Abstract
This meta-analysis examines whether exposure to HIV-prevention interventions follows self-validation or risk-reduction motives. The dependent measures used in the study were enrolling in an HIV-prevention program and completing the program. Results indicated that first samples with low prior condom use were less likely to enroll than samples with high prior condom use. Second, samples with high knowledge were less likely to stay in an intervention than were those with low knowledge. Third, samples with medium levels of motivation to use condoms and condom use were more likely to complete an intervention than were those with low or high levels. Importantly, those patterns were sensitive to the interventions' inclusions of information-, motivation-, and behavioral-skills strategies. The influence of characteristics of participants, the intervention, and the recruit procedure are reported.
Keywords: HIV prevention, behavioral interventions, retention, recruitment, attitudes
The need to develop behavioral interventions to reduce infection with HIV and other sexually transmitted infections (Centers for Disease Control [CDC], 2005) has resulted in many evidence-based interventions that attempt to increase HIV-relevant knowledge, motivation, and behavioral skills (J. D. Fisher & Fisher, 1992). Although these programs have been shown to be efficacious in meta-analytic syntheses and multisite trials (e.g., Albarracín et al., 2005; Albarracín et al., 2003; B. T. Johnson, Carey, Marsh, Levin, & Scott-Sheldon, 2003; Kim, Stanton, Li, Dickersin, & Galbraith, 1997; Mize, Robinson, Bockting, & Scheltema, 2002; Prendergast, Urada, & Podus, 2001), there is a surprising lack of understanding of the programs' outreach. However, it is important to determine if the programs reach and retain audiences that lack appropriate knowledge, motivation, and behavioral practice, and if specific aspects of these programs increase outreach and retention.
The limitations in our knowledge about outreach may be due in part to the researchers' needs to test programs under conditions that increase participation and reduce attrition. In doing so, researchers provide strong incentives for participation and perceive low retention as a serious threat to be minimized. Despite the value of these practices to assess intervention efficacy, an informed take on outreach requires understanding natural variability in intervention acceptance and retention rather than conceptualizing attrition as a rate that must be constant and low. For this reason, we meta-analyzed the HIV-prevention intervention literature with a focus on variations in the sample sizes within included studies. From sample sizes at different study points, acceptance and retention rates were calculated and then examined as a function of participants' knowledge, motivation, and behavioral practice, as well as the intervention's content.1
Self-Validation and Risk Reduction as Guides of Participation in HIV-Prevention Programs
For condom-use-promoting interventions to have a positive public-health impact, they must attract individuals who are not yet using condoms consistently. Nonetheless, previous research on selective exposure implies that only people who already use condoms may participate in prevention programs. According to Festinger (1957, 1964), people have a tendency to seek information that confirms their points of view because they feel well in these situations (Clore & Byrne, 1974). Importantly, then, if people participate in health-behavior interventions with the goal of validating their current behavior, individuals who are already in compliance with the health recommendation would be the most likely to participate. For example, individuals who already use condoms may enroll in and complete the interventions more than will individuals who do not yet use condoms.
An alternative, more encouraging, possibility is that interventions attract audiences that most need the programs. For instance, risk perception and risk-reduction goals have been proposed to underlie health-care seeking (Floyd, Prentice-Dunn, & Rogers, 2000; Rosenstock, Strecher, & Becker, 1994). Hence, people who do not use condoms may be most interested in condom-use interventions that fit with their risk-reduction goals. Another possibility, of course, is that the self-validation and risk-reduction motives operate in tandem. In this case, when condom use increases, the self-validation goal should increase, whereas the risk-reduction goal should decrease. Thus, these two patterns could produce an inverted-U relation between past condom use and exposure to the intervention. That is, high condom users may not participate because they have no risk to reduce, and low-condom users may not participate because the intervention invalidates what they do. In contrast, moderate condom users may participate because the interventions can help them to reduce their risk and do not severely invalidate what they do.
Factors other than the audience's condom use may also play a role. In particular, prior knowledge about HIV and motivation may influence participation in health promotion programs (see J. D. Fisher & Fisher, 1992; W. A. Fisher, Fisher, & Harman, 2003). Thus, our meta-analysis included knowledge about protection; how to recognize safe partners, and so on (W. A. Fisher et al., 2003); and a motivation component that includes attitudes, intentions, norms, perceived threat, and perceived behavioral control (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2005; Bandura, 1994; Rosenstock, Strecher, & Becker, 1994; for a meta-analysis, see Albarracín, Johnson, Fishbein, & Muellerleile, 2001).2 These factors may correlate with exposure to HIV-prevention interventions, whether guided by the goal of self-validation, the goal of reducing one's risk, or both. Self-validation should result in a positive association of knowledge and motivation with exposure; risk reduction should yield a negative association of knowledge and motivation with exposure; a combination of self-validation and risk reduction should yield an inverted-⋃ association.
The Role of Intervention Content
On the basis of J. D. Fisher and Fisher (1992, 2000), Albarracín et al. (2001, 2005) classified three types of interventions to induce condom use. Each intervention type respectively targets information, motivation, or behavioral skills and can be used in combination with the other two (see J. D. Fisher & Fisher, 2000). An information communication typically conveys information on the nature of HIV, modes of transmission, mechanisms of the disease, and methods of prevention (e.g., Borgia et al., 1997; Gerrard & Reis, 1989; Gillmore et al., 1997; Huszti, Clopton, & Mason, 1989; Johnson et al., 1988; Kelly, McAuliffe, et al., 1997; Kelly, Murphy, et al., 1997; O'Leary, Jemmott, Goodhart, & Gebelt, 1996; Sherr, 1987; Solomon & DeJong, 1989). Motivation interventions attempt to induce favorable attitudes, perceived vulnerability to HIV, and social norms in support of the behavior. These strategies usually consist of assertions that the behavior being advocated has personally or socially beneficial consequences (see Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980; CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group, 1997; Kamb et al., 1998). Other motivation interventions consist of normative appeals (e.g., appeals that peer group members have low HIV-risk behavior) for college students (Reeder, Pryor, & Harsh, 1997) or men who have sex with men (Kelly et al., 1991; Kelly, McAuliffe, et al., 1997; Kelly, Murphy, et al., 1997) as well as interventions to convince a variety of higher risk populations that their social network supports condom use (see CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group, 1997; Kamb et al., 1998).
According to the information–motivation– behavioral-skills model, however, HIV-prevention programs are generally not successful unless they manage to increase behavioral skills as well. Thus, interventions based on this model often contain behavioral scripts about strategies that yield successful performance of the behavior. For example, an intervention may not only recommend condom use and mention its advantages but also may describe how success in condom use depends on preparatory actions, such as carrying condoms around all the time or discussing condom use with potential partners. As another example, a widely accepted strategy is to have participants role-play condom-use negotiation or application, with the idea that the behavioral practice and the instructional feedback will facilitate the acquisition of behavioral skills. In addition to teaching behavioral skills, interventions of this type presumably increase perceptions of control (i.e., perceived behavioral control and self-efficacy), which have proven to be a critical element for behavior change (Ajzen, 1985, 1991; Albarracín et al., 2005).
Importantly, if J. D. Fisher and Fisher's (1992) model is plausible, it should have implications for participation and retention in HIV-prevention interventions. Specifically, approaches to interventions that emphasize knowledge, motivation, or behavior skills may be moderated by the audience's level of knowledge, motivation, or behavior. For example, low condom users may drop out when the intervention emphasizes behavioral skills training because the intervention invalidates their past behavior. Likewise, audiences with high knowledge or motivation may prefer interventions respectively emphasizing knowledge and motivation, presumably because these interventions support their current beliefs and motives. Alternatively, rather than seeking self-confirming interventions, audiences may stay in interventions perceived as effective at compensating for their own deficiencies. For instance, low condom users may drop out when the intervention does not include behavioral-skills training. Similarly, those who have high knowledge or motivation may prefer interventions that do not emphasize knowledge or motivation but provide them with something new. These two possibilities, one reflecting a self-validation motivation and the other reflecting a risk-reduction motivation, are explored in this meta-analysis.
The Present Meta-Analysis
A meta-analysis of the existing condom-use-intervention literature was performed to observe the associations of knowledge, motivation, and condom-use behavior with participation in different types of HIV-prevention programs. For this purpose, we calculated the acceptance and retention rates in each report. Acceptance involved the percentage of target participants who agreed to participate. Retention concerned the percentage of commencers completing the intervention. In addition, from each condition, we retrieved the commencers' levels of knowledge, motivation, and condom-use behavior as well as the types of intervention strategies (i.e., information-, motivation-, or behavioral-skills training). Analyses considered how baseline knowledge, motivation, and behavior influenced participation, both alone and in interaction with the intervention strategies. Controls for intervention duration and other potential confounds were introduced in all relevant analyses.
Method
The Literature Search
A critical factor in conducting a literature search is the selection of descriptive terms for use in computerized searches. To capture interventions designed to increase condom use, we combined the key words STD, AIDS, and HIV with intervention, behavior, knowledge, education, prevention, condoms, communication, attitudes, and message. The search included the period between 1988 and 2005. We electronically searched PubMed, PsycINFO, Medline, Cambridge, Education Resources Information Center (ERIC), and Dissertations Database as well as relevant conference databases. We also manually searched all available issues appearing during or after 1980 of the journals AIDS, AIDS Education and Prevention, AIDS and Behavior, American Journal of Public Health, Basic and Applied Social Psychology, Health Psychology, Journal of the American Medical Association, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Finally, we checked prior meta-analyses, examined cross-references in the obtained reports, and requested reports from researchers in this area.
The Selection Criteria
The selection criteria were strict because of the need to identify studies that had baseline measures of knowledge, motivation, or condom use along with precise reports of the sample sizes at the beginning and end of the intervention. Various other criteria were relevant as well.
Presence of measures of knowledge, motivation, or condom use
Given our hypotheses, we synthesized studies that provided information on the levels of baseline knowledge, motivation, or condom use of the audience on entering the intervention. These measures could be retrieved only when researchers reported the baseline levels of one of these measures for all commencers rather than only posttest completers.
Presence of a standardized condom-use-promotion intervention
To be eligible, studies had to include at least one standardized intervention designed to increase condom use among recipients. In addition, reports often included comparison and control conditions. Groups that researchers treated as “comparison” conditions but that participated in an intervention were considered treatment groups. We considered control groups only those not exposed to any kind of HIV-related intervention at the time of the study (e.g., waiting list groups, education programs on other health topics; for similar criteria in a different meta-analysis, see Albarracín et al., 2003, 2005). These control groups provide an estimate of the change that occurs in the absence of systematic exposure to an HIV-prevention intervention.
Presence of the number of commencers and completers of the intervention
Studies were eligible only when they provided statistics to calculate either or both acceptance or retention. In practice, however, all studies reporting acceptance also reported retention. Retention rates required the initial number of participants and the number of completers of the intervention or the immediate posttest. Acceptance rates required initial number of participants and the number of people targeted for recruitment.
Within-subject design
The measures of retention excluded reports containing pre- and posttest information from different audiences.
Partitioning of Studies
For each article, we retrieved each available intervention and control condition. In addition, whenever the report distinguished samples, we attempted to treat each sample separately. In three articles, however, interventions were collapsed into a single group. Hence, all statistics for these reports represent an average. In no case did we merge interventions and control groups.
Record of and Effect Sizes for Acceptance and Retention
To obtain measures of initial acceptance, we retrieved the number of target participants and also the numbers of participants who were invited, who accepted, and who were excluded by the experimenter. As measures of retention, we recorded the number of commencers and completers of the intervention. When available, these data allowed us to compute the proportion of acceptance and retention in a sample. These indices were then converted into odds (proportion of acceptance/proportion of declinations). Odds of 1 correspond to equal probability of acceptance–retention and declination–drop out. Odds greater than 1 correspond to more likely acceptance–retention than declination– drop drop out. Odds smaller than 1 correspond to more likely declination–dropout than acceptance–retention.
Change Measures
When possible, we also calculated effect sizes for use in supplementary analyses to ensure that our sample of interventions was similar to broader samples. To represent change from pretest to posttest measures, we used Becker's (1988) g, which is calculated by subtracting the mean at the posttest from the mean at the pretest and dividing the difference by the standard deviation of the pretest measure. This measure controls for the inflation in the standard deviation following treatment (for an excellent analysis of the problem, see Carlson & Schmidt, 1999). Effect sizes were also derived from exact reports of t tests, F ratios, proportions, p values, and confidence intervals (CIs). Within-subject studies can be more precisely derived by estimating the correlation between posttest and pretest measures. Because some reports did not offer this information, we adopted procedures recommended by Becker (1988) as well as by Dunlap, Cortina, Vaslow, and Burke (1996). These procedures were used by Albarracín et al. (2003, 2005) in the same context.
Coding of Moderators
Independent raters coded relevant characteristics of the reports and methods used in the study. After the initial training, the overall intercoder agreement was 95%. Occasional disagreements were resolved by discussion and further examination of the studies. For all variables, reliabilities were superior to correlations, and kappa was .70.
Past knowledge, motivation, and condom use
Assessment of knowledge about HIV or AIDS typically comprises a series of statements that the participant evaluates as true or false (e.g., “The AIDS virus can be caught through ordinary close social contact, such as sitting next to an infected person”; Rigby, Brown, Anagnostou, Ross, & Rosser, 1989, p. 149). Knowledge scores in all cases were calculated by computing the percentage of questions a participant answered correctly.
On the basis of the information–motivation–behavioral-skills framework, we created an average motivation index and used it in analyses (r among variables = .77). This index included measures of intentions, attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy.3 We included self-efficacy as part of motivation skills instead of behavioral skills on the basis of past research showing high associations of self-efficacy with intentions (r = .59 and .60 in Glasman & Albarracín, 2003). Measures of intentions assessed the intent or willingness to use condoms in the future. Typical items were “In the future, do you plan to use condoms?” (Eldridge et al., 1997, p. 67) or “In the next six months, how likely do you think it is that you will start using a condom every time you have vaginal sex with your main partner?” (CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group, 1993, p. 11). In terms of attitudes, we included only attitudes toward condom use rather than attitudes toward HIV. Attitudes toward condom use were typically measured with semantic-differential types of scales, for example, “Do you think using a condom every time you have vaginal sex with your main partner would be pleasant or unpleasant? And would you say it would be extremely, quite, or slightly (pleasant/unpleasant)?” (CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group, 1993, p. 12). With respect to measures of norms, subjective norms are influenced by a set of salient beliefs about the normative prescriptions of specific (salient) referents, weighted by the motivation to comply with each of those referents (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975). For example, a man may perceive social pressure to use condoms if he believes that his partner thinks that he should use condoms and if he is motivated to comply with the partner. Subjective norms were typically measured with probability scales in response to statements like “Would you say that most of the people who are important to you think that you should or should not use a condom for vaginal sex with your main partner?” (CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group, 1993, p. 12). Measures of self-efficacy comprised items that relate control to specific events. For example, the CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group (1993) included items like “How sure are you that you can use condoms every time for vaginal sex with your main partner when your partner doesn't feel like using them?” or “When there aren't any condoms around, how sure are you that you can wait until you get one every time before having vaginal sex with your main partner?” (p. 7).
Condom-use measures included assessments on subjective frequency scales as well as reports of the percentage and number of times participants used condoms over a period of time. For example, the CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group (1993) asked participants “When you have vaginal sex with your main partner, how often do you use a condom?” (p. 11), and participants provided their response on a scale ranging from 1 (every time) to 5 (never). To obtain a more precise report of condom use, Ploem and Byers (1997) asked participants to report the frequency of sexual intercourse over the previous 4 weeks as well as the number of occasions of sexual intercourse for which condoms were used. The researchers then derived the percentage of condom use for each participant. Similarly, Belcher et al. (1998) asked participants to list the first name of all of their sex partners in the previous 90 days. For each name listed, participants were then asked to identify the partner's gender, the partner type (regular, casual, or new), the total frequency of vaginal sex, the frequency of condom-protected vaginal sex, the total frequency of anal sex, and the frequency of condom-protected anal sex.
Importantly, to establish baseline levels of knowledge, motivation, and condom use, it was necessary to standardize measures that used many different scales. For this purpose, we converted all these measures to proportions. Specifically, the lowest possible score of a measure was subtracted from the mean score in a given group. This number was divided by the highest possible score minus the lowest possible score plus one. With these procedures, we derived ranks for the various indices and then created overall measures of knowledge, motivation, and behavior.
Intervention content
We recorded the presence or absence of the different intervention components. For this purpose, each intervention was categorized as entailing information, motivation, or active behavioral strategies. Information strategies included (a) information about HIV and/or (b) information about condom use. Motivation strategies included (c) attitudinal arguments, such as statements about the positive implications of using condoms for the health of the partners and for the romantic relationship; (d) normative arguments asserting support for condom use on the part of friends, family members, or partners; and/or (e) threat arguments, such as discussions about the recipient's personal risk of contracting HIV or other sexually transmitted infections. Behavioral strategies included (f) negotiation skills (e.g., role-playing condom-use negotiation), (g) condom-use behavioral skills (e.g., opening wrapper without tearing it, unrolling condom in proper direction), (h) HIV counseling and testing, and/or (i) provision of condoms.
Overall indices were calculated to have general measures of information-, motivation-, and behavioral-skills strategies. For that purpose, we counted the number of target components (e.g., information) addressed by the intervention and divided that by the total number of components of that intervention. For information, the maximum number of coded components was two. For motivation, the maximum number of coded components was three. For behavioral skills, the maximum number of coded components was four. The maximum total number of components used to calculate these proportions was nine. When the resulting number was multiplied by 100, this procedure yielded percentages of information-, motivation-, and behavioral-skills emphasis of each intervention.
Other descriptive moderators
We coded the following information with regard to the reports: (a) publication year, (b) country of intervention (i.e., United States vs. other countries), and (c) state of intervention for studies in the United States. We also recorded demographics of the participants as well as specific characteristics and behaviors that are associated with HIV-infection risk. These characteristics were recorded for the commencers and completers of the intervention or of the immediate follow-up. Specifically, we retrieved (a) sample size; (b) percentage of female participants in each group; (c) mean or median age; (d) percentage of participants of European, African, Latin American, Asian, and Native North American descent; (e) education (i.e., years of education and/or percentage of participants who completed at least high school); (f) percentage of married participants; (g) mean number of sexual partners; and (h) mean HIV rates. Further, we registered the inclusion of various behaviorally at-risk groups in each sample (i.e., men who have sex with men, partners of infection-drug users, commercial sex workers, multiple-partner heterosexuals, college students, participants with severe mental illness, and disadvantaged women).
We also recorded information on recruitment procedures. We classified each treatment group according to whether (a) the setting of the recruitment and intervention included hospitals or clinics, community settings, streets, bars, work settings, or schools; and (b) whether audiences were recruited via letters, by fliers, by personal contact, as patients, in community services, or by other forms of media. We also recorded if the recruiter was a risk-group peer, an outreach worker, a physician, a member of the research team, or a member of hospital staff. Further, we recorded (a) the planned number of sessions, (b) the number of minutes per session, (c) the percentage of completed sessions, and (d) the M and Mdn number of days between the treatment and the posttest.
Finally, we recorded the incentives and facilitators used to increase participation. Incentives were the amount of money paid for the study as well as the reception of services such as free health care and HIV counseling and testing. Facilitators involved the provision of child care and transportation to the intervention site.
Analytic Strategy
We first calculated weighted-mean odds as estimates of the degree of acceptance and retention and performed corrections for sample-size bias. As described before, proportions of acceptance and retention were converted into odds, and then the odds were log transformed (see Haddock, Rindskopf, & Shadish, 1998). We used Hedges and Olkin's (1985) procedures to correct the effects for sample-size bias4 as well as to calculate weighted-mean effect sizes, CIs, and homogeneity statistics. Calculations of the between-subjects variance followed procedures developed by Hedges and Olkin (1985).
Computations of effect sizes were performed with fixed- and random-effects procedures (Hedges & Olkin, 1985; Hedges & Vevea, 1998; Rosenthal, 1995; Wang & Bushman, 1999; but see Hunter & Schmidt, 2000; Raudenbush, 1994). The weights for fixed-effects models followed Hedges and Olkin's (1985) computational formulas, whereas the weights for random-effects models followed Lipsey and Wilson's (2001) approach. Unless otherwise indicated, for display purposes we present back-transformed proportions rather than log odds. However, odds can be easily calculated by dividing the proportion of participants accepting or staying in the intervention by 1 minus that proportion.
The moderator analyses also included regressions and analyses of variance by using either the fixed-effects or the random-effects weights. The analyses of variance yield QB statistics, which are similar to F ratios reflecting between-groups differences. Further, the continuous moderators of prior knowledge, motivation, and behavior were centered and included in regression equations. Interaction terms were used to model differences across groups, and beta weights were obtained for each group when differences were found. Also, both linear and quadratic models were tested when prior individual difference variables were examined. The error of the beta weights was corrected on the basis of Hedges and Olkin's (1985) recommendations.
In the moderator analyses of prior knowledge, motivation, and behavior, both the control and the intervention groups were included. The results excluding the control groups were similar to the overall results, and hence the control groups were retained in these analyses. However, the control groups were excluded in the analyses of the effects of intervention strategies. All analyses of intervention strategies covaried out the number of intervention sessions and were conducted with fixed and random effects. Some of the random-effects models for the intervention effects were nonsignificant, which led us to report only fixed effects.
Results
Sample of Intervention Groups and Control Groups
We included 59 reports, which provided 105 independent intervention groups and 27 independent control groups. Of the 59 reports, 15 provided a single data set, 28 provided two data sets, 7 provided three data sets, 7 provided four data sets, and 2 provided five data sets. Studies were published around 1998 (SD = 4 years). Although most studies were conducted in the United States (69%), 12 countries were represented. Of the American studies, 19 states were represented, with New York providing more groups than any other state. Table 1 summarizes information about prior knowledge, motivation, and condom use as well as intervention strategies, participants, and method.
Table 1.
Summary of Descriptive Statistics From Studies of HIV-Prevention Interventions
Variable | M | % | k | SD | Mdn |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline measure | |||||
Prior knowledge (standardized M score; range = .05–.94) | .59 | .18 | |||
Prior motivation (standardized M score; range = .04–.88) | .58 | .18 | |||
Prior condom use | |||||
Standardized score of past condom use | .51 | 95 | |||
Condom use over total sexual intercourse | .43 | 34 | |||
Participants using condoms during last intercourse | .45 | 29 | |||
Participants who always used condoms | .29 | 42 | |||
Participants who never used condoms | .42 | 24 | |||
| |||||
Type of intervention strategy | |||||
Information strategy | |||||
Information about HIV | |||||
Yes | 81 | 85 | |||
No | 19 | 20 | |||
Information about condom use | |||||
Yes | 56 | 59 | |||
No | 44 | 46 | |||
Motivational strategy | |||||
Attitudinal argument | |||||
Yes | 20 | 21 | |||
No | 80 | 84 | |||
Normative argument | |||||
Yes | 11 | 11 | |||
No | 89 | 94 | |||
Threat-inducing argument | |||||
Yes | 43 | 45 | |||
No | 57 | 60 | |||
Behavioral strategy | |||||
Negotiation-skills training | |||||
Yes | 31 | 32 | |||
No | 69 | 73 | |||
HIV counseling and testing | |||||
Yes | 17 | 18 | |||
No | 83 | 87 | |||
Condom-use-skills training | |||||
Yes | 20 | 21 | |||
No | 80 | 84 | |||
Condom provision | |||||
Yes | 20 | 21 | |||
No | 80 | 84 | |||
| |||||
Participant characteristic | |||||
Sample size (number of commencers = 26,780) | 202.88 | 132 | 430.02 | 86 | |
% female | 58.54 | 124 | 40.41 | 54 | |
% male | 41.46 | 124 | 40.41 | 46 | |
Age in years | 23.38 | 97 | 7.55 | 23 | |
Ethnicity | |||||
% European background | 23.16 | 106 | 30.65 | 6 | |
% African background | 54.67 | 99 | 39.54 | 59.00 | |
% Latin American background | 11.61 | 90 | 20.79 | 3.7 | |
% Asian background | 10.93 | 87 | 30.52 | 0 | |
% Native North American background | 0.16 | 87 | 0.70 | 0 | |
% high school graduate | 39.07 | 69 | 37.86 | 43.70 | |
% married | 21.23 | 42 | 24.27 | 14.75 | |
Sexual behavior | |||||
Mean number of sexual partners | 1.28 | 42 | 0.95 | 1.3 | |
% HIV+ | 7.70 | 56 | 19.53 | 0 | |
Characteristics or behaviors associated with HIV-infection risk | |||||
Inclusion of men who have sex with men | 8.3 | 11 | |||
Inclusion of partners of infection-drug users | 1.5 | 2 | |||
Inclusion of commercial sex workers | 7.6 | 10 | |||
Inclusion of multiple-partner heterosexuals | 2.3 | 3 | |||
Inclusion of college students | 7.6 | 10 | |||
Inclusion of participants with severe mental illness | 3.0 | 4 | |||
Inclusion of disadvantaged women | 19.7 | 26 | |||
Recruitment procedure | |||||
Setting of recruitment | |||||
Recruited at hospital or clinic | 37.7 | 49 | |||
Recruited at community service | 23.1 | 30 | |||
Recruited on the street | 4.6 | 6 | |||
Recruited in bars | 7.7 | 10 | |||
Recruited in work settings | 9.2 | 12 | |||
Recruited at school | 26.2 | 34 | |||
Recruitment method | |||||
n of groups recruited by letters | 3.9 | 5 | |||
n of groups recruited by fliers | 11.8 | 15 | |||
n of groups recruited by personal contact | 78.7 | 100 | |||
n of groups recruited as patients | 37 | 47 | |||
n of groups recruited in community services | 15 | 19 | |||
n of groups recruited through the media | 6.3 | 8 | |||
Characteristics of recruiters | |||||
Recruited by risk group peer | 2.7 | 2 | |||
Recruited by outreach worker | 16 | 12 | |||
Recruited by physicians | 1.3 | 1 | |||
Recruited by university or research team | 44 | 33 | |||
Recruited by hospital staff | 18.7 | 14 | |||
| |||||
Research design and implementation | |||||
Planned number of sessions | 3.55 | 109 | 3.92 | 2 | |
Number of minutes per session | 70.68 | 84 | 61.57 | 60 | |
% completed sessions | 91.20 | 67 | 17.76 | 100 | |
Days between baseline and last posttest | 148.78 | 100 | 172.62 | 90 | |
| |||||
Incentives and facilitators | |||||
Payment (US$) | 25.16 | 114 | 52.47 | 0 | |
Free health care | |||||
Yes | 14.9 | 17 | |||
No | 85.1 | 97 | |||
HIV testing | |||||
Yes | 5.3 | 6 | |||
No | 94.7 | 108 | |||
Child care | |||||
Yes | 18.2 | 4 | |||
No | 81.8 | 18 | |||
Provision of transportation | |||||
Yes | 8.7 | 2 | |||
No | 91.3 | 21 |
Note. k = number of intervention and control groups.
The mean of the baseline standardized score of past knowledge was .59 based on 55 groups. The mean of the standardized score of motivation was .58 based on 46 groups. The mean of the standardized score of past condom use was .51 based on 95 groups. Forty-three percent of the commencers used a condom over total sexual intercourses, 45% used a condom during last intercourse, 29% always used a condom, and 42% had never used a condom.
With respect to intervention strategies, 81% of the interventions contained HIV-relevant information, 56% contained information about condom use, 20% of the interventions contained arguments designed to induce a positive attitude about condom-use outcomes, 11% contained normative arguments in support of condom use, 43% included persuasive arguments designed to increase perceptions of threat among recipients, 31% included arguments designed to promote recipients' negotiation skills, 17% administered an HIV test, and an average of 20% trained participants in condom-use skills. Researchers distributed condoms to 20% of the groups. As global measures of information, motivation, and behavioral strategies, we calculated percentages of information, motivation, and behavioral strategies within each intervention by dividing the number of information, motivation, and behavioral strategies each over the total number of intervention strategies and multiplying that by 100. The mean percentage of information strategies was 50, the mean percentage of motivation strategies was 29, and the mean percentage of behavioral strategies was 16.
The last section of Table 1 summarizes participant characteristics, recruitment of participants, and research design and implementation. The mean sample size was 202.88. Samples comprised both female and male participants and were relatively young in age (Mdn = 23 years). On average, 39% of participants were high school graduates, 21% were married, and the average number of partners was 1.28. Studies included various behavioral risk groups such as men who have sex with men and commercial sex workers. Recruitment varied along recruitment method, type of recruiter, and location of recruitment. Recruitment was conducted at hospitals, community services, streets, bars, work settings, and schools. Many groups were recruited by personal contact, some by fliers, and many from the clinics in which they were patients. Recruiters were mostly members of universities and research teams. With respect to research design and implementation, on average, 3.55 sessions were planned, 71 minutes were spent per session, 91% of the sessions were completed, and 149 days passed between the baseline and last posttest. With respect to incentives and facilitators, on average, participants received $25 in exchange for the intervention, 15% of participants received free health care, 5% received HIV testing, 18% received child care, and 9% were provided transportation.
Examining Biases in Report Selection
In our integration, we excluded studies that did not describe the baseline sample sizes or any baseline measures of knowledge, motivation, or condom use. These restrictions could have produced a nonrepresentative sample of studies. To assess biases in our selection, sample characteristics and changes on knowledge, motivation, and condom use were examined. If the sample is representative, the findings should replicate the findings of a broader, independent meta-analysis. Further, we analyzed the normality of the distribution of effect sizes (the odds) to gauge potential biases in study selection.
Sample characteristics and change
The data obtained for this meta-analysis were similar to those from the past meta-analysis involving a more complete sample of studies (Albarracín et al., 2005). For example, across the present and previous syntheses, the mean percentages of female participants were 59% and 58% and of male participants 41% and 42%; the age means were 23 and 25 years; the mean percentages of participants from European backgrounds were 23% and 36%, from African background 55% and 44%, from Latin American backgrounds 12% and 14%, from Asian backgrounds 11% and 7%, and from Native North American backgrounds 0.2% and 0.4% (percentages for ethnicities have been rounded); the mean percentages of high school graduates were 39% and 35%; the percentages of inclusion of normative arguments were 11% and 12%; the percentages of inclusion of HIV counseling and testing were 17% and 14%; and the percentages of inclusion of condom-use-skills training were 20% and 18%, respectively.
We also compared our control and the intervention groups on changes on knowledge, motivation, and condom use. The weighted-mean effect sizes for intervention and control groups appear in Table 2, along with CIs. The last column of Table 2 presents QB statistics, which in this case are analogous to F ratios comparing change across intervention and control groups. As shown, changes were greater for the intervention groups than for the control groups. This finding is similar to Albarracín et al. (2005) and thus establishes generalizability of the findings from the current data set.
Table 2.
General Effects of Interventions by Information, Motivation, and Behavioral Skill
Intervention group |
Control group |
Cross-group comparison |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Skill | d | 95% CI | d | 95% CI | QB |
Knowledge | |||||
Fixed effects | 0.47 | 0.36, 0.59 | 0.10 | −0.09, 0.30 | 57.39*** |
Random effects | 0.58 | 0.45, 0.71 | 0.07 | −0.13, 0.28 | 21.82*** |
Motivation | |||||
Fixed effects | 0.22 | 0.15, 0.28 | 0.02 | −0.09, 0.13 | 27.42*** |
Random effects | 0.22 | 0.15, 0.28 | 0.02 | −0.09, 0.13 | 27.42*** |
Condom use | |||||
Fixed effects | 0.19 | 0.03, 0.36 | −0.05 | −0.28, 0.18 | 24.48*** |
Random effects | 0.13 | −0.10, 0.36 | −0.11 | −0.40, 0.17 | 2.68 |
Note. ds are Becker's g (Mposttest − Mpretest/SDpre) adjusted for sample size. Significant QBs indicate significant effects of the intervention groups relative to control groups. d = weighted mean effect sizes; CI = confidence interval; QB = between-categories homogeneity index, distributed as a chi square with number of categories − 1 degrees of freedom.
p < .001.
Normality of effect size distribution
Another way of testing for biases is to use the normal-quantile-plot method (Wang & Bushman, 1999). In a normal-quantile plot, the observed values of a variable are plotted against the expected values given normality. If the sample of effect sizes is from a normal distribution, data points cluster around the diagonal; if the sample of effect sizes is biased by publication practices or eligibility criteria, data points deviate from the diagonal (Wang & Bushman, 1999). As can be seen from Figure 1, the standardized log odds followed a straight line and generally fell within the 95% CIs of the normality line. Moreover, our findings remained unaltered after excluding the most extreme outliers from the sample of conditions (see the two extreme observations in Figure 1, Panel B). These data imply that inclusion of different studies would be unlikely to alter the conclusions of this meta-analysis.
Figure 1.
Normal probability plots. A: Normal probability plots of log-transformed odds of acceptance. B: Normal probability plots of log-transformed odds of retention.
Average acceptance and retention
Because many reports did not describe the number of persons targeted for a study, only 22 groups were identified for the acceptance measure. Acceptance was calculated by dividing the number of commencers by the number of target persons. The stem-and-leaf plot of acceptance appears in the left panel of Figure 2. On average, the proportion of participation was .53 (fixed-effects CI = .53, .53). Within control groups, the average proportion of participation was .53 (fixed-effects CI = .53, .53). (Random-effects models could not be obtained because of the low number of total effect sizes.)
Figure 2.
Stem-and-leaf plots of acceptance and retention. Each leaf corresponds to one group.
Retention in the intervention was calculated by dividing the number of completers by the number of commencers. The stem-and-leaf plot of retention appears in the right panel of Figure 2. On average, the proportions of retention were .74 (fixed-effects CI = .74, .74) and .72 (random-effects CI = .71, .72). Within control groups, the average proportions of retention were .75 (fixed-effects CI = .75, .75) and .74 (random-effects CI = .74, .74) based on fixed- and random-effects models.
Exploratory Moderator Analyses
First, change in behavior did not correlate with acceptance (r = −.11, ns, k = 6, in change in knowledge; r = −.44, ns, k = 6, in change in motivation; and no observation in change in condom use). Also, Table 3 presents various moderator analyses yielding important information. Control groups had slightly higher acceptance and retention rates than those of intervention groups. This finding suggests that participation and retention in interventions were unique and different from general participation in research. Second, conditions including larger percentages of individuals from African backgrounds, high school graduates, married persons, and HIV-positive people had lower acceptance rates than did conditions including smaller percentages of these groups. With respect to recruitment procedures, acceptance rates were lower when samples were recruited at hospitals or clinics and when recruited as patients than when they were not. In contrast, acceptance rates were higher when samples were recruited in work and school settings and through personal contact than those when they were not.
Table 3.
Exploratory Moderator Analyses of Acceptance and Retention
Cross-group comparisons |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Group | Proportion 1 | Proportion 2 | QB | β |
Acceptance | ||||
Intervention vs. control group (intervention/control) | ||||
Fixed effects | .45 | .52 | 20.46*** | |
Random effects | .47 | .49 | 0.28 | |
Gender (more than 56% female participants/more than 44% male participants) | ||||
Fixed effects | .47 | .47 | 0.36 | |
Random effects | .47 | .47 | 0.18 | |
Age (mean over 23 years/mean under 23 years) | ||||
Fixed effects | .49 | .50 | −0.33 | |
Random effects | .49 | .46 | 0.04 | |
European background (more than 21%/less than 21%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .51 | .48 | 0.14 | |
Random effects | .46 | .48 | −0.27 | |
African background (more than 50%/less than 50%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .45 | .53 | −0.76*** | |
Random effects | .46 | .51 | −0.67** | |
Latin American background (more than 10%/less than 10%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .53 | .53 | 0.45** | |
Random effects | .52 | .52 | 0.17 | |
Asian background (more than 6%/less than 6%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .54 | .45 | 0.28 | |
Random effects | .55 | .45 | 0.48 | |
High-school graduates (more than 51%/less than 51%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .47 | .53 | −0.97* | |
Random effects | .47 | .53 | −0.97* | |
Married participants (more than 15%/less than 15%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .46 | .47 | −0.72** | |
Random effects | .44 | .47 | −0.70** | |
HIV+ participants (more than 0%/0%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .43 | .48 | −0.60* | |
Random effects | .45 | .48 | −0.53 | |
Men who have sex with men (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .49 | .47 | 0.02 | |
Random effects | .49 | .47 | 0.02 | |
Female sex workers (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .54 | .47 | 5.21* | |
Random effects | .55 | .46 | 2.74 | |
Multiple-partner participants (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .42 | .47 | 0.81 | |
Random effects | .49 | .47 | 0.17 | |
College students (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .51 | .47 | 0.09 | |
Random effects | .51 | .47 | 0.08 | |
Participants with severe mental illness (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .48 | .47 | 0 | |
Random effects | .48 | .47 | 0 | |
Disadvantaged women (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .48 | .47 | 0.07 | |
Random effects | .48 | .47 | 0.08 | |
Recruited at hospital or clinic (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .43 | .52 | 45.33*** | |
Random effects | .44 | .51 | 6.22* | |
Recruited at community service (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .50 | .47 | 0.82 | |
Random effects | .50 | .47 | 0.41 | |
Recruited in work settings (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .51 | .47 | 4.63* | |
Random effects | .52 | .46 | 2.57 | |
Recruited at school (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .53 | .45 | 25.68*** | |
Random effects | .53 | .47 | 1.45 | |
Recruited by fliers (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .50 | .47 | 0.82 | |
Random effects | .50 | .47 | 0.40 | |
Recruited by personal contact (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .48 | .43 | 8.94** | |
Random effects | .50 | .43 | 3.83 | |
Recruited as patients (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .43 | .52 | 42.33*** | |
Random effects | .44 | .51 | 6.21* | |
Recruited by outreach workers (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .50 | .44 | 3.80 | |
Random effects | .50 | .45 | 1.14 | |
Recruited by university or research team (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .47 | .44 | 0.60 | |
Random effects | .47 | .45 | 0.10 | |
Recruited by hospital staff (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .41 | .45 | 3.63 | |
Random effects | .42 | .47 | 2.05 | |
| ||||
Retention | ||||
Intervention vs. control group (intervention/control) | ||||
Fixed effects | .71 | .76 | 38.51*** | |
Random effects | .73 | .81 | 18.38*** | |
Gender (more than 56% female participants/more than 44% male participants) | ||||
Fixed effects | .60 | .75 | −0.31*** | |
Random effects | .69 | .76 | −0.01 | |
Age (mean over 23 years/mean under 23 years) | ||||
Fixed effects | .63 | .72 | −0.11*** | |
Random effects | .69 | .74 | −0.31** | |
European background (more than 21%/less than 21%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .74 | .72 | 0.29*** | |
Random effects | .78 | .71 | 0.28** | |
African background (more than 59%/less than 59%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .73 | .70 | 0.16*** | |
Random effects | .73 | .71 | −0.14 | |
Latin American background (more than 10%/less than 10%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .75 | .54 | 0.40*** | |
Random effects | .82 | .53 | 0.04 | |
Asian background (more than 6%/less than 6%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .50 | .72 | −0.48*** | |
Random effects | .51 | .74 | −0.19 | |
Native North American background (more than 0%/0%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .99 | .72 | 0.16*** | |
Random effects | .99 | .72 | 0.29** | |
High-school graduates (more than 51%/less than 51%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .74 | .73 | −0.05 | |
Random effects | .77 | .77 | −0.11 | |
Married participants (more than 15%/less than 15%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .74 | .68 | 0.39*** | |
Random effects | .76 | .70 | 0.25 | |
HIV+ participants (more than 0%/0%) | ||||
Fixed effects | .66 | .73 | −0.20*** | |
Random effects | .74 | .70 | 0.03 | |
Men who have sex with men (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .77 | .72 | 5.36* | |
Random effects | .80 | .74 | 5.63* | |
Female sex workers (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .44 | .74 | 477.39*** | |
Random effects | .49 | .78 | 122.75*** | |
Partners of infection-drug users (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .78 | .72 | 1.47 | |
Random effects | .78 | .74 | 0.41 | |
Multiple-partner participants (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .90 | .72 | 130.53*** | |
Random effects | .92 | .74 | 26.66*** | |
College students (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .87 | .72 | 34.92*** | |
Random effects | .89 | .74 | 26.36 | |
Participants with severe mental illness (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .99 | .72 | 20.40*** | |
Random effects | .99 | .74 | 18.46*** | |
Disadvantaged women (included/not included) | ||||
Fixed effects | .67 | .73 | 29.17*** | |
Random effects | .72 | .75 | 3.57 | |
Number of sessions (multiple/one) | ||||
Fixed effects | .73 | .73 | −0.14*** | |
Random effects | .71 | .86 | −0.39*** | |
Minutes per session (more than 60 min/less than 60 min) | ||||
Fixed effects | .65 | .74 | −0.26*** | |
Random effects | .73 | .77 | −0.04 | |
Days between sessions (more than zero/zero) | ||||
Fixed effects | .69 | .75 | −0.17*** | |
Random effects | .75 | .79 | −0.09 | |
Number of strategies (more than three/less than three) | ||||
Fixed effects | .70 | .73 | −0.02 | |
Random effects | .72 | .74 | −0.16 | |
Payment (US$) (yes/no) | ||||
Fixed effects | .78 | .72 | 0.18*** | |
Random effects | .78 | .76 | −0.22* | |
Free health care (provided/not provided) | ||||
Fixed effects | .59 | .76 | 297.90*** | |
Random effects | .67 | .78 | 34.07*** | |
HIV testing (provided/not provided) | ||||
Fixed effects | .53 | .76 | 240.16*** | |
Random effects | .61 | .77 | 20.58*** | |
Child care (provided/not provided) | ||||
Fixed effects | .57 | .73 | 22.64*** | |
Random effects | .58 | .81 | 26.34*** | |
Transportation (provided/not provided) | ||||
Fixed effects | .60 | .61 | 0.05 | |
Random effects | .60 | .66 | 0.78 |
Note. Proportions are described in parentheses as (Proportion 1/Proportion 2). Table entries are acceptance and retention rates. Acceptance was calculated by dividing the number of targets by the number of commencers. Retention was calculated by dividing the number of commencers by the number of completers. For the weighted analyses of variance reported in this table, the acceptance and retention proportions were converted into odds (proportion of acceptance/proportion of declinations). For display purposes, the proportions in the table are back transformed from log odds. Dichotomous independent variables were analyzed with analysis of variance. Continuous independent variables were analyzed with regression, but scores for median splits are presented for display purposes. Other descriptive variables present in Table 1 could not be analyzed due to low representation in the data. QB = between-categories homogeneity index, distributed as a chi square with number of categories − 1 degrees of freedom; β = standardized regression coefficient.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Furthermore, there were also effects of the sample's characteristics on retention. Change in behavior did not correlate with retention (r = −.11, ns, k = 48, in change in knowledge; r = .01, ns, k = 39, in change in motivation; and r = −.17, ns, k = 20, in change in condom use). Samples with more male participants; more younger age groups; more people from European, African, Latin American, and Native North American backgrounds; fewer people from Asian backgrounds; more married participants; fewer HIV-positive participants; more men who have sex with men; fewer female sex workers; more multiple partner participants; more college students; more participants with mental illness; and fewer disadvantaged women were retained more often than were samples without these characteristics.5 Retention was larger when the intervention contained fewer (vs. more) number of sessions, when sessions were shorter (vs. longer), and when days between sessions were shorter (vs. longer). Monetary incentives retained participants, whereas other incentives and facilitators did not. In fact, several incentives and facilitators were probably offered when samples were hard to retain, resulting in negative associations with retention.
Test of Hypotheses
Associations with past knowledge, motivation, and condom use
We examined the relation of acceptance and retention with baseline knowledge, motivation, and condom use by using regression analyses with linear and quadratic terms. For this purpose, knowledge, motivation, and condom use were centered prior to processing. The linear term was examined alone. The quadratic term was always entered with the linear term. Table 4 presents QRs (QR = homogeneity accounted for by the regression model), regression weights, and k (k = number of effect sizes in the model) for acceptance and retention as a function of knowledge, motivation, and condom use. These analyses included both intervention groups and control groups. However, an analysis of interventions alone produced the same results.
Table 4.
Effects of Knowledge, Motivation, and Condom Use on Acceptance and Retention (Fixed Effects)
QR
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Skill | Model | Term | β | k | |
Acceptance | |||||
Knowledge | Linear | 3.64 | Linear | −0.95 | 6 |
Quadratic | 4.00 | Linear | 1.81 | ||
Quadratic | 2.77 | ||||
Motivation | Linear | 0.63 | Linear | −0.73 | 6 |
Quadratic | 0.67 | Linear | −1.20 | ||
Quadratic | 0.50 | ||||
Condom use | Linear | 8.37** | Linear | 0.49** | 20 |
Quadratic | 10.21** | Linear | 0.74** | ||
Quadratic | 0.34 | ||||
| |||||
Retention | |||||
Knowledge | Linear | 245.53*** | Linear | −0.59*** | 55 |
Quadratic | 246.58*** | Linear | −0.59*** | ||
Quadratic | −0.04 | ||||
Motivation | Linear | 9.55** | Linear | −0.14** | 46 |
Quadratic | 187.87*** | Linear | −0.35*** | ||
Quadratic | −0.65*** | ||||
Condom use | Linear | 69.00*** | Linear | −0.18*** | 95 |
Quadratic | 213.47*** | Linear | −0.18*** | ||
Quadratic | −0.26*** |
Note. QR = homogeneity accounted for by the regression model; β = standardized regression coefficient; k = number of effect sizes in the model.
p < .01.
p < .001.
As shown in Table 4, neither knowledge nor motivation was associated with acceptance rates. However, the linear model of condom use showed significant associations with acceptance. Figure 3 presents the scatter plot for these data. As shown, samples with higher condom use had higher enrollment rates than did samples with lower condom use.
Figure 3.
Effects of prior condom use on acceptance.
In terms of retention, several of the linear and the quadratic models were significant with fixed-effects models. A significant linear term indicated that samples with high knowledge were more likely to drop out than were samples with low knowledge (Figure 4, Panel A). The quadratic terms of prior motivation and condom use were significant. These suggested that samples with moderate motivation and condom use were more likely to stay in the intervention than were samples with high and low motivation and condom use (see Figure 4, Panels B and C).6 Thus, encouragingly, samples with greater knowledge deficiencies were more likely to stay in HIV-prevention programs than were those with lesser deficiencies; however, samples with low motivation and condom use were less likely to stay than were samples with moderate levels of these measures. The curvilinear tendencies of motivation and condom use might be due to the combined effect of risk-reduction and self-validation motives.
Figure 4.
Effects of prior knowledge, motivation, and condom use on retention. A: Effects of prior knowledge. B: Effects of prior motivation. C: Effects of prior condom use.
Influence of intervention contents
We next conducted analyses of the effects of the intervention content. In these tests, the control condition was excluded. Also, behavioral-training interventions typically involve more sessions than merely information strategies. Thus, the number of intervention sessions was included as a covariate representing duration. Table 5 presents the effects of intervention strategies on retention. As can be seen, participants were less likely to stay when interventions included greater percentages of motivation strategies. There were no main effects of other strategies on retention.
Table 5.
Duration-Controlled Effects of Intervention Strategies on Retention (Fixed Effects)
Percentage of strategies |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Strategy | High | Low | QR | β | k |
Information | .67 | .76 | 32.78*** | −0.02 | 114 |
N of sessions | −0.13*** | ||||
Motivation | .68 | .75 | 42.19*** | −0.07** | 114 |
N of sessions | −0.14** | ||||
Behavioral | .75 | .68 | 33.76*** | 0.03 | 114 |
N of sessions | −0.13*** |
Note. Continuous independent variables were analyzed with regression, but scores for median splits are presented for display purposes. QR = homogeneity accounted for by the regression model; β = standardized regression coefficient; k = number of effect sizes in model.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Interactions of intervention strategies with prior knowledge, motivation, and condom use
We predicted that the content of the intervention might interact with prior knowledge, motivation, and condom use. Thus, we analyzed retention as a function of (a) the indices of information, motivation, or behavioral strategies; (b) the relevant baseline levels of knowledge, motivation, or condom use; and (c) the corresponding interactions with both linear and quadratic terms. Specifically, models included the linear term for a baseline measure, the quadratic term for this same measure, the linear term for intervention content, the interaction between the intervention content and the linear term for the baseline measure, the interaction between the intervention content and the quadratic term for the baseline measure, and the linear term for the number of sessions. A summary of the results from these analyses appears in Table 6.
Table 6.
Duration-Controlled Interactions Between Knowledge, Motivation, or Condom Use and Intervention Strategies on Retention (Fixed Effects)
Variable | QR | β | k |
---|---|---|---|
Knowledge × Information Strategies | 257.35*** | 41 | |
Knowledge (linear) | −0.75*** | ||
Knowledge (quadratic) | 0.48** | ||
Information strategies | 0.49** | ||
Interaction (linear) | −0.31 | ||
Interaction (quadratic) | −0.62** | ||
N of sessions | −0.58*** | ||
Motivation × Motivation Strategies | 156.43*** | 32 | |
Motivation (linear) | −0.62*** | ||
Motivation (quadratic) | −0.86*** | ||
Motivational strategies | −0.06 | ||
Interaction (linear) | −0.12 | ||
Interaction (quadratic) | 0.48*** | ||
N of sessions | −0.03 | ||
Condom Use × Behavioral Strategies | 248.90*** | 72 | |
Condom use (linear) | −0.24*** | ||
Condom use (quadratic) | −0.38*** | ||
Behavioral strategies | 0.01 | ||
Interaction (linear) | 0.38*** | ||
Interaction (quadratic) | 0.21*** | ||
N of sessions | −0.27*** |
Note. QR = homogeneity accounted for by the regression model; β = standardized regression coefficient; k = number of effect sizes in the model.
p < .01.
p < .001.
All interaction terms involving the quadratic components were statistically significant. To interpret these interactions, we median split the intervention groups into high and low percentages of information, motivation, and behavioral strategies. We then regressed retention on the linear and quadratic components of knowledge, motivation, or condom use for each of the levels of the relevant intervention variable. A summary of these analyses appears in Table 7.
Table 7.
Effects of Knowledge, Motivation, and Condom Use on Retention as a Function of Percentage of Intervention Strategies (Fixed Effects)
High percentage |
Low percentage |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | QR | β | k | QR | β | k |
Interventions with information strategies | ||||||
Knowledge (linear) | 148.30*** | −1.41*** | 25 | 39.35*** | −0.51*** | 16 |
Knowledge (quadratic) | 0.13 | 0.20 | ||||
N of sessions | −0.85*** | −0.50*** | ||||
| ||||||
Interventions with motivation strategies | ||||||
Motivation (linear) | 37.20*** | −0.60*** | 21 | 112.98*** | −0.74*** | 11 |
Motivation (quadratic) | 0.23* | −1.06*** | ||||
N of sessions | −0.03 | 0.02 | ||||
| ||||||
Interventions with behavioral strategies | ||||||
Condom use (linear) | 57.74*** | 0.07* | 47 | 18.08*** | −0.14 | 27 |
Condom use (quadratic) | −0.13*** | −0.03 | ||||
N of sessions | −0.18*** | −0.33*** |
Note. QR = homogeneity accounted for by the regression model; β = standardized regression coefficient; k = number of effect sizes in the model.
p < .05.
p < .001.
Samples with high knowledge were more likely to drop out from the interventions with high percentage of information strategies than were samples with low knowledge (see Figure 5, Panel A1). A similar tendency was found with low percentage of information strategies, although the slope was less steep (see Figure 5, Panel A2). Of note, although the significant interaction with the quadratic term (see Table 6) suggested different quadratic slopes, neither of the quadratic terms reached significance. However, both linear and quadratic lines were plotted in the figures for interpretational purposes (Figure 5, Panels A1 and A2).
Figure 5.
Effects of prior knowledge, motivation, and condom use on retention in the context of intervention contents. A1: High percentage of information strategies. A2: Low percentage of information strategies. B1: High percentage of motivation strategies. B2: Low percentage of motivation strategies. C1: High percentage of behavioral strategies. C2: Low percentage of behavioral strategies.
When interventions had high percentage of motivation strategies, samples with low motivation had higher retention than did samples with high motivation, and samples with moderate motivation were similar to samples with high motivation (see Figure 5, Panel B1). In contrast, when the interventions had low percentage of motivation strategies, samples with moderate motivation had greater retention than did samples with both high and low motivation (see Figure 5, Panel B2). Thus, when the intervention was high in motivation strategies, the association between prior motivation and retention was negative in direction. However, when the intervention was low in motivation strategies, the inverted-⋃ pattern (see Table 4 and Figure 4, Panel B) was present.
The association between past condom use and retention was curvilinear for interventions with high percentage of behavioral strategies but nonsignificant for interventions with low percentage of behavioral strategies (see Table 7). That is, for interventions with a high percentage of behavioral-skills strategies, samples with high and low frequency of past condom use showed less retention than did samples with moderate past condom use (see Figure 5, Panel C1). In turn, interventions with high percentage of behavioral strategies did not increase retention of samples with low condom use.
In conclusion, people stayed in sessions when the interventions offered help to resolve deficiencies in knowledge and motivation. When samples contained people with low knowledge, retention was higher in predominantly information interventions. Likewise, when samples included participants with lower levels of motivation, retention was higher in predominantly motivation interventions. However, the tendency was not found in predominantly behavioral interventions. For behavioral interventions, the curvilinear pattern in Figure 4 persisted. That is, both people on the low and high ends of the behavioral continuum were likely to drop out from the interventions.
Discussion
Effects of Participants' Prior Characteristics on Retention in HIV-Prevention Intervention Programs
In this meta-analysis, we examined whether knowledge, motivation, and past behavior influence acceptance and retention in HIV-prevention intervention programs. We also examined how different intervention methods promote or hinder participation. Our findings were consistent with the possibility that self-validation and risk reduction both explained participants' attrition from HIV-prevention intervention programs. That is, in general, samples with lower need (i.e., high motivation or high condom use) were more likely to drop out from interventions than were samples with moderate or higher need (i.e., a step type of pattern).
The linear negative association between knowledge and retention suggested a risk-reduction motive, as did preferences for specific intervention strategies. Specifically, low-knowledge samples showed greater retention when the sample had a high proportion of information strategies. Similarly, low-motivation samples showed greater retention when the sample had a high proportion of motivation strategies. However, the pattern showed a curvilinear tendency for interventions with high behavioral content, with the greatest retention being achieved for samples with moderate past condom use. There is considerable risk reduction in the selection of information and motivation interventions, although both self-validation and risk-reduction patterns are apparent in the behavioral strategies.
Importantly, inclusion of some ethnic minorities and samples with higher risk correlated with lower retention rates. For example, samples with greater percentages of people from Asian backgrounds, as well as samples that included female sex workers and disadvantaged women, dropped out from the interventions more often than did samples with smaller percentages of these groups. Also, groups with greater percentages of women had lower retention than had groups with smaller percentages of women. On the basis of these findings, greater emphasis should be placed on reaching and retaining women, ethnic minorities, and other groups at disproportionate risk for HIV.
Correspondence With Information–Motivation–Behavioral-Skills Models and Stage Models of Preventive Behavior
The findings from our meta-analysis confirmed the usefulness of the information–motivation–behavioral-skills model in the area of HIV prevention. Extending this model, interventions were first divided into information-, motivation-, and behavioral-skill-based programs. Then, these components were shown to interact with baseline knowledge, motivation, and condom use. In our findings, individuals appear sensitive to the content of the preventive interventions and the degree to which these interventions matched their needs as recipients. For example, individuals sought interventions that could provide the knowledge and motivation they were lacking. In this regard, our meta-analysis suggested that the effectiveness of preventive interventions depends on the recipients' stage of behavior change (see Albarracín, 2002; Bandura, 1997; Catania, Kegeles, & Coates, 1990; Prochaska, DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992; Prochaska, Redding, Harlow, Rossi, &Velicer, 1994). Presumably, information strategies (i.e., knowledge of HIV/AIDS or information about condom use) prompt the desire to change even though people are not yet considering change. Similarly, motivation strategies (i.e., inducing favorable attitudes and norms) may be important at the initial stages and hence may attract participants with low motivation. In contrast, behavioral strategies may be best when participants have moderate condom use but not when condom use is either high or low.
Findings About Acceptance
Our meta-analysis also estimated initial acceptance by target participants, which is an important but often overlooked aspect of intervention effectiveness. In particular, a significant linear association between condom use and acceptance suggested that audiences may seek interventions that validate what they are doing. Even when this result is provocative, it was based on the characteristics of samples of commencers rather than on the characteristics of target samples, which are unknown. In the future, research must be conducted to determine who in a target sample actually enrolls in behavioral interventions. For instance, recording information about targeted participants should be standard when active recruitment takes place (e.g., referral of clients of an existing health clinic). Registering the number of targets and some basic demographic and behavioral information (e.g., age, gender, ethnicity, behavioral risks) would provide critical information about the ultimate reach of preventive interventions.
The extent of clients' knowledge of an intervention characteristic when they agree to participate is not completely clear. On the one hand, potential participants may have no clear idea of the nature of the intervention before participating in it. On the other hand, the ethical procedures that are required for research with human participants include informed consent. As a result, individuals are normally aware of the types of procedures used in the intervention and control groups to which they may be assigned. In any case, future research should include direct observation and manipulation of the information available to potential clients. For example, for participants enrolled in a study on health, researchers might observe whether the participants read HIV-relevant brochures, watch videos, or seek counseling during their visit. These various methods could be used to study acceptance as a function of the information with which the programs are introduced.
Strategies for People Who Do Not Attend Intervention Programs
This meta-analysis suggests various ways of retaining participants who start a preventive program. Knowledge-based interventions seem appropriate for people with low knowledge, and motivation-based interventions seem appropriate for people with low motivation. Importantly, however, low condom users dropped out from interventions with high behavioral content, increasing the need for retention strategies in this group.
One reason for the high attrition rate by low condom users may be low confidence in their ability to change. Thus, promoting their belief that they can change with the help of an intervention may increase retention. Another possibility is that low condom users actively resist change and require strategies that are not perceived as manipulative. For example, an audience with low condom use may be reminded that change is up to the participants and that interventions simply give them options. Such a strategy may be empowering and may signal respect for whatever decision the audience makes.
Implications for Future Research
To our knowledge, this study is the first to identify the influence of knowledge, motivation, and behavior on participation and retention in behavior change programs. In addition, the meta-analysis reveals that some disenfranchised groups, including women and behaviorally at-risk groups, are particularly unlikely to attend. Given the many inequalities that are associated with HIV transmission (del Rio, 2005; Quinn & Overbaugh, 2005), future research must be conducted to understand the barriers that reduce access of marginalized groups to efficacious HIV-prevention interventions.
Furthermore, in the synthesized studies, interventions were generally evaluated under conditions that reduced attrition and self-selection, usually by providing incentives for participation (e.g., money and other goods or the symbolic rewards of contributing to science; see Cook & Campbell, 1979). This situation implies that real acceptance and attrition may be even lower. In another meta-analysis of dropout rate on psychotherapy, the mean dropout rate was 47% (Wierzbicki & Pekarik, 1993). Thus, future work should address the behavior of potential participants when these incentives are not present.
One interesting finding of this meta-analysis is that, contrary to researchers' frequent concern, retention did not correlate with behavior change. This finding implies that samples that did change were as likely to drop out as samples that did not change, implying that attrition was unlikely to threaten detection of intervention efficacy. In addition, our meta-analysis clearly points to the need of paying attention to intervention effectiveness (see also Glasgow, Lichtenstein, & Marcus, 2003). Adequately addressing intervention reach, adoption, implementation, and maintenance will require analyzing acceptance and attrition as the main dependent measures in outcome studies.
Limitations of the Present Meta-Analysis
Despite the relevance of our findings for understanding participation and retention in HIV-prevention programs, this research integration has limitations. The most important limitation is the correlational nature of the analyses. Given this constraint, uncoded differences between studies and conditions may account for the effect of past experiences on participation and retention. For instance, individual differences other than prior condom use experiences (e.g., impulsivity) may explain the relation between prior experiences and retention. Other possible mediators may exist. Future research should further address whether self-validation and risk-reduction motives affect retention in HIV-prevention intervention programs.
Second, an assumption in the synthesized primary studies is that self-reported behaviors are accurate reflections of individuals' actual behaviors. The reliability of self-reports of sexual behavior has been established by the use of interpartner reports (Coates et al., 1986; Jaccard & Wan-Choi, 1995; McLaws, Oldenburg, Ross, & Cooper, 1990) and infection rates (CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group, 1997; Winkelstein et al., 1987). However, the accuracy of self-reports varies largely with the population and the behavior. For example, if groups have particularly high alcohol or drug consumption rates, reports by their members could be less reliable than reports by other persons. Similarly, self-reports could have different reliability for frequent or infrequent behaviors, depending on the standards people use to assess sexual events, or temporal factors, such as primacy or recency (for a review of such phenomena, see Wyer & Srull, 1989). In view of these possibilities, future work may include biological measures to gauge behavior.
Third, to test our hypotheses, we applied stringent exclusion criteria met by only 59 studies. This number of studies appears low compared with the 194 included in the most comprehensive past integration in this area (i.e., Albarracín et al., 2005). Although behavior change was comparable in both studies (see p. 8 of the present article; Albarracín et al., 2005), our results should be replicated as more reports accumulate. Moreover, we checked the composition of the sample and intervention characteristics in our study vis-à-vis those in Albarracín et al.'s (2005) study. These analyses revealed that the two meta-analyses are highly comparable and that the results of the present synthesis should be replicated with larger data sets when available. Nonetheless, the lower number of studies in the present meta-analysis implies that primary research should include more detailed reports of enrollment and attrition and characteristics of both commencers and completers.
Fourth, our analyses were based on sample-level correlations between the samples' characteristics and participation rates rather than on correlations between individual-level characteristics and participation. As mentioned before, future research should systematically study acceptance and retention at the individual level with methods that permit detecting biases that a global analysis of studies cannot detect. Nonetheless, until researchers study participation as an outcome, it appears that only a meta-analysis can determine if there are biases in acceptance and retention. For example, although dropout (a dichotomous variable that requires large sample sizes) is routinely analyzed as a function of participants' characteristics, more sensitive measures such as number of attended sessions are not routinely examined. Hence, a change in practices will be necessary before individual-level correlations with participation can be meta-analyzed.
Conclusion
An impressive number of studies have demonstrated the efficacy of interventions to persuade and train people to use condoms (see CDC Community Demonstration Projects Research Group, 1999; Cottler et al., 1998; Fogarty et al., 2001; Healton & Messeri, 1993; Kalichman, Carey, & Johnson, 1996; Kegeles, Hays, & Coates, 1996; Kelly et al., 1991, 1992; Kelly, Murphy, et al., 1997; Lauby, Smith, Stark, Person, & Adams, 2000; MacLachlan, Chimombo, & Mpeba, 1997; McCusker, Stoddard, Hindin, Garfield, & Frost, 1996; National Institute of Mental Health Multisite HIV Prevention Trial Group, 1998; O'Leary et al., 1998; Rotheram-Borus et al., 2001). However, these studies have never considered the actual reach of these programs. Our meta-analysis shows that acceptance of and retention in HIV-prevention interventions are not random but systematic. On one hand, this finding is disappointing because current interventions may not be reaching all audiences in need. On the other hand, this finding is encouraging because systematic approaches may increase acceptance and retention of efficacious programs in the future.
Acknowledgments
This research was funded by grants from the National Institutes of Health (K02-MH01861 and R01-NR08325). We thank Ece Kumkale and Allison Earl as well as the undergraduate research assistants working in Dolores Albarracín's lab during the academic years 2002–2006 for their invaluable assistance with this project. We also thank the attitudes lab at the Psychology Department at the University of Florida for a discussion of the ideas reported in this article.
Footnotes
The present meta-analysis is not part of earlier ones (Albarracín et al., 2005). Rather, a new literature search and a new coding scheme were designed, and the overall coverage was limited due to the need to extract precise information on sample size and means of knowledge, motivation, or past behavior for all participants entering the intervention.
J. D. Fisher and Fisher (1992) also included behavioral skills. However, measures of baseline behavioral skills were almost nonexistent in our database. Thus, we focused on condom use, knowledge, and motivation.
Ten conditions had reports of threat. However, threat had very low correlations with the other variables and was therefore excluded.
When the N at the pretest differed from the N at the posttest, the smaller N was used.
There is a separate article that deals with predicted effects of ethnicity. These effects represent more complex interactions and the introduction of a different conceptualization. Thus, they could not be treated here.
The interactions of condom use with knowledge and motivation were also examined for retention. Interactions were conducted only for the linear component. The interactions of condom use with both knowledge and motivation were significant at .001 (QR = 76.26, β = −1.44; and QR = 48.66, β = −1.98, respectively). With respect to knowledge, both low and high condom users showed negative correlations between prior knowledge and retention (low condom users, r = −.62, p = .001; and high condom users, r = −.44, p = .001). However, the tendency was stronger for low condom users (z = 2.48, p < .05). In terms of motivation, when condom use was low, those with higher motivation were more likely to drop out than those with lower motivation (r = −.44, p = .001). In contrast, when condom use was high, there was no difference in retention between low and high motivation individuals (r = .16, ns).
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
- Ajzen I. From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In: Kuhi J, Beckmann J, editors. Action-control: From cognition to behavior. Springer; Heidelberg, Germany: 1985. pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
- Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991;50:179–211. [Google Scholar]
- Ajzen I, Fishbein M. Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Prentice Hall; Englewood Cliffs, NJ: 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Ajzen I, Fishbein M. The influence of attitudes on behavior. In: Albarracín D, Johnson BT, Zanna MP, editors. Handbook of attitudes and attitude change: Basic principles. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 2005. pp. 173–221. [Google Scholar]
- Albarracín D. Cognition in persuasion: An analysis of information processing in response to persuasive communications. In: Zanna MP, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. Vol. 34. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 2002. pp. 61–130. [Google Scholar]
- Albarracín D, Gillette J, Earl A, Glasman LR, Durantini MR, Ho MH. A test of major assumptions about behavior change: A comprehensive look at the effects of passive and active HIV prevention interventions since the beginning of the epidemic. Psychological Bulletin. 2005;131:856–897. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albarracín D, Johnson BT, Fishbein M, Muellerleile P. Theories of reasoned action and planned behavior as models of condom use: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:142–161. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albarracín D, McNatt PS, Klein C, Ho R, Mitchell A, Kumkale GT. Persuasive communications to change actions: An analysis of behavioral and cognitive impact in HIV prevention. Health Psychology. 2003;22:166–177. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.22.2.166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen S, Tice J, Van de Perre P, Serufilira A, Hudes E, Nsengumuremyi F, et al. Effect of serotesting with counseling on condom use and seroconversion among HIV discordant couples in Africa. British Medical Journal. 1992;304:1605–1609. doi: 10.1136/bmj.304.6842.1605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker A, Heather N, Wodak A, Dixon J, Holt P. Evaluation of a cognitive-behavioral intervention for HIV prevention among injecting drug users. AIDS. 1993;7:247–256. doi: 10.1097/00002030-199302000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social cognitive theory and control over HIV infection. In: DiClemente R, Peterson J, editors. Preventing AIDS: Theories and methods of behavioral interventions. Plenum; New York: 1994. pp. 25–59. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Freeman; New York: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Becker BJ. Synthesizing standardized mean-change measures. British Journal of Mathematical & Statistical Psychology. 1988;41:257–278. [Google Scholar]
- Belcher L, Kalichman S, Topping M, Smith S, Emshoff J, Norris F, Nurss J. A randomised trial of a brief HIV risk reduction counseling intervention for women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1998;66:856–861. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.66.5.856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop GD, Wong ML. Designing sustainable health promotion: STD and HIV prevention in Singapore. In: MacLachlan M, editor. Cultivating health: Cultural perspectives on promoting health. Wiley; New York: 2001. pp. 199–226. [Google Scholar]
- Borgia P, Spadea T, Perucci CA, de Pascali V, Fano V, Schifano P, Abeni DDC. Limited effectiveness of a school-based HIV prevention campaign in Italy. European Journal of Public Health. 1997;7:411–417. [Google Scholar]
- Branson BM, Peterman TA, Cannon RO, Ransom R, Zaidi AA. Group counseling to prevent sexually transmitted diseases and HIV: A randomized controlled trial. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 1998;25:553–560. doi: 10.1097/00007435-199811000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown LK, Reynolds LA, Lourie KJ. A pilot HIV prevention program for adolescents in a psychiatric hospital. Psychiatric Services. 1997;48:531–533. doi: 10.1176/ps.48.4.531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryan AD, Aiken LS, West SG. Condom use: Evaluation of a theory-based intervention to prevent sexually transmitted diseases in young women. Health Psychology. 1996;15:371–382. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.15.5.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calsyn DA, Saxon AJ, Freeman G, Whittaker S. Ineffectiveness of AIDS education and HIV antibody testing in reducing high-risk behaviors among injection drug users. American Journal of Public Health. 1992;82:573–575. doi: 10.2105/ajph.82.4.573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey MP, Braaten LS, Maistro SA, Gleason JR, Forsyth AD, Durant LE, Jaworski BC. Using information, motivational enhancement, and skills training to reduce the risk of HIV infection for low-income urban women: A second randomized clinical trial. Health Psychology. 2000;19:3–11. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.19.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson KD, Schmidt FL. Impact of experimental design on effect size: Findings from the research literature on training. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1999;84:851–862. [Google Scholar]
- Catania JA, Kegeles SM, Coates TJ. Towards an understanding of risk behavior: An AIDS risk reduction model (ARRM) Health Education Quarterly. 1990;17:53–72. doi: 10.1177/109019819001700107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control HIV prevention strategic plan through. 2005 2005. Retrieved April 2005 from http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/resources/reports/psp/pdf/prev-strat-plan.pdf.
- The Centers for Disease Control Community Demonstration Projects Research Group . Documentation for the brief street intercept and coffee shop interview questionnaires. Author; Atlanta, GA: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- The Centers for Disease Control Community Demonstration Projects Research Group . Project Respect: Preliminary outcome study. Author; Atlanta, GA: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- The Centers for Disease Control Community Demonstration Projects Research Group Community level HIV intervention in 5 cities: Final outcome data from the CDC AIDS Community Demonstration Projects. American Journal of Public Health. 1999;89:336–345. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.3.336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheramie TA. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Mississippi State University; 1998. The effects of a peer-led HIV/AIDS education program on university students' knowledge, attitudes, and behavioral intentions concerning HIV and AIDS. [Google Scholar]
- Chernoff RA. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Southern California; 2000. An evaluation of a brief HIV/AIDS prevention intervention using normative feedback to promote risk reduction among sexually active college students. [Google Scholar]
- Choi KH, Lew S, Vittinghoff E, Catania JA, Barrett DC, Coates TJ. The efficacy of brief group counseling in HIV risk reduction among homosexual Asian and Pacific Islander men. AIDS. 1996;10:81–87. doi: 10.1097/00002030-199601000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clore GL, Byrne D. The reinforcement affect model of attraction. In: Huston TL, editor. Foundations of interpersonal attraction. Academic Press; New York: 1974. pp. 143–170. [Google Scholar]
- Coates RA, Soskilne CL, Calzavara L, Read SE, Fanning MM, Shepherd FA, et al. The reliability of sexual histories in AIDS-related research: Evaluation of an interview-administered questionnaire. Canadian Journal of Public Health. 1986;77:343–348. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook TD, Campbell DT. Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings. Houghton Mifflin; Boston: 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Cottler LB, Compton WM, Ben Abdallah A, Cunningham-Williams R, Abram F, Fichtenbaum C, et al. Peer-delivered interventions reduce HIV risk behaviors among out-of-treatment drug abusers. Public Health Reports. 1998;113:31–41. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- del Rio C. AIDS, the second wave. Archives of Medical Research. 2005;36:682–688. doi: 10.1016/j.arcmed.2005.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Clemente RJ, Wingood GM. A randomized controlled trial of an HIV sexual risk-reduction intervention for young African-American women. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1995;274:1271–1276. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diers JA. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Princeton University; 1999. Efficacy of a stage-based counseling intervention to reduce the risk of HIV in women. [Google Scholar]
- Dunlap WP, Cortina JM, Vaslow JB, Burke MJ. Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychological Methods. 1996;1:170–177. [Google Scholar]
- Ehrhardt AA, Exner TM, Hoffman S, Silberman I, Lew CS, Miller S, Levin B. A gender-specific HIV/STD risk reduction intervention for women in a health care setting: Short-and long-term results of a randomized clinical trial. AIDS Care. 2002;14:147–161. doi: 10.1080/09540120220104677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eldridge GD, St. Lawrence JS, Little CE, Shelby MC, Brasfield TL, Service JW, et al. Evaluation of an HIV risk reduction intervention for women entering inpatient substance abuse treatment. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1997;9:62–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Exner TM, Hoffman S, Parikin K, Leu CS, Erhardt AA. HIV counseling and testing: Women's experiences and the perceived role of testing as a prevention strategy. Perspectives of Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2002;34:76–83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Festinger L. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Row, Peterson; Evanston, IL: 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Festinger L. Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford University Press; Stanford, CA: 1964. [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Addison-Wesley; Reading, MA: 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher JD, Fisher WA. Changing AIDS-risk behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;111:455–474. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.3.455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher JD, Fisher WA. Theoretical approaches to individual-level change in HIV risk behavior. In: Peterson JL, DiClemente CC, editors. Handbook of HIV prevention. Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; New York: 2000. pp. 3–55. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher WA, Fisher JD, Harman J. The information-motivation-behavioral skills model: A general social psychological approach to understanding and promoting health behavior. In: Suls J, Wallston KA, editors. Social psychological foundations of health and illness. Blackwell Publishing; Malden, MA: 2003. pp. 82–106. [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald AM, Stanton BF, Terreri N, Shipena H, Li X, Kahihuata J, Ricardo IB, et al. Use of Western-based HIV risk-reduction interventions targeting adolescents in an African setting. Journal of Adolescent Health. 1999;25:52–61. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(98)00120-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floyd DL, Prentice-Dunn S, Rogers RW. A meta-analysis of research on protection motivation theory. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2000;30:407–429. [Google Scholar]
- Fogarty LA, Heilig C, Amstrong K, Cabral R, Galavotti C, Gielen AC, et al. Long term effectiveness of a peer-based intervention promoting condom and contraceptive use among HIV-positive and at-risk women. Public Health Reports. 2001;116:103–119. doi: 10.1093/phr/116.S1.103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ford N, Koestsawang S. A pragmatic intervention to promote condom use by female sex workers in Thailand. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 1999;77:888–894. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gama Vaz R, Gloyd S, Trindade R. The effect of peer education on STD and AIDS knowledge among prisoners in Mozambique. International Journal of STD and HIV. 1996;7:51–54. doi: 10.1258/0956462961917069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerrard M, Reis TJ. Retention of contraceptive and AIDS information in the classroom. The Journal of Sex Research. 1989;26:315–323. [Google Scholar]
- Gillmore MR, Morrison DM, Richey CA, Balassone ML, Gutierrez L, Farris M. Effects of a skill-based intervention to encourage condom use among high risk heterosexually active adolescents. Health Education Research. 1997;9:22–43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasgow RE, Lichtenstein E, Marcus AC. Why don't we see more translation of health promotion research to practice? Rethinking the efficacy-to-effectiveness transition. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93:1261–1267. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.8.1261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasman LR, Albarracín D. Models of condom use: A study of two cities in Argentina. AIDS and Behavior. 2003;7:183–193. doi: 10.1023/a:1023902509801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold RS, Rosenthal DA. Preventing unprotected anal intercourse in gay men: A comparison of two intervention techniques. International Journal of STD & AIDS. 1995;6:89–94. doi: 10.1177/095646249500600205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hackerman AE. Adolescent inpatient behavioral health clients: Risk factors and method of preventing an increase in HIV infection among youth. Journal of Humanistic Counseling Education and Development. 1999;11:28–44. [Google Scholar]
- Haddock CK, Rindskopf D, Shadish WR. Using odds ratios as effect sizes for meta-analysis of dichotomous data: A primer on methods and issues. Psychological Methods. 1998;3:339–353. [Google Scholar]
- Hamalainen S, Keinanen-Kiukaanniemi K. A controlled study of the effect of one lesson on the knowledge and attitudes of schoolchildren concerning HIV and AIDS. Health Educational Journal. 1992;51:135–138. [Google Scholar]
- Hannes JE. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Mississippi State University; 1998. An analysis of the efficacy of HIV/AIDS education with three methods of delivery: Interactive video disc, psychoeducational counseling and group education via pamphlets. [Google Scholar]
- Healton CG, Messeri P. The effect of video interventions on improving knowledge and treatment compliance in the sexually-transmitted disease clinic setting: Lesson for HIV health-education. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 1993;20:70–76. doi: 10.1097/00007435-199303000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press; Orlando, FL: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Hedges LV, Vevea JL. Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods. 1998;3:486–504. [Google Scholar]
- Hillman E, Hovell MF, Williams L, Hofstetter R, Burdyshaw C. Pregnancy, STDs, and AIDS prevention: Evaluation of new image teen theatre. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1991;3:328–340. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huebner DM, Davis MC, Nemeroff CJ, Aiken LS. The impact of internalized homophobia on HIV preventive interventions. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2002;30:327–348. doi: 10.1023/A:1015325303002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunter JE, Schmidt FL. Fixed effects vs. random effects meta-analysis models: Implications for cumulative research knowledge. International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 2000;8:275–292. [Google Scholar]
- Huszti HC, Clopton JR, Mason PJ. Acquired immunodeficiency syndrome educational program: Effects on adolescents' knowledge and attitudes. Pediatrics. 1989;84:986–994. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaccard J, Wan-Choi K. A paradigm for studying the accuracy of self-reports of risk behavior relevant to AIDS: Empirical perspectives on stability, recall basis, and transitory influences. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1995;25:1831–1858. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson DJ, Rakwar JP, Richardson BA, Madaliya K, Chohan BH, Bwayo JJ, et al. Decreased incidence of sexually transmitted diseases among trucking company workers in Kenya: Results of a behavioural risk-reduction programme. AIDS. 1997;11:903–909. doi: 10.1097/00002030-199707000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaworski BC, Carey MP. Effects of a brief, theory-based STD-prevention program for female college students. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2001;29:417–425. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(01)00271-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jemmott JB, Jemmott LS, Fong GT, McCaffree K. Reducing HIV risk-associated sexual behavior among African-American adolescents: Testing the generality of intervention effects. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1999;27:161–187. doi: 10.1007/BF02503158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jemmott JB, Jemmott LS, Spears H, Hewitt N, Cruz-Collins M. Self-efficacy, hedonistic expectancies, and condom-use intentions among inner-city black adolescent women: A social cognitive approach to AIDS risk behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health. 1992;13:512–519. doi: 10.1016/1054-139x(92)90016-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins RA, Jenkins PR, Nannis ED, McKee KT, Temoshok LR. Correlates of human immunodeficiency virus infection risk behavior in male attendees of a clinic of sexually transmitted disease. Clinical Infectious Diseases. 2000;30:923–929. doi: 10.1086/313744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson BT, Carey MP, Marsh KL, Levin KD, Scott-Sheldon LA. Interventions to reduce sexual risk for the human immunodeficiency virus in adolescents, 1985–2000: A research synthesis. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2003;157:381–388. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.157.4.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson JA, Sellew JF, Campbell AE, Haskell EG, Gay AA, Bell BJ. A program using medical students to teach high school students about AIDS. Journal of Medical Education. 1988;63:522–530. doi: 10.1097/00001888-198807000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalichman SC, Carey MP, Johnson BT. Prevention of sexually transmitted HIV infection: A meta-analytic review of the behavioral outcome literature. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1996;18:6–15. doi: 10.1007/BF02903934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalichman SC, Rompa D, Coley B. Experimental component analysis of a behavioral HIV-AIDS prevention intervention for inner-city women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:687–693. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.4.687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalichman SC, Rompa D, Coley B. Lack of positive outcomes from a cognitive-behavioral HIV and AIDS prevention intervention for inner-city men: Lessons from a controlled pilot study. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1997;9:299–313. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamb ML, Fishbein M, Douglas JM, Rhodes F, Rogers J, Bolan G, et al. Efficacy of risk-reduction counseling to prevent human immunodeficiency virus and sexually transmitted diseases. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1998;280:1161–1167. doi: 10.1001/jama.280.13.1161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kegeles SM, Hays RB, Coates TJ. The Mpowerment project: A community-level HIV prevention intervention for young gay men. American Journal of Public Health. 1996;86:1129–1136. doi: 10.2105/ajph.86.8_pt_1.1129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly JA, McAuliffe TL, Sikkema KJ, Murphy DA, Somlai AM, Mahy GM, et al. Reduction in risk behavior among adults with severe mental illness who learned to advocate for HIV prevention. Psychiatric Services. 1997;48:1283–1288. doi: 10.1176/ps.48.10.1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly JA, Murphy DA, Sikkema KJ, McAuliffe TL, Roffman RA, Solomon LJ, et al. Randomized, controlled, community-level HIV-prevention intervention for sexual-risk behavior among homosexual men in US cities. The Lancet. 1997;350:1500–1505. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(97)07439-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly JA, St. Lawrence JS, Diaz YE, Stevenson LY, Hauth AC, Brasfield TL, et al. HIV risk behavior reduction following intervention with key opinion leaders of population: An experimental analysis. American Journal of Public Health. 1991;81:168–171. doi: 10.2105/ajph.81.2.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly JA, St. Lawrence JS, Hood HV, Brasfield TL. Behavioral intervention to reduce AIDS risk activities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1989;57:60–67. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.57.1.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly JA, St. Lawrence JS, Stevenson LY, Hauth AC, Kalichman SC, Diaz YE, et al. Community AIDS/HIV risk reduction: The effects of endorsements by popular people in three cities. American Journal of Public Health. 1992;82:1483–1489. doi: 10.2105/ajph.82.11.1483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim N, Stanton B, Li X, Dickersin K, Galbraith J. Effectiveness of the 40 adolescent AIDS-risk reduction interventions: A quantitative review. Journal of Adolescent Health. 1997;20:204–215. doi: 10.1016/S1054-139X(96)00169-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kipke MD. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Yeshiva University; 1990. An evaluation of an AIDS risk reduction education & skills training program. [Google Scholar]
- Lauby JL, Smith PJ, Stark M, Person B, Adams J. A community-level HIV prevention intervention for inner-city women: Results of the Women and Infants Demonstration Projects. American Journal of Public Health. 2000;90:216–222. doi: 10.2105/ajph.90.2.216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- MacLachlan M, Chimombo M, Mpeba N. AIDS Education for youth through active learning: A school-based approach from Malawi. International Journal of Educational Development. 1997;17:41–50. [Google Scholar]
- Mansfield CJ, Conroy ME, Emans SJ, Woods ER. A pilot study of AIDS education and counseling of high-risk adolescents in an office-setting. Journal of Adolescent Health. 1993;14:115–119. doi: 10.1016/1054-139x(93)90095-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margolin A, Avants SK, Warburton LA, Hawkins KA, Shi J. Randomized clinical trial of a manual-guided risk reduction intervention for HIV-positive injection drug users. Health Psychology. 2003;22:223–228. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCusker J, Stoddard AM, Hindin RN, Garfield FB, Frost R. Changes in HIV risk behavior following alternative residential programs of drug abuse treatment and AIDS education. Annals of Epidemiology. 1996;6:119–125. doi: 10.1016/1047-2797(95)00128-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaws ML, Oldenburg B, Ross MW, Cooper DA. Sexual behaviour in AIDS-related research: Reliability and validity of recall and diary measures. Journal of Sex Research. 1990;27:265–281. [Google Scholar]
- McMahon RC, Malow RM, Jennings TE, Gómez CJ. Effects of a cognitive-behavioral HIV prevention intervention among HIV negative male substance abusers in VA residential treatment. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2001;13:91–107. doi: 10.1521/aeap.13.1.91.18921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mize SJ, Robinson BE, Bockting WO, Scheltema KE. Meta-analysis of the effectiveness of HIV prevention interventions for women. AIDS Care. 2002;14:163–180. doi: 10.1080/09540120220104686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute of Mental Health Multisite HIV Prevention Trial Group Social-cognitive theory mediators of behavior change in the National Institute of Mental Health Multisite HIV Prevention Trial. Health Psychology. 1998;20:369–376. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Leary A, Ambrose TK, Raffaelli M, Mailbach E, Jemmott LS, Jemmott JB, III, et al. Effects of an HIV risk reduction project on sexual risk behavior of low-income STD patients. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1998;10:483–492. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Leary A, Jemmott LS, Goodhart F, Gebelt J. Effects of an institutional AIDS prevention intervention: Moderation by gender. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1996;8:516–528. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr DP, Langefeld CD, Katz BP, Caine VA. Behavioral intervention to increase condom use among high-risk female adolescents. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1996;128:287–295. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(96)70413-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perlini AH, Ward C. Prevention interventions: The effects of role-play and behavioural commitment on knowledge and attitudes. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science. 2000;32:133–143. [Google Scholar]
- Ploem C, Byers ES. The effects of two AIDS risk-reduction interventions on heterosexual college women's AIDS-related knowl edge, attitudes and condom use. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality. 1997;9:1–24. doi: 10.1300/j056v09n01_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prendergast ML, Urada D, Podus D. Meta-analysis of HIV risk-reduction interventions within drug abuse treatment programs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2001;69:389–405. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.3.389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC, Norcross JC. In search of how people change: Application to addictive behaviors. American Psychologist. 1992;47:1002–1114. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.47.9.1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, Redding CA, Harlow LL, Rossi JS, Velicer WF. The transtheoretical model of change and HIV prevention: A review. Health Education Quarterly. 1994;21:471–486. doi: 10.1177/109019819402100410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quinn TC, Overbaugh J. HIV/AIDS in women: An expanding epidemic. Science. 2005 Jun 10;308:1582–1583. doi: 10.1126/science.1112489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quirk ME, Godkin MA, Schwenzfeier E. Evaluation of two AIDS prevention interventions for inner-city adolescent and young adult women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1993;9:21–26. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raj A, Amaro H, Cranston K, Martin B, Cabral H, Navarro A, et al. Is a general women's health promotion program as effective as an HIV-intensive prevention program in reducing HIV risk among Hispanic women? Public Health Reports. 2001;116:599–607. doi: 10.1093/phr/116.6.599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW. Random effects models. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, editors. The handbook of research synthesis. Russell Sage Foundation; New York: 1994. pp. 301–321. [Google Scholar]
- Reeder GD, Pryor JB, Harsh L. Activity and similarity in safer sex workshops led by peer educators. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1997;9:77–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rigby K, Brown M, Anagnostou P, Ross MW, Rosser BRS. Shock tactics to counter AIDS: The Australian experience. Psychology and Health. 1989;3:145–159. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenstock IM, Strecher VJ, Becker MH. The health belief model and HIV risk behavior change. In: DiClemente RJ, Peterson JL, editors. Preventing AIDS: Theories and methods of behavioral interventions. AIDS prevention and mental health. Plenum Press; New York: 1994. pp. 5–24. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R. Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118:183–192. [Google Scholar]
- Roth DL, Stewart KE, Clay OJ, van der Straten A, Karita E, Allen S. Sexual practices of HIV discordant and concordant couples on Rwanda: Effects of a testing and counseling programme for men. International Journal of STD & AIDS. 2001;12:181–188. doi: 10.1258/0956462011916992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rotheram-Borus MJ, Lee MB, Murphy DA, Futterman D, Duan N, Birnbaum JM, et al. Efficacy of a preventive intervention for youth living with HIV. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91:400–405. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.3.400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadler CS. The effect of instructor gender on the knowledge, feelings, behavior and self efficacy related to AIDS prevention education of African-American women. Journal of National Black Nurses Association. 2002;13:15–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schares DK. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Northern Iowa; 1994. AIDS education: The effect of three instructional methods on retention of changes in knowledge, attitudes, and intention to act. [Google Scholar]
- Schinke SP, Gordo AN, Weston RE. Self-instruction to prevent HIV infection among African-American and Hispanic-American adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1990;58:432–436. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.58.4.432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherr L. An evaluation of the UK government health education campaign on AIDS. Psychology and Health. 1987;1:61–72. [Google Scholar]
- Shrier LR, Ancheta R, Goodman E, Chiou VM, Lyden MR, Emans SJ. Randomized-controlled trial of a safer-sex intervention for high-risk adolescent girls. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 2001;155:73–79. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.155.1.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel DM, Aten MJ, Roghmann KJ, Enaharo M. Early effects of a school-based human immunodeficiency virus infection and sexual risk prevention intervention. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 1998;152:961–970. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.152.10.961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simbayl LC, Kalichman SC, Skinner D, Jooste S, Cain D, Cherry C, et al. Theory based HIV risk reduction counseling for sexually transmitted infections clinic patients in Cape Town, South Africa. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 2004;31:727–733. doi: 10.1097/01.olq.0000145849.35655.f1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slonim-Nevo V. The effect of HIV/AIDS prevention intervention for Israeli adolescents in residential centers: Results at 12-month follow-up. Social Work Research. 2001;25:71–84. [Google Scholar]
- Solomon MZ, DeJong W. Preventing AIDS and other STDs through condom promotion: A patient education intervention. American Journal of Public Health. 1989;79:453–458. doi: 10.2105/ajph.79.4.453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanton BF, Li X, Kahihuata J, Fitzgerald AM, Neumbo S, Kanduuombe G, et al. Increased protected sex and abstinence among Namibian youth following a HIV risk-reduction intervention: A randomized, longitudinal study. AIDS. 1998;12:2473–2480. doi: 10.1097/00002030-199818000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanton BF, Li X, Ricardo I, Galbraith J, Feigelman S, Kaljee L. A randomized, controlled effectiveness trial of an AIDS prevention program for low-income African-American youths. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine. 1996;150:363–372. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1996.02170290029004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valdiserri RO, Lyter DW, Kingsley LA, Leviton LC, Schofield JW, Huggins J, et al. The effect of group education on improving attitudes about AIDS risk reduction. New York State Journal of Medicine. 1987;87:272–278. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valente TW, Barath U. An evaluation of the use of drama to communicate HIV/AIDS information. AIDS Education and Prevention. 1999;11:203–211. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Griensen GJP, Limanonda B, Ngaokeow S, Ayuthaya SIN, Poshyachinda V. Evaluation of a targeted HIV prevention programme among female commercial sex workers in the south of Thailand. Sexually Transmitted Infections. 1998;74:54–58. doi: 10.1136/sti.74.1.54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang MC, Bushman BJ. Integrating results through meta-analytic review using SAS software. SAS Institute; Cary, NC: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Warner BD, Leukefeld CG. Assessing the differential impact of an HIV prevention intervention: Who's putting the message into practice? AIDS Education and Prevention. 2001;13:479–494. doi: 10.1521/aeap.13.6.479.21433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinhardt LS, Carey MP, Carey KB, Verdecias RN. Increasing assertiveness skills to reduce HIV risk among women living with a severe and persistent mental illness. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1998;66:680–684. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.66.4.680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wenger NS, Linn LS, Epstein M, Shapiro MF. Reduction of high-risk sexual behavior among heterosexuals undergoing HIV antibody testing: A randomized clinical trial. American Journal of Public Health. 1991;81:1580–1585. doi: 10.2105/ajph.81.12.1580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wierzbicki M, Pekarik G. A meta-analysis of psychotherapy dropout. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 1993;24:190–195. [Google Scholar]
- Winkelstein W, Jr., Lyman DM, Padian N, Grant R, Samuel M, Wiley JA, et al. Sexual practices and risk of infection by the human immunodeficiency virus: The San Francisco Men's Health Study. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1987;16:321–325. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wyer RS, Srull TK. Memory and cognition in its social context. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1989. [Google Scholar]