Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addict Biol. 2010 Apr;15(2):200–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00189.x

Consilient Research Approaches in Studying Gene x Environment Interactions in Alcohol Research

Kenneth J Sher 1, Danielle M Dick 2, John C Crabbe 3, Kent E Hutchison 4, Stephanie S O’Malley 5, Andrew C Heath 6
PMCID: PMC2883915  NIHMSID: NIHMS186388  PMID: 20148780

Abstract

This review article discusses the importance of identifying gene-environment interactions for understanding the etiology and course of alcohol use disorders and related conditions. A number of critical challenges are discussed including the fact that there is no organizing typology for classifying different types of environmental exposures, many key human environmental risk factors for alcohol dependence have no clear equivalents in other species, much of the genetic variance of alcohol dependence in human is not “alcohol specific”, and the potential range of gene-environment interactions that could be considered is so vast that maintaining statistical control of Type 1 errors is a daunting task. Despite these and other challenges, there appears to be a number of promising approaches that could be taken in order to achieve consilience and ecologically valid translation between human alcohol dependence and animal models. Foremost among these is to distinguish environmental exposures that are thought to have enduring effects on alcohol use motivation (and self-regulation) from situational environmental exposures that facilitate the expression of such motivations but do not, by themselves, have enduring effects. In order to enhance consilience, various domains of human approach motivation should be considered so that relevant environmental exposures can be sampled as well as the appropriate species to study them in (i.e., where such motivations are ecologically relevant). Foremost among these are social environments which are central to the initiation and escalation of human alcohol consumption. The value of twin studies, human laboratory studies, and pharmacogenetic studies is also highlighted.

Keywords: alcohol, animal models, environmental effects, genetics, gene-environment interaction


Hopes for identifying single genes that explain more than a small proportion of liability for common mental and physical disorders have diminished greatly in recent years as most replicable genetic effects provide, at best, very modest effects, with odds ratios in the 1.1-1.5 range (Kendler, 2005; Kraft & Hunter, 2009). This may reflect, in part, the “efficiency of natural selection in prohibiting increases in disease-associated variants in the population” (Goldstein, 2009, p. 1698). Although proponents of genome-wide association studies (GWAS) are confident that many new loci important for common diseases will be discovered in the next few years (e.g., Hirschhorn, 2009), as noted by Moffitt et al. (2005), the “main-effects approach” embodied in traditional gene finding including GWAS studies is inefficient if detection of the effect of the gene is conditional upon environmental risk. This approach assumes that genes have uniform effects across environmental conditions, and that environments affect all genotypes similarly. Alcoholism, given the long-recognized associations between alcohol use disorder in the parental generation and high-risk exposures in the offspring generation (parental separation, assaultive trauma, family conflict etc.), is arguably an ideal case for investigating gene x environment interaction (GXE) effects (Heath and Nelson, 2002). Consequently, consideration of the environment should result in improved gene finding (as well as understanding how gene effects on behavior are mediated and moderated).

The range of potential environmental influences on the development of alcohol dependence and alcohol use disorders (abuse or dependence; AUDs) in humans is vast and ranges from the chemical environment (e.g., ethanol and other drug exposures) to broad social factors (e.g., cultural influences) and includes life-stage specific factors (e.g., prenatal exposures to various biologic and nonbiologic agents, various social roles that vary over the life course). Unlike the genome which contains a very large but finite number of genes, the number and type of environmental influences is unknown. Consequently, we currently have no comparable “environome” to classify the numbers and types of environments that may be relevant for human alcohol dependence or other conditions, making the prospect of environome-wide association studies unfeasible at this time. Indeed, any “scan” for environmental effects is likely to be haphazard and unsystematic at best. When considering the number of unique single-gene by single-environment interactions, the inherent complexity of identifying valid GXE interactions becomes daunting. Clearly, if critical gene-environment interactions are to be detected, strategies must be developed that enhance the likelihood that valid, meaningful interactions are detected and spurious ones minimized. Stressful life events are a known risk factor for psychiatric disease, and perhaps the most celebrated GXE interaction finding in the psychiatric literature is the potentiating effect of multiple stressors and risk-associated effects of a short allele at the serotonin transporter gene on depression and other disorders reported in (Caspi et al., 2003) and in several subsequent studies. In commenting on the negative findings from their meta-analysis on this interaction, Risch et al., (2009) note that “few examples of gene-environment interaction exist for modest gene effects or small environmental impacts, most likely due to lack of power to characterize such an interaction” (p. 2469). When one jointly considers the universe of potential GXE interactions that can be assessed and the inherent low statistical power for detection of interactions in most statistical models (e.g., in a simple factorial ANOVA), the serious challenges that must be overcome in conducting this kind of work must be given careful consideration.

Given the Evident Challenges, How Do We Look for GXE interactions

As noted by Moffitt et al. (2005), there has long been a belief by some in behavior genetics that “GXEs are so infrequent that they can safely be ignored” (p. 473) and this attitude has only recently changed. In order to facilitate further work on human GXE interactions, Moffitt et al. recommend a seven-step strategy that goes beyond brute force scanning and, logically, should help identify the most promising candidate interactions. These steps can be summarized as: (1) “consulting” (i.e., taking leads from) the quantitative behavior-genetic literature (e.g., twin studies), (2) “identifying a candidate environmental pathogen for the disorder in question,” (3) “optimizing environmental risk measurement,” (4) “identifying candidate susceptibility genes,” (5) “testing for an interaction,” (6) “evaluating the specificity of the interaction”, and (7) “replication and meta-analysis.” To this list should be added (5a) evaluating the dependence of the observed interaction on choice of measurement scale (e.g. Jinks and Fulker, 1970) or, particularly in the case of binary phenotypes, the appropriateness of implicit modeling assumptions (e.g. Eaves, 2006; Heath et al., 2008). There is a more restrictive viewpoint that, since only true cross-over interactions cannot be removed by data-transformation, these alone should be the focus of research on gene-environment interaction. But if in biology first-order (main) effects are in general of greater magnitude than second-order interactive effects, such cross-over interactions are likely to be rare and atypical. However, it has recently been argued (Belsky et al., 2009) that such cross-over interactions are not as rare as typically assumed and are substantively meaningful. Specifically, such interactions suggest that a given genotype might be viewed as reflecting a general sensitivity to the environment, with resultant good or bad outcomes. Such cross-over interactions would therefore point to “plasticity” rather than “vulnerability.” Unfortunately, our ability to characterize the strength and form of statistical interactions with confidence is limited because most existing studies are based upon relatively small cohorts of individuals (by the standards of contemporary genetic research), only rarely enriched for specific environmental exposures.

Extension of GXE research to animal models is important scientifically because research in humans (with few, but important exceptions discussed below under Human Laboratory Models) is almost exclusively correlational and, thus, incapable of providing strong inferences concerning causation. In humans, additionally, the same environmental exposures that are most plausible candidates for interacting with individual gene effects on risk are also likely to be correlated with genetic risk. For example, alcohol risk genes as well as risky environments are passed down from parents to offspring living in the household: these are the conditions under which the dissection of GXE effects, for a variety of technical reasons, is most challenging (e.g. Purcell, 2002; Rathouz et al, 2008). Animal models provide opportunities for control of both genetic factors and environmental exposures, opportunities to observe the temporal sequencing and unfolding of behaviors and the processes presumably mediating them, and opportunities to conduct analyses across multiple levels of biological organization (e.g., neurocircuitry, cellular, genetic and epigenetic) that involve high levels of invasiveness. As noted in a recent Institute of Medicine (IOM; 2006) report, rodent models can be useful for studying a range of psychosocial environmental variables such as social relationships and many aspects of parenting although they may be limited for investigating more complex social factors such as those involving “cooperation or trust” (p. 133) which may require use of nonhuman primates. In considering ways to model the symptoms of major depression in mice, Cryan and Holmes (2005, Table 1) identified a number of possible models for most DSM-IV criteria including “indecisiveness or diminished ability to think or concentrate” where they suggested “deficits in working and spatial memory and impaired sustained attention.” However, they also concluded that “recurrent thoughts of death or suicide” or “feelings of worthlessness or excessive or inappropriate guilt” could not be modeled in mice (and presumably all other nonhuman animals). Similarly, some DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) criteria such as “ a persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control substance use” and “the substance use is continued despite knowledge [emphasis added] of having a persistent or recurrent physical or psychological problem that is likely to have been caused or exacerbated by the substance” (p. 197) may not be practical phenotypes to model in animals and therefore environmental variables that rely upon uniquely (or largely) human abilities to affect the phenotype may be impossible to model fully in any animal model. Finally, most experimental designs employed in behavior genetic research with animals incorporates enough subjects to detect main effects of genotype or treatment, but not their interaction (Wahlsten, 1991).

Thus, the first task for investigators seeking consilience between human and animal studies of GXE interactions is to identify those environment variables that are ripe for translation across human and animal models of alcohol dependence. This, by necessity, excludes some potent risk and moderating factors for either drinking and/or alcohol use disorders (e.g., culture, Hanson, 1995; religious beliefs, Haber & Jacob, 2007) that have no clear counterparts in nonhuman animal species; and others (e.g. chronic history of childhood sexual or other assaultive trauma; see Sartor et al., 2008) that involve stressors of such severity that they would be prohibited by regulations concerning the use of animals in research. Other important factors such as cost, although in humans determined largely by taxation (see Chaloupka, Grossman, & Saffer, 2002), can, in principle, be modeled via different schedules of reinforcement similarly in both humans and nonhuman animals (Etten, Higgins, & Bickel, 1995). Finally other kinds of psychosocial variables such as parental neglect can be modeled although what is considered to be neglect in humans (e.g., lack of nurturance, inadequate attention to basic physical needs, or supervision) might look phenotypically very different in rodents (e.g., lack of maternal pup retrieval to the nest and licking).

In attempting to judge consilience, the ecological meaning of an environment and the effect it has on underlying neurobiology is more critical than how phenotypically similar it appears to be to its human counterpart. Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence and so if one wants to claim that there is an animal model of a specific behavioral process (e.g., guilt) associated with a specific environment (e.g., strong social sanctions), then one would want to see this demonstrated across multiple behavioral paradigms with reference to common neural circuitry (Institute of Medicine, 2006).

