Skip to main content
HAL-INSERM logoLink to HAL-INSERM
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2007 Sep 26.
Published in final edited form as: J Sports Sci. 2006 May;24(5):509–520. doi: 10.1080/02640410500244697

Assessing the limitations of the Banister model in monitoring training

Philippe Hellard 1, Marta Avalos 2,*, Lucien Lacoste 1, Frédéric Barale 1, Jean-Claude Chatard 3, Grégoire P Millet 4
PMCID: PMC1974899  PMID: 16608765

Abstract

The aim of this study was to carry out a statistical analysis of the Banister model to verify how useful it is in monitoring the training programmes of elite swimmers. The accuracy, the ill-conditioning and the stability of this model were thus investigated. Training loads of nine elite swimmers, measured over one season, were related to performances with the Banister model. Firstly, to assess accuracy, the 95% bootstrap confidence interval (95% CI) of parameter estimates and modelled performances were calculated. Secondly, to study ill-conditioning, the correlation matrix of parameter estimates was computed. Finally, to analyse stability, iterative computation was performed with the same data but minus one performance, chosen randomly. Performances were significantly related to training loads in all subjects (R2= 0.79 ± 0.13, P < 0.05) and the estimation procedure seemed to be stable. Nevertheless, the 95% CI of the most useful parameters for monitoring training were wide τa =38 (17, 59), τf =19 (6, 32), tn =19 (7, 35), tg =43 (25, 61). Furthermore, some parameters were highly correlated making their interpretation worthless. The study suggested possible ways to deal with these problems and reviewed alternative methods to model the training-performance relationships.

Keywords: bootstrap method, inaccuracy, instability, training loads, swimming

Keywords: Adult; Female; Humans; Male; Models, Biological; Physical Education and Training; methods; Physical Fitness; physiology; Rest; physiology; Swimming; physiology; Task Performance and Analysis

Introduction

The relationships between training and performance may be of practical use to sports elite coaches when organizing their athletes’ training programs. These relationships are known to be highly individualized (Mujika et al., 1996b; Avalos et al., 2003). These differences can be attributed to genetic factors (Wolfarth et al., 2000), individual training background (Mujika et al., 1996a; Avalos et al., 2003), psychological factors (Banister et al., 1975), technical factors (Toussaint et Hollander, 1994; Wakayoshi et al., 1995) and specialty (Mujika et al., 1996a,b; Stewart and Hopkins, 2000; Avalos et al., 2003). There is a large consensus that modelling training-performance relations provides pertinent information concerning inter-individual differences enabling highly individualised training programmes (Banister et al., 1975, 1999; Morton et al., 1990, 1991, 1997; Mujika et al., 1996a, b; Avalos et al., 2003). The model proposed by Banister et al. (1975) and its different extensions (Calvert et al., 1976; Busso et al., 1997, 2002; Busso, 2003) aimed to relate training loads to performance, taking into account the dynamic and temporal characteristics of training and therefore the effects of load sequences over time. These effects may be described by two antagonistic transfer functions: first, a positive influence that synthesizes all the positive effects leading to an increase in performance, second a negative function that synthesizes all the negative effects leading to short- or long-term fatigue and having a negative influence on performance (Busso et al., 1994; Mujika et al., 1996a). The function is as follows (Banister et al., 1975; Busso et al., 1994):

pt=p0+kas=0t1e(ts)/τawskfs=0t1e(ts)/τfws

where pt is the modelled performance at time t; p0 is the initial performance level; ka and kf are the fitness and fatigue magnitude factor, respectively; τa and τf are the fitness and fatigue decay time constant, respectively; and wt is the known training load per week (or day) from the first week of training to the week (or day) preceding the performance. These parameters were interpreted as individual responses profiles (Mujika et al., 1996a), presented as usable within the training prescription; e.g. tn, the time to recover performance and tg, the time to peak performance after training completion (Fitz-Clarke et al., 1991). However, several authors (Busso et al., 1990; Mujika et al., 1996a; Taha and Thomas, 2003) reported that the practical interpretation of the positive and negative influences might be difficult. For example, Busso et al. 1990 reported a positive correlation between the testosterone level and the function of fatigue, while a negative relationship was expected. Recently, Taha and Thomas (2003) criticized the different models stemming from the original Banister model (Banister et al., 1975), stressing (i) the inability of the model to accurately predict future performance; (ii) the differences between the estimated time course of change in performance and experimental observations; (iii) the fact that most of these models were poorly corroborated by the physiological mechanisms.

Moreover, quality measures of models were commonly limited to determination coefficient (R2) associated with the F-ratio test. R2 evaluates the goodness of fit but does not necessarily guarantee accurate prediction (Bartley, 1997; Atkinson and Nevill, 1998). With the F-ratio test, the hypothesis H0: ka = 0 and kf = 0 and τa = 0 and τf = 0 is tested versus H1: ka ≠ 0 and/or kf ≠ 0 and/or τa ≠ 0 and/or τf ≠ 0 (Sen and Shrivastava, 1990). A significant F-test implies that the Banister model is better than the constant model, pt = p0, in which training load does not affect performance.

Complementary measures are needed to indicate accuracy and sensitivity (Wetherill et al., 1986; Efron and Tibshirani, 1993). Thus 95% CI’s quantifies accuracy of parameter estimates and modelled performances. The asymptotic correlation matrix of parameter estimates assesses ill-conditioning and variability, which affect the accuracy and precision of parameter estimates (Bates and Watts, 1988; Belsley, 1991; Arsac et al., 2004). A different criterion of quality concerns stability of the estimation procedure. A method is unstable if small perturbations in the data can cause significant changes in the estimations (Breiman, 1996).

Another important question is how many data points are needed per parameter to enable pertinent statistical analysis. For multiple linear regression, a nominal number of 15 observations per parameter (except the intercept parameter) is recommended (Stevens, 1986). But since the Banister model is non-linear, inference is based on asymptotic theory (Bates and Watts, 1988; Sen and Shrivastava, 1990; Davidian and Giltinan, 1995; Huet et al., 1996), which implies more data points per parameter than for a linear regression model. In fact, the studies having modelled “real” performances in elite sport did not exceed more than 20 performances per year (Mujika et al., 1996a; Millet et al., 2002).

The problem of accuracy when modelling the training-performance relationship is crucial in high-level athletes. The higher the performance level, the smaller the difference in performance. For example, during the Athens Olympic Games, the mean difference in the swimming finals was 2.16 ± 0.75% between the Olympic Champion and the poorest (8th) performance, while for the national championship, this difference was 6.55 ± 2.32%.

Therefore, the aims of the present study were 1) to assess the goodness of fit, accuracy, ill-conditioning and stability of the Banister model for real data; 2) To review and suggest alternative methods to model the training-performance relationships.

Methods

Subjects

Nine (five females, four males) elite swimmers participated in the present study. All subjects were of international level (e.g. qualified as junior or elite in the national team for the European, World Championships or Olympic Games). Written informed consent was obtained from the subjects. Their training characteristics and performances were analyzed over a whole season composed of a training period (52 weeks) and a rest period (8 weeks) (see Table 1). Individualised training programmes were prescribed by coaches for each swimmer, depending on age, training background, individual profiles and specialty. The proportion of the training performed at each intensity level was compared between the group of sprint swimmers (SS) specialised in 50 and 100m events, the group of intermediary distance swimmers (IS) specialised in 100 and 200m events, and the group of middle distance swimmers (MDS) specialised in 200 and 400m events. The season was composed of four training cycles. A linear model of periodization characterized the training cycles (Fry et al., 1992; ACSM, 2002): each training cycle, lasting between 8 and 14 weeks, commenced with high training volume and low intensity. As training progressed, volume decreased and intensity increased. The last three weeks prior the competitive period was defined as the taper phase.

Table 1.

Selected characteristics of the subjects training over the one-year period studied.

S G Age (years) Height (cm) Weight (kg) Training (km) Event Best Perf Number of Perf’s CV of Perf (%)
1 F 24 168 61 1402 100 Free 00:55:65 11 2.3
2 F 21 173 62 1856 200 Butt 02:10:8 12 1.6
3 F 26 179 59 1677 200 Free 01:59:86 21 2.2
4 M 27 185 84 1751 200 Medl 02:01:83 12 1.9
5 M 23 181 81 1340 100 Breast 01:03:51 14 1.8
6 F 26 168 50 1477 200 Back 02:15:00 18 2.4
7 M 20 186 80 1815 100 Free 00:51:5 12 2.6
8 F 19 167 52 1916 200 Free 02:03:51 11 1.6
9 M 23 188 84 1843 400 Free 03:53:42 13 1.5

Mean 23.2 177.2 68.1 1675 13.2 2.0
s.d. 2.8 8.4 14.0 215 2.4 0.4

S = Subjects, G = Gender, Perf = Performance, CV = coefficient of variation, Free = Freestyle, Butt = Butterfly, Medl = Medley, Breast = Breaststroke, Back =Backstroke. Training (km) = Total km swum during the period concerned.

Training and performance

An incremental stepwise test to exhaustion (6 × 200-m) was repeated four times in the season to determine the relationship between blood lactate concentration and swimming velocity. Then, intensity levels for swim workouts [below (I1 ≈ 2 mmol·l−1), equal to (I2 ≈ 4 mmol·l −1), slightly above (I3 ≈ 6 mmol·l−1) the onset of blood lactate accumulation, respectively; I4 ≈ 10 mmol·l−1; I5 = maximal intensity] were determined as proposed by Mujika et al. 1996a. I6 and I7 consisted in 40–70% and 70%–100% of 1 maximal repetition strength training, respectively.

For the whole group, the content of the volume, intensity and taper phases of the last training cycle before the main competitions of the year were compared (Table 2). Quantification of the training load was performed as indicated by Avalos et al. 2003; briefly, it was expressed as percentage of the maximal volume measured at each intensity level throughout the whole period studied for each subject.

Table 2.

Annual swimming volume percentage and annual dry land training percentage (including rest periods) for each intensity level and group.

SS
IS
MDS
Intensity Level IS MDS SS MDS SS IS
I1 * - * - - -
(%·yr−1) 69.3 (5.4) 57.3 (4.9) 48.1 (4.1)
I2 * - * - - -
(%·yr−1) 21.6 (2.2) 34.6 (4.5) 44.1 (5.4)
I3 - - - - - -
(%·yr−1) 4.6 (2.2) 4.7 (0.4) 3.8 (1.1)
I4 - - - # - #
(%·yr−1) 2.3 (0.2) 2.5 (0.2) 3.3 (0.3)
I5 * - * - - -
(%·yr−1) 2.3 (0.2) 0.9 (0.1) 0.7 (0.2)
I6 - - - - - -
(%·yr−1) 41.7 (20.2) 37.8 (26.7) 29.6 (0.7)
I7 - - - - - -
(%·yr−1) 58.5 (20.2) 62.2 (27.7) 70.4 (0.7)

Values are mean (s.d.). SS = sprint swimmers, IS = intermediate swimmers, MDS = middle distance swimmers.

*

Significant difference between SS and IS, for each intensity level (P ≤ 0.05).

#

Significant difference between IS and MDS, for each intensity level (P ≤ 0.05).

-Not significantly different. Mann Whitney U test for nonparametric distribution was used.

For each swimmer, performances were measured during real competitions for the same event, during the entire study period. Since performance, in the case of swimming, is represented by a time, it appears simpler to operate in terms of the percentage of the best performance mint(Pt) achieved in the course of the studied period. The performance can thus be computed in the following manner:

pt=mint(Pt)Pt100.

Performances achieved during the competition were compared with those achieved three weeks earlier during a preparatory competition.

Fitting the model

The model proposed by Banister et al. (1975) was used. Model parameters were estimated for each subject using the non-linear least squares iterative method, by minimizing the residual sum of quadratic differences between the real and the modelled performances (RSS) with a Gauss-Newton type algorithm (Bates and Watts, 1988; Sen and Shrivastava, 1990; Davidian and Giltinan, 1995; Huet et al., 1996). The values of parameters were generally reported as τa = 45 days, τf = 15 days, ka = 1 arbitrary units (a.u.) and kf = 2 a.u. (Morton et al., 1990). However, in swimming, with similar swimmers and daily training loads, these values were τa = 41.4 days, τf = 12.4 days, ka = 0.128 a.u. and kf = 0.055 a.u. (Mujika et al., 1996a). The latter were therefore retained as the initial values in the iterative procedure for each subject of the present study. Computations were completed using Matlab 2000 (version 6.0, Optimization Toolbox, Mathworks Eds). After testing the normality and homoscedasticity of the residuals, 95% CI were calculated for the estimated parameters. The determination coefficient was calculated as: R2 = 1 − (RSS / TSS), where TSS is the total sum of squares. In addition, the statistical significance of the fit was tested by analysis of variance (F-ratio test). The average standard error values (ASE) were computed as the mean difference between modelled and real performances.

Calculation of tn and tg

The time to recover performance, tn, and the time to peak performance after training completion, tg, were calculated as tn=τ1τ2τ1τ2lnk2k1 and tg=τ1τ2τ1τ2ln(k2k1τ1τ2) (subscripts “1” and “2” correspond to “a” and “f”) (Fitz-Clarke et al., 1991).

Estimating the change in performance during the rest period

The time it took each swimmer to reach peak performance was evaluated during the rest period (during the final eight weeks of the season).

Accuracy

Bootstrap method was used to calculate the 95% CI of parameter estimates and modelled performances. Bootstrap is a powerful method for estimating tests statistics like CI, especially in small samples (Efron and Tibshirani, 1993; Huet et al., 1996). Briefly, the procedure consisted of resampling the original data set with replacement to create 1000 “bootstrap replicate” data sets of the same size as the original data set. A random number generator was used to determine which data from the original data set to include in a replicate data set. Therefore a given data could be used more than once in the replicate data set, or not at all. This was repeated 1000 times. For each parameter, the estimates that fell between the 2.5th and the 97.5th percentiles of the 1000 estimates were used to construct a 95% CI for parameter estimates (τa, τf, ka, kf, tn, tg) and modelled performances. In addition the coefficient of variation (CV) was calculated as the ratio of the standard deviation over the mean of the 1000 replicates.

Ill-conditioning

Ill-conditioning is the nonlinear generalization of the collinearity problem in linear regression. The asymptotic correlation matrix of parameter estimates was checked to see whether any parameters were excessively highly correlated, since high correlations may reveal ill-conditioning problems, indicating that the model is over parameterised for the data set (Bates and Watts, 1988). The correlation matrix was obtained as follows. Let xi be the explanatory variable and yi the response variable, i = 1,…, n, where n is the number of observations. Suppose the relationship yi = f (xi, θ) + εi, where the function f is the deterministic part of the model, depending on the explanatory variable and some parameters θ = (θ1,…,θp), and εi is the random part of the model: εi ~ N(0, σ2). Let θ̂ be the non-linear least squares estimation, then, asymptotically, θ̂ ~ N(θ, σ̂2 V), where V = (Df̂′ Df̂)−1, Df̂ is the derivative matrix of f̂ with respect to θ, f̂ is the estimation of f, and σ̂2 is the standard unbiased estimator of the error variance. Elements of the correlation matrix, ρjk, j,k = 1,…,p, are then obtained from the covariance matrix as ρjk = (V)jk/[(V)jj(V)kk]1/2 (Bates and Watts, 1988; Sen and Shrivastava, 1990; Davidian and Giltinan, 1995; Huet et al., 1996).

Stability

The stability of the Banister model fitted by minimizing the RSS value was studied as follows. For each subject, iterative computation was performed with the same data, but minus one performance, that was chosen randomly. The initial parameters were the same as those presented above.

Statistical analysis

All values were reported as mean ± s.d. For all variables the hypothesis of a normal distribution was tested with the Shapiro Wilk W-test for small samples (performances) and the Kolmogorov test for the large samples (training loads) (Wetherill et al., 1986; Sen and Shrivastava, 1990). The variations in performance and in the content between each training phase were evaluated with a multiple paired t-test with Bonferonni’s correction. The training content was compared between each training group using a nonparametric Mann-Whitney U test. Bartlett test was used to verify homoscedasticity of performances. All statistical analyses were completed using Statistica 5.1. (Statsoft, Tulsa, OK, USA). Statistical significance was accepted at P < 0.05.

Results

Training characteristics and performances

For the whole group of swimmers, training volume measured during a season was 1675 ± 215 km (mean ± s.d.). During the entire study period, 13 ± 2 performances were recorded for each swimmer (see Table 1). Coefficient of variation (CV) of the actual performances was 2.0 ± 0.4%. The SS group performed more I1 training and less I2 training than the IS group (P <0.05). The MDS group performed more I4 training than the IS group (P <0.05) (see Table 2). Training volume equal to I2, decreased between the volume and the intensity phase (P <0.05), whereas training volume equal to I4 and strength training (I7) increased. Total meters swum, training volumes equals to I1 and I2 and strength training (I6, I7) decreased between the intensity and the taper phase (P <0.05), whereas training volumes equals to I3, I4 and the maximal intensity (I5) remained constant (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Respective contents of the volume, intensity and taper phases during the fourth training cycle (Before the main competition of the year).

Volume phase Intensity phase Taper phase
Total (m) 55200 ± 8772 49500 ± 7340 ** 28900 ± 3780 ++
I1 (m) 19108 ± 3375 24637 ± 2920 18841 ± 2941 +
I2 (m) 33800 ± 6229 £ 21550 ± 5340 ** 7000 ± 973 ++
I3 (m) 1333 ± 472 1125 ± 382 1366 ± 502
I4 (m) 533 ± 227 £ 1650 ± 443 * 1291 ± 483
I5 (m) 425 ± 197 537 ± 176 478 ± 181
I6 (mn) 12 ± 4 8 ± 3 ** 3 ± 1 ++
I7 (mn) 15 ± 5 £ 28 ± 7 * 12 ± 4 ++

Intensity levels for swim workouts: below (I1 ≈2 mmol·l−1), equal to (I2 ≈4 mmol·l−1), slightly above (I3 ≈6 mmol·l−1) the onset of blood lactate accumulation, respectively; I4 ≈10 mmol·l−1; I5 = maximal intensity; I6 and I7 consisted in 40–70% and 70%–100% of 1 maximal repetition strength training. Results are expressed in meters and minutes.

£

Significant differences between volume and intensity phases (P <0.05). +, ++ Significant differences between volume and taper phases (P <0.05 and P <0.01, respectively).

*, **

Significant differences between intensity and taper phases (P <0.05 and P <0.01, respectively).

Performances improved 2.24 ± 1.24 % (P <0.05) during the taper phase. The pattern of training loads and performances during a season exhibited an undulating trend (Fig. 1). The predicted peak performance was in weeks 61 and 62, that is to say three or four weeks after the end of the taper period and after complete cessation of training.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Example for subject 3 (Olympic finalist): (a) Modelled (line) and actual performances (dotted line with triangles). 95% CI for modelled performances are also presented. Performances on vertical axis were expressed in percentage of the personal record mint(Pt) and computed as pt=mint(Pt)Pt100. (b) Training loads on vertical axis are expressed as a percentage of the maximal training load performed by the subject during the course of the study. Time in horizontal axis is expressed in weeks.

Performances estimation during the rest period

For the whole group, the time to peak performance was 17 ± 9 days (range 7–35 days) after the end of the training period.

Goodness of fit and accuracy

The relationship between training and performance (using the original estimation method) was significant in all subjects: R2 = 0.79 ± 0.13, P < 0.05. ASE for all subjects was 1.05 ± 0.63%. 95% CI of the parameters and mean 95% CI width for the modelled performances are presented in Table 4. 95% CI of the parameters tn, and tg are presented in table 5. The mean CV were 32 ± 14; 42 ± 16; 64 ± 22; 98 ± 32; 44 ± 22; 41 ± 22% for τa, τf, ka, kf, tn, tg respectively.

Table 4.

Banister model parameters measured over the one-year period studied.

S P0 CI p0 ka CI ka kf CI kf τa CI τa τf CI τf R2 MIW
1 0.92 0.89, 0.95 0.002 −0.040, 0.044 0.016 −0.018, 0.051 40 8, 71 5 −6, 16 0.69* 3.17
2 0.95 0.92, 0.98 0.106 0.022, 0.189 0.129 0.071, 0.185 13 −6, 33 11 7, 15 0.84 1.99
3 0.97 0.93, 1.01 0.039 0.006, 0.071 0.048 0.016, 0.081 33 8, 57 27 8, 46 0.65* 2.69
4 0.97 0.95, 0.98 0.050 −0.029, 0.068 0.068 0.048, 0.088 27 14, 40 20 15, 25 0.97§ 0.80
5 0.98 0.95, 1.01 0.003 −0.040, 0.046 0.022 −0.012, 0.057 41 11, 70 9 −1, 19 0.78 1.80
6 0.90 0.84, 0.94 0.009 −0.014, 0.034 0.016 −0.009, 0.041 45 18, 71 18 4, 32 0.61§ 2.04
7 0.90 0.78, 1.02 0.018 −0.012, 0.046 0.028 −0.003, 0.057 57 39, 75 31 17, 46 0.95§ 1.55
8 0.95 0.92, 0.97 0.083 0.019, 0.148 0.112 0.048, 0.176 23 11, 34 16 9, 24 0.92§ 1.58
9 0.93 0.90, 0.96 0.010 −0.024, 0.046 0.012 −0.016, 0.042 65 50, 81 38 3, 64 0.73* 2.44

Mean 0.94 0.90, 0.98 0.036 −0.012, 0.077 0.050 0.014, 0.086 38 17, 59 19 6, 32 0.79 1.89
s.d. 0.03 0.038 0.044 16 11 0.13 0.49

S = subjects; p0 = the initial performance level (in % of the best performance); ka and kf = the fitness and fatigue magnitude factors, respectively (in arbitrary units); τa and τf = the fitness and fatigue decay time constants, respectively (in days). CI = Bootstrap 95% confidence interval of estimated parameters.

*

Significant, P < 0.05;

Significant, P < 0.01;

§

Significant, P < 0.001. MIW = Mean Interval Width of 95% confidence interval of modelled performances.

Table 5.

Model parameters tn and tg computed from ka and kf (the fitness and fatigue magnitude factor) and τa and τf (the fitness and fatigue decay time constant).

S tn CI tn tg CI tg
1 7 −2.15 15 −3.34
2 10 1.19 21 9.32
3 31 25.77 61 20.96
4 25 21.28 48 43.53
5 24 6.40 41 16.65
6 12 5.19 35 20.50
7 29 8.51 71 57.85
8 17 12.20 36 28.41
9 15 −14.45 64 34.95

Mean 19 7.35 43 25.61
s.d. 9 19

S = subjects, tn = the time to recover performance, tg = the time to peak performance after training completion. CI = Bootstrap 95% confidence interval of estimated parameters.

Ill-conditioning

Correlation between parameter estimates (mean absolute values ± s.d.) are shown in Table 6. Parameters τa and τf were highly correlated (0.99 ± 0.01). A high correlation was also found between ka and kf (0.91 ± 0.13). Finally, the fatigue magnitude factor was correlated with the fitness and fatigue decay time constants (0.75 ± 0.30 and 0.76 ± 0.27, respectively).

Table 6.

Correlation between parameter estimates for the Banister model (n = 9; mean absolute value ± s.d.).

Parameters Correlation
p0 – ka 0.49 ± 0.27
p0 – kf 0.31 ± 0.25
p0 – τa 0.41 ± 0.27
p0 – τf 0.41 ± 0.27
ka – kf 0.91 ± 0.13
ka – τa 0.69 ± 0.26
ka – τf 0.69 ± 0.26
kf – τa 0.75 ± 0.30
kf – τf 0.76 ± 0.27
τa – τf 0.99 ± 0.01

p0 = the initial performance level; ka and kf = the fitness and fatigue magnitude factors, respectively; τa and τf = the fitness and fatigue decay time constants, respectively.

Stability

The values of the Banister model parameters computed for each athlete with all performances minus one are shown in Table 7.

Table 7.

Banister model parameters measured over the one-year period studied. One performance chosen randomly was removed in order to assess model stability.

Subjects p0 ka kf τa τf
1 0.93 0.002 0.077 39 2
2 0.95 0.023 0.054 17 7
3 0.98 0.032 0.040 33 26
4 0.97 0.035 0.054 28 19
5 0.98 0.003 0.021 41 10
6 0.90 0.053 0.067 47 26
7 0.90 0.020 0.030 55 32
8 0.94 0.079 0.106 23 16
9 0.92 0.011 0.012 62 36

Mean 0.94 0.029 0.051 38 19
s.d. 0.03 0.025 0.029 15 12

p0 = the initial performance level; ka and kf = the fitness and fatigue magnitude factors, respectively; τa and τf = the fitness and fatigue decay time constants, respectively.

No significant differences were found between the results of Table 3 (all performances) and Table 7 (all performances minus one).

Discussion

The main findings of the present study were:

  1. For the whole group, the time to peak performance was 17 days (in average), after the end of the training period and the main competition of the season.

  2. Banister model parameters exhibited wide variability. For illustration, the 95% CI for tn and tg were, in average, (7, 35) and (25, 61) days, respectively. In addition, the coefficients of variation for the parameter estimates calculated from 1000 bootstrap replicates were greater than 30%. Conversely, the variability in modelled performances was reasonably small.

  3. The Banister model appears to be stable since no significant differences in the parameters were found when the computation was performed with one performance less.

In the present study, the characteristics of training and performances were similar to those reported previously (Mujika et al., 1995, 1996a; Stewart and Hopkins, 2000; Avalos et al., 2003). The mean positive and negative decay time constant (τa = 38 ± 16 days; τf = 19 ± 11 days) were close to those reported in endurance athletes (Mujika et al., 1996a; Morton, 1997; Bannister et al., 1999). It is noteworthy that the parameter values and the determination coefficients are near those reported by Mujika et al. (1995, 1996a,b) in a study of similar athletes (international-level swimmers) over the same duration (one season). However, these values were also close to those reported in studies with different types of athletes. For example, the present decay constant time for the fitness function is similar to the values (~40 days) calculated with less-fit athletes (Morton et al., 1990; Busso et al., 1991). It is therefore unlikely that the range of the parameter values would be similar, irrespective of the quality of the athletes. The mean standard error values for all subjects were less than the difference in performance between the first and the last swimmer in the Athens Olympic Games finals. But they were less accurate than those reported by Avalos et al. (2003) who used a linear mixed model (1.05 ± 0.63 % vs. 0.33 ± 0.11 %).

Training and performance quantification

Other methods were applied to quantify the training load and performance. The total training load was also expressed as the summation of the volume in each training level weighed by a coefficient according to relationship between the swimming velocity and the lactate level (Mujika et al., 1996a,b). Performance was also modelled using a logarithm transformation, expressed as a percentage of the world record as well as the criterion points scale proposed by Morton et al. (1990). These different methods produced a less reliable adjustment and a significant change in the ka parameter, expressing the sensitivity of the model to the scale used.

Performance prediction during the rest period

For the whole group, the modelled performance peaks occurred approximately 17 ± 9 days (range 7–35 days), after training ceased. Indeed, these results are consistent with those of Morton in 1991 who, using the original dose-response model of Calvert et al. (1976), studied by simulation the effects of variation in ten parameters (four from the dose-response model and six describing the differing shape of several seasonal training profiles). These authors reported that peak performance occurs on average 23 ± 34 days after the end of training. As the season ended with the most important competition of the year, these results are undesirable from a practical point of view: it would have been more logical for performance to peak during the key competition, and not some 17 days later. These results cannot likely be attributed to poor tapering. The taper phase lasting three weeks was characterised by an approximate 50% decline in training volume and by a decrease in low intensity and dry land training as well as maintained high-intensity as recommended in the literature (Mujika and Padilla, 2003). In addition, performances improved 2.24 ± 1.24% during the taper phase which is equivalent to the 2.2 ± 1,5% reported by Mujika et al. (2002) during the final 3 weeks of training leading to the Sidney Olympic Games and greater than the 1.48% and 2.07% reported by Bonifazi et al. (2000) in male swimmers during two seasons. Moreover, the results of the present study are not in agreement with work carried out on short-term detraining (stopping training for 2 to 4 weeks), which generates a rapid decline in maximal oxygen uptake (VO2max) (Mujika and Padilla, 2000) and decreased swim power, i.e. the ability to apply force during swimming (Neufer et al., 1987). It is noteworthy however that these qualities have been shown to be strongly linked with the level of swimming performance (Hawley et al., 1992; Wakayoshi et al., 1995). The fact that the changes in the parameters of the present model were not taken into account (time-unvarying model) may also explain why the modelled performance peaks occurred such a long time after training had ceased. With a time-varying model, Busso et al. (2002) reported a decrease in tn and tg when training frequency was reduced. The time needed to recover performance levels after a training session increased from 0.9 ± 2.1 days at the end of low-frequency training to 3.6 ± 2.0 days at the end of high-frequency training. The same author used a non-linear model introducing a variable to account for training-related changes in the magnitude and duration of exercise-induced fatigue, reported a decrease in time to peak performance when the training load was reduced from 37 days for a daily training load of 500 a.u. to 7 days for 300 a.u. (Busso, 2003). However, in the latter two models, estimates must be provided for 6 parameters implying the need for at least 15 performances per parameter (Stevens, 1986), i.e. 90 performances – totally unworkable under real sporting conditions (Mujika et al., 1996a,b; Millet et al., 2002; Avalos et al., 2003).

Another problem concerns the method used to calculate the global training load, based on a summation of the different types of training. Training in elite swimmers comprises a wide range of diversified exercises (aerobic and anaerobic training, speed exercises, strength training). These different types of training have to be individualised depending on age, training background, individual profiles and specialty (Mujika et al., 1996a; Stewart and Hopkins, 2000; Avalos et al., 2003). For example, in our study sprint swimmers performed a larger proportion of speed training compared with middle distance swimmers who had a higher proportion of ≈4 mmol·l−1 training. The nature of the immediate and long-term training effects of these different exercises on the organism are so diverse that grouping them together or considering them as making up one single training stimulus would be unrealistic (Banister et al., 1975). A similar overall training load may correspond to two very different types of training. (There is a compensation in training volume in each intensity level). Accordingly, Taha and Thomas (2003) argued that the Banister model implicitly assumed that the performance activity matches the training activity and therefore does not consider the specificity of training. Furthermore, classification in 5 intensity levels may be insufficient when compared to the large number of different training variables used by the coaches in their programmes such as arm and leg training, training in the four styles, and technical training (Mujika et al., 1996b; Stewart and Hopkins, 2000; Avalos et al., 2003). In addition, training for start and turn, which account for about 30% of the event time (Thomson and Haljand, 2000) was not taken into consideration for calculating load for the Banister model. In fact several authors pointed out the multi-faceted nature of performance excellence (Banister et al., 1975; Morton et al., 1990; Mujika et al., 1996a ; Avalos et al., 2003). Psychological, nutritional (Banister et al., 1975; Morton et al., 1990) and technical factors (Toussaint and Hollander, 1994; Wakayoshi et al., 1995) also affect performance. These two authors demonstrated that swimming efficiency, a factor that cannot be quantified in overall load, is a major factor in swimming performance. Nevertheless, the impacts of the various types of training loads on performance have an upper limit above which training does not elicit further adaptation of the subjects (Morton et al., 1997). Hence, the long-term and cumulative effects of training, that may be evidenced some macro-cycles after, may not have been taken into account (Counsilman and Counsilman, 1991; Werchoschanski, 1992; Avalos et al., 2003).

Variability in the parameters of the Banister model

The present study showed that the 95% CI in all parameters was large, in particular, for tn and tg, 19 (7, 35) and 43 (25, 61) days, respectively. The 95% CI for the modelled performances was reasonably small (1.89 ± 0.42%), representing from a practical point of view ~1 s for a 100-m event performed in 55 s. CI provides an interval of reasonable estimates, where the width of the interval is determined by the uncertainty in the point estimate. The greater the uncertainty, the wider the CI (Efron and Tibshirani, 1993; Huet et al., 1996). In addition, the coefficients of variation for the parameter estimates were high (greater than 30%) (Arsac et al., 2004). Parameter variability was much larger compared to the variability in time-dependent parameters reported by Busso et al. (1997) and Busso (2003), suggesting, unlike the latter study, that some methodological issues could be involved.

Study of the correlation matrix of parameter estimates revealed ill-conditioning problems, which are known to affect the accuracy of parameter estimates. Ill-conditioning and its effects are well known in linear regression (collinearity) (Wetherill et al., 1986; Sen and Shrivastava, 1990). In non-linear regression, the problem is more complex, and different types of ill-conditioning can be identified (Bates and Watts, 1988; Belsley, 1991). Thus, in the linear case, the inaccuracies of parameter estimates and estimated responses are generally comparable, whereas in the non-linear case, inaccuracy of parameter estimates (estimator conditioning) and inaccuracy of response estimation (data conditioning) can differ (Belsley, 1991). In the present study the 95% CI for parameters was very large, whereas the 95% CI for the modelled performances was reasonably small.

The origins of the present ill-conditioning can be explained by:

  1. A poor sample size. The most direct and obvious means for improving conditioning is through the collection and use of additional data. Unfortunately, the remedy of obtaining new data is rarely possible in studies dealing with elite athletes, having only a few precious observations. Furthermore, even if new data were obtainable, there is often no guarantee that they will be consistent with the original data or that they will indeed provide independent information (Belsley, 1991).

  2. Interaction between parameters. If parameters are inter-dependent, their interpretation is conditioned and their practical use becomes useless. Several sets of parameters can be the (best) solution for a given loads and performances data set.

  3. Misspecification of the model (Bates and Watts, 1988; Huet et al., 1996). Some hypotheses are supposed in the regression problem: normality, homoscedasticity and independence of errors (regarding the random structure), and the Banister model function (regarding the deterministic structure). If one of these hypotheses is false, the model will be misspecified. As a consequence of ill-conditioning, the estimation of the parameters tn and tg was very inaccurate. This is supported by the findings of Fitz-Clark et al. 1991 who reported that small (10–15%) changes in τf, ka and kf induced large variations in tn and tg,(41% and 21%, respectively). Thus for elite swimmers, the use of these parameters to provide valuable information for the understanding of individual responses to training and to develop individual training schedules from observational data appears to be hazardous.

Possible solutions to variability

Penalization techniques, based on decreasing variability to improve accuracy, are extensively used to resolve ill-conditioning problems in linear regression (Wetherill et al., 1988; Sen and Shrivastava, 1990). Subset selection and ridge regression are the two main penalization procedures. The former consists in selecting a subset of the most relevant variables (Sen and Shrivastava, 1990; Breiman, 1996). Its adaptation to the present problem consists in selecting a subset of parameters. The studies of Busso et al. 1991 and Busso (2003) can be understood from a parameter subset selection viewpoint. Thus, the goodness of f it of models with different numbers of components (i.e. different numbers of parameters) was examined.

Ridge regression imposes a constraint on parameters (Wetherill et al., 1988; Sen and Shrivastava, 1990; Breiman, 1996). To date, ridge regression has not been applied to sport data and only few studies have dealt with the adaptation of ridge regression to non-linear regression (see, for example, Minor et al., 1996; O’Sullivan and Saha, 1999; Zhou et al. 2002).

Short of new data, the introduction of appropriate prior information is another available solution to the ill-conditioning problem. Procedures to introduce prior information are, for example, mixed-estimation techniques (Belsley, 1991; Davidian and Giltinan, 1995). An application of mixed models to sport data was proposed by Avalos et al. (2003).

The re-parameterization of the Banister model may improve conditioning (Bates and Watts, 1988; Huet et al., 1996). Since a high correlation between the fitness and fatigue decay time constants and between the fitness and fatigue magnitude factors was found, dissymmetrizing fitness and fatigue functions may overcome the parameters correlation. For example, one of the decay time constants and one of the magnitude factors could be inversed: υa = 1/τa, la = 1/ka. These operations do not affect the interpretation of parameters (as initial parameters can be easily computed from the new ones), however, they may be numerically advantageous.

Misspecification problems are probably involved in the present study: (i) the Banister model does not take into account the possible dependence between performances, a typical problem in longitudinal data studies (Avalos et al., 2003); (ii) In the Banister model, training impulses are proportional to the training loads then, greater loads induce more fitness and fatigue acquisition. But previous studies reported that the impact of training loads on performance may have an upper limit above which training does not elicit further adaptation of the organism (Fry et al., 1992; Morton, 1997); (iii) The procedure assumes the parameters remain constant over time, an assumption that is not consistent with observed time-dependent alterations in responses to training (Busso et al., 1997; Avalos et al., 2003; Busso, 2003).

Stability

The Banister model estimated by the non-linear least squares method seemed to be stable since no significant differences were found when the computation was performed with one performance less. The change in the temporal parameters were ~1–3 days for most of the subjects. However, in one subject (n° 6) the difference in τf was 8 days. Moreover, the values of tn and tg are greatly modified by little change in τa or τf. For example, in subject 1, the values of tn and tg were decreased from 12 to 7 days and 24 to 15 days, respectively, when τf decreased by three days and in subject 9 the values of tn and tg were increased from 7 to 17 days and 54 to 66 days, when τa decreased by 3 days and τf by 2 days. These changes may have important practical implications in planning training loads.

Therefore proposing a single set of parameters might be dubious. Breiman (1996) showed that the averaged estimator of the bootstrap estimations is more stable than the original one. Stabilizing non-linear methods can give non-linear estimators with improved accuracy. This technique was used in the present study but a large variability was also observed in all parameters of the model and accuracy was not improved.

Perspectives for future study and use of the Banister model

Further studies should be conducted to determine whether the parameters estimation of the Banister model would be more accurate under standardised experimental conditions. Such standardised conditions would allow multiple recordings of performance and use of a single type of exercise, e.g. as in the study reported by Busso (2003) where exercises were limited to ergometer cycling. The accuracy and the stability of the performance estimations observed in the present study suggest that the pattern of performance changes could be assessed as a function of training load. Nevertheless, the mean error of performance estimates was greater than reported by Avalos et al. (2003) suggesting that the results need to be validated experimentally by another complementary method

Other procedures

Another possibility is to use nonparametric regression. Edelmann-Nusser et al. (2002) suggested applying a non-parametric model (multilayer perceptron neural networks) to model training load-performance relationships. These authors used an unconventional method giving a surprisingly small prediction error (0.04%). Indeed, the model was fitted with data pertaining to one particular swimmer and then used to predict the performance of a different swimmer, despite the consensus that reigns about the singularity of training responses (Mujika et al., 1996a,b; Busso et al., 1997; Millet et al., 2002; Avalos et al., 2003; Busso, 2003).

Neural networks may be particularly useful when the primary goal is outcome prediction, but these techniques are a “black box” and have limited ability to explicitly identify possible causal relationships. Thus, the interpretation of the results obtained is not straightforward. However, in any given parametric problem, the parameters have meaningful interpretations (Hastie et al., 2001).

Recently, Perl (2002) developed a dynamic meta model based on two antagonistic systems (two internal buffer potentials: one positive and one negative, which influence the performance potential alternately). This meta model seems conceptually very rich because it takes into account the collapse effect in the wake of an overloaded training period, atrophy following a period of detraining, not to mention the long-term behaviour of the training-performance relationship (Perl et al., 2001). However, to date, no statistical study has validated the quality of this meta model.

Finally, few authors have used multiple regressions to create a model for the relationship between training and performance. Mujika et al. (1996b), using stepwise regression, reported a very close match with the Banister model. Multiple regressions make it possible to integrate different training loads as independent variables and can take into account the effects of load sequences over time with short-term deferred effects during the three weeks that precede the performance (weeks 0, -1, -2), intermediate effects (weeks -3, -4 and –5) and long-term effects (weeks -6, -7, -8), as indicated by Avalos et al. 2003. The results we obtained with this method, using data from the present study, showed improved statistical accuracy in estimated parameters and modelled performances (more accurate CI). Moreover, in multiple regression each training variable could be transformed for instance by a quadratic function (or higher order functions) to take into account a potential parabolic relationship between the quantity of training loads and performance (Sen and Shrivastava, 1990). It is also possible to take into account any effects of interaction by associating the different input variables (Sen and Shrivastava, 1990).

Conclusion

The aim of this study was to assess whether the Banister model could be used to monitor the training process in the “real world”, i.e. with real international-level performances. The present study assessed the goodness of fit, accuracy and stability of the Banister model as applied to training loads and performances in elite swimmers. The model showed substantial variability in its parameters, making it imprecise. To conclude, it seems inappropriate to use these parameters to monitor the training process in elite swimmers. Nevertheless, the variability in modelled performances was reasonably small and the Banister model appeared to be stable. Further research should be conducted to determine whether associating these Banister model qualities with other methods of modelling could provide pertinent information for monitoring training.

References

  1. American College of Sports Medicine. Progression models in resistance training for healthy adults. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2002;34:364–380. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200202000-00027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arsac LM, Thiaudière E, Diolez P, Gerville-Réache L. Parameter estimation in modeling phosphocreatine recovery in human skeletal muscle. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 2004;91:419–424. doi: 10.1007/s00421-003-1001-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Atkinson G, Nevill AM. Statistical methods for assessing measurement error (reliability) in variables relevant to sports medicine. Sports Medicine. 1998;26:217–238. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199826040-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Avalos M, Hellard P, Chatard JC. Modeling the training-performance relationship using a mixed model in elite swimmers. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2003;35:838–846. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000065004.05033.42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Banister EW, Calvert TW, Savage MV, Bach A. A system model of training for athletic performance. Australian Journal of Sports Medicine. 1975;7:170–176. [Google Scholar]
  6. Banister EW, Carter JB, Zarkadas PC. Training theory and taper: validation in triathlon athletes. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 1999;79:182–191. doi: 10.1007/s004210050493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bartley R. The use and abuse of statistics in sport and exercise sciences. Journal of Sports Sciences. 1997;15:1–2. doi: 10.1080/026404197367524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bates DM, Watts DG. Nonlinear regression analysis and its applications. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 1988. pp. 52–53.pp. 67–70.pp. 80–81.pp. 90–92. [Google Scholar]
  9. Belsley DA. Collinearity and weak data in regression. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 1991. 1991. Conditioning diagnostics; pp. 342–378. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bonifazi M, Saldella F, Luppo C. Preparatory versus main competitions: differences in performances, lactate responses and pre-competition plasma cortisol concentrations in elite male swimmers. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 2000;82:368–373. doi: 10.1007/s004210000230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Breiman L. Heuristics of instability and stabilization in model selection. Annals of Statistics. 1996;24:2350–2383. [Google Scholar]
  12. Busso T. Variable dose-response relationship between exercise training and performance. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2003;35:1188–1195. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000074465.13621.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Busso T, Hakkinen K, Pakarinen A, Carasso C, Lacour JR, Komi PV, Kauhanen H. A systems model of training responses and its relationship to hormonal responses in elite weight-lifters. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 1990;61:48–54. doi: 10.1007/BF00236693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Busso T, Carasso C, Lacour JR. Adequacy of a systems structure in the modeling of training effects on performance. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1991;71:2044–2049. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1991.71.5.2044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Busso T, Candau R, Lacour JR. Fatigue and fitness modelled from the effects of training on performance. European Journal of Applied Physiology. 1994;69:50–54. doi: 10.1007/BF00867927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Busso T, Denis C, Bonnefoy R, Geyssant A, Lacour JR. Modeling of adaptations to physical-training by using a recursive least-squares algorithm. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1997;82:1685–1693. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1997.82.5.1685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Busso T, Benoit H, Bonnefoy R, Feasson L, Lacour JR. Effects of training frequency on the dynamics of performance response to a single training bout. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2002;92:572–580. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00429.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Calvert TW, Banister EW, Savage MV. A systems model of the effects of training on physical performance. SMC Systems, Man and Cybernetics. 1976;2:94–102. [Google Scholar]
  19. Counsilman BE, Counsilman JE. The residual effects of training. Journal of Swimming Research. 1991;7:5–12. [Google Scholar]
  20. Davidian M, Giltinan DM. Nonlinear models for repeated measurement data. London: Chapman and Hall; 1995. pp. 26–43.pp. 63–71. [Google Scholar]
  21. Edelmann-Nusser J, Hohmann A, Henneberg B. Modeling and prediction of competitive performance in swimming upon neural networks. European Journal of Sport Science. 2002;2:1–12. [Google Scholar]
  22. Efron B, Tibshirani R. Monographs on statistics and applied probability. New York: Chapman and Hall; 1993. An introduction to the Bootstrap; pp. 237–238.pp. 242–243. [Google Scholar]
  23. Fitz-Clarke JR, Morton RH, Banister EW. Optimizing athletic performance by influence curves. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1991;71:1151–1158. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1991.71.3.1151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fry RW, Morton AR, Keast D. Periodisation of training stress-A review. Canadian Journal of Sport Sciences. 1992;17:234–240. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hastie T, Tibshirani R, Friedman J. Springer Series in Statistics. New York: Springer; 2001. The elements of statistical learning. Data mining, inference and prediction. [Google Scholar]
  26. Hawley
  27. Huet S, Bouvier A, Gruet MA, Jolivet E. Statistical tools for nonlinear regression: a practical guide with S-PLUS examples. New York, PA: Springer-Verlag; 1996. pp. 12–13.pp. 30–38.pp. 89–90.pp. 111–115. [Google Scholar]
  28. Millet GP, Candau RB, Barbier B, Busso T, Rouillon JD, Chatard JC. Modelling the transfers of training effects on performance in elite triathletes. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 2002;23:55–63. doi: 10.1055/s-2002-19276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Minor JM, Namini H, Watson GA. Generalized ridge analysis with application to population pharmacokinetics/dynamics. Journal of Biopharmaceutical Statistics. 1996;6:105–114. doi: 10.1080/10543409608835125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Morton RH. Modelling training and overtraining. Journal of Sports Sciences. 1997;15:335–340. doi: 10.1080/026404197367344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Morton RH. The quantitative periodization of athletic training: a model study. Sports Medicine Training and Rehabilitation. 1991;3:19–28. [Google Scholar]
  32. Morton RH, Fitz-Clarke JR, Banister EW. Modelling human performance in running. Journal of Applied Physiology. 1990;90:1171–1177. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1990.69.3.1171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mujika I, Padilla S. Scientific bases for precompetition tapering strategies. Medicine and Sciences in Sports and Exercise. 2003;35:1182–1187. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000074448.73931.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mujika I, Padilla S, Pyne D. Swimming performance changes during the final 3 weeks of training leading to the Sidney 2000 Olympic games. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 2002;23:582–587. doi: 10.1055/s-2002-35526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mujika I, Padilla S. Detraining: loss of training-induced physiological and performance adaptations. Part I: short term insufficient training stimulus. Sports Medicine. 2000;30:79–87. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200030020-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mujika I, Busso T, Lacoste L, Barale F, Geyssant A, Chatard JC. Modelled responses to training and taper in competitive swimmers. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 1996a;28:251–258. doi: 10.1097/00005768-199602000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mujika I, Chatard JC, Busso T, Geyssant A. Use of swim-training profiles and performance data to enhance training effectiveness. Journal of Swimming Research. 1996b;11:23–29. [Google Scholar]
  38. Mujika I, Chatard JC, Busso T, Geyssant A, Barale F, Lacoste L. Effects of training on performance in competitive swimming. Canadian Journal of Applied Physiology. 1995;20:395–406. doi: 10.1139/h95-031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Neufer PD, Costill DL, Fielding RA, Flynn MG, Kirwan JP. Effect of reduced training on muscular strength and endurance in competitive swimmers. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 1987;19:486–490. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. O’Sullivan F, Saha A. Use of ridge regression for improved estimation of kinetic constants from PET data. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging. 1999;18:115–125. doi: 10.1109/42.759111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Perl J, Dausher P, Hawlitzky M. On the long term behaviour of the performance potential-metamodel Perpot. International Journal of Computer Science in Sport. 2001;1:56–57. [Google Scholar]
  42. Perl J. Adaptation, antagonism and system dynamics. In: Ghent G, Kluka D, Jones D, editors. The Multidisciplinary Series of Physical Education and Sport Science. Vol. 4. Meyer & Meyer Sport; 2002. pp. 105–125. [Google Scholar]
  43. Sen A, Shrivastava M. Regression analysis: theory, methods, and applications. New York, Berlin, Paris: Springer Verlag; 1990. pp. 37–40.pp. 60–62.pp. 100–111.pp. 218–220.pp. 233–238.pp. 256–262.pp. 298–310. [Google Scholar]
  44. Stevens J. Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1986. p. 72. [Google Scholar]
  45. Stewart AM, Hopkins WG. Seasonal training and performance of competitive swimmers. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2000;18:873–884. doi: 10.1080/026404100750017805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Taha T, Thomas SG. Systems modeling of the relationship between training and performance. Sports Medicine. 2003;33:1061–1073. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200333140-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Thomson KG, Haljand R, Mc Laren DP. An analysis of selected kinematic variables in national and elite male and female 100-m and 200-m breastroke swimmers. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2000;18:421–431. doi: 10.1080/02640410050074359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Toussaint HM, Hollander AP. Energetics of competitive swimming - Implications for training-programs. Sports Medicine. 1994;18:384–405. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199418060-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wakayoshi K, D’Acquisto LJ, Cappaert JM, Troup JP. Relationship between oxygen uptake, stroke rate and swimming velocity in competitive swimming. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 1995;16:19–23. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-972957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Werchoschanski JW. Efficient training. (L’entraînement efficace) Paris, PA: Presses Universitaires de France; 1992. pp. 38pp. 43–44.pp. 124–125.pp. 166–167. [Google Scholar]
  51. Wetherill GB, Duncombe P, Kenward M. Regression analysis with applications. London: New York; 1986. pp. 82–86.pp. 102–104.pp. 180–182.pp. 226–227.pp. 240–246. [Google Scholar]
  52. Wolfarth B, Rivera MA, Oppert JM, Boulay MR, Dionne FT, Chagnon M, Gagnon J, Chagnon Y, Perusse L, Keul J, Bouchard C. A polymorphism in the alpha 2a-adrenoceptor gene and endurance athlete status. Medicine and Sciences in Sports and Exercise. 2000;32:1709–1712. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200010000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Zhou Y, Huang SC, Bergsneider M, Wong DF. Improved parametric image generation using spatial-temporal analysis of dynamic PET studies. Neuroimage. 2002;15:67–707. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES