Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Oct 30.
Published in final edited form as: J Neurosci. 2009 Jan 21;29(3):620–629. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5486-08.2009

LONG-TERM HOMEOSTASIS OF EXTRACELLULAR GLUTAMATE IN THE RAT CEREBRAL CORTEX ACROSS SLEEP AND WAKING STATES

Michael B Dash 1,2, Christopher L Douglas 1, Vladyslav V Vyazovskiy 1, Chiara Cirelli 1, Giulio Tononi 1
PMCID: PMC2770705  NIHMSID: NIHMS92871  PMID: 19158289

Abstract

Neuronal firing patterns, neuromodulators, and cerebral metabolism change across sleep waking states, and the synaptic release of glutamate is critically involved in these processes. Extrasynaptic glutamate can also affect neural function and may be neurotoxic, but whether and how extracellular glutamate is regulated across sleep-waking states is unclear. To assess the effect of behavioral state on extracellular glutamate at high temporal resolution, we recorded glutamate concentration in prefrontal and motor cortex using fixed-potential amperometry in freely behaving rats. Simultaneously, we recorded local field potentials (LFP) and electroencephalograms (EEG) from contralateral cortex. We observed dynamic, progressive changes in the concentration of glutamate that switched direction as a function of behavioral state. Specifically, the concentration of glutamate increased progressively during waking (0.329 ± 0.06 %/min) and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (0.349 ±0.13 %/min). This increase was opposed by a progressive decrease during non-REM (NREM) sleep (0.338 ± 0.06 %/min). During a 3-hr sleep deprivation period, glutamate concentrations initially exhibited the progressive rise observed during spontaneous waking. As sleep pressure increased, glutamate concentrations ceased to increase and began decreasing despite continuous waking. During NREM sleep, the rate of decrease in glutamate was positively correlated with sleep intensity, as indexed by LFP slow wave activity. The rate of decrease doubled during recovery sleep after sleep deprivation. Thus, the progressive increase in cortical extrasynaptic glutamate during EEG-activated states is counteracted by a decrease during NREM sleep that is modulated by sleep pressure. These results provide evidence for a long-term homeostasis of extracellular glutamate across sleep-waking states.

Keywords: glutamate, in vivo amperometry, sleep, rat, cerebral cortex, EEG, slow wave activity

Introduction

The transition between wakefulness, NREM, and REM sleep is accompanied by neuromodulatory changes. Acetylcholine, norepinephrine, serotonin, histamine, and hypocretin levels are high in wakefulness and low in NREM sleep, and noradrenergic, serotonergic and histaminergic cells become silent during REM sleep (Jones, 2005). Following neuromodulation, the EEG and the underlying patterns of neuronal activity also change, especially in cerebral cortex. The low-voltage fast-activity EEG of wakefulness and REM sleep is called “activated” because neurons are steadily depolarized and fire tonically (Steriade et al., 2001). During NREM sleep, when large slow waves appear in the EEG, neurons exhibit periods of hyperpolarization lasting hundreds of milliseconds, during which synaptic activity ceases altogether (Steriade et al., 2001). Accompanying these changes in neuronal activity are variations in energy metabolism. In waking and REM sleep overall brain metabolism is high in both animals and humans (Madsen et al., 1991; Maquet, 1995). During deep NREM sleep, instead, cerebral glucose metabolism is ∼30–40% lower than during waking (Kennedy et al., 1982; Maquet, 1995), and so are cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism (−18% and −25%, respectively (Madsen et al., 1991)).

Neuromodulators and changes in firing pattern affect glutamate release and clearance (Tzingounis and Wadiche, 2007). Moreover, most of the brain energy budget is required to sustain synaptic activity at glutamatergic synapses (Attwell and Laughlin, 2001). Thus, glutamatergic activity may also change as a function of behavioral state, though it is unclear how. Knowing whether and how it changes would be important, since glutamatergic signaling is critical for brain function at many levels, including information transmission, plasticity, and neurotoxicity. Unfortunately, the available evidence is limited and inconsistent. Some found increased glutamate release during EEG-activated states in cortex (Jasper et al., 1965; Lopez-Rodriguez et al., 2007) and anterior hypothalamus (Azuma et al., 1996). Others found no changes in cortex (Lena et al., 2005), an increase in NREM sleep in thalamus (Kekesi et al., 1997), in REM sleep in medulla (Kodama et al., 1998), and in waking in pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus (Kodama and Honda, 1999). These studies had low temporal resolution, with sampling rates of 1–5 min (average duration of NREM and REM sleep episodes in rats is ∼3.6 and 1.6 min (Tang et al., 2007)). Almost all studies used microdialysis, whose sensitivity to spontaneous synaptic release has been questioned (Timmerman and Westerink, 1997). Also, samples were collected for at most 2–6 hours during the light period, and without resorting to sleep deprivation, so the effects of waking duration and sleep pressure on glutamate levels could not be assessed.

Here we use in vivo amperometry, which is sensitive to spontaneous synaptic release and has a temporal resolution of <1 sec (Burmeister et al., 2000; Rutherford et al., 2007), to measure changes in glutamate levels across the sleep-waking cycle in two cortical areas. Data were continuously collected for up to 4 days in baseline conditions and after sleep deprivation. We find that glutamate concentration increases rapidly and progressively during wakefulness and REM sleep, and decreases during NREM sleep, compensating for previous increases. Thus, the concentration of extracellular glutamate is kept within a homeostatic range across sleep-waking states.

Materials and Methods

Surgery

Male Wistar Kyoto rats (N = 10; Charles River, Wilmington, MA), 300–350 grams at time of surgery, were used in the present study. One week prior to surgery animals were individually housed in their recording chambers under controlled conditions (24 ± 1 °C; 12h light/dark cycle lights on at 10:00) with access to food and water ad libitum. The recording chamber consisted of a clear Plexiglas enclosure (36.5 × 25 × 46 cm) in a sound-attenuating box.

Under isoflurane anesthesia (3% induction; 1.5% maintenance) local field potential (LFP) electrodes (Rhodes Medical Instruments, Summerland, CA) were implanted in the left parietal cortex (mm from bregma: anteroposterior, AP - 3.5; mediolateral, ML −3) and right motor cortex (AP +2, ML +3). Two gold plated epidural screws affixed to the scalp, one frontal (AP + 4, ML − 2) and one cerebellar (AP −9 ML 0), served as EEG electrodes. Silver wire electrodes were implanted in the nuchal muscles to monitor the electromyogram (EMG). Microelectrode arrays (MEAs; see below), were implanted in either motor cortex (N=3; AP +2, ML −3 dorsoventral, DV −1.5) or prefrontal cortex (N=5; AP +3.2 ML −.8 DV −4.5), with a Ag/AgCl reference electrode placed occipitally (AP-6.5 ML +3). Two additional rats were also used in pilot studies (anesthesia and whisker stimulation, see below). For these studies, a MEA was implanted in motor cortex in one rat and barrel cortex (AP −2.3 ML −3.8 DV −2) in the other. Dental cement was used to affix the electrodes to the animal’s skull. Recordings began immediately following surgery. All animal procedures followed the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and facilities were reviewed and approved by the IACUC of the University of Wisconsin-Madison, and were inspected and accredited by AAALAC.

Recordings of behavioral states and sleep staging

EEG and EMG recording signals were connected to the commutator with a flexible cable. The signals were then sent to a polygraph (Grass Technologies, Model 8–16 C, West Warwick, RI), amplified and filtered (EEG and LFP signals: high-pass at 0.1 Hz, low-pass at 70 Hz; EMG signals: high-pass at 5 Hz, low-pass at 70 Hz). All signals were notch-filtered at 60 Hz. Analog signals were digitized with the A/D board (National Instruments, Austin, TX) and sampled at 128 Hz (VitalRecorder, Kissei Comtec, Nagoya, Japan). Vigilance states were determined offline by visual inspection of EEG and EMG signals and scored in 4-sec epochs as either awake (low-voltage, high-frequency EEG, high EMG), NREM sleep (high-voltage, low-frequency EEG, low EMG), or REM sleep (low-voltage, high-frequency EEG, low EMG activity). Vigilance state could be determined for all epochs.

Ag/AgCl reference electrode preparation

Ag/AgCl reference electrodes were prepared as previously described (Rutherford, 2007). Briefly, a Teflon-coated silver wire (Cooner Wire, Chatsworth, CA) was stripped at both ends (0.25 inch). A gold-plated socket (Ginder Scientific, Nepean, ON Canada) was soldered to one end and connected to the cathode of a 9V DC current power supply. A second silver wire was stripped and attached to the anode of the power supply. These wires were immersed in 1M HCl saturated with NaCl and current was applied for 15 min to coat the reference electrode with AgCl.

Microelectrode preparation

Ceramic based microelectrode arrays (MEAs; Center for Microelectrode Technology, Louisville, Kentucky) containing four, 15 µM x 333 µM platinum/iridium electrodes were modified for chronic implantation in freely moving rats as previously described (Rutherford et al., 2007). Implantable assemblies were cleaned by submersion in three baths (5 min Citrisolv, 5 min isopropyl alcohol, 5 min DI water; all reagents from Sigma, St. Louis, MO) and dried for 15 min in an 80°C oven. Following cleaning, a coating solution of L-glutamate oxidase (GluOx; Associates of Cape Cod Incorporated, East Falmouth, MA), glutaraldehyde (Sigma), and bovine serum albumin (BSA; Sigma) was applied to two recording channels. GluOx catalyzes the conversion of glutamate to α-ketoglutarate and hydrogen peroxide. With an applied potential, H2O2 can then oxidize at the surface of the recording channels and acts as a reporter molecule for glutamate. Glutaraldehyde and BSA were included to affix the GluOx to the recording channels. The remaining two recording channels were coated with only glutaraldehyde and BSA to serve as sentinel recording channels (Fig. 1A). These sentinel channels measure background current from possible interferents only. Signal and sentinel channels are in close proximity in the microelectrode array, with a distance of 100 µM separating the edge of the signal channels from the edge of the sentinel channels (see Fig.1A). Subtracting current sensed by sentinel channels from current sensed by signal channels provides a signal reflecting glutamate concentration, removes background current produced by other analytes and reduces noise (Burmeister and Gerhardt, 2001; Rutherford et al., 2007). Following application of the coatings, the MEAs were allowed to dry overnight at 4°C. The following day the MEAs were submerged in a 5 mM m-phenylenediamine (MPD; Sigma) solution of 0.05 M phosphate-buffered solution (PBS). Cyclic amperometry (Peak-Peak: 0.25, offset: −0.5, frequency 0.05) was applied for 30 min to electroplate the MEA with a MPD exclusion layer. This size-exclusion layer serves to limit interference in vivo from electroactive analytes such as ascorbate without disrupting detection of the analyte of interest, glutamate.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

In vitro calibration and in vivo assessment of microelectrode arrays. A) Schematic of the self-referencing microelectrode arrays. Signal channels are coated with glutamate oxidase (GluOx) and are responsive to glutamate while sentinel channels are coated only with the cross linking proteins bovine serum albumin (BSA) and glutaraldehyde (GA). Signal and sentinel channels both have an m-phenylenediamine exclusion layer (mPD). B) Typical in vitro calibration of signal (black line) and sentinel (gray line) channels. Arrows indicate the time of application of ascorbic acid (A; 500 µM), glutamate (G;10 µM), and peroxide (P). Both channels show little or no response to ascorbic acid but respond robustly to peroxide, while only the signal channel responds robustly to glutamate. Inset, calibration curve demonstrating the linear response of the signal channels to changes in glutamate concentration. C) Changes in glutamate concentration following pentobarbital injection in one rat (x; 50 mg/kg i.p.). The first 90-sec episode of arousal with locomotion was observed at (y), but full motor activity was regained only at (z). D) Rapid increase in glutamate concentration during whisker stimulation contralateral to an electrode implanted in the barrel cortex in one rat. The rat received 15 stimulations applied over the course of 90 minutes. The average of all 15 stimulations, time-locked to stimulation onset, is depicted in 4-sec intervals. Stimulation onset began at arrow and each stimulation lasted on average 23.72± 3.28 sec.

Electrode calibration, in vivo amperometry, and post-processing

Microelectrodes were calibrated in vitro prior to implantation in freely moving animals. The microelectrodes were submerged in 40 mL of 0.05 M PBS (pH 7.4) which was stirred and maintained at 37°C by means of a water bath (Gaymar Co., Orchard Park, NY). Fixed-potential amperometry (+ 0.7 V vs. Ag/AgCl reference) was applied to recording electrodes using the FAST-16 potentiostat (Fast Analytical Sensing Technology, Quanteon LLC, Nicholasville, KY). Responses of both signal and sentinel channels to ascorbic acid (final concentration: 250 µM; Sigma), monosodium glutamate (four sequential additions of 10 µM increments; Sigma), and hydrogen peroxide were recorded and stored offline.

For in vivo recordings, fixed-potential amperometry (+0.7 V vs. Ag/AgCl reference) was performed using a four channel potentiostat (FAST-16). The applied potential of +0.7 V was chosen to enable oxidization of hydrogen peroxide (the reporter molecule for glutamate) while minimizing interference from other analytes that could otherwise oxidize at a higher applied potential. Implantable MEAs were connected to a headstage amplifier (500X, Rat Hat; Quanteon LLC) to amplify current produced from oxidation at the surface of the electrodes. The headstage amplifier was connected with a flexible cable to a commutator (Airflyte, Bayonne, NJ) to enable freedom of movement within the recording apparatus. The commutator was connected to the potentiostat where the signal was furthered amplified (10X). Analog outputs from the potentiostat were digitized with an A/D board (National Instruments, Austin, TX) and recorded to hard drive with a sampling rate of 128 Hz (VitalRecorder, Kissei Comtec Cc., Ltd, Nagano, Japan). In vivo recordings began immediately following surgery and lasted for up to 4 days post-surgery. In one pilot animal, pentobarbital anesthesia (50 mg/kg, i.p.) was administered one hour after light onset on post surgery day 2 while the concentration of glutamate was recorded in motor cortex. Following loss of righting reflex, the animal was placed on an electric heating pad to maintain proper body temperature. Upon full behavioral recovery, the heating pad was removed and recordings continued uninterrupted. In a second pilot animal, whisker stimulation was performed by gently deflecting whiskers contralateral to the implanted MEA with a small paintbrush while the extrasynaptic concentration of glutamate in barrel cortex was assessed. The rat remained awake throughout the course of the stimulations that were administered over the course of 90 min.

As previously described (Rutherford et al., 2007), the absolute strength of the glutamate signal recorded from signal channels declined over time, likely as a result of degradation or fouling of the applied enzyme. In our experiments, this was evident as a slow, but progressive decay in total current sensed by the signal channels across several days. To account for this decay and facilitate comparison between experimental days, the linear component of this decay was removed before analysis of glutamate concentrations. Examples of the removal of linear decay can be seen in Supplemental Figure 1.

Signals from microelectrode recordings were processed with Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA). Each signal was low-pass filtered (chebyshev type II, passband = 18 Hz, stopband = 19 Hz). The concentration of glutamate was calculated by dividing the raw voltage recorded during the experiment by the sensitivity of individual electrodes (volts/µM glutamate) determined from prior in vitro calibration of each individual electrode. Final glutamate concentration was obtained by subtracting sentinel channels from signal channels. To measure changes in glutamate concentration based on behavioral state, the concentration of glutamate was binned and averaged over 4-sec epochs previously scored as waking, NREM, or REM sleep. To remove technical artifacts present in some recordings, a 5 minute moving median filter was applied to each signal and values that deviated from this median by ± 40% were removed. This procedure reliably removed large technical artifacts and for each chronic recording removed no more than 5% of data points. Statistical tests were performed using Matlab and Statistica (Statsoft, Tulsa, OK).

Histology

At the end of the experiment rats were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane anesthesia (3% in oxygen) and perfused transcardially with saline immediately followed by 4% solution of paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.1 M sodium phosphate, pH 7.2. Brains were post-fixed overnight in 4% PFA, cryoprotected in increasing concentration of sucrose (15, 20, 30%) in PBS, and rapidly frozen on dry ice. Brains were then cut into 40µm serial coronal sections, mounted, and stained with cresyl violet. Placement of glutamate and LFP electrodes was determined using light microscopy. In all cases electrodes were in the targeted cortical regions and did not extend to the white matter below cortical layer VI. No excessive or unexpected tissue damage was noted.

Results

In vitro calibration shows the specificity of the glutamate signal

Each individual electrode was calibrated prior to implantation. Figure 1 shows a typical calibration experiment, which assessed the responsiveness of a glutamate-sensitive signal channel as well as a glutamate-insensitive sentinel channel (see Methods for details). Across all calibrations, signal channels (n=20) were highly sensitive to changes in glutamate concentrations (0.048 ± 0.006 V/µM) and displayed limits of detection in the low micromolar range (0.845 ± .154 µM). Signal channels responded linearly to changes in glutamate concentrations, demonstrating that changes in output voltage reflect changes in glutamate concentration. Sentinel channels, instead, were unresponsive to glutamate.

Ascorbic acid and hydrogen peroxide challenges were administered to test the selectivity of the electrodes and ensure that signal and sentinel channels differed only in their responsiveness to glutamate (Fig. 1B). The MPD exclusion layer applied to the surface of the electrodes limited the response of the electrodes to interference from ascorbic acid without affecting either the signal or sentinel channels’ robust responses to a hydrogen peroxide challenge. For subsequent in vivo recordings, electrodes were only used if the selectivity to glutamate over ascorbic acid was greater than 50:1, and if the response to the hydrogen peroxide challenge did not differ by > 15% in the signal relative to the sentinel channels.

Cortical glutamate concentrations reflect increases and decreases in synaptic activity

Glutamate levels as measured by amperometry are sensitive to block of action potentials by tetrodotoxin (Day et al., 2006; Oldenziel et al., 2006), whereas this treatment does not consistently affect the concentration of glutamate as measured with microdialysis (Timmerman and Westerink, 1997). Thus, this method may reflect glutamate released from synapses better than microdialysis (Timmerman and Westerink, 1997; Baker et al., 2002; van der Zeyden et al., 2007). To further assess whether the microelectrodes we used are sensitive to changes in glutamate concentration in vivo, pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, i.p.), an anesthetic known to augment inhibition, decrease neuronal activity, and depress excitatory neurotransmission (Nicoll and Madison, 1982; Richards and Strupinski, 1986; Wan et al., 2003), was administered to one rat while recording the concentration of glutamate in motor cortex (Fig. 1C). As expected pentobarbital administration produced immobilization, loss of righting reflex, and, within 30 min from the injection, an EEG pattern dominated by slow waves that persisted for ∼ 2 hours. During this time, the concentration of glutamate progressively decreased and reached a nadir of 14.01 µM; a 42% decrease from its initial concentration of 24.26 µM. The first brief arousal was observed 2.5 hours after administration, when the rat was mobile for ∼ 90 sec and the EEG showed a high frequency low amplitude “activated” pattern similar to that of normal waking. The animal then remained immobile for 3 more hours before full motor activity was regained, 5.5 hours after the initial anesthetic injection. Glutamate concentrations began to increase immediately during the 90-sec episode of arousal, and continued to rise afterwards up to a concentration of 18.69 µM immediately prior to full recovery of motor activity. Glutamate returned to pre-pentobarbital levels 8.5 hours after the initial administration. Hence, pentobarbital, known to depress synaptic activity, produced a large decrease in the extrasynaptic concentration of glutamate as recorded by our microelectrodes. These decreases are consistent with published data under urethane anesthesia (Rutherford et al., 2007).

The experiment under pentobarbital anesthesia demonstrates that a decrease in the concentration of glutamate can be observed following a treatment known to depress synaptic activity. To investigate whether the opposite is true, namely that an increase in the concentration of glutamate can be observed following an increase in synaptic activity, another pilot experiment was undertaken in which the effects of whisker stimulation on the concentration of glutamate in barrel cortex were assessed. Whisker stimulation activates neurons in the contralateral barrel cortex, producing a complex pattern of spikes that are mediated by glutamatergic transmission (Armstrong-James et al., 1993). A single rat was subjected to 15 whisker stimulations (each lasting ∼ 20 sec, 3–4 min apart), which reliably produced an increase in the extrasynaptic concentration of glutamate in the contralateral barrel cortex (Fig. 1D). Specifically, we observed a rapid increase in the concentration of glutamate that persisted throughout the duration of each stimulation. The concentration of glutamate at the conclusion of each stimulation was always elevated as compared to pre-stimulation levels. On average, glutamate concentrations increased by 0.459 ± 0.08 µM during whisker stimulations (average stimulus duration: 23.72 ± 3.28 s), consistent with prior reports (Pomerleau et al., 2003). In the same rat, no increase was observed during stimulation of whiskers ipsilateral to the microelectrode (N=7; Supplementary Fig. 2). Thus, the microelectrodes in the present study are sensitive to bidirectional changes in the concentration of glutamate following either an increase or a decrease in synaptic activity.

Sleep-wake parameters

Simultaneous polysomnographic and amperometric recordings were performed continuously in freely-behaving animals. In previous experiments polygraphic recordings started at least one week after surgery, to allow full recovery. In the current experiments, however, due to the progressive decay in the absolute strength of the glutamate signal (see Methods), all rats were recorded starting 24 hours after the surgery. Recordings lasted at least 2 full days, and in some cases up to 4 days.

Table 1 shows sleep quantity and quality for the 2 baseline recording days (post-surgery day 2 and 3). Since there was no significant difference between the 2 days in any of the sleep parameters investigated the data were pooled. Rats spent ∼ 60% of the 24-hour period asleep, as compared to ∼ 50% in fully recovered rats (e.g. (Alfoldi et al., 1991)), but they still were more awake at night (52% of the time) than during the day (29%). The total increase in sleep was mainly due to more time spent in NREM sleep at night (NREM, 61% and 44% in light and dark period, respectively, vs 55% and 25% in fully-recovered rats; REM sleep, 10% and 4% vs 12% and 6% in fully-recovered rats; (Alfoldi et al., 1991)). The average duration of NREM and REM sleep episodes (episode criteria: minimum duration:16 seconds; maximum allowed interruption: 16 seconds) was 3.0 ± 0.2 min and 1.54 ± 0.1 min, respectively, similar to those reported for rats on day 2 and 3 after surgery (Tang et al., 2007)). Waking activities such as grooming, exploration, eating and drinking did not show obvious abnormalities although subjects typically appeared less active than fully recovered rats.

Table 1.

Sleep parameters (mean ± SEM, n=8 rats) during two baseline days. Since day 1 and day 2 were not significantly different, data were pooled.

Daily Total (Hrs) Light (%) Dark (%) Episode Duration (min)
WAKE 9.63 (0.39) 29.29 (1.61) 51.71 (1.86) 2.47 (0.13)
NREM 12.48 (0.35) 60.72 (1.30) 44.05 (1.76) 3.02 (0.16)
REM 1.70 (0.13) 9.96 (0.71) 4.24 (0.47) 1.54 (0.06)

Cortical glutamate concentration reflect changes in sleep and waking independent of time of day

Chronic amperometry was employed to assess changes in the extracellular concentration of glutamate across the sleep/waking cycle in motor cortex or prefrontal cortex. Across the entire 2-day baseline period the average concentration of glutamate was 18.85 ± 6.4 µM in motor cortex (N=3) and 15.27 ± 4.1 µM in prefrontal cortex (N=5). Since results were not significantly different between the two areas (t(6) = 0.50, p = 0.64; independent samples, two-tailed), the data were pooled. The average concentration of glutamate across all rats was 16.6 ± 3.3 µM, a value within the range previously reported in the cerebral cortex of other strains of rats (Rutherford et al., 2007) and in mice (Hascup et al., 2008). As in previous studies, we also found significant interindividual variability. Specifically, average glutamate concentrations ranged from as low as 6–8 µM in a few animals to as high as 26–30 µM in others. Of note, the concentration of glutamate sampled with amperometry typically (including the present study) exceeds the range of concentrations reported by microdialysis studies (1–3 µM; (van der Zeyden et al., 2007)), most likely due to closer proximity of microelectrodes to synapses, higher sensitivity of amperometry recordings to neuronal activity, and higher sampling rates (Rutherford et al., 2007).

Despite the variability in absolute values, in all rats the concentration of glutamate was not static, but varied throughout the recording period. This is shown in Figure 2, where absolute glutamate levels during a 24-hour baseline period are plotted for each consecutive 4-sec epoch scored as waking or sleep. The rat in panel A (each panel refers to a different animal) was well entrained to the light-dark cycle and, as typically seen in fully recovered rats, was mainly asleep during the day and showed long periods (as long as 58.5 min) of continuous waking during the night. It is clear that glutamate levels increase for as long as the rat is awake, and decrease or stay low during sleep. The rat in panel B was also mainly awake during the night and asleep during the day, but showed a more fragmented sleep pattern, with episodes of continuous waking no longer than 20 min: in this rat glutamate levels also increased throughout waking, and decreased during sleep. The rat in panel C instead slept significantly also during the night. In this rat glutamate levels kept decreasing for most of the light period and the first hours of the dark period, and then remained low. Finally, panel D refers to a rat that showed long periods of waking also at the beginning of the light periods: glutamate levels increased during this time as long as the rat was awake, and reached peak values as high as at the end of long waking bouts during the night. Overall, these data show that in the rat prefrontal and motor cortex glutamate levels 1) increase during waking; 2) decrease or remain low during sleep; 3) are not affected by the transition from light to dark per se; 4) are not affected by time of day independent of behavioral state.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Changes in glutamate concentration across the light/dark period. (ABCD) Concentration of extrasynaptic glutamate during sleep and waking across the light/dark period for 4 individual rats. Each data point represents the average concentration of glutamate across a 4-sec window of waking (red) or sleep (blue). Absolute glutamate concentrations are highly variable across the light/dark period but display consistent changes within behavioral states.

Cortical glutamate levels increase progressively during both waking and REM sleep, and decrease progressively in NREM sleep

Since NREM sleep and REM sleep differ significantly in terms of metabolism and neuronal activity, we examined whether they also have different effects on cortical glutamate levels. Figure 3 shows absolute glutamate levels plotted for each consecutive 4-sec epoch in 2 rats, but unlike in Figure 2, NREM sleep and REM sleep are scored separately.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Dynamic changes in extrasynaptic glutamate concentration during waking, NREM, and REM sleep. A shows a 24-hour recording in the prefrontal cortex of a single rat. Each data point represents the average concentration of glutamate across a 4-sec window of waking (red), NREM (blue), or REM (green). The 2-hour window boxed in A is depicted at higher resolution in B, with the inset showing a single NREM to REM transition. C and D are organized as A and B, but show 24-hour recording in the motor cortex of a different rat.

It is clear that both in prefrontal and motor cortex glutamate levels increase not only during waking, but also during REM sleep. Moreover, as evident from Figure 2 and Figure 3, changes in concentration were not confined to the transitions between behavioral states, but occurred progressively throughout spontaneous waking, NREM, and REM periods. To quantify these changes over time, we considered all consolidated periods of sleep or waking during the 2 baseline days, defined as those episodes whose duration was equal or greater than the mean duration for each behavioral state (2.5, 3, and 1.5 min for waking, NREM, or REM sleep, respectively). These criteria enabled the characterization of a large number of episodes (mean ± SEM in 24 hr/rat, waking: 43.5 ± 4.1; NREM: 91.9 ± 4.0; REM: 24.0 ± 2.4) that were sufficiently long to minimize technical variability in assessing glutamate levels, which is more pronounced for short episodes containing only a few 4-sec epochs. For each rat, we calculated the percent change in glutamate concentration for each consolidated episode of sleep or waking relative to each rat’s overall 2-day mean concentration (across all behavioral states), and averaged them to provide an index of the typical changes in glutamate concentration for waking, NREM, and REM sleep. As can be seen in Figure 4, the concentration of glutamate progressively increased during the vast majority of waking and REM episodes and progressively decreased during most NREM episodes. Within each behavioral state, the average changes that were observed in prefrontal cortex did not significantly differ from those observed in motor cortex (waking: t(6) = 0.073, p = 0.944; NREM: t(6) = 1.37, p = 0.221; REM: t(6) = 0.663 p = 0.532). Consequently, data from these two regions were pooled for further analyses. Across all rats, glutamate concentrations increased 0.329 ± 0.06 %/min during waking and 0.349 ± 0.13 %/min during REM sleep, and decreased 0.338 ± 0.06 %/min during NREM sleep. Changes in glutamate concentration during waking or REM sleep were significantly different than changes during NREM (repeated measure ANOVA, F(2,14)=16.94, p<0.001; post-hoc Tukey’s HSD, p<.001) while no significant difference was observed between waking and REM. It should be pointed out that these results, based on mean episode duration, did not significantly change when glutamate levels were calculated across a wide range of episode length (1–5 min for waking and NREM, 0.5–3 min for REM), or when the median episode duration for each behavioral state was considered.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Behavioral state dependent changes in glutamate concentration. Histogram of percent change in glutamate concentration during consolidated periods of waking (all episodes >2.5 min), NREM (>3 min), or REM (>1.5 min) for all rats (n=8). The observed changes in prefrontal and motor cortex were not significantly different from one another for any behavioral state, and thus data were pooled (n=8). Values outside of x-axis scale (those with >2%/ min) were grouped in the edge bins.

There were a few episodes during which glutamate concentrations exhibited atypical changes for a given behavioral state as shown in Figure 4. Specifically, a small minority (6.6%) of all consolidated NREM periods showed the opposite trend, i.e. a progressive increase in glutamate concentration during NREM sleep that was at least as large as the average decrease normally observed during this stage of sleep. We could not find an obvious explanation for the opposite behavior of glutamate in these NREM episodes, as factors such as the occurrence of a REM episode or a brief awakening immediately afterwards did not seem to matter. On the other hand, as shown in Figure 4, a subset (12.3%) of consolidated waking periods also showed the opposite trend, i.e. the concentration of glutamate progressively decreased, and the reduction over time was as significant as the increase normally seen during waking. The waking episodes with decreasing glutamate levels were more often (60% of cases) interspersed within long and consolidated NREM periods (at least 85% of NREM sleep during a >10-min recording period), which are typical of sleep under high sleep pressure (i.e., sleep at the beginning of the major sleep phase, and sleep after sleep deprivation). Hence, glutamate concentrations exhibit a progressive rise during spontaneous waking with the exception of some short periods of waking interspersed within NREM episodes during high sleep pressure. Finally, as in waking, in a subset (15.5%) of REM epochs glutamate concentrations showed a decrease rather than an increase. We do not have a clear explanation for these atypical epochs, but it is likely that at least some of these apparent changes in the opposite direction reflect higher technical variability, due to the shorter duration of REM bouts.

As mentioned before, recordings lasted for up to 4 days starting 24 hours after the surgery. Thus, the changes in glutamate described above during waking, NREM, and REM may have been influenced by the close proximity to surgery, since our rats spent more time asleep than fully recovered animals, especially at night. To determine whether this was the case we ran 2 sets of analyses. First, we focused on the fourth day post-surgery in a single animal in which the strength of the glutamate signal remained high throughout that day. This rat displayed a sleep pattern more similar to that seen in fully recovered animals (% of 24 hr, waking: 53.8%; NREM: 43.1%; REM: 3.0%; % of dark period, NREM: 32.0%; REM: 3.2%) and yet, the changes in glutamate concentration during this day were not significantly different from those observed in the same animal on day 1 and day 2 post-surgery (p > 0.05 for waking, NREM and REM). In a second analysis we selected, among all baseline recording days, those that showed sleep patterns the most (n=3) or the least (n=3) similar to that of fully recovered rats (most similar, % of 24 hr, waking: 49%; NREM: 45%; REM: 6%; % of dark period, NREM: 34%; REM: 4%; least similar, % of 24 hr, waking: 36%; NREM: 58%; REM: 6%; % of dark period, NREM: 51%; REM: 3%). Again, there was no significant difference in the changes in glutamate within waking, NREM, or REM between the 2 groups. Thus, our results do not seem to be affected by the extent to which the sleep pattern was disrupted because of the surgery.

Changes in glutamate levels during sleep deprivation

The progressive increase in glutamate during most consolidated waking episodes raises the possibility that glutamate levels would continue to rise if waking was not interrupted by NREM sleep. To further characterize the effect of prolonged wakefulness on the time course of changes in glutamate, a subset of animals (N=5) were deprived of sleep for 3 hours by introducing novel objects to their cage to stimulate exploratory behavior. Pilot studies (N=3) had indicated that despite the proximity to surgery (sleep deprivation started at light onset the fourth day after surgery) we could reliably maintain wakefulness with this method for the duration of the deprivation. After 3 hours of sleep deprivation, rats were then allowed to sleep ad libitum.

Figure 5 depicts changes in glutamate concentration in the prefrontal cortex of 3 rats before, during, and after 3 hours of sleep deprivation. During sleep deprivation glutamate concentrations initially rose in all 3 animals, consistent with the changes observed during spontaneous waking. Unlike during spontaneous waking, however, glutamate levels did not continue to rise for the entire duration of forced waking. The time when glutamate levels no longer increased varied among animals, being as short as 30 min in one rat (Fig. 5B) and as long as 120 min in another (Fig. 5C). Strikingly, across all 5 rats, the initial rise in glutamate continued only until the concentration reached a level similar to the highest concentration observed during spontaneous waking. Afterwards, in spite of continued wakefulness as assessed by both behavioral and EEG analysis, glutamate concentrations either leveled (Fig. 5B), began to decrease (Fig. 5C), or first leveled and then decreased (Fig. 5A). Interestingly, the duration of the “increase” phase seems to depend on the previous sleep/waking history, so that the rat that had spent most of the last 2 hours asleep (Fig. 5C) showed the longest increase, while the rat that was mostly awake for 1–2 hours before the sleep deprivation began (Fig. 5B) showed the shortest increase. Indeed, across all rats (n=5), the amount of time spent in NREM sleep during the 90 minutes preceding the sleep deprivation was significantly correlated (r = 0.961, p < 0.01) with the duration of the increase phase (defined as the time required for glutamate levels to reach 95% of the peak value observed during sleep deprivation). Thus, glutamate concentrations increased for a longer time during sleep deprivation in those rats that were predominantly in NREM sleep prior to the start of sleep deprivation.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

The effects of 3 hours of sleep deprivation and subsequent recovery sleep on the concentration of glutamate in the prefrontal cortex. Panels A,B,C depict glutamate concentrations and behavioral state across a 24-hour baseline day, 3 hours of sleep deprivation, and 4 hours of recovery sleep for 3 individual rats. Each data point represents the average concentration of glutamate across a 4-sec epoch of waking (red), NREM (blue), or REM (green). Horizontal grey bars indicate sleep deprivation (SD). The vertical grey line indicates the ∼ 10 min between the end of the baseline day (time 24; 10AM) and the onset of sleep deprivation (time 0) during which animal care was performed. D. Averaged response to 3 hours of sleep deprivation and subsequent 4 hours of recovery sleep for all rats (± SEM, N=5).

Figure 5D shows the pooled data across the 5 animals: during the first hour of sleep deprivation the concentration of glutamate increased by 0.401 ± 0.151 %/min, an increase that was not significantly different from that observed during periods of spontaneous waking (t(4) = 0.583, p = 0.591; dependent samples t-test, two-tailed). During the second hour, however, the progressive increase in glutamate concentration slowed and flattened (0.133 ± 0.135 %/min), and during the third hour of deprivation glutamate concentration declined (−0.430 ± 0.106 %/min), significantly different from spontaneous waking (t(4) = 8.44, p<0.01; dependent samples t-test, two-tailed). Of note, there was a progressive increase in sleepiness throughout the sleep deprivation period, as indicated by an increase in the number of sleep attempts (defined as any period longer than 2 sec of NREM sleep) as the deprivation progressed, especially between hour 1 and hours 2–3 (number of sleep attempts/hour, mean ± SEM, hour 1 = 4 ± 0.95; hour 2= 6.8 ± 2.2; hour 3 =8.2 ± 2.8).

During recovery sleep following sleep deprivation a large, progressive decrease in the concentration of glutamate was observed (−0.710 ± 0.131 %/min for the first two hours after sleep deprivation). The rate of this decrease, occurring under the high sleep pressure that follows sleep deprivation, was significantly faster than the average rate of decline (−0.338 ± 0.06 %/min) during spontaneous NREM sleep (t(4) = 4.48, p <0.05; dependent samples t-test, two-tailed) suggesting again that intense sleep typical for high sleep pressure can modulate the changes in glutamate concentration associated with behavioral state. Moreover, glutamate levels at the end of recovery sleep reached values as low as, but not lower, than those observed after consolidated periods of baseline sleep during the light period. Overall, these data indicate that 1) glutamate concentrations rise during spontaneous and enforced wakefulness; 2) the increase in glutamate concentration during enforced waking does not persist indefinitely and reaches a peak concentration similar to that observed during spontaneous waking; 3) upon reaching this apparent threshold, glutamate concentrations plateau or decrease despite continued wakefulness and 4) glutamate concentrations decrease rapidly under high sleep pressure.

Glutamate concentration and SWA

The EEG pattern of NREM sleep is dominated by slow waves. Slow wave activity (SWA) is defined as the EEG power in the 0.5–4 Hz band during NREM sleep, and is a reliable measure of the amplitude and number of slow waves during this phase of sleep. SWA is an established marker of sleep pressure, increasing with the duration of waking and dissipating throughout sleep (Achermann and Borbely, 2003). As expected, all rats showed the typical SWA profile across the light period, i.e. SWA was high immediately after sleep onset and declined progressively afterwards (Fig. 6A). During the first 2 hours of the light period, when sleep pressure and SWA were high, the average rate of decrease in glutamate was also high (−0.330 ± 0.079 %/ min). Conversely, at the end of the major sleep phase (hour 9–10 of the light period), when sleep pressure and SWA were low, the average rate of decrease in glutamate levels during NREM sleep was also low (−0.181 ± 0.076 %/ min) and was significantly less than that observed during the first two hours (t(7) = 2.92, p<0.05; dependent samples t-test, two-tailed). To further assess the relationship between sleep pressure and changes in extrasynaptic glutamate we also compared the rate of change in glutamate levels with the average SWA for each consolidated (>3 min) NREM period during the light phase. All NREM sleep episodes were divided in quintiles based on average SWA levels, from the highest (top 20% average SWA) to lowest (bottom 20% average SWA). During NREM episodes with the highest SWA the average decrease in glutamate concentration was −0.348 ± 0.054 %/ min, significantly larger (t (7) = 2.55, p < 0.05; dependent samples t-test, two-tailed) than the decrease in glutamate concentration during NREM sleep episodes with the lowest SWA (bottom 20% SWA; −0.194 ± 0.045 %/min). Overall, we found that the rate of glutamate decline during NREM sleep and the levels of SWA were positively correlated (r = 0.41, p < .01; Fig. 6B). Thus, sleep pressure and sleep intensity as measured by SWA appear to change in parallel with the rate of decline in glutamate concentration during NREM sleep.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Slow wave activity (SWA) and decreases in glutamate concentration during NREM sleep across the light period. A) Slow wave activity (SWA) exhibits a typical decline across the light period. B) The rate of glutamate decline during NREM sleep was positively correlated (r = 0.41) with the amount of SWA. Each data point represents the average SWA and change in glutamate concentration during NREM sleep across 2-hour blocks during the light period for each rat. One outlier (> 3x standard deviation) was removed, but the correlation remains significant even with its inclusion; (p = 0.0496). *, p < 0.05.

Discussion

As summarized in the introduction, several studies used microdialysis to assess glutamate levels across the sleep/waking cycle, but the results were inconsistent. Other studies used indirect markers to assess the effects of sleep and waking on glutamatergic signaling. Some reports measured tissue glutamate content in whole brain after total or REM sleep deprivation, with mixed results: no change (Karadzic et al., 1971; Himwich et al., 1973) or an increase in glutamate after sleep loss (Davis et al., 1969; Bettendorff et al., 1996). REM sleep deprivation was also shown to increase glutamine turnover (Mark et al., 1969), and protein levels of glutamine synthetase (Sallanon-Moulin et al., 1994). Transcript levels of genes coding for enzymes involved in glutamate synthesis (glutamine synthase, glutaminase), glutamate receptor subunits (GluR2, GluR3, (Cirelli and Tononi, 2000)) and intracellular proteins implicated in glutamate receptor clustering (Homer/Vesl, Narp) are also upregulated during waking (Cirelli et al., 2004). Finally, the number of GluR1-containing AMPA receptors increases after waking relative to sleep in cortex and hippocampus (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008b). Thus, there is evidence that changes in glutamatergic signaling occur across behavioral states, but the link with glutamatergic neurotransmission is indirect.

Here we used in vivo amperometry to measure cortical extracellular glutamate continuously for several days with second-by second resolution. This allowed us to reach several novel conclusions. First, within each sleep-wake cycle (<30 min in rats) glutamate levels increased rapidly and progressively during waking and REM sleep, and decreased progressively during NREM sleep. These dynamic within-state changes could not be detected with microdialysis due to its poor temporal resolution. Supporting our finding, a recent study focusing on the posterior hypothalamus used glutamate biosensors similar to those employed here and found rapid increases in glutamate levels in waking and REM sleep during a 7-hour window in the middle of the day (John et al., 2008).

Second, the minute-by-minute glutamate levels reflected the sleep-wake history of the previous several hours: glutamate levels were low in an awake rat if it had been asleep for several hours, and high in a sleeping rat if it had been mostly awake for the previous hours. Thus, the previous sleep-wake history contributes to the absolute levels of glutamate at any given time, while the direction of these changes is determined by the current behavioral state. Glutamatergic activity is associated with high energy demand (Attwell and Laughlin, 2001), and brain energy consumption is lower during NREM sleep relative to waking and REM sleep (Madsen et al., 1991; Maquet, 1995). Strikingly, cerebral metabolism in mice is also contingent upon the previous sleep-waking history (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008a). Specifically, the uptake of 2-deoxyglucose during quiet waking is 15–20% lower in mice that had slept for ∼ 2.5h prior to the injection of the tracer, compared to mice that had been awake prior to the injection (Vyazovskiy et al., 2008a).

Third, the previous sleep-waking history not only affected the absolute concentration of glutamate, but also modulated its rate of change. SWA is a reliable indicator of sleep pressure, and increases as a function of the duration of prior waking (Achermann and Borbely, 2003). Glutamate levels decreased at a faster rate during NREM sleep periods with high SWA than during those with low SWA. Overall, the rate of glutamate decline during NREM sleep was positively correlated with SWA. Also, the rate of decrease in glutamate during sleep following sleep deprivation, when sleep pressure is high, was roughly twice as fast as the average decrease during spontaneous NREM sleep. Hence, high sleep pressure enhances the NREM-related decline in glutamate levels.

Another factor modulating the changes in glutamate concentration during waking appears to be sleepiness. During sleep deprivation the frequency of sleep attempts increased, indicating an increase in sleepiness. Meanwhile, glutamate levels increased during the first hour of sleep deprivation, but by the third hour they started to decrease. Thus, towards the end of the deprivation, when rats attempted to sleep the most, the concentration of glutamate declined despite continued wakefulness. Changes in glutamate levels during spontaneous waking may also be affected by sleepiness, because most “atypical” waking epochs (associated with decreasing glutamate concentrations), occurred during short waking periods within long, consolidated NREM sleep periods.

The second-by-second concentration of glutamate in extrasynaptic space is the result of two opposite processes: extracellular release of glutamate from neurons and astrocytes, and intracellular uptake by high-affinity transporters. The fluctuations in extrasynaptic glutamate with behavioral state could therefore be due to changes in release or in transporter activity. Most glutamate uptake in cortex is mediated by the glutamate transporter-1, GLT-1 and the glutamate/aspartate transporter, GLAST (Tzingounis and Wadiche, 2007). Their expression/activity is modulated by many factors downstream of neuronal activity, including cAMP and kinases (Duan et al., 1999), and their block results in increased extracellular glutamate levels and neurodegeneration (Rothstein et al., 1996). The changes in glutamate levels observed in our study could be accounted for by decreased uptake during waking and REM sleep, and increased uptake during NREM sleep. There is no evidence, however, that GLT-1 and GLAST activity is reduced during waking. GLAST mRNA expression in rat cortex increases by 30% following 8h of waking (Cirelli and Tononi, 2000), and GLAST expression increases following exposure to glutamate, resulting in increased uptake (Duan et al., 1999). Thus, if present, changes in glutamate transporter expression may serve to oppose the progressive increases in glutamate during waking and REM sleep, rather than contribute to it. The latter changes, instead, may reflect changes in glutamate release.

Under physiological conditions, glutamate is released into the extrasynaptic space by spillover from synapses (Asztely et al., 1997; Biro and Nusser, 2005; Sem'yanov, 2005), exocytosis from astrocytes (Santello and Volterra, 2008), and active transport by the cystine-glutamate antiporter (Baker et al., 2002). Extrasynaptic glutamate as measured by our microelectrodes is affected by block of action potentials by tetrodotoxin, indicating that it reflects neuronal activity (Day et al., 2006; Hascup et al., 2008). Astrocytic release of glutamate can be coupled with neuronal activity (Santello and Volterra, 2008), but the relative contribution of synaptic spillover and astrocytic release cannot be determined in our experimental conditions. Nevertheless, activity-induced release of glutamate is likely to underlie the changes in extrasynaptic glutamate concentration across the sleep-wake cycle.

Initial studies suggested that firing rates in cortex are higher during waking and REM sleep and lower during NREM sleep (Desiraju, 1972; Noda and Adey, 1973). Recent reports indicate that neuronal firing rates during NREM sleep are similar to waking during the up state of the slow oscillation, but are close to zero during the down state (Steriade et al., 2001). Therefore, the progressive rise of glutamate during waking and REM sleep could result from the inability of glutamate transporters to maintain a constant extrasynaptic glutamate concentration during high levels of neuronal activity. Meanwhile, during NREM sleep, the occurrence of down states could allow transporters to gradually reduce the extrasynaptic concentration of glutamate. Given that glutamate transporters complete a full cycle in ∼70 ms (Wadiche et al., 1995) and that down states typically last 80–300 ms (Ji and Wilson, 2007), each transporter could conceivably remove 1–4 molecules of glutamate per down state. Large slow waves, indicative of high sleep pressure, are associated with even longer silent periods of neuronal activity (Vyazovskiy V, unpublished observations; (Calvet et al., 1973)). This could account for the faster decline in the concentration of glutamate during NREM sleep with high SWA. It is tempting to speculate that NREM sleep may be a propitious time for replenishing presynaptic glutamate reserves depleted by tonic glutamate release during wakefulness, though there is so far little indication that the presynaptic glutamate pool may constitute a limited resource over long periods of time.

The progressive changes in extracellular glutamate reported here shows that, for whatever reason, glutamate release and clearance mechanisms are not kept in full balance in the short term. Instead, glutamate levels systematically increase during EEG activated states, raising the question whether they may become neurotoxic. In vitro studies of cultured neurons report glutamate-induced excitotoxicity at concentrations below those observed here (1–12 µM; (Rosenberg et al., 1992; Cheung et al., 1998). However, astrocyte-rich cultures are much less sensitive to glutamate excitotoxicity (EC50 = 205 µM; (Rosenberg et al., 1992); EC50 = 100–200 µM; (Regan and Choi, 1991)), and high glutamate levels are required to produce excitotoxicity in vivo (Mangano and Schwarcz, 1983; Choi, 1988). While it remains unclear whether the progressive increase of glutamate concentrations in waking can approach excitotoxic conditions, the present results show that NREM sleep effectively opposes glutamate accumulation in the extrasynaptic space. Thus, thanks to the alternation of wakefulness and NREM sleep, glutamate levels are kept within a homeostatic range, remaining relatively constant across the 24-hour period. Moreover, as shown by the positive correlation between the rate of glutamate decline during NREM sleep and the levels of SWA, the homeostasis of extracellular glutamate in the cortex may be linked to sleep homeostasis. At this point, it is unknown whether NREM sleep is in any way critical to glutamate homeostasis, and it is likely that, in the absence of sleep, other compensatory mechanisms come into play. Perhaps the glutamate-decreasing effect of NREM sleep is especially relevant in pathological conditions, such as ischemia, that can lead to glutamate-induced excitotoxicity.

Supplementary Material

Supp1. Supplementary Figure 1.

Due to an observed decrease in absolute signal strength across the recording duration, signals were rectified by removing a linear decay component before subsequent analyses. ABCD) The original (grey) and rectified (black) signal from four individual rats across the duration of the recording period. Data were analyzed for up to 4 days post-surgery during which time the proper functioning of signal channels (as indicated by positive [glutamate]) was always observed. A positive signal was observed for as long as 6 days post-surgery.

Supp2. Supplementary Figure 2.

No increase in glutamate concentration following whisker stimulation ipsilateral to an electrode implanted in the barrel cortex in one rat. The rat received 7 stimulations. The average of all 7 stimulations, time-locked to stimulation onset, is depicted in 4-sec intervals. Stimulation onset began at arrow and each stimulation lasted on average 20.0 ± 1.75 sec.

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (P20 MH077967 to CC) and by the NIH Director’s Pioneer award (to GT). We thank Francois Pomerleau at the University of Kentucky for technical advice.

References

  1. Achermann P, Borbely AA. Mathematical models of sleep regulation. Front Biosci. 2003;8:s683–s693. doi: 10.2741/1064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alfoldi P, Franken P, Tobler I, Borbely AA. Short light-dark cycles influence sleep stages and EEG power spectra in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 1991;43:125–131. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(05)80062-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Armstrong-James M, Welker E, Callahan CA. The contribution of NMDA and non-NMDA receptors to fast and slow transmission of sensory information in the rat SI barrel cortex. J Neurosci. 1993;13:2149–2160. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-05-02149.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Asztely F, Erdemli G, Kullmann DM. Extrasynaptic glutamate spillover in the hippocampus: dependence on temperature and the role of active glutamate uptake. Neuron. 1997;18:281–293. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80268-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Attwell D, Laughlin SB. An energy budget for signaling in the grey matter of the brain. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2001;21:1133–1145. doi: 10.1097/00004647-200110000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Azuma S, Kodama T, Honda K, Inoue S. State-dependent changes of extracellular glutamate in the medial preoptic area in freely behaving rats. Neurosci Lett. 1996;214:179–182. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(96)12918-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baker DA, Xi ZX, Shen H, Swanson CJ, Kalivas PW. The origin and neuronal function of in vivo nonsynaptic glutamate. J Neurosci. 2002;22:9134–9141. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-20-09134.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bettendorff L, Sallanon-Moulin M, Touret M, Wins P, Margineanu I, Schoffeniels E. Paradoxical sleep deprivation increases the content of glutamate and glutamine in rat cerebral cortex. Sleep. 1996;19:65–71. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Biro AA, Nusser Z. Synapse independence breaks down during highly synchronous network activity in the rat hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:1257–1262. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04304.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burmeister JJ, Gerhardt GA. Self-referencing ceramic-based multisite microelectrodes for the detection and elimination of interferences from the measurement of L-glutamate and other analytes. Anal Chem. 2001;73:1037–1042. doi: 10.1021/ac0010429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Burmeister JJ, Moxon K, Gerhardt GA. Ceramic-based multisite microelectrodes for electrochemical recordings. Anal Chem. 2000;72:187–192. doi: 10.1021/ac9907991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Calvet J, Fourment A, Thiefry M. Electrical activity in neocortical projection and association areas during slow wave sleep. Brain Res. 1973;52:173–187. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90657-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cheung NS, Pascoe CJ, Giardina SF, John CA, Beart PM. Micromolar L-glutamate induces extensive apoptosis in an apoptotic-necrotic continuum of insult-dependent, excitotoxic injury in cultured cortical neurones. Neuropharmacology. 1998;37:1419–1429. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00123-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Choi DW. Glutamate neurotoxicity and diseases of the nervous system. Neuron. 1988;1:623–634. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(88)90162-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cirelli C, Tononi G. Gene expression in the brain across the sleep-waking cycle. Brain Res. 2000;885:303–321. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)03008-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cirelli C, Gutierrez CM, Tononi G. Extensive and divergent effects of sleep and wakefulness on brain gene expression. Neuron. 2004;41:35–43. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00814-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Davis JM, Himwich WA, Stout M. Cerebral amino acids during deprivation of paradoxical sleep. Biol Psychiatry. 1969;1:387–390. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Day BK, Pomerleau F, Burmeister JJ, Huettl P, Gerhardt GA. Microelectrode array studies of basal and potassium-evoked release of L-glutamate in the anesthetized rat brain. J Neurochem. 2006;96:1626–1635. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.03673.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Desiraju T. Discharge properties of neurons of the parietal association cortex during states of sleep and wakefulness in the monkey. Brain Res. 1972;47:69–75. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90252-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Duan S, Anderson CM, Stein BA, Swanson RA. Glutamate induces rapid upregulation of astrocyte glutamate transport and cell-surface expression of GLAST. J Neurosci. 1999;19:10193–10200. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-23-10193.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hascup KN, Hascup ER, Pomerleau F, Huettl P, Gerhardt GA. Second-by-second measures of L-glutamate in the prefrontal cortex and striatum of freely moving mice. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2008;324:725–731. doi: 10.1124/jpet.107.131698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Himwich WA, Davis JM, Stout M. Cerebral amino acids of rats deprived of paradoxical sleep. Biol Psychiatry. 1973;6:37–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jasper HH, Khan RT, Elliott KA. Amino Acids Released from the Cerebral Cortex in Relation to Its State of Activation. Science. 1965;147:1448–1449. doi: 10.1126/science.147.3664.1448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ji D, Wilson MA. Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:100–107. doi: 10.1038/nn1825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. John J, Ramanathan L, Siegel JM. Rapid changes in glutamate levels in the posterior hypothalamus across sleep-wake states in freely behaving rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2008 doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.90541.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Jones BE. From waking to sleeping: neuronal and chemical substrates. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2005;26:578–586. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2005.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Karadzic V, Micic D, Rakic L. Alterations of free amino acids concentrations in cat brain induced by rapid eye movement sleep deprivation. Experientia. 1971;27:509–511. doi: 10.1007/BF02147566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kekesi KA, Dobolyi A, Salfay O, Nyitrai G, Juhasz G. Slow wave sleep is accompanied by release of certain amino acids in the thalamus of cats. Neuroreport. 1997;8:1183–1186. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199703240-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kennedy C, Gillin JC, Mendelson W, Suda S, Miyaoka M, Ito M, Nakamura RK, Storch FI, Pettigrew K, Mishkin M, Sokoloff L. Local cerebral glucose utilization in non-rapid eye movement sleep. Nature. 1982;297:325–327. doi: 10.1038/297325a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kodama T, Honda Y. Acetylcholine and glutamate release during sleep-wakefulness in the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus and norepinephrine changes regulated by nitric oxide. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;53:109–111. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.1999.00543.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kodama T, Lai YY, Siegel JM. Enhanced glutamate release during REM sleep in the rostromedial medulla as measured by in vivo microdialysis. Brain Res. 1998;780:178–181. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Lena I, Parrot S, Deschaux O, Muffat-Joly S, Sauvinet V, Renaud B, Suaud-Chagny MF, Gottesmann C. Variations in extracellular levels of dopamine, noradrenaline, glutamate, and aspartate across the sleep--wake cycle in the medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats. J Neurosci Res. 2005;81:891–899. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lopez-Rodriguez F, Medina-Ceja L, Wilson CL, Jhung D, Morales-Villagran A. Changes in extracellular glutamate levels in rat orbitofrontal cortex during sleep and wakefulness. Arch Med Res. 2007;38:52–55. doi: 10.1016/j.arcmed.2006.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Madsen PL, Schmidt JF, Wildschiodtz G, Friberg L, Holm S, Vorstrup S, Lassen NA. Cerebral O2 metabolism and cerebral blood flow in humans during deep and rapid-eye-movement sleep. J Appl Physiol. 1991;70:2597–2601. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1991.70.6.2597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mangano RM, Schwarcz R. Chronic infusion of endogenous excitatory amino acids into rat striatum and hippocampus. Brain Res Bull. 1983;10:47–51. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(83)90073-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Maquet P. Sleep function(s) and cerebral metabolism. Behav Brain Res. 1995;69:75–83. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00017-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mark J, Godin Y, Mandel P. Biosynthesis of aspartic, glutamic, gamma-aminobutyric acids and glutamine in brain of rats deprived of total sleep or paradoxical sleep. J Neurochem. 1969;16:1263–1272. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1969.tb05975.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Nicoll RA, Madison DV. General anesthetics hyperpolarize neurons in the vertebrate central nervous system. Science. 1982;217:1055–1057. doi: 10.1126/science.7112112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Noda H, Adey WR. Neuronal activity in the association cortex of the cat during sleep, wakefulness and anesthesia. Brain Res. 1973;54:243–259. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Oldenziel WH, Dijkstra G, Cremers TI, Westerink BH. In vivo monitoring of extracellular glutamate in the brain with a microsensor. Brain Res. 2006;1118:34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Pomerleau F, Day BK, Huettl P, Burmeister JJ, Gerhardt GA. Real time in vivo measures of L-glutamate in the rat central nervous system using ceramic-based multisite microelectrode arrays. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;1003:454–457. doi: 10.1196/annals.1300.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Regan RF, Choi DW. Glutamate neurotoxicity in spinal cord cell culture. Neuroscience. 1991;43:585–591. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90317-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Richards CD, Strupinski K. An analysis of the action of pentobarbitone on the excitatory postsynaptic potentials and membrane properties of neurones in the guinea-pig olfactory cortex. Br J Pharmacol. 1986;89:321–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1986.tb10263.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Rosenberg PA, Amin S, Leitner M. Glutamate uptake disguises neurotoxic potency of glutamate agonists in cerebral cortex in dissociated cell culture. J Neurosci. 1992;12:56–61. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-01-00056.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rothstein JD, Dykes-Hoberg M, Pardo CA, Bristol LA, Jin L, Kuncl RW, Kanai Y, Hediger MA, Wang Y, Schielke JP, Welty DF. Knockout of glutamate transporters reveals a major role for astroglial transport in excitotoxicity and clearance of glutamate. Neuron. 1996;16:675–686. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80086-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rutherford EC, Pomerleau F, Huettl P, Stromberg I, Gerhardt GA. Chronic second-by-second measures of L-glutamate in the central nervous system of freely moving rats. J Neurochem. 2007;102:712–722. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.04596.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sallanon-Moulin M, Touret M, Didier-Bazes M, Roudier V, Fages C, Tardy M, Jouvet M. Glutamine synthetase modulation in the brain of rats subjected to deprivation of paradoxical sleep. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1994;22:113–120. doi: 10.1016/0169-328x(94)90038-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Santello M, Volterra A. Synaptic modulation by astrocytes via Ca(2+)-dependent glutamate release. Neuroscience. 2008 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.03.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sem'yanov AV. Diffusional extrasynaptic neurotransmission via glutamate and GABA. Neurosci Behav Physiol. 2005;35:253–266. doi: 10.1007/s11055-005-0003-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Steriade M, Timofeev I, Grenier F. Natural waking and sleep states: a view from inside neocortical neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2001;85:1969–1985. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.5.1969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tang X, Yang L, Sanford LD. Sleep and EEG spectra in rats recorded via telemetry during surgical recovery. Sleep. 2007;30:1057–1061. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.8.1057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Timmerman W, Westerink BH. Brain microdialysis of GABA and glutamate: what does it signify? Synapse. 1997;27:242–261. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(199711)27:3<242::AID-SYN9>3.0.CO;2-D. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Tzingounis AV, Wadiche JI. Glutamate transporters: confining runaway excitation by shaping synaptic transmission. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:935–947. doi: 10.1038/nrn2274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. van der Zeyden M, Oldenziel WH, Rea K, Cremers TI, Westerink BH. Microdialysis of GABA and glutamate: Analysis, interpretation and comparison with microsensors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007 doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Vyazovskiy VV, Cirelli C, Tononi G, Tobler I. Cortical metabolic rates as measured by 2-deoxyglucose-uptake are increased after waking and decreased after sleep in mice. Brain Res Bull. 2008a;75:591–597. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.10.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Vyazovskiy VV, Cirelli C, Pfister-Genskow M, Faraguna U, Tononi G. Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nat Neurosci. 2008b;11:200–208. doi: 10.1038/nn2035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Wadiche JI, Arriza JL, Amara SG, Kavanaugh MP. Kinetics of a human glutamate transporter. Neuron. 1995;14:1019–1027. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90340-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Wan X, Mathers DA, Puil E. Pentobarbital modulates intrinsic and GABA-receptor conductances in thalamocortical inhibition. Neuroscience. 2003;121:947–958. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp1. Supplementary Figure 1.

Due to an observed decrease in absolute signal strength across the recording duration, signals were rectified by removing a linear decay component before subsequent analyses. ABCD) The original (grey) and rectified (black) signal from four individual rats across the duration of the recording period. Data were analyzed for up to 4 days post-surgery during which time the proper functioning of signal channels (as indicated by positive [glutamate]) was always observed. A positive signal was observed for as long as 6 days post-surgery.

Supp2. Supplementary Figure 2.

No increase in glutamate concentration following whisker stimulation ipsilateral to an electrode implanted in the barrel cortex in one rat. The rat received 7 stimulations. The average of all 7 stimulations, time-locked to stimulation onset, is depicted in 4-sec intervals. Stimulation onset began at arrow and each stimulation lasted on average 20.0 ± 1.75 sec.

RESOURCES