Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Mar 19.
Published in final edited form as: J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010 Jan 5;51(7):799–808. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02207.x

Maternal emotion coaching, adolescent anger regulation, and siblings’ externalizing symptoms

Joann Wu Shortt 1, Mike Stoolmiller 1, Jessica N Smith-Shine 1, J Mark Eddy 1, Lisa Sheeber 2
PMCID: PMC3601745  NIHMSID: NIHMS191034  PMID: 20059622

Abstract

Background

Increases in externalizing behaviors during the transition to adolescence may put children at risk for developing mental disorders and related problems. Although children’s ability to regulate their emotions appears to be a key factor influencing risk for maladjustment, emotion processes during adolescence remain understudied. In this longitudinal study, we examined a multi-level mediational model in which emotion coaching by parents was posited to influence the ability of adolescents to regulate their emotions, which in turn influences their expression of problem behaviors.

Methods

We recruited a representative community sample of 244 families with biological sibling pairs comprising a child in late elementary school and a child in middle school. Maternal meta-emotion interviews were coded for mother emotion coaching and adolescent difficulty with anger. Mothers also completed questionnaires on adolescent irritability. Ratings of adolescent problem behaviors were obtained from mother and teacher questionnaires completed at two time points. Using structural equation modeling, constructs were partitioned into components across older and younger siblings to examine shared and nonshared variance and contextual effects.

Results

Cross-sectional data indicated that mothers’ emotion coaching of anger was related to better anger regulation in adolescent siblings, which was, in turn related to less externalizing behavior. Although support for mediational effects was limited in the longitudinal data, both older and younger siblings’ difficulties in regulating anger predicted adolescent externalizing behavior three years later. Additional longitudinal predictors of externalizing behavior were observed for younger siblings. In particular, emotion coaching of anger by mothers was associated with decreased externalizing behavior, while conversely, older siblings’ externalizing behavior was associated with increased externalizing behavior in the younger siblings over time.

Conclusions

The findings highlight the importance of considering family emotion processes in understanding adolescent problem behavior. Both maternal emotion coaching of adolescent anger and adolescent difficulty in regulating anger influenced adolescent externalizing behavior. Emotion coaching interventions seem worthy of consideration for enhancing the impact of prevention and intervention programs targeting youth externalizing behaviors.

Keywords: Parenting, anger, emotion regulation, adolescence, externalizing problem behavior, siblings


Adolescence is a time of increased risk for externalizing behaviors and related disorders (Steinberg, 2004). Externalizing behaviors include academic failure, school drop out, substance use, and delinquent peer affiliation (e.g., Dishion, Nelson, & Bullock, 2004; Eccles et al., 1993; Petersen, Leffert, Graham, Alwin, & Ding, 1997). For a subset of youth, these behaviors lead to the genesis of serious clinical disorders, such as conduct disorder (Offord, Adler, & Boyle, 1986). Moreover, for many adolescents, negative behavioral patterns evident during adolescence continue into adulthood (Shortt, Capaldi, Dishion, Bank, & Owen, 2003), with serious ramifications for later adjustment (Lipsey & Derzon, 1998). It is likely that the numerous biological, social, and cognitive changes experienced during adolescence (Steinberg, 2008), along with the associated increases in negative affect which occur during this developmental period (Larson & Sheeber, 2008), contribute to these difficulties.

A significant body of research has examined factors that render some children particularly likely to demonstrate externalizing behaviors (e.g., Reid, Patterson, & Snyder, 2002). In particular, studies with both community and clinical samples have repeatedly documented the association between adverse family contexts, problematic social interactional processes, and externalizing behaviors and disorders during childhood and adolescence (e.g., Burke, Pardini, & Loeber, 2008; Reid et al., 2002). Moreover, attention has been directed increasingly on the role that children’s and adolescent’s ability to regulate their own emotions plays in the development of these difficulties (e.g., Chaplin & Cole, 2005; Cicchetti, Ackerman, & Izard, 1995). Indeed it appears that family processes, particularly those that relate to emotion socialization, may operate to promote or hamper the development of the emotion regulatory abilities necessary for long term healthy adjustment (e.g., Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, 2007).

In this study, we examined the associations between positive maternal socialization behavior, adolescents’ ability to regulate anger, and adolescents’ externalizing behavior. We focused, in particular, on maternal emotion coaching, a socialization process wherein parents provide guidance in understanding and coping with emotions. Coaching behaviors that are respectful of the adolescents’ emotional experience include responses such as offering comfort, providing guidance about the nature of emotions and appropriate ways to express them, setting behavioral limits, and discussing goals and strategies for coping with emotionally arousing situations (Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1997).

