The commentary by Sherlock and Zietsch raises important questions about how the science of human psychological development moves forward as we make dramatic strides in understanding the role of genetics. In this response we will return to these larger questions, but we begin by addressing S&Z’s specific argument that “genetic contributions should almost always be considered when dealing with associations between parent and child behavior.” In our 60-year study of the association between childhood warmth and late-life security of attachment, we did not mention the potential influence of genetic factors, and we regret this omission. Moreover, we acknowledge that in some places in the manuscript we used causal language that was not justified by the design of the study. Although we explicitly stated that the association between childhood warmth and late-life security of attachment is correlational and not causal, we went on to interpret these findings as indications of support for the influence of childhood environment on adult wellbeing. Although this is a plausible interpretation, S&Z are justified in arguing that genetic influences could contribute to this link and that our article should have noted this. The editorial review process encourages investigators to highlight the implications of their work, and in our enthusiasm to convey the importance of an association that spans 6 decades, we did not adequately address possible alternative explanations for this association.
S&Z’s commentary prompts us to consider two sets of issues – those particular to the field of attachment research and broader concerns about how we make sense of genetic and environmental influences in human development. S&Z raise important questions about the role of genes and the environment in shaping attachment phenemona. The twin studies cited by S&Z consistently find genetic influences on attachment styles and behavior. These studies (Brussoni et al, 2000; Piccardi et al, 2011; Fearon et al, 2014) estimate genetic influences at 25–45% of the variance in attachment outcomes, with the larger proportion accounted for by shared and nonshared environmental influences. Low estimates of shared environmental influence in some twin studies (e.g., Fearon et al, 2014) are often cited as evidence that parents do not significantly influence children’s security of attachment. This interpretation involves reducing the influences of family upbringing to shared environment. However, each child – even a twin – has different experiences within the same family, as each child has a unique relationship with parents and plays different roles. Parents are thus likely to contribute to the nonshared as well as the shared environment that impacts each child.
With respect to the determinants of attachment across the lifespan, it is critical to recognize that attachment is not a unitary construct. There is ample empirical evidence that elements of attachment vary in important ways across developmental epochs and types of relationships (parents vs. romantic partners), and that the mode of assessment (interview vs. self-report vs. observation) shapes the nature of the construct being assessed (Roisman et al, 2007). None of the research cited by S&Z examines predictors of late-life romantic attachment, which was the focus of our study. Instead, all focus on infancy, childhood, and young adulthood. Moreover, the Brussoni et al. and Picardi et al. studies cited by S&Z use self-report measures of attachment. There is evidence that the interview-based measure used in our work assesses aspects of attachment that may not be captured by traditional self-report approaches (Roisman et al, 2007). Therefore, there exists little empirical basis for estimating what portions of variance in late-life attachment – particularly as assessed by the Current Relationship Interview – are accounted for by genes, shared environment, and nonshared environment. It is possible – even likely – that there is a genetic component to late-life romantic attachment, but the studies cited by S&Z do not provide any specific evidence that this is the case. Employing the Current Relationship Interview in a genetically informative study could greatly enhance our understanding of the causal role of genes and environment in late-life attachment.
Rigorous experimental work with animals provides persuasive evidence for the influence that parenting and early environments can have on development, including relationship behaviors relevant to attachment (e.g., Harlow et al, 1965; Soumi et al, 1987; Weaver, Meaney, & Szyf, 2006). Opportunities to trace connections from childhood to late-life attachment across 6 decades are exceedingly rare, and the challenge is to understand the mechanisms that are responsible for such long-term links. The field would benefit from continued research with animals and humans that can help shed light on the mechanisms by which both environments and genes contribute to long-term developmental outcomes.
The broader questions for our field concern the underlying goals of psychological science and the best pathways toward unearthing causal mechanisms that underlie psychological traits. Part of the wisdom in S&Z’s commentary is their caution about oversimplifying complex phenomena. Determining the degree to which a particular behavioral phenomenon is shaped by genes and environmental processes is complicated. There is disagreement about how heritability estimates derived from twin studies should be interpreted and about their potential limitations and utility (Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). One danger of focusing too narrowly on estimates of the strength of heritability is that such emphasis might discourage investigators from investigating non-heritable influences. For example, the fact that height is estimated to be highly heritable across human populations does not preclude the profound environmental impact of preventable factors such as malnutrition and disease (Silventoinen et al, 2003).
The notion that genes and environment make identifiably separate contributions to human psychological development engenders a dichotomy that runs counter to much of what we know about biology (Gottlieb, 2007). Heritability studies may inadvertently fuel this dichotomous thinking by parsing variance into discrete packets. Although some phenotypes (e.g., cystic fibrosis) can be traced to specific genes and are predominantly genetic in origin, the origins of most phenotypes are decidedly mixed. This reflects the fact that genes and environment interact in every moment of development. DNA codes for varieties of RNA, which in turn code for proteins which build cells that form complex structures that underlie psychological traits. At every juncture there is opportunity for feedback from and interaction with the environment and other critical biological systems (e.g., neural, hormonal) to shape how genes are expressed. Thus, a DNA sequence operating in different environments can generate different products in different amounts at the cellular and phenotypic levels. This interactionist emphasis, of course, also implies that the same environment operating on people with different genetic makeups can generate different outcomes. Pinker (2004) reminds us that the extremes of both rigid interactionism and rigid genetic or environmental determinism fail to account for the complexity of most behavioral and developmental phenotypes.
As psychological scientists, we endeavor both to discover new knowledge and to identify possible targets for improving the human condition. Three decades ago, Rutter (1987) wisely pointed out that prediction alone is not sufficient; understanding mechanisms is critical to this endeavor. A key aspect of our study involved examining the adaptiveness of midlife emotion-regulatory styles as a possible mechanism by which childhood nurturance might result in more secure late-life attachment. The finding that adaptive emotion-regulatory styles did, in fact, partially mediate the link between childhood experience and late-life functioning points to a potential target for intervention to help those emerging from less nurturing backgrounds to achieve more stable and adaptive intimate relationships. The findings do not prove causation and clearly require replication, but they constitute a pointer toward what may be a helpful avenue for intervention.
In an era when research is based increasingly on technical expertise that is not shared across the field, integration of methods and findings is challenging but essential. Both experimental and non-experimental research have important limitations. The benefits to internal validity that are the centerpiece of experimental research derive in large part from their isolated study of one or two mechanisms. Non-experimental research – whether genetic or psychosocial – is hindered by difficulties ruling out potential confounds and can also be simplified in ways that do not match the complexity of the phenomena under investigation. Our psychosocial, non-experimental study is but one small contribution to a rich body of empirical investigation linking parent-child relationships to later functioning. Spurred by enthusiasm for our points of view, the temptation to overstate afflicts us all. Humility and appreciation of the limits of any one perspective are of paramount importance in presenting our work. We view the world through the lenses of our particular intellectual traditions. In this respect, to paraphrase an ancient Indian parable, we are all blind men and women trying our best to understand this infinitely complex elephant that is human psychological development.
Footnotes
Author Contributions
Drs. Waldinger and Schulz jointly conceptualized and wrote this commentary.
Contributor Information
Robert Waldinger, Massachusetts General Hospital.
Marc Schulz, Bryn Mawr College.
References
- Brussoni MJ, Jang KL, Livesley WJ, Macbeth TM. Genetic and environmental influences on adult attachment styles. Personal Relationships. 2000;7(3):283–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00017.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fearon P, Shmueli-Goetz Y, Viding E, Fonagy P, Plomin R. Genetic and environmental influences on adolescent attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2014;55(9):1033–1041. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottlieb G. Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science. 2007;10(1):1–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00556.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harlow HF, Dodsworth RO, Harlow MK. Total social isolation in monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1965;54(1):90–97. doi: 10.1073/pnas.54.1.90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picardi A, Fagnani C, Nisticò L, Stazi MA. A twin study of attachment style in young adults. Journal of Personality. 2011;79(5):965–992. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00707.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinker S. Why nature & nurture won't go away. Daedalus. 2004;133(4):5–17. doi: 10.1162/0011526042365591. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Roisman GI, Holland A, Fortuna K, Fraley RC, Clausell E, Clarke A. The adult attachment interview and self- reports of attachment style: An empirical rapprochement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007;92:678–697. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Psychosocial resilience and protective mechanisms. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1987;57:316–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1987.tb03541.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silventoinen K, Sammalisto S, Perola M, Boomsma DI, Cornes BK, Davis C, Dunkel L, De Lange M, Harris JR, Hjelmborg JVB, Luciano M, Martin NG, Mortensen J, Nisticò L, Pedersen NL, Skytthe A, Spector TD, Stazi MA, Willemsen G, Kaprio J. Heritability of adult body height: A comparative study of twin cohorts in eight countries. Twin Research. 2003;6(5):399–408. doi: 10.1375/136905203770326402. PMID 14624724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suomi SJ. Genetic and maternal contributions to individual differences in rhesus monkey biobehavioral development. In: Krasnegor N, Blass E, Hofer M, Smotherman W, editors. Perinatal development: A psychobiological perspective. New York: Academic Press; 1987. pp. 397–420. [Google Scholar]
- Turkheimer E, Waldron M. Nonshared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126(1):78–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkheimer E. Weak genetic explanation 20 years later: Reply to Plomin et al. (2016) Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2016;11(1):24–28. doi: 10.1177/1745691615617442. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615617442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weaver ICG, Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Maternal care effects on the hippocampal transcriptome and anxiety-mediated behaviors in the offspring that are reversible in adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103(9):3480–3485. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507526103. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0507526103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]