Why Do Humans Drink?

Alcohol-seeking, like all addiction-related phenomena, can be viewed as representing the outcome of two competing processes: (1) approach motivation and (2) inhibition/self-control (e.g., Orford, 2001). Full understanding of addictive behavior, then, requires an understanding of impelling and restraining tendencies although a given animal model doesn’t necessarily have to address both at the same time.

Approach motivation in humans has been extensively studied in self-report and and factor analyses of self-reported reasons for drinking which reliably yield a multidimensional, four-factor structure (Cooper, 1994): (1) social motives (e.g., “to be sociable”), (2) enhancement motives (e.g., “to get high,” “because it’s fun”), (3) coping motives (e.g., “to forget your worries,” “because it helps when you feel depressed or nervous”), and (4) conformity motives (e.g., “to fit in”). Notably, enhancement and coping motives are strongly associated with drinking, heavy drinking, and drinking problems in both adolescents and adults (Cooper, 1994; Cooper, Frone, Russell & Mudar, 1995), with conformity and social drinking motives somewhat less strongly correlated with consumption levels. A growing body of literature indicates that the temperamental traits of extraversion and sensation-seeking tend to correlate with enhancement motivation and that neuroticism/negative affectivity tends to correlate with coping motivation (Kuntsche, Knibbe, Gmel & Engels, 2006). These findings point to specific motivational pathways arising from individual differences that are temperamentally based and can help guide the selection of environmental variables and appropriate animal models. For example, to study the effects of alcohol on socialization or conformity (indeed, if conformity in its typical sense is relevant at all to nonhuman animals), one would select a species for study where socialization is an important part of adaptive behavior. Additionally, initial evidence suggests that motives for alcohol use may also be linked to similar genetic factors. Drinking motives involving coping with negative moods appear to be heritable, especially in females, (Agrawal et. al., 2007; Prescott et al., 2004) and a substantial portion of genetic variation in AUDs appears to overlap with drinking to manage mood states (Prescott et al., 2004).

The other side of the approach/restraint conflict is addressed by Dick et al. (in press) who note that impulsivity (arguably, in most but not all cases, the lack of restraint) is, like drinking motivations, multidimensional and so the specific types of phenotypes relevant for consilient translation between human and animal species will depend upon the specific facet of restraint/impulsivity considered (e.g., ability to inhibit a prepotent response, negative affect urgency). (We note that one aspect of impulsivity, novelty seeking is probably best thought of as approach; Dick et al. [in press, this issue] specifically discuss which types of phenotypes are potentially ripe for translation between human and nonhuman animals.)

An important perspective on alcohol dependence is that it is just one manifestation of the types of generalized externalizing behavior problems that first appear in childhood and adolescence (e.g., Achenbach, 1978; Jessor & Jessor, 1977). There are strong genetic correlations among alcohol dependence and conduct disorder (e.g., Slutske et al., 1998) and personality traits associated with disinhibition (e.g., Slutske et al., 2002). In addition, traits-related to disinhibition prospectively predict the onset of alcohol dependence (Sher et al., 1999, 2005). Factor analytic approaches to psychopathology indicate that all externalizing disorders (e.g., conduct disorder, adult antisocial behavior, drug use disorders, and AUDs) are highly correlated and form a distinct psychopathological factor of “externalizing disorder” with common personality correlates (e.g., Krueger et al., 2005; Krueger & Markon, 2006, 2008). While models of generalized externalizing behavior (e.g., Jessor, Donovan, & Costa, 1991; Patterson, Reid, & Dishion, 1992) tend to highlight the importance of parenting and deviant peer groups, both twin studies (e.g., Slutske et al., 1998) and molecular genetics studies (e.g., Dick, 2007) suggest that common genetic influences are also contributing factors. However, there is evidence from twin studies suggesting that there is more complexity to the comorbidity among different forms of substance dependence (and other externalizing behavior problems) than would be expected on the basis of a single spectrum of externalizing behavior. For example, best-fitting multivariate models of the genetic architecture of common genetic factors among abuse of alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, caffeine, and cocaine suggest at least two distinguishable (although very highly correlated) common genetic factors (one principally associated with licit substances and one principally associated with illicit substances) as well as substance-specific genetic factors, especially for nicotine and caffeine (Kendler, Meyers, & Prescott, 2007). These findings were in contrast to a similar study of the genetic architecture of illicit substance dependence which found evidence only for common genetic influences on cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogen, sedative, stimulant, and opiate dependence (Kendler, Jacobson, et al., 2003): individual genetic paths to risk for abuse of each drug separately were not strong in this study. Collectively these findings highlight the importance of shared genetic mechanisms among a range of externalizing and substance pathology but also to a potentially important role for unique genetic and nongenetic factors on specific types of substance dependence including alcohol dependence.

(At Least) Two Different Ways Environments Can Operate to Affect Alcohol Consumption

Most of the literature on GXE for behavioral phenotypes in animal models focuses on the role of early experiences or later severe stressors inducing some relatively durable change in the organism. Commonly studied environments in other species include maternal behavior, how enriched or impoverished an environment is, various kinds of physical and social stressors, or early drug exposures (e.g., Barr et al., 2004; Cryan & Slattery, 2007; Holmes et al., 2005; Stevens et al., 2009). While these types of experiences appear relevant to drinking in some animal models (esp. the effect of peer-rearing and early exposure to ethanol in primates; Barr et al., 2004), implicit in all of these types of studies is that environmental exposures are creating some functional (and perhaps even structural) change in the organism that persists for some duration of time and is motivationally relevant to general approach or restraint tendencies. Most GXE interactions are studied within this framework.

However, a number of human environmental influences on excessive drinking appear to represent situational goads or constraints that are likely to interact with genetically influenced traits, but not necessarily by getting “under the skin” and influencing underlying physiology. (Clearly, there is a continuum here of “how much” a given environmental exposure induces a transient versus durable effect.) Such situational goads are many and diverse and include simple availability, specific occasions (e.g., celebrations and holiday), specific activities (e.g., tailgating, prepartying, drinking games), housing, social networks, community variables, and price. Some of these types of situational factors (e.g., availability and price) could be modeled in animals and could be important for both identifying GXE interactions highly related to variable aspects of drinking. Moreover, systematically varying these situational variables could prove to be a useful strategy in establishing the robustness of other genetic and environmental manipulations.

It might be tempting to dismiss highly situational effects as not relevant for understanding the genetics of alcoholism in humans. However, such effects may represent some of the most heritable phenotypes. For example, the maximum number of drinks in a drinking day has been shown to be a heritable alcohol consumption-based phenotype for which evidence for genetic linkage is found (Saccone et al., 2000; Saccone et al. 2005) and in a collegiate sample, close to 50% of all 21st birthday celebrants established their highest lifetime maximum drinks (until age 21) on that occasion (Rutledge et al., 2008). While birthday celebrations may not be relevant to animal models, some other drinking situations that promote high consumption (e.g., certain drinking schedules (Crabbe et al, 2009; Falk et al., 1972) could be highly translatable and can be modeled across species.

Using Behavior Genetic Studies to Identify Relevant Environments

Animal models allow us to study environmental effects in genotypes with self-defined levels of alcohol drinking. In the first report of inbred mouse strain differences in two-bottle preference drinking, data on individual mice from 5 inbred strains were presented (McClearn and Rodgers, 1959). Even within the high-drinking C57BL strain, there were individual differences in alcohol preference, and within some other strains such as C3H, the individual differences were substantial. Such differences among individuals within a strain must reflect gene-environment interaction, but the source of the environmental effect is entirely unknown. A strong possibility is that the individual differences within a strain are due to differences in gene expression. Molecular technology now permits the genome-wide analysis of gene expression differences, and pronounced individual differences within inbred strains have been shown for genes and gene networks. For example, genes in a functional group may be up-regulated in one brain tissue and down-regulated in another in the same individual brain, in addition to differing from expression patterns across individuals of the same genotype (Cowley et al., 2009).

In rodent behavior genetic models of drinking, cross-fostering designs (where the genetic background and rearing environment are experimentally crossed) can be extremely informative for identifying relevant environments and GXEs. Within the limitations to interpretation discussed above, animal models could be used to study the effects of maternal and peer environments on adult drinking. Randall (Randall and Lester, 1975a) compared two inbred mouse strains well known to differ in alcohol preference, the high drinking C57BL/6 strain and the low-drinking DBA/2 strain. She cross-fostered some DBA/2 newborns at birth to C57BL/6 mothers, and vice versa. Mice were tested at adulthood for two-bottle alcohol preference drinking. The strong main effect of genotype was clearly seen (C57BL/6 drank more than DBA/2), however, DBA/2 mice reared by a C57BL/6 dam drank more than those reared by a different DBA/2 dam. Thus, there was a clear GXE effect on adult drinking between genotype and postnatal environment.

Social facilitation could not have contributed to these results (there was no alcohol offered during rearing), but in a subsequent experiment, adult, normally reared C57BL/6 and DBA/2 mice were housed with mice of the opposite or the same strain from weaning (3 weeks) for 7 weeks (Randall and Lester, 1975b). Here, alcohol and water bottles were available. At 10 weeks, mice were individually housed and preference drinking was measured for one week. Both genotypes showed an effect of post-natal rearing. DBA/2 mice reared throughout adolescence with C57BL/6 adults drank more than those reared with other DBAs, and C57BL/6 mice reared with DBAs drank less than the DBAs reared with DBA cage mates.

However, the comparable design in humans, the adoption design, is problematic for several reasons. Human adoptions tend to select for strong genetic effects (due to deviant biological parents) and low variance in rearing conditions (i.e., adoption agencies tend not to place adoptees in problem homes) (Heath et al, 1997). Thus, it is not necessarily surprising that most human adoption studies have failed to find main environmental effects of exposure to parental alcoholism (Cadoret et al.., 1985, 1987; Cloninger et al., 1985; Goodwin et al., 1974; McGue, Sharma, & Benson, 1996); and such GXE effects as have been identified (e.g. involving adoptive parent marital status: Cadoret et al. 1986) will be of uncertain generalizability to the general population.

Moreover, in the classic twin design, and in sibling designs more generally, GXE interactions are confounded with additive genetic effects (in the case of genes*shared environment) and with unique environmental effects (in the case of genes*unique environment) (Eaves, 1977; Heath and Nelson, 2002). However, extended twin-family designs that control for genetic background in parents while examining differences in rearing conditions can elucidate where some GXE interactions might occur. Specifically, in one study using this approach, the absence of paternal AUD (a protective environmental factor) reduced the impact of high genetic risk for the development of an AUD in the offspring of (dependence-discordant) twins (Jacob, Waterman, Heath, True, Bucholz, Haber, et al., 2003) although similar findings were not found in another study employing a similar design (Slutske et al., 2008). Thus, perhaps the “biggest news” to come from adoption designs and children-of-twins designs is that the presumed environmental effect of living with an alcohol parent, per se, is not an important risk factor for alcohol dependence if genetic factors are adequately controlled.

However, twin studies can provide opportunities for identifying important environmental influences operating both as main effects and as GXE interactions. When information about the environment is explicitly measured, this can be incorporated into twin models to test whether the importance of genetic and environmental effects varies as a function of the measured environment (Dick, Rose, Viken, Kaprio, & Koskenvuo, 2001b; Purcell, 2002). One of the earliest illustrations of gene-environment interaction in the area of substance use research demonstrated that genetic influences on alcohol use were greater among unmarried women, whereas having a marriage-like relationship reduced the impact of genetic influences on drinking (Heath, Jardine, & Martin, 1989). Religiosity has also been shown to moderate genetic influences on alcohol use among females, with genetic factors playing a larger role among individuals without a religious upbringing (Koopmans, Slutske, van Baal, & Boomsma, 1999). Genetic influences on adolescent substance use are also enhanced in environments with lower parental monitoring (Dick et al., 2007c) and in the presence of substance-using friends (Dick et al., 2007b). Similar effects have been demonstrated for more general externalizing behavior: genetic influences on antisocial behavior were higher in the presence of delinquent peers (Button et al., 2007) and in environments characterized by high parental negativity (Feinberg, Button, Neiderhiser, Reiss, & Hetherington, 2007), low parental warmth (Feinberg et al., 2007), and high paternal punitive discipline (Button, Lau, Maughan, & Eley, 2008).

Socioregional or neighborhood-level influences have also been reported to moderate the importance of genetic influences on substance use. Genetic influences for late adolescent alcohol use and early adolescent behavior problems are enhanced in urban environments, communities characterized by greater migration, and neighborhoods with higher percentages of slightly older adolescents/young adults (Rose, Dick, Viken, & Kaprio, 2001; Dick, Rose, Viken, Kaprio, & Koskenvuo, 2001a; Dick et al., 2009). These moderation effects presumably reflect differences in availability of alcohol, role models, neighborhood stability, and community-level monitoring across different areas. Indeed, twin studies suggest that many of the important moderating effects of the environment associated with alcohol use and related externalizing behavior reflect differences in situational constraints and/or opportunities, as discussed above, but this type of GXE has been largely unexplored in measured gene studies.

Examples of Specific Environments that Could be Modeled Across Species: Early Alcohol Exposures and the Peer Environment

Early Alcohol Exposures

Animal models have shown clearly that fetal exposure to alcohol may lead to dysmorphology and behavioral deficits that resemble human Fetal Alcohol Syndrome. In addition, fetal alcohol exposures may lead to the development of specific drug sensitivities and preferences that may contribute to the development of AUDs (Abel, Bush, Dintcheff, 1981; Alati et al., 2006; Arias & Chotro, 2005; Dominguez, Lopez & Molina, 1998; Osborn, Yu, Gabriel & Weinberg, 1998). There is also increasing evidence to indicate that youth exposure to alcohol may be relevant to the development of AUDs. One of the most reliable findings in the epidemiology of AUDs is that early onset of drinking is associated with increased likelihood of developing an AUD (Grant & Dawson, 1997; Hingson, Heeren, & Winter, 2006). What is not clear at this point is the extent that these findings represent a clear causal mechanism (perhaps because of increased sensitivity of the brain to alcohol or alcohol-related cues) or are an artifact of a common “third variable” associated with both early deviant behavior and the development of AUDs. Currently the data are ambiguous, with some evidence suggesting that the early alcohol drinking → AUD link is an spurious effect of correlated disinhibitory problems (Prescott & Kendler, 1999), some a true genotype x environment interaction effect (Agrawal et al., 2009) and it remains unclear whether common third-variable causation is a sufficient explanation (McGue & Iacono, 2008; Odgers et al., 2008).

It is possible that early alcohol exposure could contribute to risk for dependence by at least two different mechanisms: (1) by altering ethanol sensitivity or alcohol-seeking processes, and (2) by inducing neurocognitive impairment that ultimately could relate to impaired self-control processes. With respect to altering ethanol sensitivity and alcohol seeking, adolescent rats (like humans) tend to consume more alcohol than their adult counterparts and this is not just a function of global consummatory behavior (hyperphagia, hyperdipsia) suggesting enhanced rewarding or decreased aversive responses at this stage of life (Vetter, Doremus-Fitzwater, & Spear, 2007). Adolescent alcohol exposure in rats has been shown to be associated with high levels of appetitive motivation for alcohol (e.g., Pautassi et al., 2008), increased hypothermia (Ristuccia & Spear, 2008), increased stress-induced alcohol consumption (Fullgrabe, Vengelience, & Spanagel, 2007), altered alcohol-induced social facilitation s (Varlinskaya & Spear, 2007), and decreased sensitivity to some but not all symptoms of “hangover” (acute withdrawal; Doremus-Fitzwater & Spear, 2007). At present, it remains unclear to what extent adolescent exposure and drinking behaviors predicts adult variation in alcohol consumption in rats (Vetter et al., 2007). However, adolescence is the primary period of initiation for alcohol consumption in humans and rodent data indicate that adolescents respond differently than adults to both reinforcing and punishing effects of alcohol and they drink different.

Accumulating evidence suggests that adolescent humans and rats may be more sensitive than adults to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol (Monti et al., 2005). Heavy exposure to alcohol in adolescence has been associated with structural and functional brain deficits, and also deficits in cognitive functioning (Clark, Thatcher & Tapert, 2008; De Bellis, Clark, Beers, Soloff, Boring, Hall, et al., 2000; Hargreaves, Quinn, Kashem, Matsumoto & McGregor, 2009; Tapert, Brown, Baratta, Brown, 2004; Zeigler, Wang, Yoast, Dickinson, McCaffree, Robinowitz, et al., 2005). The seemingly heightened sensitivity of the adolescent brain to alcohol-related insult is thought to be associated with neurodevelopmental vulnerability to disruption of the extensive remodeling of the brain that takes place in adolescence (e.g., synaptic pruning; Clark, Thatcher, & Tapert, 2008). The associated neurocognitive deficits, especially those associated with deficits in executive functioning (Monti et al., 2005), could pose added risk for a range of externalizing behavior problems in addition to alcohol dependence. Although a definite causal relation in humans has yet to be established, rodent models of adolescent ethanol exposure (Crews et al., 2000; Spear, 2000; Swartzwelder, Wilson, & Tayyeb, 1995) demonstrate that adolescence appears to be a time of heightened sensitivity to persistent neurologic damage as well as priming enhanced sensitivity for some alcohol effects and tolerance for others.

The Peer Social Environment

Alcohol consumption in humans, at least consumption that leads to intoxication, is largely a social phenomenon. Perhaps the strongest environmental correlate of alcohol use and alcohol problems is peer use, with adolescents and young adults showing a strong resemblance with their peers with respect to their substance use. This similarity reflects two complementary and interacting processes: socialization or causation, and selection (Andrews et al. 2002). Socialization describes the phenomenon where an individual’s alcohol use is shaped by influence from the peer group. In this case, affiliations with substance using peers may encourage greater involvement with alcohol through various mechanisms, including social learning, peer group influence, modeling, and social facilitation (Fergusson et al. 2002; Deater-Deckard 2001). Conversely, the process of selection occurs when adolescents seek affiliation with peers who display similar patterns of substance use or deviant behavior. Research suggests that adolescents from disadvantaged, dysfunctional, or disturbed environments or those with a predisposition towards antisocial behavior are most likely to become involved with deviant peer groups through the selection process (Fergusson et al. 1999). In either case, the proportion of peer associates who use alcohol and engage in deviant behavior is a powerful predictor of the development of alcohol abuse and dependence in adolescence (Fergusson et al. 2002; Windle 2000).

The peer use environment illustrates some of the dilemmas surrounding the consideration of environmental risk factors since a substantial amount of this ostensible “environmental risk factor,” perhaps as much as 50% by adulthood, is attributable to additive genetic effects (Kendler et al., 2007). The apparent, genetically influenced “niche seeking” should not be interpreted to mean that the environment does not play an important causal role in the outcome of alcohol-related pathology. It does suggest, however, that it is not just naïve but probably incorrect to view the peer environment as an independent, environmental risk factor. Moreover, even in the context of strong selection into high drinking social environments (e.g., college fraternities and sororities), there remains nontrivial socialization effects that appear to reinforce the selection or even lead to onset or escalation in lighter drinkers (Park et al., 2009). Twin studies have suggested that the correlation between peers’ substance use and an adolescent’s own substance use reflects both genetic (selection) and environmental (socialization) processes (Dick et al., 2007a).

When considering the peer environment as an important environmental factor in drinking, simple social influence (e.g., coercive or imitative modeling) and availability are not the only mechanisms that may be considered. Research on drinking motivations (Cooper et al., 1994) and alcohol expectancies (e.g., Brown et al., 1980; Fromme et al., 1993) indicates that drinkers report that alcohol enhances social reinforcement and that they drink to enhance social interaction. Experimental studies (e.g., Sher, 1985) indicate that alcohol is perceived as more pleasurable and activating when consumed in group versus solitary contexts. Indeed, most human consumption occurs in social contexts.

The possibility that some of the most etiologically relevant of alcohol’s reinforcing effects are conditional upon social interaction, at least early in a drinker’s drinking career (e.g., during adolescence), is consistent with neuropharmacological views of alcohol (and other drugs of abuse affecting dopamine circuits; Berridge and Robinson, 1998) increasing the salience of natural incentives. There is direct evidence that interaction with familiar conspecifics is reinforcing in rats (Douglas, Varlinskaya, & Spear, 2004) and that ethanol facilitates social interactions (esp. in adolescents). Moreover, opiate antagonists block some aspects of social interaction (e.g., play fighting) implicating opioid mechanisms in this seemingly consilient ethanol effect (Varlinskaya, & Spear, 2009). Note, however, as drinking progresses to dependence in vulnerable individuals, it seems likely that the salience of the drug effect (independent of enhanced social reinforcement) increases, in effect, “hijacking” brain circuits for social and other natural reinforcers. Thus, the notion of alcohol’s rewarding properties being heightened in the context of social interaction is likely a function of stage of one’s drinking career as well as stage of development.

The role of the social environment in animal modeling of GXE presents a number of interesting opportunities and challenges. The opportunities stem from the ability to carefully manipulate aspects of the social environment to determine the exact nature of the social environmental effect (e.g., imitative modeling, pharmacological enhancement of normal social reinforcement), specifying the neurobiological mechanisms underpinning such an effect, and relating such mechanisms to candidate genes. The challenges arise when trying to find appropriate models or even species where relevant social behaviors “have evolved in their natural ethological and ecological contexts” (IOM [2006], p. 134). From this perspective, whether or not an animal appears to imitate another animal consuming alcohol isn’t a sufficient criterion for deeming it a consilient model. The issue is why it is doing so and whether it serves a similar function.

The Potential Importance of Species Selection for Building Consilient Models

As noted above, alcohol use in humans is largely a social phenomenon. Moreover, human pair bonding (and parenthood) is associated with both general decreases in alcohol consumption (“the marriage effect”) and reciprocal dyadic influences within the marital dyad (Leonard & Rothbard, 1999). Use of rat and rodent species that vary greatly from humans in both social activity and pair bonding may limit consilience and, thus, at least for studying some environmental influences, other species may be considered.

Perhaps the most obvious class of animals for building consilient animal models for human alcohol consumption is primates. The value of using primates to study GXE interactions involving childhood and proximal stressors on drinking has been clearly demonstrated (Barr et al., 2004, 2008) in the work in Rhesus macaques. However, the cost of conducting this type of research and heightened animal welfare concerns regarding primates constrain the extent that primates can be used and additional species should be considered.

Prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) are social and monogamous in the wild, forming life-long pair bonds, a trait shared with only 5% of mammalian species (Young et al., 1998). Many of these behavioral characteristics can also be studied in the laboratory. In contrast to a promiscuous and asocial vole species, montane voles (Microtus montanus), when studied in triads of one male and two females in a laboratory, male prairie voles huddle with one female selectively after mating with her. The behaviors were shown not to be a function of mate availability or female receptivity, but seem to represent a high degree of affiliative behavior (Shapiro and Dewsbury, 1990). Much of this research has been devoted to defining the neuroendocrine correlates of pair bonding, which predominantly feature signaling differences in arginine vasopressin (especially in the vasopressin 1a receptor) and oxytocin systems (Insel, 2003). Interestingly, one recent study suggested that polymorphisms in the human AVPR1A receptor gene were associated with pair bonding in humans (Walum et al., 2008). Some studies have also examined paternal care, male other aspects of social behavior, and various aspects of agonistic behavior (Young et al., 1998).

Could pair bonding, paternal care of pups, or other social behaviors in prairie voles offer an environmental behavioral context for exploring the importance of GXE on alcoholism? One recent study suggests that this might be possible. Bosch et al. allowed male and female prairie voles to pair bond for several days, during which time they presumably copulated and entered the post-copulatory period (after the first 24-72 hr of exposure), although mating was not recorded. Some pairs were then separated. Separated animals showed longer durations of floating in the forced swim test, and increased durations of “passive stress-coping” (i.e., hanging without struggling) in the tail suspension test but showed no differences from controls in the elevated plus maze (Bosch et al., 2009). Further studies in this well-controlled experiment implicated the CRF-1 receptor in these behavioral changes. Thus, the pair bonding behavior appears to be amenable to experimental manipulation.

However, it is not entirely clear how one would interpret such studies. The Bosch study used these particular tests as putative analogs of “depression-like” behavior and “anxiety-like” behavior. In other words, they were used to assess presumed negative emotional states upon removal of the presumably rewarding state of pair bonding. Others have noted that the behavior of floating appears to signify different emotional states and behavioral coping strategies to different observers – i.e., its construct validity is questionable (Petit-Demouliere et al., 2005). In contrast, most of the literature surrounding the comparison of prairie and montane voles has cast them in the role of an animal model of autistic behavior (Hammock and Young, 2006). Finally, as mentioned above, most of the vole research in this area has been at the molecular level.

A recent study paired adult prairie voles with one male sib for five days and then individually housed the animals. Some were housed with continued “social,” or at least visual, auditory, and olfactory, access to their sibling through a wire mesh screen, and others were housed alone. All had access to water and ethanol and drinking preference was assessed. Isolated voles drank less than those with social access to a littermate. Drinking in littermates housed with access was highly correlated, while sibling drinking in isolated pairs was not. (Anacker & Ryabinin, 2009).

Besides the above unpublished study, that this is an area with intriguing possibilities for alcohol research is based almost entirely on the biological data implicating vasopressin and oxytocin. In the 1980’s, vasopressin was shown to restore normal ethanol drinking in Brattleboro rats genetically lacking vasopressin (Rigter and Crabbe, 1982). More recently, mice with a targeted disruption of the Avp1r gene showed increased alcohol consumption compared to wild type (Sanbe et al., 2008). The vasopressin 1A receptor has also been implicated in partially mediating some of the differences in maternal behavior transmitted epigenetically by the group of Michael Meaney and collaborators (Champagne et al., 2003). Other social isolation-related behaviors have been studied extensively in rats and mice, and there is some evidence for a role for vasopressin as well. For example, brain-regional receptor densities for V1aR, oxytocin, and CRF receptors predict the response of rats to isolation housing, specifically the potentiation of the startle reflex induced by isolation (Nair et al., 2005). Manipulations of the pre and postnatal environment using cross fostering and /or ova transplant designs could be applied to prairie voles or other high or low drinking rodent genotypes to begin to tease apart the influences of drinking from observing drinking in cage mates. Similarly, such a study could be performed in a genetically segregating genotype by screening mice at weaning for high or low drinking individuals to use as cage mates. Voles (or possibly mice) could be offered the opportunity to huddle with one of two previous cage mates, a high drinker with which it had previously been paired vs a low drinker. These sorts of designs could be applied to prairie voles or other high or low drinking genotypes to begin to tease apart the influences of drinking from observing drinking in cage mates. It may be useful to pursue the role of vasopressin more widely in such experiments. Similarly, such a study could be performed in a genetically segregating genotype by screening mice at weaning and selecting high or low drinking individuals for cage mates. Such studies might help to sort out GXE from gene-environment correlation.

Human Laboratory Models

Human laboratory models offer an opportunity to examine the effects of GXE interactions on ‘translational phenotypes’ that bridge human and animal models (Hutchison, 2008). Translational phenotypes can be derived from either animal or human studies that are designed to elucidate the neuroanatomical and pharmacological mechanisms that underlie the etiology of addiction. For example, animal models can help identify and evaluate GXE interactions comparing animals with different genetic backgrounds that are exposed to specific environmental variables. Likewise, human laboratory models can be used to compare humans that differ on specific polymorphisms and have been exposed to specific environmental manipulations.

As an example, the motivation to use drugs has been linked to the mesolimbic and mesocortical networks in the brain (the neural substrates that putatively underlie the attribution of incentive motivation to drugs of abuse) and is thought to be an important factor in the etiology of addiction (Berridge & Robinson, 1998; Kalivas & Volkow, 2005; Robinson & Berridge, 1993; Wise, 1988). The most widely used animal model for investigating the neurobiology of drug-seeking behavior is the drug reinstatement model (Epstein, Preston, Stewart, & Shaham, 2006). Here, the animal is exposed chronically to a drug and a drug-related cue, withdrawn from the drug, and then subsequently re-exposed to the cue. Although some concerns regarding the validity of the model have been raised (see Epstein et al., 2006), this approach has been extremely useful for identifying the neurobiological mechanisms that underlie drug-seeking behavior. To examine a gene by environment interaction, animals that have been genetically modified by “knocking in” human genes with different versions of a specific polymorphism or more generally “knocking out” the gene can be exposed to specific environmental variables (e.g., repeated exposure to a drug or drug cues). Key neuroanatomical loci and pathways in the rat include the VTA, nucleus accumbens, prefrontal cortex, dorsal striatum, and basolateral amygdala (see Kalivas & Volkow, 2005). Changes in the loci and pathways that result from the combination of the gene and the environmental exposure can be conceptualized as a GXE interaction.

In the specific example above and more generally, there are three points where animal and human research can make significant contact in terms of the study of GXE interactions. One point is represented by common ground in specific experimental models of exposure to a given environmental variable. A second point of contact is the genetic variable in question (e.g., gene, gene network), and the third point of contact is the biological measures that are analogous across the human and animal studies.

For example, neuroimaging approaches have been used to examine the effect of specific environmental manipulations such as exposure to a drug cue or drug itself on neurocognitive function in humans. In fact, studies with humans have proven to be largely consistent with the animal literature, leading to the integration of translational models that span animal and human findings (Kalivas & Volkow, 2005). Thus, for a GXE study, there are clearly approaches that provide common ground in terms of the environmental manipulation (e.g., exposure to drug cues) and phenotypes (e.g., changes in neurobiological variables). There are also a number of human studies that have examined the effect of GXE interactions in the context of neuroimaging. For example, several studies have demonstrated an interaction between an environmental manipulation (exposure to a drug cue) and a specific polymorphism on brain function (e.g., Hutchison et al., 2008; Filbey et al., 2008, McClernon et al., 2007). It seems likely that these observations actually reflect the long-term consequences of a gene by chronic drug exposure interaction that alters the neurobiological pathways that mediate the attribution of incentive salience to drug related stimuli.

Another point of contact between animal and human studies of G x E interactions can be found in pharmacogenetic studies which we can view as a special case of GXE where the “environmental” variable is exposure to a medication. Recent examples of these studies in humans include studies on the interaction between exposure to alcohol and genetic variation in the OPRM1 gene and naltrexone (Ray & Hutchison, 2007) and the interaction between genetic variation in the CHRNA4 gene and the acute effects of nicotine (Hutchison et al., 2007).

It is important to highlight that with regard to OPRM1, existing studies have generally not found a significant association with substance use disorder diagnoses (Arias et al., 2006). However, a functional single nucleotide polymorphism of the OPRM1 gene may predict response to naltrexone, an opiate antagonist that has actions at mu opiate receptors. Specifically, carriers of the OPRM1 +118G allele have been found to respond better to naltrexone therapy for alcohol dependence relative to homozygotes for the OPRM1 +118A allele (Anton et al., 2008; Oslin et al, 2003); although not all studies support this finding (e.g., Gelernter et al., 2007).

Human laboratory studies have identified that carriers of the G allele have increased subjective alcohol effects (Ray & Hutchison, 2004), stronger cue-induced craving (van den Wildenberg et al., 2007) and stronger automatic approach tendencies toward alcohol and other appetitive stimuli (Wiers et al., 2009). Moreover, these various facets of alcohol approach motivation and reward such as alcohol stimulation, positive mood, craving and enjoyment are blunted by naltrexone, particularly for those with the G allele (Ray & Hutchison, 2007). It is therefore of considerable interest that seemingly consilient findings can be observed in nonhuman animals. For example, deletion of the mu opioid receptor gene in mice abolishes ethanol drinking (Roberts et al., 2000) and reduces motor impulsivity and the enhancement of motor impulsivity by ethanol (Olmstead et al., 2009). In primates, carriers of a functionally equivalent variant of the mu-opioid receptor had higher alcohol preference that naltrexone reduced providing additional evidence supporting the therapeutic potential of naltrexone. Efforts such as these that work across human and animal studies to examine how genes modify responses to pharmacological agents may ultimately inform our understanding of different pathways to the development of alcohol dependence without finding an association with alcohol dependence per se (Weirs et al., 2009).

Finally, one other critical environmental variable to examine in the context of GXE interactions, in both animal and human models, is stress (for review see Clarke & Schumann, 2009). Recent studies have suggested that exposure to stress has a important impact on the progression of psychopathology, especially during specific developmental periods, and this impact is moderated by genetic factors (Stevens et al., 2009). While there are excellent experimental models of the effects of stress in animals (e.g., Heilig & Koob, 2007) and in humans (Greeley & Oei, 1999; Sher, 1987; Sher & Grekin, 2007), it is important to note that the intensity and duration of stressors that can be ethically administered to human participants in the laboratory is limited, thus, restricting our ability to examine traumatic stress exposures in the laboratory. Consequently, the examination of gene x stress interactions may be largely limited to retrospective/observational studies in humans if severe or traumatic stress is the target of inquiry. (Although in recent years electronic diaries and ecological momentary assessment have been used to study alcohol/stress interactions in “real time” in the natural environment [see Sher & Grekin, 2007], the base rate of traumatic exposures is likely too low to employ this technology to assessment traumatic stress.) Animal models can therefore provide critical information that may be difficult to obtain in humans.

Clearly, experimental approaches are more feasible when employing nontraumatic levels of stress in humans. Indeed, some of the largest individual differences in alcohol sensitivity associated with family history of alcoholism are in the domain of autonomic stress reactivity (e.g., Finn, Zeitouni, & Pihl, 1990; Levenson, Oyama, & Meek, 1987). Additionally, there are a number of paradigms for assessing negative affect/stress that translate across human and mammalian species for examining stress reactivity using psychophysiological measures (e.g., affect-potentiated startle) and behavioral measures (e.g., passive avoidance) that have been shown to be alcohol sensitive (e.g., Donohue, Curtin, & Lang, 2007; Sher, 1987; Sher & Grekin, 2005). Moreover, human laboratory models of acute exposure to stress (e.g., Sinha, 2001; Sinha et al., 2006) and the examination of genetic variation that may interact with acute stress (see Clarke & Schumann, 2009) has proven valuable in the study of relapse and clinical alcohol dependence. Given that there is some controversy over the role of stress in the progression of alcohol dependence, translational models that bridge the animal and human work are sorely needed to develop a better understanding of stress and genetic factors that may interact with exposure to stress.

CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS

One phenotype where we would expect to find compelling evidence for gene-environment interaction effects is the alcohol dependence phenotype, considering its strong association in the parental generation with childhood environmental adversities in the offspring generation and heightened opportunities for social learning of excessive consumption patterns. It is plausible, but arguably not yet convincingly established, that greater consideration of such GXE effects will facilitate the goals of identifying more of the genes that are contributing to risk of alcohol dependence, as well as of understanding how (including under which environmental conditions) they are having effects. In these undertakings, however, it is critical to remember that much of the genetic effects on alcohol dependence appear to represent broad liabilitities to a range of externalizing behaviors and other substance dependence. This suggests that understanding relevant GXE interactions for alcohol dependence may involve studying nonspecific genes and environmental exposures and not focusing exclusively on alcohol-related phenotypes. Characterizing relevant endophenotypes that can be generalized across species and that are relevant to multiple behavior problems would be advanced, but further delineation of the relevant neurocircuitry and behavioral processes that are believed to subserve alcohol dependence more specifically would still be needed.

Achieving consilience faces a number of potent challenges. In the human literature, we lack an organizing taxonomy of environmental exposures. Moreover, the lack of experimental control over environmental exposures, and in particular the tendency of multiple adverse exposures to cluster in the same individuals, families, neighborhoods etc., both cross-sectionally and over time, makes disentangling individual risk-factor effects that might interact with genetic differences a major challenge. While latent variable methods (e.g. inferring genetic effects from twin data) hold considerable promise for identifying major environmental domains that may moderate aggregate effects of genetic variants such methods, involving comparison of differences in familial correlation (e.g. in MZ-DZ correlation differences) as a function of environmental exposure, have relatively low power and thus can detect only large interaction effects (although it remains an open question whether they will yield greater insight into GXE effects than SNP by SNP GXE analyses, given the small effect sizes anticipated for the latter). Isolated reports of GXE effects in individual samples have in general not yet led to the coordinated efforts at cross-study replication, including meta-analyses, that would make findings convincing both in terms of confirmation of GXE effects, and through clarification of the specific aspects of environmental exposure (e.g. of the many environmental differences associated with urban versus rural residence) that may be contributing to interaction effects.

In the domain of human measured genotype studies, GWAS studies offer the prospect of novel genetic findings (albeit most probably on a more limited scale than was initially hoped). However, in the alcohol field, such studies have been designed based on the AUD phenotype, without regard to stratification by environmental exposure history. Thus, they will also have limited power to model GXE effects and use these to inform gene discovery. Despite high initial enthusiasm for the single genetic variant – single exposure approach that was illustrated in the serotonin transporter work of Caspi et al. (2003), the ultimate failure of replication attempts (Munafo et al., 2009; Risch et al), and low power of studies reporting positive findings (Munafo et al., 2009) cautions against undue confidence in the existing literature. In particular, studies based on sample sizes that are at least an order of magnitude larger than the first generation of studies (which typically had under a thousand individuals per sample) will be needed to give some confidence in findings (esp. for interactions involving rare environmental exposures). Studies in the human experimental laboratory, using experimentally controlled stressors or other manipulations, with human subjects selected on the basis of a genetic variant, and perhaps stratified by early environmental exposure, offer some hope of furthering understanding interaction effects with known genetic variants. Yet, even here, likely modest gene effect sizes (c.f. Flint and Munafo, 2007), except for variables such as metabolism, and greater limitations on feasible sample size, will be an important limiting factor.

We have noted that non-human research offers many attractions for characterizing gene-environment interaction effects, through its ability to work at multiple levels of biological organization (neurocircuitry, cellular, genetic, epigenetic), including in particular specific genotype x specific environment interaction effects. The challenge for work in this area is to identify consilient environmental exposures and associated neurobehavioral processes that are analogous in human and nonhuman species even if not strictly homologous. Such processes can be found in multiple domains including punishment and reward systems (associated with motivations to consume alcohol), self-regulatory systems (associated with the ability to inhibit consumption in the context of drinking motivation), and neurocognitive abilities (e.g.learning processes more generally). It seems possible, if not likely, that more careful consideration of specific species for studying specific environmental variables of relevance to humans will result in more theoretically informed and ecologically valid models.

While most environments studied to date have been viewed as having durable effects on constitutional, motivational propensities and self-regulation, there are a range of possible situational environments that are associated with likelihood of consumption in humans and nonhuman animals. Although some of these would seem to be uniquely human (e.g., various celebrations and socially sanctioned rituals; MacAndrew & Edgerton, 1969), others related to availability, cost, competing demands, and social stimulation would appear to be directly translatable and important for understanding human “real world drinking” where situational goads and constraints represent powerful but not absolute effects on behavior. The ultimate goal is to inform human alcohol use and dependence; establishing meaningful consilience of human and nonhuman animal studies represents considerably more than demonstrating similarity of inputs and genes on alcohol-related behaviors and establishing similar mediating processes linking genes and environments to phenotypes of interest.

Acknowledgements

The work on this manuscript was supported in part by NIH grants AA010760 (JCC), AA013519 (JCC), AA015416 (DMD), AA011998 (ACH), AA007728 (ACH), AA017688 (ACH), AA012238 (KEH), AA017242 (KJS), AA016392 (KJS), AA014715 (SSO), AA012870 (SSO) and the US Department of Veterans Affairs (JCC).

Footnotes

The Institute of Medicine (2006) report’s chapter, “The Role of Animal Models,” notes (p.132) that “the commissioned paper submitted by Steve W. Cole was used in the preparation of [the] report.”

Contributor Information

Kenneth J. Sher, University of Missouri-Columbia and the Midwest Alcoholism Research Center

Danielle M. Dick, Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University

John C. Crabbe, Portland Alcohol Research Center, Veterans Affairs Medical Center and the Oregon Health & Science University

Kent E. Hutchison, University of New Mexico and the Mind Research Network

Stephanie S. O’Malley, Yale University School of Medicine

Andrew C. Heath, Washington University School of Medicine and the Midwest Alcoholism Research Center

Reference List

  1. Abel EL, Bush R, Dintcheff BA. Exposure of rats to alcohol in utero alters drug sensitivity in adulthood. Science. 1981;212:1531–1533. doi: 10.1126/science.7233243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. The classification of child psychopathology: a review and analysis of empirical efforts. Psychol Bull. 1978;85:1275–1301. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Agrawal A, Dick DM, Bucholz KK, Madden PA, Cooper ML, Sher KJ, Heath AC. Drinking expectancies and motives: A genetic study of young adult women. Addiction. 2008;103:194–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.02074.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Agrawal A, Sartor CE, Lynskey MT, Grant JD, Pergadia ML, Grucza R, Bucholz KK, Nelson EC, Madden PAF, Martin NG, Heath AC. Evidence for an interaction between age at 1st drink and genetic influences on DSM-IV alcohol dependence symptoms. Alc Clin Exp Res. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01044.x. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alati R, AlMamum A, Williams GM, O’Callagham M, Najman JM, Bor W. In utero alcohol exposure and prediction of alcohol disorders in early adulthood: A birth cohort study. Arch of GenPsychiatry. 2006;63:1009–1016. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.9.1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Author; Washington, DC: 2000. Text Revision. [Google Scholar]
  7. Anacker AMJ, Ryabinin AE. The effect of social affiliations or social isolation on alcohol drinking in prairie voles. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2009;33((6) Suppl. 210A) [Google Scholar]
  8. Andrews JA, Tildesley E, Hops H, Li F. The influence of peers on young adult substance use. Health Psychol. 2002;21(4):349–57. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.21.4.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Anton RF, Oroszi G, O’Malley S, Couper D, Swift R, Pettinati H, et al. An evaluation of mu-opioid receptor (OPRM1) as a predictor of naltrexone response in the treatment of alcohol dependence: results from the Combined Pharmacotherapies and Behavioral Interventions for Alcohol Dependence (COMBINE) study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(2):135–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.2.135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Arias A, Feinn R, Kranzler HR. Association of an Asn40Asp (A118G) polymorphism in the mu-opioid receptor gene with substance dependence: a meta-analysis. Drug Alc Dependence. 2006;83:262–268. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Arias C, Chotro MG. Increased preference for ethanol in the infant rat after prenatal ethanol exposure, expressed on intake and taste reactivity tests. Alcoholism: Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:337–346. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000156115.35817.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Barr CS, Chen SA, Schwandt ML, Lindell SG, Sun H, Suomi SJ, Heilig M. Suppression of alcohol preference by naltrexone in Rhesus Macaque: A critical role of genetiv variation at the mu-opioid receptor gene locus. Biol Psychiatry. 2009 Sept 11; doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barr CS, Dvoskin RL, Yuan Q, Lipsky RH, Gupte M, Hu X, Zhou Z, Schwandt ML, Lindell SG, McKee M, Becker ML, Kling MA, Gold PW, Higley D, Heilig M, Suomi SJ, Goldman D. CRH haplotype as a factor influencing cerebrospinal fluid levels of corticotropin-releasing hormone, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity, temperament, and alcohol consumption in rhesus macaques. Arch Gen Psychiat. 2008;65(8):934–944. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.8.934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Barr CS, Newman TK, Lindell S, Shannon C, Champoux M, Lesch KP, Suomi SJ, Goldman D, Higley JD. Interaction between serotonin transporter gene variation and rearing condition in alcohol preference and consumption in female primates. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:1146–1152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Belsky J, Jonassaint C, Pluess M, Stanton M, Brummett B, Williams R. Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Mol Psychiat. 2009;14:746–754. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998;28(3):309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bosch OJ, Nair HP, Ahern TH, Neumann ID, Young LJ. The CRF system mediates increased passive stress-coping behavior following the loss of a bonded partner in a monogamous rodent. NPP. 2009;34:1406–1415. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brown SA, Goldman MS, Inn A, Anderson L. Expectations of reinforcement from alcohol: their domain and relation to drinking pattern. J Cons Clin Psychol. 1980;48:419–426. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.48.4.419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Button TM, Corley RP, Rhee SH, Hewitt JK, Young SE, Stallings MC. Delinquent peer affiliation and conduct problems: A twin study. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:554–564. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Button TM, Lau JY, Maughan B, Eley TC. Parental punitive discipline, negative life events and gene-environment interplay in the development of externalizing behavior. Psychol Med. 2008;38:29–39. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cadoret RJ, O’Gorman TW, Troughton E, Heywood E. Alcoholism and antisocial personality. Interrelationships, genetic and environmental factors. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42:161–167. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1985.01790250055007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cadoret RJ, Troughton E, O’Gorman TW. Genetic and environmental factors in alcohol abuse and antisocial personality. J Stud Alcohol. 1987;48:1–8. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1987.48.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cadoret RJ, Troughton E, O’Gorman TW, Heywood E. An adoption study of genetic and environmental factors in drug abuse. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43:1131–1136. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800120017004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chaloupka FJ, Grossman M, Saffer H. The effects of price on alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems. Alc Res Hlth. 2002;26(1):22–34. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Champagne FA, Francis DD, Mar A, Meaney MJ. Variations in maternal care in the rat as a mediating influence for the effects of environment on development. Physiol Behav. 2003;79:359–371. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00149-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Clark DB, Thatcher DL, Tapert SF. Alcohol, psychological dysregulation, and adolescent brain development. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:375–385. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00601.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Clarke TK, Schumann G. Gene-environment interactions resulting in risk alcohol drinking behaviour are mediated by CRF and CRF1. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;93(3):230–236. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cloninger CR, Bohman M, Sigvardsson S, von Knorring AL, The Stockholm Adoption Study Psychopathology in adopted-out children of alcoholics. Recent Dev Alcohol. 1985;3:37–51. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-7715-7_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cooper ML. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(2):117–128. [Google Scholar]
  30. Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P. Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69:990–1005. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Cowley MJ, Cotsapas CJ, Williams RB, Chan EK, Pulvers JN, Liu MY, Luo OJ, Nott DJ, Little PF. Intra- and inter-individual genetic differences in gene expression. Mamm Genome. 2009;20:281–295. doi: 10.1007/s00335-009-9181-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Crabbe JC, Metten P, Rhodes JS, Yu C-H, Brown LL, Phillips TJ, Finn DA. A line of mice selected for high blood ethanol concentrations shows drinking in the dark to intoxication. Biol Psychiat. 2009;65:262–270. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Crews FT, Braun CJ, Hoplight B, Switzer RC, Knapp DJ. Binge ethanol consumption causes differential brain damage in young adolescent rats compared with adult rats: Alcohol effects on the fetus, brain, liver, and other organ systems. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2000;24:1712–1723. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Cryan JF, Holmes A. The ascent of mouse: advances in modelling human depression and anxiety. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2005;4(9):775–790. doi: 10.1038/nrd1825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Cryan JF, Slattery DA. Animal models of mood disorders: Recent developments. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2007;20:1–7. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280117733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Deater-Deckard K. Annotation: Recent research examining the role of peer relationships in the development of psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(5):565–579. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. De Bellis MD, Clark DB, Beers SR, Soloff PH, Boring AM, Hall J, Kersh A, Keshavan MS. Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:737–744. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dick DM, Bernard M, Aliev F, Viken R, Pulkkinen L, Kaprio J, Rose RJ. The role of socio-regional factors in moderating genetic influences on early adolescent behavior problems and alcohol use. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;33(10):1739–1748. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01011.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dick DM, Pagan JL, Holliday C, Viken R, Pulkkinen L, Kaprio J, Rose RJ. Gender differences in friends’ influences on adolescent drinking: A genetic epidemiological study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007a;31:2012–2019. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00523.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Dick DM, Pagan JL, Viken R, Purcell S, Kaprio J, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ. Changing environmental influences on substance use across development. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007b;10:315–326. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.2.315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dick DM, Rose RJ, Viken RJ, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M. Exploring gene-environment interactions: Socioregional moderation of alcohol use. J Abnorm Psychol. 2001a;110:625–632. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.4.625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dick DM, Smith G, Olausson P, Mitchell SM, Leeman RF, O’Malley SS, Sher KJ. Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addict Biol. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2009.00190.x. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Dick DM, Viken R, Purcell S, Kaprio J, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ. Parental monitoring moderates the importance of genetic and environmental influences on adolescent smoking. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007c;116:213–218. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Dominguez HD, Lopez MF, Molina JC. Neonatal responsiveness to alcohol odor and infant alcohol intake as a function of alcohol experience during late gestation. Alcohol. 1998;16:109–117. doi: 10.1016/s0741-8329(97)00169-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Donohue K, Curtin JJ, Patrick CJ, Lang AR. Intoxication level and emotional response. Emotion. 2007;7:103–112. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Doremus- Fitzwater TL, Spear LP. Developmental differences in acute ethanol withdrawal in adolescent and adult rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31(9):1516–1527. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00457.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Douglas LA, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Rewarding properties of social interactions in adolescent and adult male and female rates: Impact of social versus isolate housing of subjects. Dev Psychobiol. 2004;45(3):153–162. doi: 10.1002/dev.20025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Eaves LJ. Inferring the causes of human variation. J Roy Stat Soc A. 1977;140:324–355. [Google Scholar]
  49. Eaves LJ. Genotype x Environment interaction in psychopathology: fact or artifact? Twin Res Hum Genet. 2006;9:1–8. doi: 10.1375/183242706776403073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Englund MM, Egeland B, Oliva EM, Collins WA. Childhood and adolescent predictors of heavy drinking and alcohol use disorders in early adulthood: a longitudinal developmental analysis. Addiction. 2008;103(Suppl. 1):23–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02174.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Epstein DH, Preston KL, Stewart J, Sharham Y. Toward a model of drug relapse: An assessment of the validity of the reinstatement procedure. Psychopharmacology. 2006;189(1):1–16. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0529-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Falk JL, Samson HH, Winger G. Behavioral maintenance of high concentrations of blood ethanol and physical dependence in the rat. Science. 1972;177:811–813. doi: 10.1126/science.177.4051.811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Feinberg ME, Button TM, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Hetherington EM. Parenting and adolescent antisocial behavior and depression: evidence of genotype x parenting environment interaction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:457–465. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.4.457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Fergusson DM, Swain-Campbell NR, Horwood LJ. Deviant peer affiliations, crime and substance use: a fixed effects regression analysis. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2002;30(4):419–430. doi: 10.1023/a:1015774125952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Fergusson DM, Woodward LJ, Horwood LJ. Childhood peer relationship problems and young people’s involvement with deviant peers in adolescence. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1999;27(5):357–369. doi: 10.1023/a:1021923917494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Filbey FM, Ray L, Smolen A, Claus ED, Audette A, Hutchison KE. Differential neural response to alcohol priming and alcohol taste cues is associated with DRD4 VNTR and OPRM1 genotypes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32(7):1113–1123. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00692.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Finn PR, Zeitouni NC, Pihl RO. Effects of alcohol on psychophysiological hyperreactivity to nonaversive and aversive stimuli in men at high risk for alcoholism. J Abnorm Psychol. 1990 Feb;99(1):79–85. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.99.1.79. PubMed PMID: 2307770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Flint J, Munafo NR. The endophenotype concept in psychiatric genetics. Psychol Med. 2007;37:163–180. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706008750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Fromme K, Stroot E, Kaplan D. Comprehensive effects of alcohol: development and psychometric assessment of a new expectancy questionnaire. Psychological Assess. 1993;5:19–26. [Google Scholar]
  60. Füllgrabe MW, Vengeliene V, Spanagel R. Influence of age at drinking onset on the alcohol deprivation effect and stress-induced drinking in female rats. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2007;86:320–326. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gelernter J, Gueorgueva R, Kranzler H, Zhang H, Cramer J, Rosenheck R, Krystal JH. Opioid receptor gene (OPRM1, OPRK1, and OPRD1) variants and response to naltrexone treatment for alcohol dependence: Results from the VA cooperative study. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31(4):555–563. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00339.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Goldstein DB. Common genetic variation and human traits. N Engl J Med. 2009;360:1696–1698. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp0806284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Moller N, Hermansen L, Winokur G, Guze SB. Drinking problems in adopted and nonadopted sons of alcoholics. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974;31(2):164–169. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1974.01760140022003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Grant BF, Dawson DA. Age at onset of alcohol use and its association with DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J Subst Abuse. 1997;9:103–110. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(97)90009-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Greeley J, Oei T. Alcohol and tension reduction. In: Leonard K, Blane H, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. 2nd ed. Guilford; New York: 1999. pp. 14–53. [Google Scholar]
  66. Haber JR, Jacob T. Alcoholism risk moderation by a socio-religious dimension. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68:912–922. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Hammock EA, Young LJ. Oxytocin, vasopressin and pair bonding: implications for autism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;361:2187–2198. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hanson DJ. Preventing alcohol abuse: Alcohol, culture, and control. Praeger; Westport, CT: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  69. Hargreaves GA, Quinn H, Kashem MA, Matsumoto I, McGregor IS. Proteomic analysis demonstrates adolescent vulnerability to lasting hippocampal changes following chronic alcohol consumption. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2009;33:86–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00814.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Heath AC, Jardine R, Martin NG. Interactive effects of genotype and social environment on alcohol consumption in female twins. J Stud Alcohol. 1989;50:38–48. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1989.50.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Heath AC, Lynskey MT, Waldron M. Child and Adolescent Substance Use and Substance Use Disorder. In: Rutter M, Bishop DM, Pine D, Scott S, Stevenson J, Taylor E, Thapar A, editors. Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 5th Edition Wiley-Blackwell; Oxford: 2008. pp. 565–586. [Google Scholar]
  72. Heath AC, Nelson EC. Effects of the interaction between genotype and environment: Research into the genetic epidemiology of alcohol dependence. Alcohol Res Health. 2002;26:193–201. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Heath AC, Slutske WS, Bucholz KK, Madden PAF, Martin NG. Behavioral genetic methods in prevention research: An overview. In: Bryant K, Windle M, West SG, editors. The Science of Prevention: Methodological Advances from Alcohol and Substance Abuse Research. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 1997. pp. 123–163. [Google Scholar]
  74. Heilig M, Koob GF. A key role for corticotropin-releasing factor in alcohol dependence. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(8):399–406. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hingson RW, Heeren T, Winter MR. Age of alcohol-dependence onset: Associations with severity of dependence and seeking treatment. Pediatrics. 2006;118:755–763. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Hirschhorn JN. Genomewide association studies: Illuminating biologic pathways. N Engl J Med. 2009;360:1699–1701. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp0808934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Holmes A, le Guisquet AM, Vogel E, Millstein RA, Leman S, Belzung C. Early life genetic, epigenetic and environmental factors shaping emotionality in rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:1335–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Hutchison KE. Alcohol dependence: neuroimaging and the development of translational phenotypes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32(7):1111–1112. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00691.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Hutchison KE, Allen DL, Filbey FM, Jepson C, Lerman C, Benowitz NL, Stitzel J, Bryan A, McGeary J, Haughey HM. CHRNA4 and tobacco dependence: from gene regulation to treatment outcome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(9):1078–1086. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.9.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Hutchison KE, Haughey H, Niculescu M, Schacht J, Kaiser A, Stitzel J, Horton WJ, Filbey F. The incentive salience of alcohol: translating the effects of genetic variant in CNR1. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(7):841–850. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.7.841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Insel TR. Is social attachment an addictive disorder? Physiol Behav. 2003;79:351–357. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00148-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Institute of Medicine . Genes, behavior, and the social environment: Moving beyond the nature/nurture debate. National Academies Press; Washington, DC: 2006. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Jacob T, Waterman B, Heath A, True W, Bucholz KK, Haber R, Scherrer J, Fu Q. Genetic and environmental effects on offspring alcoholism: New insights using an offspring-of-twins design. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:1265–1272. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Jessor R, Donovan JE, Costa FM. Beyond adolescence: Problem behavior and young adult development. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  85. Jessor R, Jessor SL. Problem behavior and psychosocial development: A longitudinal study of youth. Academic Press; San Diego, CA: 1977. [Google Scholar]
  86. Jinks JL, Fulker DW. Comparison of the biometrical genetical, MAVA and classical approaches to the analysis of human behavior. Psychol Bull. 1970;73:311–349. doi: 10.1037/h0029135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Kalivas PW, Volkow ND. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(8):1403–1413. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Kendler KS. “A gene for…”: the nature of gene action in psychiatric disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(7):1243–1252. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.7.1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Gardner CO, Gillespie N, Aggen SA, Prescott CA. Creating a social world: a developmental twin study of peer-group deviance. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(8):958–965. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Neale MC. Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants, and opiates in male twins. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(4):687–695. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Kendler KS, Myers J, Prescott CA. Factors for symptoms of cannabis, cocaine, alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(11):1313–1320. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Koopmans JR, Slutske WS, van Baal GCM, Boomsma DI. The influence of religion on alcohol use initiation: Evidence for genotype x environment interaction. Behav Genet. 1999;29:445–453. doi: 10.1023/a:1021679005623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Kraft P, Hunter DJ. Genetic risk prediction--are we there yet? N Engl J Med. 2009;360(17):1701–1703. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp0810107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Krueger RF, Markon KE. Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2006;2:111–133. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Krueger RF, Markon KE. Understanding psychopathology: Melding behavior genetics, personality, and quantitative psychology to develop an empirically based model. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008;15:113–117. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00418.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Krueger RF, Markon KE, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG. Externalizing psychopathology in adulthood: A dimensional-spectrum conceptualization and its implications for DSM-V. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:537–550. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Who drinks and why? A review of socio-demographic, personality, and contextual issues behind the drinking motives in young people. Psychol Addict Behav. 2006;31:1844–1857. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.12.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Leonard KE, Rothbard JC. Alcohol and the marriage effect. J Stud Alcohol. 1999:139–146. doi: 10.15288/jsas.1999.s13.139. Supplement No 13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Levenson RW, Oyama ON, Meek PS. Greater reinforcement from alcohol for those at risk: parental risk, personality risk, and sex. J Abnorm Psychol. 1987 Aug;96(3):242–53. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.96.3.242. PubMed PMID: 3680764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. MacAndrew C, Edgerton R. Drunken Comportment: A Social Explanation. Aldine; Chicago, IL: 1969. [Google Scholar]
  101. McClearn GE, Rodgers DA. Differences in alcohol preference among inbred strains of mice. Q J Stud Alc. 1959;20:691–695. [Google Scholar]
  102. McClernon FJ, Hutchison KE, Rose JE, Kozink RV. DRD4 VNTR polymorphism is associated with transient fMRI-BOLD responses to smoking cues. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;194(4):433–441. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0860-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. McGue M, Iacono WG. The adolescent origins of substance use disorders. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2008;17(Suppl. 1):S30–S38. doi: 10.1002/mpr.242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. McGue M, Sharma A, Benson P. Parent and sibling influences on adolescent alcohol use and misuse: evidence from a U.S. adoption cohort. J Stud Alcohol. 1996;57(1):8–18. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1996.57.8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M. Strategy for investigating interactions between measured genes and measured environments. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:473–481. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Monti PM, Miranda R, Nixon K, Sher KJ, Swartzwelder HS, Tapert SF, White A, Crews FT. Adolescence: Booze, brains and behavior. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:207–220. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000153551.11000.f3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Munafò MR, Durrant C, Lewis G, Flint J. Gene X environment interactions at the serotonin transporter locus. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Nair HP, Gutman AR, Davis M, Young LJ. Central oxytocin, vasopressin, and corticotropin-releasing factor receptor densities in the basal forebrain predict isolation potentiated startle in rats. J Neurosci. 2005;25:11479–11488. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2524-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Odgers CL, Caspi A, Nagin DS, Piquero AR, Slutske WS, Milne BJ, Dickson N, Poulton R, Moffitt TE. Is it important to prevent early exposure to drugs and alcohol among adolescents? Psychol Sci. 2008;19(10):1037–1044. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02196.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Olmstead MC, Ouagazzal A, Kieffer BL. Mu and delta opioid receptors oppositely regulate motor impulsivity in the signaled nose poke task. PLoS One. 2009;4:1–11. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Oslin DW, Berrettini W, Kranzler HR, Pettinati H, Gelernter J, Volpicelli JR, O’Brien CP. A Functional Polymorphism of the μ -Opioid Receptor Gene is Associated with Naltrexone Response in Alcohol-Dependent Patients. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:1546–1552. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Orford J. Excessive appetites: A psychological view of addictions. Wiley; Chichester, England: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  113. Osborn JA, Yu C, Gabriel K, Weinberg J. Fetal ethanol effects on benzodiazepine sensitivity measured by behavior on the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;60:625–633. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(98)00039-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Park A, Sher KJ, Krull JL, Wood PK. Dual mechanisms underlying accentuation of risky drinking via fraternity/sorority affiliation: The role of personality, peer norms, and alcohol availability. J Abnorm Psychol. 2009;118:241–245. doi: 10.1037/a0015126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Patterson GR, Reid JB, Dishion TJ. Antisocial Boys. Castalia; Eugene, OR: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  116. Pautassi RM, Myers M, Spear LP, Molina JC, Spear NE. Adolescent but not adult rats exhibit ethanol-mediated appetitive second-order conditioning. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32(11):2016–2027. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00789.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Petit-Demouliere B, Chenu F, Bourin M. Forced swimming test in mice: a review of antidepressant activity. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2005;177:245–255. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2048-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Prescott CA, Kendler KS. Age at first drink and risk for alcoholism: A noncausal association. Alc Clin Exp Res. 1999;23:101–107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Prescott CA, Cross RJ, Kuhn JW, Horn JL, Kendler KS. Is Risk for Alcoholism Mediated by Individual Differences in Drinking Motivations? Alc: Clin Exp Res. 2004;28(1):29–39. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000106302.75766.F0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Purcell S. Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Res. 2002;5:554–571. doi: 10.1375/136905202762342026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Randall CL, Lester D. Cross-fostering of DBA and C57Bl mice. Increase in voluntary consumption of alcohol by DBA weanlings. J Stud Alcohol. 1975a;36:973–980. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1975.36.973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Randall CL, Lester D. Social modification of alcohol consumption in inbred mice. Science. 1975b;189:149–151. doi: 10.1126/science.1138373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Rathouz PJ, Van Hulle CA, Rodgers JL, Waldman ID, Lahey BB. Specification, testing, and interpretation of gene-by-measured-environment interaction models in the presence of gene-environment correlation. Behav Genet. 2008;38:301–315. doi: 10.1007/s10519-008-9193-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Ray LA, Hutchison KE. Effects of naltrexone on alcohol sensitivity and genetic moderators of medication response: a double-blind placebo-controlled study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(9):1069–1077. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.9.1069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Rigter H, Crabbe JC. Ethanol preference in homozygous diabetes insipidus (Brattleboro) rats: effect of vasopressin fragments. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1982;394:663–671. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1982.tb37485.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang KY, Eaves L, Hoh J, Griem A, Kovacs M, Ott J, Merikangas KR. Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2009;301:2462–2471. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Ristuccia RC, Spear LP. Autonomic responses to ethanol in adolescent and adult rats: a dose-response analysis. Alcohol. 2008;42:623–629. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2008.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Roberts A, Mcdonald JS, Heyser CJ, Kieffer BL, Matthes HWD, Koob GF, Gold LH. Mu-opioid receptor knockout mice do not self-administer alcohol. J Pharm Exp Ter. 2000;293:1002–1008. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev. 1993;18:247–291. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Rose RJ, Dick DM, Viken RJ, Kaprio J. Gene-environment interaction in patterns of adolescent drinking: Regional residency moderates longitudinal influences on alcohol use. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2001;25:637–643. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Rutledge P, Park A, Sher KJ. 21st birthday drinking: Extremely extreme. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76:511–516. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Saccone NL, Kwon JM, Corbett J, Goate A, Rochberg N, Edenberg HJ, Foroud T, Li TK, Begleiter H, Reich T, Rice JP. A genome screen of maximum number of drinks as an alcoholism phenotype. Am J Med Genet. 2000;96:632–637. doi: 10.1002/1096-8628(20001009)96:5<632::aid-ajmg8>3.0.co;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Saccone SF, Saccone NL, Neuman RJ, Rice JP. Genetic analysis of the maximum drinks phenotype. BMC Genet. 2005;6(Suppl 1):S124. doi: 10.1186/1471-2156-6-S1-S124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Sanbe A, Takagi N, Fujiwara Y, Yamauchi J, Endo T, Mizutani R, Takeo S, Tsujimoto G, Tanoue A. Alcohol preference in mice lacking the Avpr1a vasopressin receptor. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2008;294:R1482–R1490. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00708.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Sartor CE, Agrawal A, McCutcheon VV, Duncan AE, Lynskey MT. Disentangling the complex associaiton between childhood sexual abuse and alcohol-related problems: A review of methodological issues and approaches. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2008;69:718–727. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Shapiro LE, Dewsbury DA. Differences in affiliative behavior, pair bonding, and vaginal cytology in two species of vole (Microtus ochrogaster and M. montanus) J Comp Psychol. 1990;104:268–274. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.104.3.268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Sher KJ. Subjective effects of alcohol: Influence of setting and individual differences in alcohol expectancies. J Stud Alcohol. 1985;46:137–146. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1985.46.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Sher KJ. Stress response dampening. In: Blane HT, Leonard K, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. Guilford; New York: 1987. pp. 227–271. [Google Scholar]
  139. Sher KJ, Grekin E. Alcohol and affect regulation. In: Gross J, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. Guilford; New York: 2005. pp. 560–580. [Google Scholar]
  140. Sher KJ, Grekin E, Williams N. The development of alcohol problems. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:493–523. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.144107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Sher KJ, Trull TJ, Bartholow BD, Vieth A. Personality and alcoholism: Issues, methods, and etiological processes. In: Leonard K, Blane HT, editors. Psychological theories on drinking and alcoholism. Guilford; New York: 1999. pp. 54–105. [Google Scholar]
  142. Sinha R. How does stress increase risk of drug abuse and relapse? Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;158(4):343–359. doi: 10.1007/s002130100917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Sinha R, Garcia M, Paliwal P, Kreek MJ, Rounsaville BJ. Stress-induced cocaine craving and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses are predictive of cocaine relapse outcomes. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(3):324–31. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Slutske WS, D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Harden KP, Heath AC, Martin NG. Searching for an environmental effect of parental alcoholism on offspring alcohol use disorder: A genetically informed study of children of alcoholics. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008;117:534–551. doi: 10.1037/a0012907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Slutske WS, Heath AC, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PA, Bucholz KK, Dunne MP, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Common genetic risk factors for conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. J Abnorm Psychol. 1998;107:363–374. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.3.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Slutske WS, Heath AC, Madden PA, Bucholz KK, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Personality and the genetic risk for alcohol dependence. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002;111:124–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Spear LP. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:417–463. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00014-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Stevens HE, Leckman JF, Coplan JD, Suomi S. Risk and resilience: Early manipulation of macaque social experience and persistent behavioral and neurophysiological outcomes. J Am Acad Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(2):114–127. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318193064c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Swartzwelder HS, Wilson WA, Tayyeb MI. Age-dependent inhibition of long-term potentiation by ethanol in immature versus mature hippocampus. Alc Clin Exp Res. 1995;19:1480–1485. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01011.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Tapert SF, Brown GG, Baratta MV, Brown SA. fMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women. Addict Behav. 2004;29:33–50. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. van Etten ML, Higgins ST, Bickel WK. Effects of response cost and unit dose on alcohol self-administration in moderate drinkers. Behav Pharm. 1995;6(7):754–758. doi: 10.1097/00008877-199511000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. van den Wildenberg E, Wiers RW, Janssen RGJH, Lambrichs EH, Smeets HJM, van Breukelen GJP. A functional polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) influences cue-induced craving for alcohol in male heavy drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:1–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00258.x. SHE. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Ethanol-induced social facilitation in adolescent rats: Role of endogenous activity at Mu opioid receptors. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res. 2009;33(6):991–1000. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.00920.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Chronic tolerance to the social consequences of ethanol in adolescent and adult Sprague-Dawley rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2007;29:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2006.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Vetter CS, Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Spear LP. Time course of elevated ethanol intake in adolescent relative to adult rats under continuous voluntary-access conditions. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31(7):1159–1168. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00417.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Wahlsten D. Insensitivity of the analysis of variance to hereditary-environment interaction. Behavioural and Brain Sciences. 1991;13:109–120. [Google Scholar]
  157. Walum H, Westberg L, Henningsson S, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Igl W, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Pedersen NL, Eriksson E, Lichtenstein P. Genetic variation in the vasopressin receptor 1a gene (AVPR1A) associates with pair-bonding behavior in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:14153–14156. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803081105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Wiers RW, Rinck M, Dictus M, van den Wildenberg E. Relatively strong appetitive action-tendencies in male carriers of the OPRM1 G-allele. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8:101–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00454.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Windle M. Parental, sibling, and peer influences on adolescent substance use and alcohol problems. Appl Dev Sci. 2000;4:98–110. [Google Scholar]
  160. Wise RA. The neurobiology of craving: implications for the understanding and treatment of addiction. J Abnorm Psychol. 1988;97(2):118–132. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.97.2.118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Young LJ, Wang Z, Insel TR. Neuroendocrine bases of monogamy. Trends Neurosci. 1998;21:71–75. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(97)01167-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Zeigler DW, Wang CC, Yoast RA, Dickinson BD, McCaffree MA, Robinowitz CB, Sterling ML. The neurocognitive effects of alcohol on adolescents and college students. Prev Med. 2005;40:23–32. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES