Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 May 29.
Published in final edited form as: J Neurosci. 2009 Feb 11;29(6):1648–1656. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4578-08.2009

Neural activity associated with the passive prediction of ambiguity and risk for aversive events

Dominik R Bach 1,*, Ben Seymour 1, Raymond J Dolan 1
PMCID: PMC2688025  EMSID: UKMS3995  PMID: 19211872

Abstract

In economic decision making, outcomes are described in terms of risk (uncertain outcomes with certain probabilities) and ambiguity (uncertain outcomes with uncertain probabilities). Humans are more averse to ambiguity compared to risk with a distinct neural system suggested as mediating this effect. However, there has been no clear disambiguation of activity related to decisions themselves from perceptual processing of ambiguity. In a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment we contrasted ambiguity, defined as a lack of information about outcome probabilities, to risk where outcome probabilities are known or ignorance where outcomes are completely unknown and unknowable. We modified previously learned Pavlovian CS+ stimuli such that they became an ambiguous cue and contrasted evoked brain activity both with an unmodified predictive CS+ (risky cue), and a cue that conveyed no information about outcome probabilities (ignorance cue). Compared to risk, ambiguous cues elicited activity in posterior inferior frontal gyrus and posterior parietal cortex during outcome anticipation. Furthermore, a similar set of regions was activated when ambiguous cues were compared with ignorance cues. Thus, regions previously shown to be engaged by decisions about ambiguous rewarding outcomes are also engaged by ambiguous outcome prediction in the context of aversive outcomes. Moreover, activation in these regions was seen even when no actual decision is made. Our findings suggest that these regions subserve a general function of contextual analysis when search for hidden information during outcome anticipation is both necessary and meaningful.

Keywords: ambiguity, risk, uncertainty, probability distribution, probabilistic outcome prediction, Pavlovian conditioning, fear conditioning, fMRI, BOLD

Introduction

Many predictions an organism makes about the world contain uncertainty. In decision making, economists distinguish different kinds of uncertainty. Risk refers to situations in which we know the precise probabilities of each outcome (Bernoulli, 1738). Decision making under risk is axiomised in expected utility theory to provide the basis of rational choice (von Neumann and Morgenstern, 1944). These axioms are violated when outcome probabilities are not known with certainty (that is, they don't correspond to a point estimate), a situation referred to as ambiguity (Ellsberg, 1961).

People tend to avoid outcomes associated with ambiguity (Becker and Brownson, 1964; Slovic and Tversky, 1974; Larson, 1980; Curley et al., 1986; Pulford and Colman, 2008) where one critical feature is lack of information about outcome probabilities (Larson, 1980; Camerer, 1995). It has been suggested that the amygdala, dorsomedial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex mediate decision making under ambiguity (Hsu et al., 2005). However, choices based upon ambiguous monetary gambles are also reported to engage lateral prefrontal cortex, anterior insula and parietal regions (Huettel et al., 2006). A limitation of both these studies is that they conflate activity associated with the perception of ambiguity, and decisions that ensue from this perception. Brain activations in these studies might therefore be attributable decision making as well as to what has been termed in a recent neuroeconomic framework the representation process, that is, the identification and assessment of external and internal states (Rangel et al., 2008). Furthermore, since both studies used ambiguous rewarding outcomes, it is not known whether brain areas activated in these studies also encode ambiguity about aversive outcomes.

The importance of a distinction between outcome prediction with, and without, choice is embedded in theoretical accounts of a distinction between Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning. Thus, Pavlovian conditioning has often been employed to study the distinction between prediction and control (see e.g. Dayan and Balleine, 2002). Accordingly, to study the neural basis of ambiguity perception per se, we used a Pavlovian conditioning procedure and provided participants with ambiguous situations in which an outcome prediction is made, but no actual decision required. Within this context, we defined ambiguity as “known-to-be-missing information, or not knowing relevant information that could be known” (Camerer, 1995; p.645). This definition of ambiguity implies the fact that information is hidden from the observer, and draws distinction with ignorance, in which probabilities of outcomes are unknown, but are also unknowable (unless exploration and learning are permitted).

We degraded previously learned conditioned visual stimuli (CS+) so that, under the ambiguity condition, they only allowed a probabilistic prediction of outcome probabilities. These stimuli were contrasted with the original CS+ that conveyed a point estimate of outcome probabilities. Critically, to distinguish responses to lack of information alone from lack of information that could potentially be known (that is, hidden information), we also included completely novel stimuli that carried no predictive information, thereby corresponding to a uniform prediction of outcome probabilities (see figure 1). Thus, we could identify brain areas responding to probabilistic prediction of outcome probabilities as opposed to certain outcome probabilities and to completely unknown outcome probabilities. Our design ensured that on average, all conditions carried the same outcome probability.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Examples for outcome prediction after risky, ambiguous or ignorance cues, visualised by a second order distribution of outcome probabilities. In the risk condition, prediction of outcome probability corresponds to a point estimate (left). In ambiguous trials, outcome probabilities can be predicted using a second-order distribution, thus rendering outcome predictions probabilistic. Ignorance cues convey no information about outcomes, the outcome probability could therefore have any value, and its prediction corresponds to a uniform distribution.

Materials and methods

Design and participants

The study utilized a Pavlovian conditioning paradigm within a single factorial design with four levels (CS−, risk, ambiguity, ignorance). 20 healthy right-handed participants (10 male, 10 female, mean age ± standard deviation: 27.4 ± 5.8 years) were recruited from the general population and given monetary compensation of £40 for participation. Handedness was controlled with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971; mean ± standard deviation: 83.6 ± 23.2). All participants gave written informed consent, and the study was approved by the local ethics committee.

Independent variable

There were four levels for the independent variable condition, which varied on each trial in an event-related design. (1) A perceptually distinct CS− served as internal baseline condition and signalled the absence of the UCS on this trial. (2) In the risk condition, one of three previously learned compound CS+'s, signalling three different CS-UCS contingencies, was presented with a white frame indicating that no additional noise was added. CS-UCS-contingencies were .25, .50, and .75 respectively. (3) In the ambiguity condition, noise was added to the previously learned compound CS+ by randomly flipping its four information bits at a noise rate of 20% per bit. Thus, participants were unable to generate a point estimate of outcome probabilities, although a probabilistic prediction was possible by taking into account the second order distribution of underlying risky CS+. This condition was signalled to participants by a grey frame around the CS+. Each of 16 possible stimuli appeared at least once, while their frequencies were determined by the noise rate. After the expectancy period, together with the UCS, the original underlying CS+ was shown on the screen. UCS contingency of ambiguous cues was determined by the UCS contingency of the underlying risky cue. Critically, occurrence of the underlying CS+, and thus the frequency of electric shocks, was identical between the risk and the ambiguity condition (see figure 2F). (4) In the ignorance condition, a completely new set of CS+ was presented, which had the same internal structure and CS-UCS contingency as the ambiguity stimuli. However, different symbols were used, and the internal pattern of each stimulus was reversed from left to right. Thus, it was not possible to predict the outcome of these stimuli, and each single stimulus did not occur often enough to fully learn outcome contingencies.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Stimulus set 1 (left) and 2 (right) each of which were used on half of the participants to exclude condition effects due to simple graphical differences. A: CS− that was unchanged throughout the experiment, both in the preceding learning task and at test. B: 3 CS+ that were used in the preceding learning task with different CS-UCS contingencies of 0.25, 0.50, and 0.75 as indicated. C: Risk condition used the three original CS+ cues and was signalled by a white frame around the original CS+. D: Example for an ambiguous stimulus, derived from stimulus C by flipping one of its information bits. The ambiguity condition was signalled by a grey frame around the degraded CS+. E: Example for a novel stimulus, corresponding to stimulus D by exchanging colours and geometric symbols, and reversing its information bits from left to right. F: All 16 possible ambiguous cues from set 1 (similarly for set 2 with geometric symbols instead of colour bars). Each cue was surrounded by a grey frame (omitted in the figure). For each cue, its frequency during the whole experiment is indicated as well as the probability of electric shock after this cue, averaged over the whole experiment. Both frequency and shock probability were determined by the chance that this cue was derived from any of the three underlying risky cues, given a noise rate of 0.2 per information bit.

Stimuli: CS

CS+ stimuli in all conditions consisted of four pieces (i.e. information bits) that could take two different states. In one stimulus set, a combination of four rectangles was used that could be either yellow or blue. In the other set, each of four geometrical symbols could be either a circle or a triangle (see figure 2B). The use of these two stimulus sets was balanced across participants, so that one half received the coloured rectangles as risk/ambiguity stimuli and the dark green symbols as ignorance stimuli, and vice versa for the other half. A white frame indicated the risk, or noiseless, condition, and a grey frame (40%) the ambiguity, or noise, condition (figure 2 C and D). No frame was presented in the ignorance condition (figure 2E). A light blue rectangle without frame served as CS− in both stimulus sets (see figure 2A). All stimuli were presented on a black background, randomly either above or below the screen centre.

Stimuli: UCS

As unconditioned stimulus, participants received an aversive 500 Hz train of electrical pulses (square wave, individual pulse duration: 200 μs, total duration: 500 ms, individually adjusted current, mean ± standard deviation: 14.3 ± 4.3 mA), delivered via paediatric ECG electrodes (DENIL10026, Spes Medica srl, Battipaglia, Italy) to the left hand. Stimulation intensity was determined before the experiment started and was set slightly below the pain tolerance. After the procedure was explained to participants, discomfort and pain thresholds were roughly assessed with ascending stimulation intensity. Random stimulus intensities were then delivered around the pain threshold to establish a stimulus-percept function. The random procedure was repeated after the experiment. There was no change in perception of the stimuli between the pre and the post measurement (p> .70).

Experimental procedure: Pavlovian fear conditioning

After participants arrived in the laboratory, the task was fully explained and the UCS level was determined as described above. Participants then engaged in a Pavlovian learning task in order to condition the different CS-UCS contingencies (CS−: no UCS, CS+: .25, .50 and .75). They were not informed about the nature of the learning task; however, instructions included a statement that “after some symbols, electric stimulation will be more frequent than after others”. Pavlovian conditioning was conducted in the scanner environment with the scanner running to ensure an identical context between learning and the subsequent critical experimental paradigm. Each of the four CS (CS− and three CS+) occurred twenty times in pseudo-randomized order.

To ensure learning, skin conductance responses (SCR) were recorded in a subsample of 14 participants, while in the remaining 6 participants, SCR was not recorded due to a technical failure. Recordings were made on thenar/hypothenar of the left hand using 8 mm Ag/AgCl cup electrodes (EL258, Biopac Systems Inc., Goleta CA, USA). Constant voltage was provided by an integrated skin conductance pre-amplifier (AT64, Autogenic Systems, Wood Dale IL, USA). The signal was converted to an optical pulse (minimum resolution 7 Hz), digitally converted with 100 Hz sampling rate (Micro1401, Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK), and recorded (Spike2, Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). Further analysis was carried out in Matlab (Version 7.1., MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). The signal was temporally filtered with a bidirectional Gaussian kernel of 0.5 s full duration at half maximum and high pass filtered with a first order Butterworth filter and a cut-off frequency of 0.0159 Hz, corresponding to a time constant of 10 s. After visual inspection for artefacts, anticipatory conditioned reaction was calculated as mean SCR during CS presentation, corrected for 1 s baseline before stimulus onset. Learning was modelled using a simple Rescorla-Wagner rule (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972), assuming a learning rate of α = 0.3 and initial outcome prediction values of 0.5 for all four CS. For each individual participant, a trial-by-trial general linear model was then fitted, using predicted outcome, variance of the outcome distribution, and prediction error on the last trial as regressors. Beta estimates of these models were analyzed on the group level using one-sample t-tests. Responses to CS+ and CS− were additionally averaged across trials and analyzed in a one way ANOVA model using the GLM approach in SPM 12.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago IL, USA).

Experimental procedure: Scanning

After the Pavlovian conditioning, the concept of noisy reception was explained as well as the occurrence of completely new symbols in the ignorance condition (see instructions in supplementary methods). During the scanning experiment, 45 stimuli for the risk, ambiguity, and ignorance conditions were presented in pseudo-randomized order. 15 interspersed CS− served as internal baseline (that is, as null events) to allow recovery of BOLD signal in regions of interest.

Intra-trial procedure

At the beginning of each trial, a CS was presented for 5.2 seconds (see figure 3). Participants were tasked to indicate the position of the CS (above or below the screen centre) with a button press as quickly as possible, while between responses, they held down a resting button. After the CS disappeared, the outcome was signalled at the centre of the screen by a lightning-style sign to indicate shock (which was delivered concurrently) or with the words “no shock”. In the ambiguity condition, the original “risky” CS+ was shown in place of the ambiguous CS+. After 0.5 seconds, these signs disappeared and 0.7 s later, a fixation cross was shown during the intertrial period. The inter trial interval (ITA) was jittering between 4.8 s and 7.8 s resulting in a mean trial onset asynchrony of 12.7 s.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

A: Study Design. After an initial Pavlovian conditioning task with one CS− and three CS+ indicating three different outcome contingencies of 0.25, 0.5, and 0.75, these CS+ were degraded in an ambiguity condition to resemble the original CS+ but allow no accurate prediction of outcomes. This was explained to participants as “noisy reception” and indicated by a grey frame. The original cues were shown in a risk condition as indicated by a white frame. In an ignorance condition, completely novel stimuli served as control. Additionally, the CS− was interleaved and served as internal baseline. B: Intra-trial timeline. Each cue was shown for 5.2 seconds, during which participants had to respond to its position on the screen (above or below the screen centre). Then, the outcome was indicated for 0.5 s and delivered. In ambiguous trials, the underlying original CS+ was shown together with the outcome indication.

Image acquisition

Images were acquired on a 3 T Allegra head scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, Erlangen, Germany) with a head coil for RF transmission and signal reception. Field maps were acquired with a standard manufacturer's double echo gradient echo field map sequence (TE, 10.0 and 12.46 ms; TR, 1020 ms; matrix size, 64 × 64), using 64 slices covering the whole head (voxel size, 3 × 3 × 3 mm).

For functional images, we used BOLD signal-sensitive T2*-weighted transverse single-shot gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI; flip angle α, 90°; bandwidth BW, 3551 Hz/pixel; phase-encoding (PE) direction, anterior-posterior; bandwidth in PE direction BWPE, 47.3 Hz/pixel; TE, 30 ms; effective TR, 2600 ms). The manufacturer's standard automatic 3D-shim procedure was performed at the beginning of each experiment. Each volume contained 40 slices of 2 mm thickness (1mm gap between slices; field of view, 192 × 192 mm2; matrix size, 64 × 64). BOLD sensitivity losses in the orbitofrontal cortex and the amygdala due to susceptibility artefacts were minimized by applying a z-shim gradient moment of −0.4 mT/m*ms, a slice tilt of −30° and a positive PE gradient polarity (Weiskopf et al., 2006; 2007). In two scanning sessions, 377 and 382 functional whole-brain volumes were acquired. The first 4 volumes, or 10.4 s, of each session were discarded to obtain steady-state longitudinal magnetization. Each session was concluded by 8 volumes, or 20.8 s, without stimuli.

Whole-brain anatomical scans were acquired using a modified driven equilibrium Fourier transform (MDEFT) sequence with optimized parameters as described previously (Deichmann et al., 2004). 176 sagittal partitions were acquired with an image matrix of 256 × 224 (read × phase) and two-fold oversampling in read direction (head/foot direction) to prevent aliasing (isotropic spatial resolution 1 mm; α, 15°; TR/TE/TI, 7.92 ms/2. 4 ms/910 ms; BW, 195 Hz/Px). Spin tagging in the neck was performed to avoid flow artefacts in the vicinity of blood vessels. The flip angle of the tagging pulse was chosen to be 160° to account for B1 losses in the neck. Special RF excitation pulses were used to compensate for B1 inhomogeneities of the transmit coil in superior/inferior and anterior/posterior directions. Images were reconstructed by performing a standard 3D Fourier Transform, followed by modulus calculation. No data filtering was applied in k-space or in the image domain.

Image analysis

Images analyzed with statistical parametric mapping (SPM 5; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) on Matlab (Version 7.1., MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). EPI images were generated offline from the complex k-space raw data using a generalized reconstruction method based on the measured EPI k-space trajectory to minimize ghosting. They were then corrected for geometric distortions caused by susceptibility-induced field inhomogeneities. A combined approach was used which corrects for both static distortions and changes in these distortions due to head motion (Andersson et al., 2001; Hutton et al., 2002). The static distortions were calculated for each subject from a field map that was processed using the FieldMap toolbox as implemented in SPM5. Using these parameters, the EPI images were then realigned and unwarped, a procedure that allows the measured static distortions to be included in the estimation of distortion changes associated with head motion. The motion-corrected images were then co-registered to the individual's anatomical MDEFT image using a 12-parameter affine transformation, and normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) T1 reference brain template (re-sampled voxel size 2 × 2 × 2 mm). Normalized images were smoothed with an isotropic 8 mm full width at half maximum Gaussian kernel. The time series in each voxel were high-pass filtered at 1/128 Hz to remove low-frequency confounds. We modelled the presentation of cues for each trial type as a separate boxcar regressor convolved with a canonical haemodynamic response function. In fear conditioning studies, phasic responses to CS+ onset are often observed in the amygdala (see for example Büchel et al., 1998; Labar et al., 1998) and occur early rather than late during CS+ presentation (Cheng et al., 2007). To assess such responses, we modelled cue onset with a stick function, thus enhancing sensitivity for early responses to cue onset, and analysed responses to CS+ > CS− and to the shock probability. In both models, outcome onset and outcome valence (shock or no shock) were included as additional regressors. To account for serial acquisition of different slices in one volume, time derivatives for each regressor were included into the model.

Position of cue (and thus, the type of motor response), response latency, motor speed and the correctness of response were normalized for each participant, convolved with a haemodynamic response function, and, together with their time derivatives, orthogonalized with respect to each other and included into the model as regressors of no interest. Further regressors of no interest were movement parameters derived from the realignment procedure and correction regressors for variance caused by the cardiac cycle (Glover et al., 2000). Statistical parametric maps were generated from linear contrasts of interest (ambiguity > risk, ambiguity >ignorance) in each participant. A second level random effect analysis (RFX) was then performed using one sample t–tests on contrast images obtained in each participant for each comparison of interest (df = 19). To identify areas that show an enhanced response to ambiguity compared to both other conditions, a conservative conjunction analysis was performed on the second level, testing against conjunction null (Friston et al., 2005; Nichols et al., 2005). We report clusters with a voxel-level threshold of p < 0.001 (uncorrected). In regions of interest for which we had prior hypotheses (posterior inferior frontal gyrus/sulcus, anterior insula, posterior parietal cortex, orbitofrontal and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala), results were small volume corrected for family-wise error within a sphere of 15 mm diameter around peak coordinates as reported by Huettel et al. (2006) and Hsu et al. (2005), and reported at a voxel-level threshold of p < .05. For the conjunction analysis, small volume correction was performed on peak coordinates from main contrasts. BOLD responses outside of regions of interest are reported at a cluster-level threshold of p < .05, whole-brain corrected for family-wise error.

Behavioural data analysis

The effect of the condition factor on reaction time measures and task performance was analyzed in a one-way factorial design, using GLM procedures in SPSS 12.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago IL, USA).

Results

Pavlovian conditioning

During Pavlovian conditioning, anticipatory skin conductance responses (conditioned reactions, CR) were greater for the CS+ than for the CS− (F1, 12 = 22.9; p < .001). Predicted outcome was significantly correlated with CR (t13 = 2. 4; p < .05), showing that participants learned the CS-UCS contingencies. After conditioning, we tested for typical brain responses associated with conditioned stimuli. By modelling phasic responses to predictive cue onset we observed enhanced activity in left amygdala for the contrast of CS+ (i.e. risky cues) compared to CS−, surviving small volume correction with a sphere of 15 mm diameter around peak coordinates reported previously for phasic amygdala responses in fear conditioning (Morris and Dolan, 2004). Across the three CS+, activity in the ventral striatum showed a linear increase with increasing probability of negative outcome, surviving small volume correction with a sphere of 15 mm diameter around peak coordinates reported for representation of outcome probability (Tobler et al., 2007) (additional information can be found in supplementary table 1).

Behavioural responses to the concurrent task

During the critical phase of the experiment, participants were tasked to respond to the position of the cue, in order to provide a broad measure for attention. Thus, we were able to test whether our experimental manipulation influenced cognitive processes related to a concurrent task. There was no effect of the condition factor on mean and variance of overall reaction times or response latency, or on task performance (p> .30 for all ANOVAs).

BOLD responses to contrasts of interest

Our critical contrasts of interest related to the post-learning period and involved evoked responses to presentation of ambiguous and risky, and ambiguous and ignorance cues, respectively. Enhanced differential responses to ambiguous compared to risky stimuli were found bilaterally in posterior inferior frontal gyrus (pIFG), extending into middle frontal gyrus (MFG), and bilaterally in the posterior superior parietal lobule (pPAR) in Brodmann area 7 (see table 1 at the end of the text, figure 4A and B). All of these clusters survived small volume correction around peak coordinates as reported by Huettel et al. (2006). Additional clusters that survived whole-brain correction were found in occipito-temporal cortex including higher visual areas. We did not observe any activation that was greater for risky compared to ambiguity cues.

Table 1.

Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) responses to contrasts of interest. All clusters are reported at a voxel-level significance threshold of p < .001, small volume corrected with p < .05 for family-wise error (FWE) in regions of interest for a 15 mm sphere around peak coordinates as reported by Huettel et al. (2006), and cluster-level corrected with p < .05 for family-wise error (FWE) outside regions of interest (marked *).

Brain regions Brodmann
area of
local
maxima
Hemisphere Voxel
number
Voxel t
score
Montreal Neurological Institute brain
template coordinates of local maxima
Ambiguity > risk
Posterior inferior frontal gyrus 44 & 45 right 50 4.53 52, 14, 14;58, 10, 26
44 & 9 left 147 4.87 −52, 10, 26;−42, 2, 30
Posterior superior parietal lobule 7 right 44 4.41 34, −50, 50;30, −58, 44
7 left 93 4.97 −26, −54, 46
Middle frontal gyrus 9 right 15 4.22 40, 10, 30
Fusiform gyrus, inferior
temporal gyrus &
parahippocampal gyrus*
19 right 536 5.97 32, −56, −8; 30, −68, −10;44, −54, −4
Fusiform gyrus& middle
temporal gyrus*
37 left 188 5.41 −44, −58, 0;−52, −60, 2;−28, −68, −10

Ambiguity > ignorance
Posterior inferior frontal gyrus&
middle frontal gyrus
44, 46, 9 right 605 5.47 42, 16, 24; 58, 16, 16; 44, 4, 28
Posterior inferior frontal gyrus 46 right 69 4.82 36, 32, 8;34, 24, 4
Posterior inferior frontal gyrus &
middle frontal gyrus
9 left 232 5.26 −52, 8, 30;−44, 6, 30; −48, 6, 38
Posterior superior & inferior
parietal lobule
7 right 86 4.48 30, −58, 44; 30, −64 , 50
Occipital cortex* 18, 37, 17 bilateral 4229 8.31 −30, −82, −8; 32, −46, −14; 10, −88, 6
Middle temporal gyrus & precuneus* 19 left 173 5.27 −34, −82, 20;−28, −76, 38

Conjunction: (Ambiguity > risk) & (ambiguity > ignorance)
Posterior inferior frontal gyrus 9 right 10 4.0 54, 14, 14
Middle frontal gyrus 44 right 26 4.17 44, 12, 30
Posterior inferior frontal gyrus 9 left 69 4.26 −50, 8, 26
Posterior superior parietal lobule 7 right 3 3.68 30, −60, 44
Fusiform gyrus &
parahippocampal gyrus*
19 & 37 right 566 4.77 28, −70, −14;38, −52, −14; 30, −56, −8
Fusiform gyrus & lingual gyrus* 18 & 19 left 344 5.29 −28, −68, −10; −20, −74, −12; −26, −80, −8

Figure 4.

Figure 4

BOLD responses to ambiguous cues, as compared to risky or ignorance cues. Clusters are overlaid on a mean T1-weighted image from all participants, and displayed at a voxel-level threshold of p < .001 (uncorrected) and small volume correction with regard to peak coordinates of previous studies as indicated in the methods section. A: Bilateral pIFG responses to ambiguous as compared to risky cues. B: Bilateral pPAR and occipital responses to ambiguous as compared to risky cues. C: Bilateral pIFG responses to ambiguous as compared to ignorance cues. D: Right pPAR and bilateral occipital responses to ambiguous as compared to ignorance cues. E: Bilateral pIFG responses to ambiguous as compared to both risky and ignorance cues (conjunction analysis, testing against conjunction null). F: Right pPAR and bilateral occipital responses to ambiguous as compared to both risky and ignorance cues (conjunction analysis, testing against conjunction null).

Our second contrast of interest involving ambiguous compared to ignorance stimuli revealed a set of clusters similar to the above contrast. Specifically, we observed pIFG clusters extending into middle frontal gyrus (MFG) on the right (see figure 4C and D). The contrast also showed enhanced posterior parietal responses in the right hemisphere. All of these clusters survived small volume correction around peak coordinates as reported by Huettel et al. (2006). Additional clusters that survived whole-brain correction were found in occipito-temporal regions. We did not observe activations that were stronger in response to ignorance than to ambiguity cues.

In order to identify brain areas that showed a greater response to ambiguous than to both risky and ignorance cues, we performed a conjunction analysis. This analysis revealed enhanced responses in bilateral pIFG, extending into right MFG. A small cluster in right pPAR also showed conjoint responses across both contrasts (see figure 4E and F and figure 5). All of these clusters survived small volume correction around peak coordinates from individual contrasts. Additionally, occipito-temporal areas, including visual areas, responded to ambiguous cues and survived whole-brain correction.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Parameter estimates for the three conditions risk, ambiguity, and ignorance, contrasted with the CS− that served as internal baseline. For each participant, estimates were averaged within the clusters displayed in figure 4E and F. Values are stated in arbitrary units (mean across participants ± standard error).

In an exploratory analysis, ignorance and risk were contrasted. No activations were observed that were significantly greater to ignorance than to risk cues. Cerebral responses in the primary visual cortex were greater for risk than for ignorance cues and are summarized in supplementary table 2.

Discussion

The present study examined neural responses to perception of ambiguity in the absence of choice. We show that within posterior inferior frontal gyrus (pIFG) and posterior parietal cortex (pPAR), two brain regions previously reported to encode decision making about ambiguous outcomes (Huettel et al., 2006) respond to the perception of ambiguity in the absence of decision making. As our paradigm involved negative outcomes, namely painful electric shocks, it appears brain regions previously associated with ambiguity of reward prediction respond also to ambiguity in aversive predictions. Critically, these responses occur only when a lack of information in outcome predictions could potentially be known. That is, neural responses evoked by ambiguity are not correlated with the distribution width of the outcome prediction, which is highest under complete ignorance, but rather with the fact that information was hidden from the observer.

Economic ambiguity as a contextual cue

The lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) is implicated in component processes related to behavioural planning (Lee et al., 2007; Tanji et al., 2007; Sakagami and Watanabe, 2007). Most notably for the present study, where no actual behaviour was required in response to the cues, it was suggested that the LPFC is involved in maintaining a state representation in a given environment (Lee et al., 2007). The LPFC area has also been parsed into functionally specific subregions on a dorsal/ventral (Tanji and Hoshi, 2008) or on an anterior/posterior axis (Koechlin and Summerfield, 2007). In the present study, LPFC activation was observed in a posterior dorsal area, the pIFG. It has been suggested that the ventral LPFC is more involved in processing specific, object-related information, while the dorsal prefrontal cortex is involved in more general functions of monitoring strategic behaviour (Tanji and Hoshi, 2008). A framework for a hierarchical anterior/posterior distribution of functions, the cascade model (Koechlin and Summerfield, 2007), proposes that simple stimulus-response relationships are controlled by premotor regions alone, while more contextual cues draw on the posterior prefrontal cortex, with conflicting information drawing on even more anterior regions. Within these two accounts of LPFC subregions, dorsal pIFG activation noted in the present study, as well as in the study by Huettel et al., might reflect a representation of a contextual cue that requires more than mere sensori-motor control. Although in the experimental task, no behavioural response to the cues was necessary, in an everyday environment, economic ambiguity might provide a strong incentive to search for hidden information during an anticipation period, thus overriding simple sensorimotor processes. This search could be under the control of pIFG, even in the absence of an actual decision. This is in line with a previous study examining a non-economic context and showing activity in the same areas during outcome prediction when contextual cues implied uncertainty (Huettel et al., 2005). This speculation is also supported by the finding that the pIFG responds to economic situations requiring further information search, for example sudden changes in the reward landscape within a dynamic economic game (Li et al., 2006).

We found a region within pPAR (namely BA 7) activated in response to ambiguity as opposed to risk and ignorance. This area shares connections with the prefrontal cortex (Tanji and Hoshi, 2008) which have been suggested to convey integrated sensory information to executive areas, the strength of which may mediate the effectiveness of executive control (Jung and Haier, 2007). Parietal areas responsible for sensory integration represent a critical decision variable in perceptual decision making (Gold and Shadlen, 2007) and are implicated in valuation of economic choices (see for example Platt and Glimcher, 1999; Dorris and Glimcher, 2004; Glimcher et al., 2005). The responses we observed in this region can be construed as reflecting value calculations, and these calculations are necessarily more complex for ambiguous than for risky or ignorance trials due to the second order probability distribution of ambiguous outcomes. However, since experimental cues differed not only in the information they conveyed but also in colour and shape, pPAR responses could conceivably reflect these sensory differences. We think this is unlikely given that differences in these sensory attributes would be expected to be reflected in differential responses in earlier stages of processing, such as visual areas in the occipital cortex.

Finally, a competing explanation for different responses between conditions relates to general attentional differences as, for example, between risk and ambiguity. However, our behavioural data indicate no such difference with respect to reaction time measures and task performance. For additional control, we also covaried reaction times and task performance out of the BOLD signal. Finally, we note there is no easy formalization of attention and in so far as risk and ambiguity embody greater or lesser requirements for information search then it can be argued that each of these processes necessarily entail different levels of attention.

Economic ambiguity and uncertainty of outcome

It has been argued that the critical feature of economic ambiguity is a lack of information (Ellsberg, 1961). In economic studies, this is often operationalized as gambles including a second order distribution of outcome probabilities (Becker and Brownson, 1964; Larson, 1980; Curley et al., 1986; Pulford and Colman, 2008), which implies a probabilistic prediction of outcome probabilities. This key concept of lack of information about outcome probabilities has been translated as uncertainty of outcomes, or the distribution width of outcome predictions (Yates and Zukowski, 1976). In our experiment, any measure of distribution width of possible outcomes is highest for ignorance and lowest for the risk condition. Since in many experiments on economic ambiguity, the second order outcome is hidden from the observer, an alternative view involves reformulation of economic ambiguity as “not knowing relevant information that could be known” (Camerer, 1995; p.645). This quantity is highest in the ambiguity condition in our study.

We failed to identify any brain region responding to uncertainty of outcomes that showed greater responses for ignorance over ambiguity and at the same time for ambiguity over risk. Indeed, virtually all neural responses to ambiguity compared to risk were also evident when ambiguity was compared to ignorance. Therefore, the critical aspect of ambiguity in our experiment, in terms of observable neural activity, was not uncertainty in itself but the fact that the missing information was hidden to the observer and could potentially be revealed. This fits an interpretation of ambiguity as a contextual cue necessitating further search for information before the outcome occurs, i.e. during the anticipation period. In contrast, under complete ignorance of outcome distributions, an optimal strategy might involve sampling from outcomes rather than looking for information in the environment. This is reflected in phenomena such as novelty seeking (see e.g. Wittmann et al., 2008), as opposed to ambiguity aversion. The potential to gain knowledge of hidden outcome information is therefore a critical factor for brain responses observed here.

One could argue that in the study by Huettel et al. (2006) that showed similar brain responses as our study, ambiguous gambles in fact involved a uniform outcome probability distribution, and that this translates as complete ignorance. The intra-trial design of this experiment however strongly emphasized the fact that probabilities were merely hidden rather than unknown, since they were revealed after a decision had been made. This is very similar to our own study where underlying risky cues were shown during the outcome phase of ambiguous trials, thus emphasizing the fact that there was information present that could potentially be revealed, and therefore makes our results on the effect of ambiguity over ignorance compatible with those of Huettel et al. (2006).

An alternative explanation for these findings relates to the computational demand which is also highest in the ambiguity condition compared to both other condition. Both explanations however indicate that uncertainty of outcomes is not the factor modulating cerebral responses in the present study. Whether it is also critical for actual economic decisions under ambiguity cannot be answered within our paradigm.

Ambiguity and the amygdala

In contrast to the present experiment, a previous fMRI study has suggested that the amygdala, dorsomedial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex underlie decision making under ambiguity (Hsu et al., 2005). This is consistent with a framework of the amygdala as a detector of ambiguity (Whalen, 1998) elaborated to explain why the amygdala is more activated in response to fearful (that supposedly do not clearly indicate whether a threat is present) than to angry faces (e.g. Whalen et al., 2001), and why amygdala responses during fear conditioning quickly habituate (supposedly because they reflect uncertainty about CS-UCS contingencies; e.g. Buchel et al., 1998). Although it is obvious that the amygdala responds to some kinds of uncertainty (e.g. temporal unpredictability, Herry et al., 2007), different forms of uncertainty have not been formally compared with regard to such responses. The kind of outcome uncertainty described in the aforementioned work is likely to be different from the economic definition applied in the present study (e.g. the lack of knowledge about CS-UCS contingencies in fear conditioning paradigms corresponds to the ignorance and not the ambiguity condition in the present study). The study by Hsu et al. (2005), although concerned with an economic definition of ambiguity, in fact collapsed different kinds of “ambiguous” situations for analysis of fMRI data, that is, monetary gambles following a strict economic definition, but also quizzes, and uninformed gambles against an informed opponent. Together, there is no entirely convincing empirical evidence that the amygdala responds to ambiguity as defined in a strict economic sense, an inference upheld by our present findings, although such a role of the amygdala cannot be discounted entirely (Seymour and Dolan, 2008).

Individual attitudes to risk and ambiguity

The approach taken in the present paradigm entailed dispensing with any active decisions in order to study the brain response to perception of stimuli per se. This makes it difficult to directly infer participants' attitudes towards these cues as in terms of formalised neuroeconomic models that incorporate individual risk and ambiguity preferences. Previous imaging studies have shown that neural responses to risky and ambiguous situations might depend on such individual attitudes (e.g. Hsu et al., 2005; Huettel et al., 2006). The present study however was not designed to test such relations though we acknowledge their likely importance.

Conclusions

Ambiguity is an important aspect of uncertainty in decision making. Here, we draw on the definition of ambiguity as a lack of information that could potentially be known and operationalized it as a probabilistic prediction of outcomes in a Pavlovian conditioning paradigm. In the absence of any decisions, we show that a network comprising the pIFG and pPAR responds to the perception of ambiguity as opposed to risk (point estimate of outcome probabilities) or ignorance (uniform distribution of outcome probabilities). This corresponds to a network previously been implicated in decisions involving ambiguity. We conclude that the critical factor for these responses to ambiguity is anticipation of an outcome when information about outcome probabilities is hidden, but potentially knowable. pIFG responses might subserve contextual analysis when search of hidden information is both necessary and meaningful prior to the occurrence of an outcome.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by a Programme Grant to RJD from the Wellcome Trust and by a Personal Grant to DRB from the Swiss National Science Foundation. We wish to thank Mkael Symmonds for many helpful discussions.

References

  1. Andersson JL, Hutton C, Ashburner J, Turner R, Friston K. Modeling geometric deformations in EPI time series. Neuroimage. 2001;13:903–919. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Becker SW, Brownson FO. What price ambiguity? Or the role of ambiguity in decision making. J Polit Econ. 1964;72:62–73. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bernoulli D. Specimen theoriae novae de mensura sortis. Commentarii Academiae Scientiarum Imperialis Petropolitanae. 1738;5:175–192. [Google Scholar]
  4. Buchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:947–957. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80476-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Büchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:947–957. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80476-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Camerer C. Individual decision making. In: Kagel JH, Roth AE, editors. Handbook of experimental economics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1995. pp. 587–704. [Google Scholar]
  7. Cheng DT, Richards J, Helmstetter FJ. Activity in the human amygdala corresponds to early, rather than late period autonomic responses to a signal for shock. Learn Mem. 2007;14:485–490. doi: 10.1101/lm.632007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Curley SP, Yates F, Abrams RA. Psychological sources of ambiguity avoidance. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1986;38:230–256. [Google Scholar]
  9. Dayan P, Balleine BW. Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning. Neuron. 2002;36:285–298. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00963-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Deichmann R, Schwarzbauer C, Turner R. Optimisation of the 3D MDEFT sequence for anatomical brain imaging: technical implications at 1.5 and 3 T. Neuroimage. 2004;21:757–767. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dorris MC, Glimcher PW. Activity in posterior parietal cortex is correlated with the relative subjective desirability of action. Neuron. 2004;44:365–378. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ellsberg D. Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms. Q J Econ. 1961;75:643–669. [Google Scholar]
  13. Friston KJ, Penny WD, Glaser DE. Conjunction revisited. Neuroimage. 2005;25:661–667. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Glimcher PW, Dorris MC, Bayer HM. Physiological utility theory and the neuroeconomics of choice. Games Econ Behav. 2005;52:213–256. doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Glover GH, Li TQ, Ress D. Image-based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR. Magn Reson Med. 2000;44:162–167. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200007)44:1<162::aid-mrm23>3.0.co;2-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gold JI, Shadlen MN. The neural basis of decision making. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:535–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Herry C, Bach DR, Esposito F, Di Salle F, Perrig WJ, Scheffler K, Luthi A, Seifritz E. Processing of temporal unpredictability in human and animal amygdala. J Neurosci. 2007;27:5958–5966. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5218-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hsu M, Bhatt M, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Camerer CF. Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science. 2005;310:1680–1683. doi: 10.1126/science.1115327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Huettel SA, Song AW, McCarthy G. Decisions under uncertainty: probabilistic context influences activation of prefrontal and parietal cortices. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3304–3311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5070-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Huettel SA, Stowe CJ, Gordon EM, Warner BT, Platt ML. Neural signatures of economic preferences for risk and ambiguity. Neuron. 2006;49:765–775. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hutton C, Bork A, Josephs O, Deichmann R, Ashburner J, Turner R. Image distortion correction in fMRI: A quantitative evaluation. Neuroimage. 2002;16:217–240. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jung RE, Haier RJ. The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence. Behav Brain Sci. 2007;30:135–154. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Koechlin E, Summerfield C. An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:229–235. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Labar KS, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: a mixed-trial fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:937–945. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80475-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Larson JR. Exploring the external validity of a subjectively weighted utility model of decision making. Organ Behav Hum Perform. 1980;26:293–304. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lee D, Rushworth MF, Walton ME, Watanabe M, Sakagami M. Functional specialization of the primate frontal cortex during decision making. J Neurosci. 2007;27:8170–8173. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1561-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Li J, McClure SM, King-Casas B, Montague PR. Policy adjustment in a dynamic economic game. PLoS ONE. 2006;1:e103. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Morris JS, Dolan RJ. Dissociable amygdala and orbitofrontal responses during reversal fear conditioning. Neuroimage. 2004;22:372–380. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Nichols T, Brett M, Andersson J, Wager T, Poline JB. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage. 2005;25:653–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Platt ML, Glimcher PW. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature. 1999;400:233–238. doi: 10.1038/22268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Pulford BD, Colman AM. Size doesn't really matter. Ambiguity aversion in Ellsberg urns with few balls. Exp Psychol. 2008;55:31–37. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.55.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rangel A, Camerer C, Montague PR. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:545–556. doi: 10.1038/nrn2357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black AH, Prokasy WF, editors. Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  35. Sakagami M, Watanabe M. Integration of cognitive and motivational information in the primate lateral prefrontal cortex. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1104:89–107. doi: 10.1196/annals.1390.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Seymour B, Dolan Emotion, Decision Making, and the Amygdala. Neuron. 2008;58:662–671. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Slovic P, Tversky A. Who accepts Savage's axiom? Behav Sci. 1974;19:368–373. [Google Scholar]
  38. Tanji J, Hoshi E. Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioral control. Physiol Rev. 2008;88:37–57. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00014.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Tanji J, Shima K, Mushiake H. Concept-based behavioral planning and the lateral prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:528–534. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Tobler PN, O'Doherty JP, Dolan RJ, Schultz W. Reward value coding distinct from risk attitude-related uncertainty coding in human reward systems. J Neurophysiol. 2007;97:1621–1632. doi: 10.1152/jn.00745.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. von Neumann J, Morgenstern O. Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1944. [Google Scholar]
  42. Weiskopf N, Hutton C, Josephs O, Deichmann R. Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: a whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T. Neuroimage. 2006;33:493–504. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Weiskopf N, Hutton C, Josephs O, Turner R, Deichmann R. Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex: compensation of susceptibility-induced gradients in the readout direction. MAGMA. 2007;20:39–49. doi: 10.1007/s10334-006-0067-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Whalen PJ. Fear, vigilance and ambiguity: initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1998;7:177–188. [Google Scholar]
  45. Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Fischer H, Wright CI, Rauch SL. A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. Emotion. 2001;1:70–83. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wittmann BC, Daw ND, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. Striatal activity underlies novelty-based choice in humans. Neuron. 2008;58:967–973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Yates JF, Zukowski LG. Characterization of Ambiguity in Decision-Making. Behav Sci. 1976;21:19–25. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material

RESOURCES