight were more likely to refuse to be weighed. However, although missing cases inevitably pose a source of bias, we think it unlikely that exclusion of ELSA respondents with missing weight data would have resulted in substantial over- or under-estimation of the mediation effect because the discrimination-wellbeing association was the same in that group as in the total included group. Weight discrimination was self-reported and was therefore subject to recall bias and only reflected participants’ own perceptions of discrimination. These results therefore estimate the impact of believing that one has been a target of weight discrimination as opposed to the impact of weight discrimination per se. It is possible that the timing of discriminatory experiences was years prior to when they were reported, which would make mediation effects less plausible as the purported mediator would be temporally prior to the purported IV. The sample comprising older adults may have implications as high BMI over the life course is a predictor of premature mortality (40); thus the obese individuals in this sample may be positively selected on some trait that both helped them to stay alive, and remain healthy enough to participate in a major data collection effort. In addition, as the sample was predominantly white (98%) results may not generalise to other ethnic groups. Finally, our analyses were restricted to participants with data on discrimination, BMI, and at least one psychological outcome; and although participants in the analysed sample matched the total ELSA sample at wave 5 on age and sex, they were on average slightly wealthier and heavier, so results may not be population-representative.

Future research could extend our findings by investigating the extent to which weight discrimination mediates associations between obesity and other measures of wellbeing, such as self-esteem. It would also be interesting to examine differences in associations between obesity, weight discrimination, and wellbeing across demographic subgroups. The greater social pressures on women than men to maintain a slender physique may mean obese women are more vulnerable to the adverse psychological effects of weight discrimination, so the mediating effect may be stronger in women than men. Likewise, it is possible that weight discrimination explains a larger proportion of the association between obesity and wellbeing in younger populations, where obesity-associated health problems, which may lead to poorer wellbeing among obese individuals, have had less time to develop.

In summary, our results indicate that a substantial proportion of the association between obesity and psychological wellbeing can be explained by perceptions of weight discrimination. Concerted efforts to reduce weight stigma in society could therefore help to alleviate the psychological burden of obesity.

Supplementary Material

Supp TableS1-S2

What is already known about this subject?

  • Obesity is associated with poorer psychological wellbeing on a number of domains, including depression and quality of life.

  • In addition, many obese individuals report being the subjects of weight-related stigmatisation and discrimination in their everyday lives.

  • Weight stigma is often cited as a potential mechanism leading from obesity to poorer psychological wellbeing, but only one previous study has explicitly assessed the role of weight discrimination as a mediator of obesity–wellbeing relationships.

What does this study add?

  • This study indicates that perceived weight discrimination explains approximately 40% of the association between obesity and quality of life, life satisfaction, and depressive symptoms.

  • Mediation effects were specific to weight discrimination; no evidence for significant mediation of obesity–wellbeing associations was observed for perceived discrimination on the basis of other attributes (age and sex).

  • There was also some evidence that an individual’s psychological state may influence the perception of weight discrimination, with those with poorer wellbeing scores being more likely to perceive discrimination.

Acknowledgements

SEJ designed the study, analysed the data, and drafted the manuscript. RJB and JW revised the manuscript for intellectual content. All authors were involved in writing the paper and had final approval of the submitted and published versions.

The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing is funded by the National Institute on Aging (grants numbers 2RO1AG7644-01A1 and 2RO1AG017644) and a consortium of UK government departments coordinated by the Office for National Statistics. SEJ is supported by ELSA funding. RJB and JW are supported by Cancer Research UK.

Footnotes

1

Note: The prevalence of weight discrimination was slightly higher in participants for whom weight data were missing than in the analysed sample (6.9% vs. 4.6%).

Conflicts of interest statement

The authors have no competing interests.

References

  • 1.National Institutes of Health, National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Clinical guidelines on the identification, evaluation, and treatment of overweight and obesity in adults: the evidence report. Obes Res. 1998;6 51–209S. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.National Institutes of Health. Health Implications of Obesity. Consensus Development Conference Statement; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ul-Haq Z, Mackay DF, Fenwick E, Pell JP. Meta-analysis of the association between body mass index and health-related quality of life among adults, assessed by the SF-36. Obesity. 2013;21:E322–E327. doi: 10.1002/oby.20107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. Obesity and body image. Body Image. 2004;1:43–56. doi: 10.1016/S1740-1445(03)00007-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Miller CT, Downey KT. A Meta-Analysis of Heavyweight and Self-Esteem. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 1999;3:68–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Luppino FS, de Wit LM, Bouvy PF, et al. Overweight, obesity, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:220–229. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Simon GE, Von Korff M, Saunders K, et al. Association between obesity and psychiatric disorders in the US adult population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:824–830. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rand CS, Macgregor AM. Successful weight loss following obesity surgery and the perceived liability of morbid obesity. Int J Obes. 1991;15:577–579. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Stunkard AJ, Faith MS, Allison KC. Depression and obesity. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:330–337. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00608-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Puhl RM, Heuer CA. The stigma of obesity: a review and update. Obesity. 2009;17:941–964. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Jackson SE, Steptoe A, Beeken RJ, Croker H, Wardle J. Perceived weight discrimination in England: a population-based study. Int J Obes. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.186. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Andreyeva T, Puhl RM, Brownell KD. Changes in Perceived Weight Discrimination Among Americans, 1995–1996 Through 2004–2006. Obesity. 2008;16:1129–1134. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Annis NM, Cash TF, Hrabosky JI. Body image and psychosocial differences among stable average weight, currently overweight, and formerly overweight women: the role of stigmatizing experiences. Body Image. 2004;1:155–167. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2003.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hatzenbuehler ML, Keyes KM, Hasin DS. Associations between perceived weight discrimination and the prevalence of psychiatric disorders in the general population. Obes Silver Spring Md. 2009;17:2033–2039. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Carr D, Friedman MA. Is obesity stigmatizing? Body weight, perceived discrimination, and psychological well-being in the United States. J Health Soc Behav. 2005;46:244–259. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rosenberger PH, Henderson KE, Grilo CM. Correlates of body image dissatisfaction in extremely obese female bariatric surgery candidates. Obes Surg. 2006;16:1331–1336. doi: 10.1381/096089206778663788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Steptoe A, Breeze E, Banks J, Nazroo J. Cohort profile: the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42:1640–1648. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hill AJ, Williams J. Psychological health in a non-clinical sample of obese women. Int J Obes. 1998;22:578–583. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0800631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sutin AR, Terracciano A. Perceived Weight Discrimination and Obesity. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e70048. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hyde M, Wiggins RD, Higgs P, Blane DB. A measure of quality of life in early old age: the theory, development and properties of a needs satisfaction model (CASP-19) Aging Ment Health. 2003;7:186–194. doi: 10.1080/1360786031000101157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J Assess. 1985;49:71–75. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Turvey CL, Wallace RB, Herzog R. A revised CES-D measure of depressive symptoms and a DSM-based measure of major depressive episodes in the elderly. Int Psychogeriatr IPA. 1999;11:139–148. doi: 10.1017/s1041610299005694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Banks J, Karlsen S, Oldfield Z. Socio-economic position. 2003 [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wang Y, Beydoun MA. The Obesity Epidemic in the United States—Gender, Age, Socioeconomic, Racial/Ethnic, and Geographic Characteristics: A Systematic Review and Meta-Regression Analysis. Epidemiol Rev. 2007;29:6–28. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxm007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Zaninotto P, Falaschetti E, Sacker A. Age trajectories of quality of life among older adults: results from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Qual Life Res. 2009;18:1301–1309. doi: 10.1007/s11136-009-9543-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Murrell SA, Himmelfarb S, Wright K. Prevalence of Depression and Its Correlates in Older Adults. Am J Epidemiol. 1983;117:173–185. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a113528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sobel ME. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociol Methodol. 1982;13:290–312. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51:1173. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004;36:717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol Methods. 2002;7:422–445. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Phinney JS, Madden T, Santos LJ. Psychological Variables as Predictors of Perceived Ethnic Discrimination Among Minority and Immigrant Adolescents1. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1998;28:937–953. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40:879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Vartanian LR, Smyth JM. Primum Non Nocere: Obesity Stigma and Public Health. J Bioethical Inq. 2013;10:49–57. doi: 10.1007/s11673-012-9412-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.MacLean L, Edwards N, Garrard M, Sims-Jones N, Clinton K, Ashley L. Obesity, stigma and public health planning. Health Promot Int. 2009;24:88–93. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dan041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lillis J, Hayes SC, Bunting K, Masuda A. Teaching acceptance and mindfulness to improve the lives of the obese: a preliminary test of a theoretical model. Ann Behav Med Publ Soc Behav Med. 2009;37:58–69. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9083-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Puhl RM, Andreyeva T, Brownell KD. Perceptions of weight discrimination: prevalence and comparison to race and gender discrimination in America. Int J Obes 2005. 2008;32:992–1000. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kolotkin RL, Crosby R, Pendleton R, Strong M, Gress R, Adams T. Health-Related Quality of Life in Patients Seeking Gastric Bypass Surgery vs Non-Treatment-Seeking Controls. Obes Surg. 2003;13:371–377. doi: 10.1381/096089203765887688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Miras AD, Al-Najim W, Jackson SN, et al. Psychological characteristics, eating behavior, and quality of life assessment of obese patients undergoing weight loss interventions. Scand J Surg SJS Off Organ Finn Surg Soc Scand Surg Soc. 2014 doi: 10.1177/1457496914543977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Berrington de Gonzalez A, Hartge P, Cerhan JR, et al. Body-Mass Index and Mortality among 1.46 Million White Adults. N Engl J Med. 2010;363:2211–2219. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1000367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp TableS1-S2

RESOURCES