Abstract
BACKGROUND
Because dopamine neurons signal a mismatch between expected and actual reward called prediction error (PE), aberrant PE signals in schizophrenia have been attributed to known dopaminergic abnormalities. However, dysfunction of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors on cortical γ-aminobutyric acid neurons, as hypothesized in schizophrenia, could lead to excess glutamate release in the substantia nigra (SN) and affect reward processing.
AIMS
The aim of this study was to investigate the contribution of SN glutamate to PE signals in healthy controls (HC) and patients with schizophrenia (SZ).
METHODS
We recruited 22 medicated SZ and 19 HC. We obtained (1) functional magnetic resonance imaging during a probabilistic monetary reward task to assess PE-related blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal and (2) magnetic resonance spectroscopy to measure Glx (glutamate+glutamine) in the SN. To identify group differences in regions where the BOLD signal varies as a function of PE, we analyzed PEs generated during the task as parametric modulators of reward delivery. Finally, we examined the correlation of PE-related BOLD signal and SN Glx in each group.
RESULTS
Relative to HC, PE-related BOLD signals in SZ were significantly different in the midbrain/SN and ventral striatum. In SZ, SN Glx was significantly elevated. In HC, but not in SZ, PE-related BOLD signal in SN was positively correlated with SN Glx.
CONCLUSIONS
These results suggest a role of glutamate in the neural coding of PE in controls. They indicate that glutamatergic dysfunction might contribute to abnormal PE coding in schizophrenia, suggesting the use of glutamate-targeted approaches to improve these deficits.
INTRODUCTION
Reinforcement learning models posit that, to maximize reward, learning from our environment occurs by comparing expected outcomes with attained outcomes. Prediction error (PE) signals are generated when outcomes deviate from predictions, which leads to updating of reward expectations. Reward processing and, most specifically, PE are linked to dopaminergic function.1 Electrophysiological studies of midbrain dopamine neurons in primates show the firing of neurons increase when a reward exceeds what was predicted and decrease when a reward is less than predicted.2 In schizophrenia, it is hypothesized that known dopamine abnormalities3 could lead to aberrant encoding of PE signals.4 In this context, some symptoms could stem from aberrant attribution of salience to irrelevant stimuli, such as delusions, or from reduced attribution of salience to rewarding events, such as anhedonia.5,6 Imaging studies in schizophrenia have registered aberrant PE signals during reward processing and related these to symptoms.7–10
The revised glutamatergic hypothesis of schizophrenia proposes that blockade of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors on γ-aminobutyric acid neurons could result in a disinhibition of glutamatergic neurons leading to excess glutamate release in projection areas.11,12 Because both the substantia nigra (SN) and the striatum receive glutamatergic projections from cortical areas,13,14 abnormal cortical glutamate transmission could affect these regions. Consistent with this model, a recent proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS) study found higher glutamate+glutamine (Glx) levels in the striatum of antipsychotic-naive patients with schizophrenia.15 We previously reported the results of a 1H-MRS study of the SN in medicated patients. Although we did not observe differences in Glx, the Glx/N-acetyl-aspartate ratio was significantly elevated in patients, possibly indexing a glutamatergic dysfunction.16 Therefore, glutamate dysfunction in the SN could affect reward processing. However, little is known about the contribution of glutamate to reward both in general and also to its dysfunction in schizophrenia.
The aim of this study was to investigate the contribution of glutamate to PE signals in healthy controls (HC) and patients with schizophrenia. We combined functional MRI (fMRI) during PE processing with single-voxel 1H-MRS in the SN to obtain Glx measurements. We hypothesized that we would replicate findings of abnormal PE-related neural signals in the SN in patients. In addition, for the first time, we explore the contribution of Glx to PE-related neural signals and its implication in schizophrenia.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
We enrolled 22 medicated participants with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (SZ), recruited from outpatient clinics at the University of Alabama at Birmingham, and 19 matched HC, recruited via advertisement. After being deemed able to give consent,17 informed consent was provided. Approval by the Institutional Review Board was obtained. All participants were recruited for a multimodal neuroimaging study of reward. Neurometabolite measurements of some participants have been included in another report.16
Diagnoses were established through review of medical records, two clinician consensus, and the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies. Exclusion criteria were major medical/neurological conditions, pregnancy, substance abuse within 6 months of enrollment, and head injury with loss of consciousness. HC were without history of Axis I disorders or family history of psychosis. Cognitive functioning was characterized by the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status,18 and symptom severity by the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale.19
Reward task
After a training session, subjects performed a probabilistic monetary reward decision task modeled after Rolls et al.20 during fMRI (six runs of 25 trials; Figure 1). Each trial consisted of three conditions (Decision, Decision Display and Reward Presentation). Each condition was displayed in a pseudo-randomly jittered fashion lasting 2, 4, or 6 s (total of 10 s per trial). During Decision, participants selected either a large reward of 30¢ or a smaller reward of 10¢ by pushing a right or left box. Although the probability of receiving 10¢ remained constant (0.9), the probability of receiving 30¢ varied between runs (0.1, 0.33, and 0.9). Participants were informed that the left/right position of the different reward amounts and probability of receiving the reward of higher magnitude would change from run to run, but remain constant within a given run. That is, for a given run, the left/right position of the 10¢/30¢ choice would not change. During Decision Display, to indicate a response had been made, the color of the box selected changed. During Reward Presentation, the reward magnitude (RM) earned during a given trial (0¢, 10¢, or 30¢) was displayed. Subjects were instructed to sample both sides offered in each run to determine which selection was more advantageous, with the goal of maximizing the amount of money earned. As previously reported,20 subjects took less than 10 trials to adjust to change in probability and develop an expected value (EV) of that run. After the 10th trial of each run, the EV (RM × probability) for selecting 10¢ throughout the run was 9¢ and, based on increasing probability levels, the EVs for selecting 30¢ were 3¢, 10¢, or 27¢. Thus, after the 10th trial, the task was such that subjects generated PE. PE was calculated as the difference between the RM for each trial and EV for that run (RM − EV; that is, if EV = 9¢ (0.9 × 10¢), but RM = 0¢, then PE = −9). PE could take any one of the following values: −27, −10, −9, −3, 1, 3, 20, and 27.
Image acquisition
All scanning was done on a 3 T Siemens Allegra head-only scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). A high-resolution structural scan was acquired for anatomic reference (magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE); TR/TE/inversion time = 2,300/3.93/1,100 ms, flip angle = 12°, 256 × 256 matrix, 1 mm isotropic voxels).
fMRI data were acquired using the gradient-recalled echo-planar imaging sequence (repetition time/echo time (TR/TE) = 2,000/25 ms, flip angle = 70°, field of view = 192 mm, 6 mm slice thickness, 32 axial slices). An IFIS-SA system (MRI Devices, Corp., Waukesha, WI, USA) running E-Prime (version 1.2, Psychology Software Tools Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, USA) was used to control stimulus delivery and record responses.
We used a turbo spin echo sequence with magnetization transfer contrast to visualize the SN and aid in placement of an 1H-MRS voxel (13 × 13 × 13 mm; Figure 2) positioned around the left SN. Following manual shimming, water-suppressed spectra were collected with the point-resolved spectroscopy sequence (TR/TE = 2,000/80 ms, 640 averages; for details see refs 16, 21).
Statistical analyses
Demographics and behavioral data
Independent samples’ t-tests and χ2-tests were used to compare groups on demographic and behavioral variables. A general linear model was used to determine whether HC and SZ performed the task in a similar manner. Each participant’s response during every trial was binarized to indicate a left or a right button press. These values were entered as the dependent variable in a linear regression. Fixed independent factors were entered to define each of the six sessions and each of the 25 trials. Group was entered as a random factor and participant identification was entered as a covariate. A planned contrast was conducted for the outcome of diagnostic status as a predictor of trial response.
fMRI
Data were analyzed using SPM8 (Wellcome Trust, London, UK). Preprocessing included slice-timing correction, realignment, artifact and motion correction using ArtRepair, coregistration to the structural scan, normalization to Montreal Neurological Institute space, and smoothing (4 mm) using DARTEL.22
First-level analyses were conducted for each participant with a general linear model to determine the relationship between observed event-related blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal and regressors representing expected neural responses to trial events. To examine the effects of reward separate from learning, the first 10 trials of each run were excluded from analysis.20 Decision events (at the time of button press) and reward presentations (at the midpoint of the reward presentation window) were modeled as stick functions in the general linear model along with their first-order temporal derivatives. In addition, in order to identify regions where the BOLD signal changed as a function of PE, reward presentation events were parametrically modulated (correlated) by their respective PE, with values ranging from − 27 to 27. The first-order PE regressor was orthogonalized to the reward presentation to ensure it was uniquely specified and validly estimated.23 Contrasts were carried forward to the second level for within- and between-group analyses. Whole-brain analyses were corrected for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate with significance level set to P<0.01. In addition, we conducted region of interest analyses using masks from the WFU pickatlas24 for the midbrain/SN (TD lobes) and bilateral ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens (IBASPM 71). The significance level was set to P<0.05 using small-volume corrections (SVC).
1H-MRS
1H-MRS data were quantified in the time domain, incorporating prior knowledge derived from in vitro and in vivo metabolite spectra (for details see refs 25–27). Cramer-Rao lower bounds, an estimate of uncertainty, were calculated for each peak; data with Cramer-Rao lower bounds >30% were excluded. Glx was quantified with respect to creatine, and will hereafter be referred to as Glx. Spectroscopy data were not obtained in 1 SZ completing the reward task, spectral quality was poor in 4 HC and 5 SZ, and 1 SZ was excluded as an outlier (>3 s.d. above mean), leaving 15 HC and 15 SZ in analyses involving Glx. An analysis of covariance with age and smoking as covariates was performed to assess group differences in Glx.
Combined fMRI/MRS
Regression analyses were performed in SPM8 to identify regions in the midbrain/SN and ventral striatum where the linear relationship between PE and BOLD during Reward Presentation was correlated with SN Glx. The analysis was performed in HC and SZ using the same masks as above with SVC (P<0.05). To visualize the distribution of variance associated with these analyses, we extracted the first eigenvariate (from the main effect of PE-related BOLD signal for each individual) in the midbrain area where SN Glx was found to be correlated. We then plotted the first eigenvariate of the PE-related BOLD signal against SN Glx.
RESULTS
We found no differences in demographics, but SZ more commonly were smokers and, as expected, had lower Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status scores. There were no differences between the groups in the amount of reward earned or the amount of PE generated by their response pattern. In addition, the analysis of task performance indicated that the distribution of choices made (right versus left) was not statistically different between groups (Table 1). SN Glx was significantly higher in SZ (0.69 ± 0.21) compared with HC (0.57 ± 0.24; F = 5.60; P = 0.03).
Table 1.
SZ (n =22) | HC (n = 19) | t/X2 | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Gender (% male) | 77.3 | 57.9 | 1.77 | 0.31 |
Age | 39.41 (6.70) | 36.47 (12.12) | −0.74 | 0.47 |
Parental occupationb | 6.70 (5.05) | 7.50 (4.76) | 12.52 | 0.25 |
Smoking status (% smokers) | 72.7 | 42.1 | 4.82 | 0.03 |
Smoking (packs per day) | 0.66 (0.60) | 0.36 (0.50) | −1.74 | 0.09 |
Diagnosis | ||||
Schizophrenia | 15 | |||
Schizoaffective disorder | 7 | |||
Illness duration (in years) | 17.68 (11.53) | |||
Antipsychotic medication | ||||
First generation | 2 | |||
Second generation | 17 | |||
First and second generations | 1 | |||
Clozapinec | 2 | |||
BPRSd | ||||
Total | 30.27 (8.86) | |||
Positive | 5.77 (3.74) | |||
Negative | 4.27 (1.75) | |||
RBANS total index | 76.14 (9.33) | 93.32 (11.49) | 5.28 | < 0.01 |
Prediction error task | ||||
Total reward earned ($) | 11.71 (1.59) | 12.41 (1.14) | 1.59 | 0.12 |
Mean prediction error ($) | −0.19 (0.33) | −0.31 (0.33) | −1.13 | 0.26 |
Task performancee | 0.03 (0.01) | 0.01 (0.02) | 2.32f | 0.13 |
Abbreviations: HC, healthy controls; RBANS, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status; SZ, schizophrenia.
Mean (s.d.) unless indicated otherwise.
Ranks determined from Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (1–18 scale); higher rank (lower numerical value) corresponds to higher socioeconomic status. Parental occupation unknown in three HC and two patients with schizophrenia, n = 36.
One SZ with clozapine monotherapy and one SZ with combination of clozapine and ziprasidone.
Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (1–7 scale); positive (conceptual disorganization, hallucinatory behavior, and unusual thought content); negative (emotional withdrawal, motor retardation, and blunted affect).
To assess diagnostic status as predictor of trial response, linear regression was conducted (button press as dependent variable, sessions and trials as fixed independent factors, and group as random factor). Values reported in parentheses are s.e.
Reported value is F-statistic.
fMRI results
In HC (Supplementary Table 1, Supplementary Figure S1), we found positive changes in the BOLD signal as a function of PE (positive PE-related BOLD changes) in the orbitofrontal cortex, bilateral caudate, angular gyrus, and occipital cortex, as well as negative PE-related BOLD changes in frontal regions including the anterior cingulate cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, parietal cortices, insula, basal ganglia, and thalamus that are largely consistent with prior findings.20,28
Between-group analyses
PE-related BOLD signals in SZ (Supplementary Table 1, Supplementary Figure S1) were significantly different than those of HC in the following regions: inferior and middle frontal cortices, insula, caudate/ventral striatum, pallidum/putamen, thalamus, and midbrain/SN (also see Table 2). In region of interest analyses (Figure 3), PE-related BOLD signal was significantly different between SZ and HC in the ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens (cluster 1: t = 3.45, kE = 18, x = 8, y = 7, z = − 7; cluster 2: t = 3.05, kE = 19, x = − 16, y = 9, z = −13) and the midbrain/SN (cluster 1: t = 4.11, kE = 1,414, x = 6, y = −30, z = −12).
Table 2.
Brain regions | Voxels in cluster | Hem. | Voxels in region |
Peak coordinatesa
|
Peak t | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
X | Y | Z | |||||
PE-related BOLD signal | |||||||
HC>SZ | |||||||
None | |||||||
SZ>HC | |||||||
Cluster 1 | 269 | 35 | 30 | 24 | 4.20 | ||
Inferior frontal cortex | B | 156 | |||||
Middle frontal cortex | B | 105 | |||||
Cluster 2 | 473 | 14 | 14 | −2 | 2.87 | ||
Inferior frontal cortex | R | 43 | |||||
Insula | R | 23 | |||||
Caudate/ventral striatum | R | 163 | |||||
Pallidum/putamen | R | 108 | |||||
Cluster 3 | 346 | 18 | 11 | 15 | 4.81 | ||
Insula | R | 10 | |||||
Caudate | R | 168 | |||||
Putamen | R | 22 | |||||
Cluster 4 | 540 | 6 | −23 | 3 | 4.58 | ||
Thalamus | R | 130 | |||||
Cluster 5 | 695 | 15 | −36 | −38 | 5.53 | ||
Midbrain/substantia nigra | B | 695 |
Abbreviations: B, bilateral; BOLD, blood oxygen level dependent; HC, healthy controls; Hem., hemisphere; PE, prediction error; R, right; SZ, schizophrenia.
Reported in Montreal Neurologic Institute coordinates.
Combined fMRI and MRS results
In HC, but not SZ, we found a significant correlation between the PE-related BOLD signal in SN and SN Glx (Figure 4a; t = 4.60, kE = 100, x = −12, y = −23, z = −19). Figure 4b scatterplots showing the distribution of variance in the relationship between Glx and PE BOLD response in HC (r = 0.74) and SZ (r = 0.30).
DISCUSSION
To our knowledge, this is the first study reporting the relationship between PE processing and SN Glx and its implications in schizophrenia. In SZ, we found abnormal PE-related neural response in the midbrain, ventral striatum, caudate, thalamus, orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices as well as significantly elevated SN Glx. In HC, but not in SZ, the neural response to PE in the SN was positively correlated with SN Glx. These results suggest a role of glutamate in the neural coding of PE in HC, and that glutamatergic dysfunction might contribute to its abnormal coding in patients with schizophrenia.
Despite differences in experimental design and analyses, several studies in medicated and unmedicated patients have identified neural abnormalities during the encoding of PE, most prominently in the ventral striatum.29 Although some studies investigated reward-conditioning paradigms on a trial-by-trial level,4,8,10 others7,9 examined PE trials generated after conditioning completion like we did. Starting after the 10th trial, the behavioral analyses of our PE task show that SZ had learned the contingencies of the task during the first 10 trials. Compared with HC, there were no differences in the amount of reward earned or the amount of PE generated by their response pattern. In addition, the analysis of task performance indicated that the distribution of choices made (right versus left) were not statistically different between groups. This finding is consistent with the results of others.30–32 After the 10th trial, when the expected value of each trial was known to the participants, PEs were analyzed as parametric modulators of reward delivery. Like others,4,7–10 we found abnormalities of PE in dopamine-rich areas, including the SN, ventral striatum, caudate, and thalamus. In addition, in HC, correlation with the PE signal was also observed in dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and posterior parietal cortex, implying that executive processes were engaged. Interestingly, those regions were not differentially associated with the PE signal in patients, suggesting that abnormalities in PE in patients originate from bottom-up rather than top-down processing. Like others, we observed abnormal PE signals in patients who were medicated (mainly second-generation antipsychotics), indicating that dopamine D2 blockade does not normalize PE abnormalities. Interestingly, a study in first-episode patients found normalization of a reward anticipation-related ventral striatum hypofunction after treatment.33 It remains to be determined whether treatment does in fact reduce, but obviously not normalize, PE abnormalities, and whether treatment has a differential effect on PE dysfunction in first episode compared with chronic patients with schizophrenia.
Our finding of elevated SN Glx in SZ is consistent with previous finding of elevated Glx measured in the striatum,15 putatively suggesting excessive glutamate release from cortical glutamatergic projections to basal ganglia. However, although elevated striatum Glx was observed in unmedicated patients, our observations derive from medicated patients. The participants enrolled in this study overlap with those included in a prior report where, although no differences in Glx were observed, the Glx/N-acetyl-aspartate ratio was significantly elevated in patients, possibly indexing a glutamatergic dysfunction.16 Our findings are also consistent with the identification of abnormal expression of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor-associated intracellular proteins in the SN in schizophrenia.34 In addition to cortical projections, glutamatergic projections to the ventral tegmental area originate from subcortical structures, including the subthalamic nucleus and the pedunculopontine tegmentum.35 Increased SN Glx might also reflect a local, rather than a projected disturbance. Recent optogenetic studies in rodents have demonstrated that mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons release glutamate in the nucleus accumbens, suggesting colocalization of glutamate and dopamine receptors in some midbrain neurons.36,37 Glutamatergic abnormalities have now been identified in schizophrenia in the basal ganglia, hippocampus, and medial prefrontal cortex.15,38–40 It remains to be determined whether these abnormalities originate from a similar local dysfunction, such as γ-aminobutyric acid interneuron abnormalities, or whether they are connected, one impacting the others, and the extent to which they are affected by treatment.
In HC, we observed a correlation between the PE signal and SN Glx in the SN. Given that the burst firing of dopamine neurons recorded during PE signals can be driven by application of glutamate to dopamine neurons or by stimulation of glutamatergic afferents,41 it is tempting to speculate that this correlation reflects the drive of glutamatergic projections to dopamine neurons in the SN. In the context of elevated SN Glx, the correlation between PE signal and Glx was not present in patients, suggesting that glutamatergic dysfunction could contribute to aberrant PE signaling. Consistent with our findings, low-dose administration of the N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist ketamine disrupts error-dependent associative learning in healthy subjects.42
There are several limitations in our study. We quantified Glx using creatine as an internal reference because we did not collect unsuppressed water spectra or image an external phantom. As there may be creatine abnormalities in schizophrenia43 (but also see ref. 44), this study should be repeated using absolute quantitation. We did not correct Glx values for voxel gray matter content because of the limitations of our acquisition protocol. Future studies would benefit from acquiring a three-dimensional image with magnetization transfer contrast for the purposes of tissue segmentation.45 We used a large number of averages to increase the signal-to-noise ratio, which leads to a long scan time. Although this was well tolerated among our participants, it may not be ideal for all clinical populations. Our participants with schizophrenia were medicated, which could confound 1H-MRS measurements.38,46 Additional studies will be needed to determine the effect of antipsychotic medications. Illness stage and clinical status are important factors to consider in future studies, as they may also account for some variability in findings.
In summary, our results suggest a role of glutamate in the neural coding of PE and that glutamatergic dysfunction, such as the one we identified in the SN, might contribute to abnormal PE coding in schizophrenia. Because aberrant PE signals are found both in medicated and unmedicated patients, it suggests that dopamine D2 blockade may not reverse those deficits. Therefore, our results support the use of glutamate-targeted approaches to improve these deficits.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The authors thank all the volunteers with schizophrenia who took part in this project. The authors also thank Dr Luke Stoeckel and Dr Kathy Avsar for their contribution to this project and Dr Tim Gawne for comments on the manuscript.
FUNDING: Adrienne C Lahti received funding from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH081014, R01 MH102951). This work was funded by National Institute of Mental Health; Grant numbers: R01 MH081014, R01 MH102951.
Footnotes
CONTRIBUTIONS
A.C.L. was responsible for conception and design of the study and oversaw data acquisition, analysis, and interpretation as well as drafting and critical revision of the manuscript. DMW contributed to data acquisition, analysis and interpretation, and drafting of the manuscript. NVK contributed to data interpretation and drafting of the manuscript. MAR contributed to data acquisition, analysis, and drafting of the manuscript.
COMPETING INTERESTS
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Supplemental Information accompanies the paper on the npj Schizophrenia website (http://www.nature.com/npjschz)
References
- 1.Schultz W, Dickinson A. Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:473–500. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mirenowicz J, Schultz W. Importance of unpredictability for reward responses in primate dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:1024–1027. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.2.1024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kegeles LS, Abi-Dargham A, Frankle WG, Gil R, Cooper TB, Slifstein M, et al. Increased synaptic dopamine function in associative regions of the striatum in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:231–239. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Murray GK, Corlett PR, Clark L, Pessiglione M, Blackwell AD, Honey G, et al. Substantia nigra/ventral tegmental reward prediction error disruption in psychosis. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13:239, 267–276. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Smith A, Li M, Becker S, Kapur S. Dopamine, prediction error and associative learning: a model-based account. Network. 2006;17:61–84. doi: 10.1080/09548980500361624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Strauss GP, Waltz JA, Gold JM. A review of reward processing and motivational impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(Suppl 2):S107–S116. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Waltz JA, Schweitzer JB, Gold JM, Kurup PK, Ross TJ, Salmeron BJ, et al. Patients with schizophrenia have a reduced neural response to both unpredictable and predictable primary reinforcers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1567–1577. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Koch K, Schachtzabel C, Wagner G, Schikora J, Schultz C, Reichenbach JR, et al. Altered activation in association with reward-related trial-and-error learning in patients with schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2010;50:223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Morris RW, Vercammen A, Lenroot R, Moore L, Langton JM, Short B, et al. Disambiguating ventral striatum fMRI-related BOLD signal during reward prediction in schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17:235, 280–289. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gradin VB, Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Stickle C, Milders M, et al. Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain. 2011;134(Pt 6):1751–1764. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Olney JW, Farber NB. Glutamate receptor dysfunction and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:998–1007. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240016004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Olney JW, Wozniak DF, Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Farber NB, Bittigau P, Ikonomidou C. Drug-induced apoptotic neurodegeneration in the developing brain. Brain Pathol. 2002;12:488–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2002.tb00467.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Frankle WG, Laruelle M, Haber SN. Prefrontal cortical projections to the midbrain in primates: evidence for a sparse connection. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1627–1636. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sesack SR, Carr DB. Selective prefrontal cortex inputs to dopamine cells: implications for schizophrenia. Physiol Behav. 2002;77:513–517. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00931-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.de la Fuente-Sandoval C, Leon-Ortiz P, Favila R, Stephano S, Mamo D, Ramirez-Bermudez J, et al. Higher levels of glutamate in the associative-striatum of subjects with prodromal symptoms of schizophrenia and patients with first-episode psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:1781–1791. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Reid MA, Kraguljac NV, Avsar KB, White DM, den Hollander JA, Lahti AC. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the substantia nigra in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2013;147:348–354. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.04.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Carpenter WT, Jr, Gold JM, Lahti AC, Queern CA, Conley RR, Bartko JJ, et al. Decisional capacity for informed consent in schizophrenia research. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:533–538. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.6.533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Randolph C, Tierney MC, Mohr E, Chase TN. The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS): preliminary clinical validity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1998;20:310–319. doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.3.310.823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Overall J. The brief psychiatric rating scale. Psychological Rep. 1962;10:799–812. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rolls ET, McCabe C, Redoute J. Expected value, reward outcome, and temporal difference error representations in a probabilistic decision task. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:652–663. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bolding MS, Reid MA, Avsar KB, Roberts RC, Gamlin PD, Gawne TJ, et al. Magnetic transfer contrast accurately localizes substantia nigra confirmed by histology. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73:289–294. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. 2007;38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Henson RN, Flandin G, Friston KJ, Mattout J. A Parametric Empirical Bayesian framework for fMRI-constrained MEG/EEG source reconstruction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31:1512–1531. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Burdette JH. An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fmri data sets. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1233–1239. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hutcheson NL, Reid MA, White DM, Kraguljac NV, Avsar KB, Bolding MS, et al. Multimodal analysis of the hippocampus in schizophrenia using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Schizophr Res. 2012;140:136–142. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.06.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kraguljac NV, Reid MA, White DM, den Hollander J, Lahti AC. Regional decoupling of N-acetyl-aspartate and glutamate in Schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:2635–2642. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Reid MA, Stoeckel LE, White DM, Avsar KB, Bolding MS, Akella NS, et al. Assessments of function and biochemistry of the anterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:625–633. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lin A, Adolphs R, Rangel A. Social and monetary reward learning engage overlapping neural substrates. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;7:274–281. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Deserno L, Boehme R, Heinz A, Schlagenhauf F. Reinforcement learning and dopamine in schizophrenia: dimensions of symptoms or specific features of a disease group? Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:172. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Keri S, Kelemen O, Szekeres G, Bagoczky N, Erdelyi R, Antal A, et al. Schizophrenics know more than they can tell: probabilistic classification learning in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2000;30:149–155. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Weickert TW, Terrazas A, Bigelow LB, Malley JD, Hyde T, Egan MF, et al. Habit and skill learning in schizophrenia: evidence of normal striatal processing with abnormal cortical input. Learn Mem. 2002;9:430–442. doi: 10.1101/lm.49102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Weickert TW, Goldberg TE, Callicott JH, Chen Q, Apud JA, Das S, et al. Neural correlates of probabilistic category learning in patients with schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2009;29:1244–1254. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4341-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nielsen MO, Rostrup E, Wulff S, Bak N, Broberg BV, Lublin H, et al. Improvement of brain reward abnormalities by antipsychotic monotherapy in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:1195–1204. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mueller HT, Haroutunian V, Davis KL, Meador-Woodruff JH. Expression of the ionotropic glutamate receptor subunits and NMDA receptor-associated intracellular proteins in the substantia nigra in schizophrenia. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2004;121:60–69. doi: 10.1016/j.molbrainres.2003.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Geisler S, Derst C, Veh RW, Zahm DS. Glutamatergic afferents of the ventral tegmental area in the rat. J Neurosci. 2007;27:5730–5743. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0012-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tecuapetla F, Patel JC, Xenias H, English D, Tadros I, Shah F, et al. Glutamatergic signaling by mesolimbic dopamine neurons in the nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7105–7110. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0265-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stuber GD, Hnasko TS, Britt JP, Edwards RH, Bonci A. Dopaminergic terminals in the nucleus accumbens but not the dorsal striatum corelease glutamate. J Neurosci. 2010;30:8229–8233. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1754-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kegeles LS, Mao X, Stanford AD, Girgis R, Ojeil N, Xu X, et al. Elevated prefrontal cortex gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamate-glutamine levels in schizophrenia measured in vivo with proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:449–459. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kraguljac NV, White DM, Reid MA, Lahti AC. Increased hippocampal glutamate and volumetric deficits in unmedicated patients with schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:1294–1302. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.2437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Stan AD, Ghose S, Zhao C, Hulsey K, Mihalakos P, Yanagi M, et al. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy and tissue protein concentrations together suggest lower glutamate signaling in dentate gyrus in schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:1–7. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Grace AA. Dopamine modulation of forebrain pathways and the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. In: Iversen LL, Iversen DD, Dunnett SS, Bjorklnd A, editors. Dopamine Handbook. Oxford University Press; Oxford, England: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Corlett PR, Honey GD, Aitken MR, Dickinson A, Shanks DR, Absalom AR, et al. Frontal responses during learning predict vulnerability to the psychotogenic effects of ketamine: linking cognition, brain activity, and psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:611–621. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.6.611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Öngür D, Prescot AP, Jensen JE, Cohen BM, Renshaw PF. Creatine abnormalities in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2009;172:44–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kraguljac NV, Reid M, White D, Jones R, den Hollander J, Lowman D, et al. Neurometabolites in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder—a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2012;203:111–125. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Helms G, Draganski B, Frackowiak R, Ashburner J, Weiskopf N. Improved segmentation of deep brain grey matter structures using magnetization transfer (MT) parameter maps. Neuroimage. 2009;47:194–198. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.de la Fuente-Sandoval C, Leon-Ortiz P, Azcarraga M, Stephano S, Favila R, Diaz-Galvis L, et al. Glutamate levels in the associative striatum before and after 4 weeks of antipsychotic treatment in first-episode psychosis: a longitudinal proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:1057–1066. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.