Though prior work has primarily focused on younger children (e.g., Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998), more recent studies suggest that parents’ emotion-socialization behavior continues to be important during adolescence. For example, punishing and neglectful responses by parents in response to adolescent affect have been shown to be associated with adolescent behavioral problems (Klimes-Dougan et al., 2007), and further, parental negative expressiveness is associated with higher levels of adolescent externalizing symptoms (Stocker, Richmond, Rhoades, & Kiang, 2007). Conversely, maternal emotion coaching is associated with less aversive parent-adolescent interactions (Katz & Hunter, 2007).

Emerging evidence suggests that the association between parental socialization behavior and externalizing problems may be mediated by difficulties in child and adolescent emotion regulation. Valiente and colleagues (2007) reported that school-aged children whose parents responded more supportively to their negative emotions demonstrated both better effortful control and less externalizing behavior. In one of the few longitudinal studies available, maternal positive expressivity, assessed when the child was 9 years old, was associated with better child effortful control at age 11, which in turn predicted fewer externalizing problems at age 13, controlling for stability effects (Eisenberg et al., 2003).

Finally, evidence suggests that externalizing behaviors are associated with deficits in the regulation of anger. For example, the tendency to display angry affect has been found to predict externalizing behavior in young children over time (Rydell, Berlin, & Bohlin, 2003). Notably, this propensity toward anger has also been associated with externalizing behavior in adolescent boys (Keltner, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1995). These findings are consistent with theorizing that specific emotions co-occur with types of child psychopathology (e.g., Muris & Ollendick, 2005; Rothbart & Bates, 1998) and may involve different neurological motivational systems (Fowles, 1987; Gray, 1994).

Unfortunately, there are relatively few studies of emotional processes, as they relate to either problem behavior or family functioning, during the adolescent period (Morris et al., 2007). This is notable because the confluence of biological and social changes makes adolescence a period of increased vulnerability. In particular, increased emotionality and independence that characterize adolescence, combined with the still developing neural regulatory structures, account in part for the increases in externalizing behavior that emerge at this juncture (Steinberg, 2008). However, a better understanding of the role parents play in adolescent emotional development could render this developmental period one of increased opportunity for adolescents to learn effective emotion regulation strategies as well. Though adolescents have increased competencies to regulate their own emotions (Cole & Kaslow, 1988), they are not yet independent in this regard. Hence, the nature of parental responses to adolescent affect, and in particular, parental ability to scaffold the developing regulatory skills of their adolescent children may be an important predictor of adolescent externalizing behavior.

Present Study

The purpose of the study was to examine the hypothesis that maternal emotion coaching would be associated with better anger regulation on the part of the adolescent, which in turn would be associated with lower levels of adolescent externalizing behaviors, both concurrently and over time. We focused on variability in levels of externalizing behavior, rather than on diagnostic categories because a substantial body of research suggests that externalizing behaviors exist, and are best considered, on a continuum (e.g., McMahon & Forehand, 1988; Murrie et al., 2007; Reid et al., 2002). Moreover, associations between family processes and externalizing behavior appear to be similar in clinical and community samples (e.g., Hinshaw, 2002). As such, we recruited a community sample of adolescents, rather than a clinical sample, in order to provide a wide distribution in each of the constructs of interest. As shown in Figure 1, the proximal predictor of externalizing behavior was difficulty regulating anger, an aspect of emotional functioning which maps onto the externalizing dimension of psychopathology. The distal predictor was maternal emotion coaching of adolescents’ anger. We hypothesized that the association between maternal emotion coaching and adolescent externalizing behavior would be mediated by adolescent anger regulation.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Conceptual model

This study has a number of methodological strengths. First, data on externalizing behavior were collected at two time points, enabling us to examine the association of predictor variables both concurrently and prospectively, and provided by teachers as well as mothers, yielding a valuable source of independent information. Second, the use of a sibling design facilitated the differentiation of shared from nonshared family influences (e.g., Reiss, 1993). The family environment comprises a number of social interactional micro-systems (e.g., Dunn & McGuire, 1994) such that the nature of parenting that children experience may differ considerably within the same family. Nonetheless, the vast majority of studies involve only one child per family (Plomin, Asbury, & Dunn, 2001), and as such, underestimate the complexity of family processes. To the extent that nonshared influences predominate in child and adolescent development, it would suggest that within family processes and differential experiences specific to each child will have implications for future work on emotion socialization (e.g., Reiss, Neiderhiser, Hetherington, & Plomin, 2000).

Methods

Sample

Participants were 244 mothers and sibling pairs from families with same-sex biological siblings (122 brothers). Three collaborating school districts from a moderately sized metropolitan area used their enrollment databases to identify families with: (a) a child in middle school, (b) a same-sex sibling in elementary school, and (c) a mother residing in the home. After an initial letter from the school, families were removed from the sampling pool if they informed the school that they did not want to be contacted by study staff. Remaining families received a letter and phone call from study staff, which was followed by a home recruitment visit for interested families. Of the 448 families whose names were provided by the school districts, 1% could not be contacted and 18% were not eligible (e.g., siblings did not share two biological parents). Of the 364 eligible families, 67% chose to participate and completed the assessment at Time 1 (T1). Informed consent was obtained at the time of the home recruitment visit. At Time 2 (T2), approximately 3 years after the initial assessment, 215 families (111 brothers) participated, yielding a retention rate of 88% (4% could not be contacted and 8% declined to participate). None of the variables measured in the study significantly predicted attrition at T2.

Demographics

At (T1), older siblings were 13.33 years old (SD = .45) and younger siblings were 10.87 years old (SD = .59). Mothers reported that 7% of the adolescents were of Hispanic ethnicity, 83% were Euro-American, 16% were of mixed race, and less than 1% each were Native American or Asian American/Pacific Islander. These percentages reflect that of the community from which the sample was recruited, the majority of siblings (74%) lived with both parents. Regarding education, 21% of the mothers had attained a high school education or less, 58% had attended at least some college, and 21% had received graduate-level training. Approximately 19% of families were receiving some form of public assistance (e.g., food stamps; unemployment insurance).

Procedures

Families participated in lab visits at both T1 and T2. T1 data collection relevant to this report included videotaped interviews with mothers and mother-report measures of adolescent irritability (Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire; EATQ, Capaldi & Rothbart, 1992) and adolescent externalizing behavior (Child Behavior Checklist; CBCL, Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). The CBCL was repeated at T2. Each measure was completed for older and younger siblings. Each adolescent’s teacher also provided reports on the Teacher Report Form of the CBCL at both time points.

Meta-Emotion Interview

The Parent Meta-Emotion Interview (MEI; Katz & Gottman, 1986) was administered at T1. It is a semi-structured interview comprising a series of open-ended questions about parents’ thoughts and feelings regarding their own and their adolescents’ emotional experiences (e.g., what is it like for you when your adolescent is angry?). Interviews with the parents were video-recorded for coding by trained research assistants using an adaptation (Smith, Thomas, & Nederland, 2002) of the Meta-Emotion Coding System (Hooven, Katz, & Mittman, 1996). In addition to the micro-analytic coding, both interviewers and coders provided macro global ratings of parent socialization behavior and adolescent emotion regulation. Coded data as well as the interviewer and coder global ratings were used to derive latent measures of both mothers’ coaching of adolescent anger, and adolescent difficulty regulating anger as described below. Inter-rater reliability, computed on 15% of the data, ranged from r = .71 to r = .75 for the coded interviews and r = .50 to r = .66 for the global ratings.

Constructs

Mother coaching of adolescent’s anger

Two indicators – a coder indicator tapping coded items and coder ratings, and an interviewer indicator tapping interviewer ratings, composed a latent Emotion Coaching construct. The coder indicator was the average of two measured variables: 1) the mean ratings on micro-coded MEI items forming an Emotion Coaching Scale (e.g., when adolescent is upset, mother talks about situation and emotion), and 2) the mean of four global ratings of emotion coaching (e.g., mother’s remediation technique for adolescent’s anger is to approach). The Emotion Coaching Scale was formed by applying a principal components analysis to coded items tapping both acceptance and coaching of adolescent’s anger. This analysis resulted in a single 11-item factor that accounted for 41% of the variance for older siblings and 46% for younger siblings which had alphas of .85 and .87 for older and younger siblings respectively. The scale based on the global coder ratings had alphas of .80 for older siblings and .77 for younger siblings. The interviewer indicator was the mean of two interviewer global ratings (i.e., mother talked about responding to adolescent’s anger in appropriate and positive ways; mother talked about adolescent going to mother about anger to talk, to get comfort, etc.).

Adolescent’s difficulty regulating anger

Two indicators were used to compose a latent construct reflecting adolescent Anger Regulation. The first indicator, “anger difficulty”, was computed as the mean of three measured variables: 1) the mean of coded MEI items forming an Anger Regulation Scale (e.g., mother is concerned about adolescent’s experience and expression of anger), 2) a single MEI coder global rating (i.e., adolescent has difficulty regulating anger), and 3) the mean of two interviewer global ratings (i.e., mother talked about adolescent having difficulty managing anger; mother talked about adolescent having difficulty getting over feeling anger). The Anger Regulation Scale was formed by applying a principal components analysis to coded items tapping anger regulation. This analysis resulted in a single 7-item factor that accounted for approximately 55% of the variance for older siblings and 54% for younger siblings, with internal consistencies of approximately α = .86 in both groups. The second indicator called was “irritability” as assessed by the EATQ irritability scale (e.g., adolescent gets very frustrated when making a mistake in schoolwork). The internal consistency of the 8-item irritability scale was .73 and .76 for older and younger siblings respectively.

Adolescent’s externalizing problem behavior at T1 and T2

Mother and teacher reports on the broad-band externalizing scale of the CBCL (comprised of rule-breaking and aggressive behavior scales) were included as indicators of a latent construct labeled Externalizing. Internal consistency for the scales across time, siblings, and reporters ranged from .64 to .94. Scores were square root transformed for the modeling analyses.

Statistical Methods

Modeling analyses were conducted to determine the effects of the distal predictor (Emotion Coaching) on the mediator (Anger Regulation), and in turn, the effects of the mediator (Anger Regulation) on the outcome (Externalizing; MacKinnon, Lochwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002; MacKinnon, 2008). Gender was included as a control variable. All structural equation models were estimated using full information maximum likelihood and included participants with partial data. Missing data were assumed to be missing at random conditional on covariates included in the model, the so called MAR assumption. MAR implies that variables included in the model can predict attrition, but that there are no other omitted predictors of both missingness and outcome variables. Model-based likelihood methods that assume MAR are the recommended standard (Schafer & Graham, 2002) because the older alternative methods (complete case analysis or listwise deletion), based on an even more unrealistic assumption of missing completely at random, tend to perform worse in terms of bias and power. Estimation was carried out using Mplus (Muthen & Muthen, 2006). Missingness by variable ranged from 0% to 18% and was highest for the teacher-report measure at T2. On a pairwise basis (i.e., covariance coverage) the highest amount of missingness was 20%. Each construct was partitioned across older and younger siblings and by source in order to examine shared versus nonshared and contextual effects. The decomposition for Externalizing is shown in Figure 2. The inclusion of both mother- and teacher-report indices on older and younger siblings allows partitioning of variance into that which is: (1) shared across siblings and common across context (home versus school), (2) nonshared for siblings and common across context, (3) shared across siblings but unique to context, and (4) nonshared for siblings and unique to context. Means and standard deviations for each construct are provided in Table 1 and correlations between constructs are provided in Table 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Decomposition of adolescent externalizing behavior

Table 1.

Means and standard deviations of constructs by sibling status

Older sibling Younger sibling
Emotion Coaching construct scores 3.51 (.66) 3.53 (.66)
Anger Regulation construct scores 2.87 (.70) 2.96 (.75)
T1 Externalizing construct scores 4.63 (5.10) 4.28 (5.18)
 T-scores, mother report 49.24 (9.90) 49.11 (9.93)
 T-scores, teacher report 49.38 (8.03) 49.38 (8.60)
T2 Externalizing construct scores 4.71 (5.47) 5.24 (5.81)
 T-scores, mother report 50.35 (9.93) 49.67 (9.99)
 T-scores, teacher report 48.16 (6.86) 49.37 (8.13)

Table 2.

Construct correlations

Emotion Coaching Anger Regulation T1 Externalizing T2 Externalizing
Emotion Coaching ----- -.36*** -.31*** -.19**
Anger Regulation -.23*** ----- .49*** .39***
T1 Externalizing -.23*** .44*** ----- .62***
T2 Externalizing -.19*** .37*** .74*** -----

Note. Older sibling data are above the diagonal and younger sibling data are below the diagonal in italics.

**

p < .01;

**

p < .001

Results

Overall Model Fit

The overall structural model included both cross-sectional and longitudinal components in order to examine the relations between maternal emotion coaching, adolescent anger regulation, and adolescent externalizing behavior both within and across time. In addition to hypothesized associations, five additional parameters were added to improve model fit, resulting in a model that fit the data well (χ2 = 152.27, df = 151, p = .456, CFI (Bentler, 1990) = .999, TLI (Bentler & Bonnet, 1980) = .999, RMSEA (Steiger, 1990) = .006, RMSEA 90%, CI = 0, .031). Three of these parameters were for strong but unanticipated effects of T1 predictors on T2 teacher-rated externalizing behavior in younger siblings, as described in the longitudinal model below. Additionally, the model was modified to allow a larger nonshared teacher report variance for younger relative to older siblings for T2 externalizing. Finally, negative error variances for T2 shared teacher reported externalizing and T1 and T2 shared (mother and teacher reported) externalizing were fixed to zero, which resulted in standardized effects that were 1.0 and necessitated removing T1 shared anger regulation on T2 shared externalizing.

Cross-Sectional Structural Model

Overall, the cross-sectional results were consistent with the hypothesis that maternal emotion coaching would be inversely associated with adolescent difficulty regulating anger, which would in turn be positively associated with adolescent externalizing behavior. As depicted in Figure 3, shared aspects of emotion coaching negatively predicted shared aspects of anger regulation and shared aspects of anger regulation positively predicted shared aspects of externalizing. Shared aspects of irritability also positively predicted shared aspects of mother reported externalizing. Similarly, nonshared aspects of emotion coaching negatively predicted nonshared aspects of anger regulation and nonshared aspects of anger regulation positively predicted nonshared aspects of mother reported externalizing. Nonshared aspects of irritability also positively predicted nonshared aspects of (mother and teacher reported) externalizing. There were no direct effects of emotion coaching on externalizing. Gender, which was included as a control variable, was modestly related to anger regulation, indicating that boys had more difficulty regulating anger than girls, and to irritability, signifying that mothers reported higher levels of irritability for girls compared to boys.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Cross-sectional model. Note. Only significant paths are indicated; + the residual variance of T1 shared externalizing was negative and fixed to zero, which resulted in a standardized effect that was 1.0; approximate * p < .05, ** p < .01; *** p < .001

Longitudinal Structural Model

The longitudinal model is shown in Figure 4. For ease of viewing, only significant paths are drawn. As expected, both shared and nonshared T1 externalizing behavior was a strong predictor of both shared and nonshared T2 externalizing behavior. Hypotheses regarding the associations between adolescent anger regulation and externalizing behavior were supported, with regard to nonshared but not to shared effects. Nonshared aspects of T1 anger regulation positively predicted nonshared aspects of T2 mother reported externalizing behavior over and above the corresponding stability effect. The only mediated pathway found was for nonshared effects from maternal emotion coaching to adolescent anger regulation at T1 and from anger regulation at T1 to mother reported externalizing at T2. A Sobel test (Sobel, 1982) indicated that the indirect effect was significant (z = -2.11, p = .035).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Longitudinal model. Note. Only significant paths are indicated; + the residual variances of T1 and T2 shared externalizing were negative and fixed to zero, which resulted in standardized effects that were 1.0; approximate * p < .05, ** p < .01; *** p < .001

For younger siblings, shared aspects of T1 emotion coaching negatively predicted T2 teacher reported externalizing. However, contrary to expectations this association was not mediated by anger regulation. Two other unanticipated findings emerged: shared aspects of T1 teacher reported externalizing and T1 (mother and teacher reported) older sibling externalizing positively predicted T2 teacher reported younger sibling externalizing.

Discussion

The findings of this study highlight the salience of family emotional processes in adolescent adjustment and substantiate the role of adolescents’ anger regulation in their externalizing behavior. Cross-sectional analyses supported the hypothesized model in which maternal coaching of anger was associated with adolescents’ anger regulation, which in turn was associated with adolescent externalizing behavior. Results of the longitudinal model were less clear. As hypothesized, adolescent anger regulation predicted mother-reported adolescent externalizing behavior three years later, replicating the effect observed in the cross-sectional data. However, support for the mediational model was limited to nonshared effects and specific to mother reported externalizing. It is likely that the strong stability of externalizing behavior limited our ability to detect cross-lag effects. Finally, though we anticipated that the association between emotion socialization and externalizing behavior would be mediated by adolescent anger regulation, the longitudinal data revealed that mothers’ emotion coaching had a direct effect on teacher-reported younger sibling externalizing behavior that was not mediated. This finding may reflect differences in mothers’ emotion coaching toward younger siblings specific to their age group. However, as this effect was limited to one measure within the longitudinal model, replication is necessary before drawing firm conclusions.

As much of the research on the role of emotion regulation as a mediator between parent emotion socialization behaviors and child functioning has been conducted with young children, the findings of this study are important in demonstrating the ongoing significance of family processes for emotional development into adolescence. Maternal coaching of adolescent anger was associated concurrently with adolescent emotion regulation. Difficulties regulating anger, moreover, predicted externalizing behavior problems concurrently and longitudinally in both older and younger siblings, underscoring the role of adolescents’ emotion regulation abilities in adolescent adjustment. Though as noted, prior work has suggested parallels in the association between parenting behavior and child functioning in community and clinical samples, it will, nevertheless, be important to replicate these findings in a clinical sample in order to test the hypothesis that emotion socialization would be operative in similar ways in adolescents demonstrating externalizing disorders.

One strength of the study was the focus on mother emotion coaching of two adolescents in the same family. Shared effects across siblings and nonshared child-specific effects emerged in the cross-sectional and longitudinal effects. Identification of effects as shared or nonshared has implications for understanding the mechanisms accounting for observed findings. For example, the association of anger regulation with externalizing behavior was a nonshared effect, highlighting the involvement of child-specific processes and sibling differences within families. The shared effects predicting younger sibling externalizing behavior over time, on the other hand, indicated family influences across siblings. Next steps would be to determine whether the shared and nonshared effects involved genetic or environmental factors.

Another strength was the use of multiple reporters. This strategy, in regards to externalizing behavior, not only increases the reliability and validity of constructs but also provides important information about contextual effects that may have implications for intervention (Lochman, 2004). Differences in findings as a function of whether mothers or teachers provided information may reflect the context in which adolescents’ externalizing behaviors were observed as well as the perspectives of family and non-family reporters (e.g., Youngstrom, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2000). The involvement of familial emotional processes in adolescent functioning that takes place at home and in the community is suggested by the findings linking maternal emotion coaching to adolescent emotion regulation and adolescent emotion regulation to mother-reported externalizing behavior. It is possible that source variance contributed to the observed associations between emotion processes derived from mother report and adolescent externalizing behavior as reported by mothers. On the other hand, older siblings’ externalizing behavior was found to be a risk factor for younger siblings’ externalizing behavior as reported by teachers. With school-based externalizing behavior largely fueled by peer interaction, older siblings can be a negative influence by exposing and including younger siblings in antisocial activities and delinquent peer groups (Snyder, Bank, & Burraston, 2005).

One measurement weakness which should be noted was the relatively modest inter-rater reliability obtained for the global interview ratings, which may have limited the predictive accuracy of the latent construct. As well, a potential limitation of the broader measurement strategy was that indices of anger regulation and emotion coaching were both derived from maternal report, and hence, may be inflated by shared source variance. It should be noted, however, that interviewer and coder ratings, though based on the interview with mother, reflected their judgment about the mothers’ functioning rather than the mothers’ self-evaluation. Moreover, recent work suggests that mothers’ emotion coaching as assessed with the MEI is correlated with observed maternal behavior during parent-adolescent interaction (Shortt, Smith-Shine, & McDade, 2008). In particular, mothers rated high in emotion coaching demonstrated more validating and affectionate behavior and less contemptuous and belligerent behavior during the interactions. Nonetheless, the relation of emotion coaching meta-emotion philosophy to parenting behavior is not well established and the study would have benefitted from observational measures.

Though the frame we have given to our questions reflects a parent-driven mediational model, alternative adolescent-driven effects or reciprocal processes cannot be ruled out. That children with externalizing symptomatology are more likely to evoke negative emotions from others (Ge et al., 1996) suggests the involvement of nonshared effects and the possibility that child characteristics contribute to eliciting the parenting behavior by which they are then influenced (Caspi et al., 2004). In fact, prior findings suggest that adolescent characteristics do evoke changes in parenting over time (Brody & Ge, 2001). Unfortunately, as we do not have data on emotion coaching or adolescent anger regulation at T2, we are unable to examine these potential influences. Further studies are needed to better elucidate the effects of adolescent characteristics on parent emotion socialization behavior.

Another direction to pursue in future research relates to the specificity of effects. Though studies on children suggest some specificity in relations between emotions and symptoms (e.g., Kim, Walden, Harris, Karrass, & Catron, 2007), there is limited examination of such during adolescence. Some evidence suggests that the association between emotional functioning and psychopathology may be more diffuse (e.g., Steinberg & Avenevolli, 2000). For example, depressive disorder in adolescents is associated with disruptions in both anger and sadness (Sheeber et al., 2009). To the best of our knowledge, the specificity of maternal socialization behaviors has not been established. Some evidence suggests specificity with links between maternal dismissiveness to adolescents’ positive affect and adolescent depressive symptoms (Yap, Allen, & Ladouceur, 2008). The role of specific emotions in adolescent psychopathology will be an important direction for ongoing work.

In conclusion, the findings of this study support the growing evidence that emotion coaching, a form of positive emotion socialization behavior, in which children and adolescents’ affect are accepted and their regulation skills are scaffolded, is associated with healthy emotional development. Adolescents whose mothers coached them through episodes of anger demonstrated better regulation of anger and less externalizing behavior. Moreover, adolescents with poor anger regulation skills were at increased risk for developing externalizing problems. These findings, taken together with those of previous studies, suggest that interventions designed to promote positive parent emotion socialization behavior may promote youths’ ability to regulate emotions, and hence, may prove to be effective components of preventative interventions for child and adolescent externalizing behavior.

Key Points.

  • Increases in externalizing behavior during the transition to adolescence may put children at risk for developing mental disorders and related problems.

  • Although children’s ability to regulate their emotions appears to be a key factor in whether they experience maladjustment, emotion processes during adolescence remain understudied.

  • Maternal emotion coaching of adolescent anger influenced adolescents’ anger regulation concurrently, and adolescents’ difficulty regulating anger influenced adolescents’ externalizing behavior concurrently and longitudinally.

  • Interventions promoting adaptive parent socialization of emotion behavior have the potential to strengthen adolescent emotion regulatory abilities which in turn may reduce adolescent psychopathology.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the families for their participation, the collaborating Oregon school districts Eugene 4J, Springfield, and Bethel for their assistance in identifying families, Deborah Capaldi for her guidance throughout the project, Alice Holmes for coordinating the project, research staff and students for interviewing, data management, and coding, Diana Strand for editorial assistance, and Sara Hodges for her comments on the manuscript. Additional information on the modeling analyses can be obtained from the authors. The project described was supported by Award Number R01 MH 58337 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or National Institutes of Health.

References

  1. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;107:238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bentler PM, Bonnett DG. Significant tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin. 1980;88:588–606. [Google Scholar]
  4. Brody GH, Ge Z. Linking parenting processes and self-regulation to psychological functioning and alcohol use during early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology. 2001;15(10):82–94. doi: 10.1037//0893-3200.15.1.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Burke JD, Pardini DA, Loeber R. Reciprocal relationships between parenting behavior and disruptive psychopathology from childhood through adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36(5):679–692. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9219-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Capaldi DM, Rothbart MK. Development and validation of an early adolescent temperament measure. Journal of Early Adolescence. 1992;12(2):153–173. [Google Scholar]
  7. Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Morgan J, Rutter M, Taylor A, Arseneault L, Polo-Tomas M. Maternal expressed emotion predicts children’s antisocial behavior problems: Using monozygotic-twin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40(2):149–161. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chaplin TM, Cole PM. The role of emotion regulation in the development of psychopathology. In: Hankin BL, Abela JRZ, editors. Development of psychopathology: A vulnerability-stress perspective. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2005. pp. 49–74. [Google Scholar]
  9. Cicchetti D, Acherman BP, Izard CE. Emotions and emotion regulation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7:1–10. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cole PM, Kaslow NJ. Interactional and cognitive strategies for affect regulation: Developmental perspective on childhood depression. In: Alloy LB, editor. Cognitive processes in depression. NY: Guilford; 1988. pp. 310–343. [Google Scholar]
  11. Dishion TJ, Nelson SE, Bullock BM. Premature adolescent autonomy: Parent disengagement and deviant peer process in the amplification of problem behaviour. Journal of Adolescence. 2004;27(5):515–530. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dunn J, McGuire S. Young children’s nonshared experiences: A summary of studies in Cambridge and Colorado. In: Hetherington EM, Reiss D, Plomin R, editors. Separate social worlds of siblings The impact of nonshared environment on development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  13. Eccles JS, Midgley C, Wigfield A, Buchanan CM, Reuman D, Flanagan C, Mac Iver D. Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment fit on young adolescents’ experiences in schools and in families. American Psychologist. 1993;48(2):90–101. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.48.2.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Eisenberg N, Cumberland A, Spinrad TL. Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry. 1998;9:241–273. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Morris AS, Fabes RA, Cumberland A, Reiser M, Losoya S. Longitudinal regulations among parental emotional expressivity, children’s regulation, and quality of socioemotional functioning. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39(1):3–19. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fowles DC. Application of a behavioral theory of motivation to the concepts of anxiety and impulsivity. Journal of Research in Personality. 1987;21(4):417–435. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ge X, Conger RE, Cadoret RJ, Neiderhiser JM, Yates W, Troughton E, Stewart MA. The developmental interface between nature and nurture: A mutual influence model of child antisocial behavior and parent behaviors. Developmental Psychology. 1996;32(4):574–589. [Google Scholar]
  18. Gottman JM, Katz LF, Hooven C. Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotionally. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbuam; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  19. Gray JA. Personality dimensions and emotion systems. In: Ekman P, Davidson RJ, editors. The nature of emotions: Fundamental questions. NY: Oxford; 1994. pp. 329–331. [Google Scholar]
  20. Hinshaw SP. Intervention research, theoretical mechanisms and causal processes related to externalizing behavior patterns. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14(4):789–818. doi: 10.1017/s0954579402004078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hooven C, Katz LF, Mittman A. The meta-emotion coding system: Parent meta-emotion philosophy coding manual. University of Washington, Department of Psychology; 1996. Unpublished manual. [Google Scholar]
  22. Katz LF, Gottman JM. The meta-emotion interview. University of Washington, Department of Psychology; 1986. Unpublished manual. [Google Scholar]
  23. Katz LF, Hunter EC. Maternal meta-emotion philosophy and adolescent depressive symptomatology. Social Development. 2007;16(2):343–360. [Google Scholar]
  24. Keltner D, Moffitt TE, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Facial expressions of emotion and psychopathology in adolescent boys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104(4):644–652. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.4.644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kim G, Walden T, Harris V, Karrass J, Catron T. Positive emotion, negative emotion, and emotion control in the externalizing problems of school-aged children. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2007;37:221–239. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0031-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Klimes-Dougan B, Brand AE, Zahn-Waxler C, Usher B, Hastings PD, Kendziora K, Garside RB. Parental emotion socialization in adolescence: Differences in sex, age and problem status. Social Development. 2007;16(2):326–342. [Google Scholar]
  27. Larson RW, Sheeber LB. The daily emotional experience of adolescents: Are adolescents more emotional, why, and how is that related to depression? In: Allen NB, Sheeber LB, editors. Adolescent emotional development and the emergence of depressive disorders. UK: Cambridge; 2008. pp. 11–32. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lipsey MW, Derson JH. Predictors of violent or serious delinquency in adolescence and early adulthood: A synthesis of longitudinal research. In: Loeber R, Farrington DP, editors. Serious and violent juvenile offenders: Risk factors and successful interventions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1998. pp. 86–105. [Google Scholar]
  29. Lochman JE. Contextual factors in risk and prevention research. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2004;50(3):311–325. [Google Scholar]
  30. MacKinnon DP. Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. NY: Erlbaum; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  31. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods. 2002;7(1):83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. McMahon RJ, Forehand R. Conduct disorders. In: Mash EJ, Terdal LG, editors. Behavioral assessment of childhood disorders. NY: Guilford Press; 1988. pp. 105–153. [Google Scholar]
  33. Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development. 2007;16(2):361–388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Muris P, Ollendick TH. The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2005;8(4):271–289. doi: 10.1007/s10567-005-8809-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Murrie DC, Marcus DK, Douglas KS, Lee Z, Salekin RT, Vincent G. Youth with psychopathy features are not a discrete class: A taxometric analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48(7):714–723. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01734.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. Fourth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  37. Offord DR, Adler RJ, Boyle MH. Prevalence and socio-demographic correlates of conduct disorder. American Journal of Social Psychiatry. 1986;6:272–278. [Google Scholar]
  38. Petersen AC, Leffert N, Graham B, Alwin J, Ding S. Promoting mental health during the transition into adolescence. In: Schulenberg J, Maggs JL, Hurrelmann K, editors. Health risks and developmental transitions during adolescence. UK: Cambridge; 1997. pp. 471–497. [Google Scholar]
  39. Plomin R, Asbury K, Dunn J. Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;46:225–233. doi: 10.1177/070674370104600302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Reid JB, Patterson GR, Snyder J. Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for intervention. Washington, DC: APA; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  41. Reiss D. Genes and the environment: Siblings and synthesis. In: Plomin R, McClearn GE, editors. Nature, nurture & psychology. Washington, DC: APA; 1993. pp. 417–432. [Google Scholar]
  42. Reiss D, Neiderhiser JM, Hetherington EM, Plomin R. The relationship code. Deciphering genetic and social influences on adolescent development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  43. Rothbart MK, Bates JE. Temperament. In: Damon W, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development. 5. NY: Wiley; 1998. pp. 105–176. [Google Scholar]
  44. Rydell AM, Berlin L, Bohlin G. Emotionality, emotion regulation, and adaptation among 5- to 8-year-old children. Emotion. 2003;3(1):30–47. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods. 2002;72(2):147–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sheeber LB, Allen NB, Leve C, Davis B, Shortt JWu, Katz LF. Dynamics of affective experience and behavior in depressed adolescents. 2009 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02148.x. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Shortt JWu, Capaldi DM, Dishion TJ, Bank L, Owen LD. The role of adolescent peers, romantic partners, and siblings in the emergence of the adult antisocial lifestyle. Journal of Family Psychology. 2003;17:521–533. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Shortt JWu, Smith-Shine JN, McDade ME. Associations between maternal meta-emotion philosophy, observed affect, and reported parenting behavior toward adolescent siblings. In: Sheeber LB, Hunter EC, editors. Meta-emotion philosophy and socialization of adolescent affect; Symposium at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research on Adolescence; Chicago, Illinois. 2008, March. [Google Scholar]
  49. Smith JN, Thomas A, Nederland A. The meta-emotion coding system: Parent meta-emotion philosophy coding manual: Revised for the Peer Adolescent Linkages for Siblings project. Oregon Social Learning Center; 2002. Unpublished manual. [Google Scholar]
  50. Snyder J, Bank L, Burraston B. The consequences of antisocial behavior in older male siblings for younger brothers and sisters. Journal of Family Psychology. 2005;19(4):643–653. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sobel ME. Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology. 1982;13:290–312. [Google Scholar]
  52. Steiger JH. Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1990;25:173–180. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence: What changes, and why? In: Dahl RE, Patia L, editors. Adolescent brain development: Vulnerabilities and opportunities Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. Vol. 1021. NY: Academy of Sciences; 2004. pp. 51–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Steinberg L. A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review. 2008;28(1):78–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Steinberg L, Avenevolli S. The role of context in the development of psychopathology: A conceptual framework and some speculative propositions. Child Development. 2000;71(1):66–74. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Stocker CM, Richmond MK, Rhoades GK, Kiang L. Family emotional processes and adolescents’ adjustment. Social Development. 2007;16(2):310–325. [Google Scholar]
  57. Valiente C, Lemery-Chalfant K, Reiser M. Pathways to problem behaviors: Chaotic homes, parent and child effortful control, and parenting. Social Development. 2007;16:249–267. [Google Scholar]
  58. Yap M, Allen NB, Ladouceur C. Maternal socialization of positive affect: The impact of “dampening” on adolescent emotion regulation and depressive symptomatology. Child Development. 2008;79:1415–1431. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01196.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Youngstrom E, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Patterns and correlates of agreement between parent, teacher, and male adolescent ratings of externalizing and internalizing problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68(6):1038–1050. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.6.1038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES