Abstract
Background
Using a knock-out mouse model, it was shown that NETO1 is a critical component of the NMDAR complex, and that loss of Neto1 leads to impaired hippocampal long term potentiation and hippocampal-dependent learning and memory. Moreover, hemizygosity of NETO1 was shown to be associated with autistic-like behavior in humans.
Purpose of the Research
We examined the association between schizophrenia and the neuropilin and tolloid-like 1 gene (NETO1). First, we selected eight single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) within the NETO1 locus, based on the Japanese schizophrenia genome wide association study (JGWAS) results and previously conducted association studies. These SNPs were genotyped in the replication sample comprised of 963 schizophrenic patients and 919 healthy controls. We also examined the effect of associated SNPs on scores in the Continuous Performance Test and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Keio version (schizophrenic patients 107, healthy controls 104).
Results
There were no significant allele-wise and haplotype-wise associations in the replication analysis after Bonferroni correction. However, in meta-analysis (JGWAS and replication dataset) three association signals were observed (rs17795324: p = 0.028, rs8098760: p = 0.017, rs17086492: p = 0.003). These SNPs were followed up but we could not detect the allele-specific effect on cognitive performance measured by the Continuous performance test (CPT) and Wisconsin Card Sorting test (WCST).
Major Conclusions
We did not detect evidence for the association of NETO1 with schizophrenia in the Japanese population. Common variants within the NETO1 locus may not increase the genetic risk for schizophrenia in the Japanese population. Additionally, common variants investigated in the current study did not affect cognitive performance, as measured by the CPT and WCST.
Introduction
The glutamate hypothesis of schizophrenia (GHS) emerged in the early 1980 s as an alternative to the prevailing theory of altered dopamine neurotransmission. The GHS is based on the observation that non-competitive antagonists of the N-methyl-d-asparate (NMDA) subtype of glutamate receptors, such as phencyclidine (PCP), ketamine and MK-801, induce a psychotic reaction in healthy individuals that resembles schizophrenia (both the positive and negative symptoms). When the same compounds are administered to patients with schizophrenia, exacerbation of psychotic symptoms can be the outcome [1]. Together, these observations suggest that diminished function of the NMDA receptor (NMDAR) may play a role in the pathoetiology of schizophrenia. Moreover, evidence from morphological, clinical and neuroimaging studies have provided support for the GHS by mapping cognitive impairment, alterations in blood flow and changes in neuronal morphology to particular brain areas, including the frontal and cingulate cortices, both of which are areas with extensive excitatory glutamatergic neurotransmission [2].
The N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor (NMDAR), a major excitatory ligand-gated ion channel in the central nervous system, is composed of a heterotetramer between two NR1 and two NR2 subunits. Moreover, the NMDAR is a principal mediator of synaptic plasticity [3]. It has been shown that corticolimbic NMDAR hypofunction is one of the core molecular mechanisms relevant for phenotypes observed in animal models of schizophrenia [4]. One of the genes that regulate NMDAR function is neuropilin and tolloid-like 1 gene (NETO1). NETO1 maps to the 18q22-q23 and three alternative splicing variants (mRNA level) have been observed [5]. Specifically, variants 1 and 2 are detected in the retina while variant 3 is specific for fetal and adult brain. NETO1 is a transmembrane protein, which has two extracellular CUB domains, a low-density lipoprotein class A (LDLa) domain, a transmembrane domain and classical type I PDZ-domain binding motif [5] (Figure 1). Deletion of Neto1 leads to deficits in synaptic plasticity in mice while stimulation of the AMPA receptor can partially compensate for deficits caused by Neto1 deletion [6]. NETO1 interacts with the core NMDAR subunits, NR2A and NR2B and a scaffolding protein, postsynaptic density-95 (PSD-95), maintaining the abundance of NR2A-containing NMDARs in the postsynaptic density of the hippocampus. PSD-95 is a protein that is almost exclusively located in the postsynaptic density of neurons, and is important in anchoring synaptic proteins [7]. Increase in surface NR2A, but not NR2B, occurs in hippocampal neurons derived from dysbindin-null mutant mice (Dys-/-). Dysbindin controls hippocampal LTP by selective regulation of the surface expression of NR2A [8]. In situ hybridization studies of schizophrenia detected decreased transcript expression of the NR1 subunit, increased transcript expression of the NR2B subunit and unchanged transcript expression of the NR2A subunit in hippocampus [9]. Therefore, regulation of NR2 in hippocampus in schizophrenia may be relevant for the etiology of schizophrenia and NETO1 may play an important role in the molecular mechanism by maintaining the abundance of NR2A-containing NMDARs in the postsynaptic density of hippocampal neurons. Moreover, NETO1 interacts with kainate receptors (KAR), one of the glutamate receptors, in mouse brain. NETO1 modulates the KAR affinity for the endogenous ligand glutamate. NETO1 modulates not only kinetics, but also the amplitude of slow excitatory postsynaptic current in KAR (KAR-EPSC) [10], [11]. NETO1 fundamentally alters the function and neuronal localization of GluK1-containing KAR [10], [11]. Therefore, NETO1 may influence glutamate neurotransmission through modulation of KAR and NMDAR properties.
Figure 1. NETO1-gene centric data.
LD: linkage disequilibrium. NETO1 maps to the 18q22-q23 and three alternative splicing variants (mRNA level) have been observed. Specifically variants 1 and 2 are detected in retina while variant 3 is specific for fetal and adult brain (mRNA level panel). NETO1 is a transmembrane protein, which has two extracellular CUB domains, a low-density lipoprotein class A (LDLa) domain, a transmembrane domain and a classical type I PDZ-domain binding motif (protein level panel).
Genetic studies suggested that the NETO1 locus could harbor genetic variants that are relevant for susceptibility to neuropsychiatric disorders. Specifically, hemizygosity of NETO1 was shown to be associated with a autistic-like behaviors in humans [12]. Although unequivocal genome wide evidence (p<10−8) for association at the NETO1 locus has not been reported, it is of note that in the recent GWASs of Alzheimer's disease and schizophrenia, suggestive association signals were observed (rs1109070; p = 0.000669 [13] and rs9962470; p = 0.000154 [14] in Alzheimer's disease and schizophrenia, respectively). Moreover, several weak association signals (P<0.05) within the NETO1 locus were detected in the first GWAS of schizophrenia conducted in the Japanese population (JGWAS) [15]. It is of note that in the JGWAS, genome wide evidence for association was not detected, and the non-genome wide level of statistical significance should be interpreted with caution. However due to the relatively small sample size, type II errors (false negative result) cannot be excluded – especially in the case of small odds ratios (OR) which are expected for common SNPs associated with schizophrenia [16].
Based on the aforementioned biological studies, NETO1 can be seen as a promising candidate gene for schizophrenia. However, to the best of our knowledge, no genetic association study specifically designed to evaluate the association between NETO1 and schizophrenia has been conducted. The goal of the present study was to evaluate the association between NETO1 and schizophrenia based on the JGWAS. Additionally, as deficits were found in LTP and learning and memory in Neto1-null mice [7], we performed a cognitive function analysis that targeted the relationship between common SNPs selected based on the JGWAS and cognitive function assessed by the CPT and the WCST.
Results
In the replication sample set, we did not detect any association between eight SNPs and schizophrenia after Bonferroni correction was applied (Table 1). Using the replication sample set, we have conducted haplotype-wise analysis in order to investigate association between haplotypes within the NETO1 locus and schizophrenia. We did not detect evidence for haplotype-wise association after Bonferroni correction was applied (Table S1). In order to maximize the power, we performed meta-analysis combining results from the JGWAS and the replication dataset. In this analysis we could detect three suggestive association signals (rs17795324: P = 0.028, rs8098760: P = 0.017, rs17086492: P = 0.003). In the test of heterogeneity, we detected four SNPs, which showed significant high heterogeneities (Breslow-Day test; rs17795324: P = 0.04, rs6566674: P = 0.01, rs8098624: P = 0.003, rs1109070: P = 0.0001) (Table 2). However, we could not detect any association between these three SNPs (rs17795324, rs8098760 and rs17086492) and CPT and WCST scores (Table S4). We investigated the association between the NETO1 gene and schizophrenia stratified by gender. We could detect no association signals either for males or females in the meta-analysis after Bonferroni correction (Tables S2 and S3).
Table 1. Results of JGWAS (N = 1108).
SNP | Positiona | Minor allele | Caseb | Controlb | p-valuec | OR | L95d | U95d |
rs9962470 | 68616086 | A | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.31 | 0.79 | 0.50 | 1.24 |
rs17086334 | 68626752 | C | 0.10 | 0.08 | 0.05 | 1.34 | 1.00 | 1.80 |
rs17795324 | 68654033 | G | 0.27 | 0.33 | 0.003 | 0.76 | 0.63 | 0.91 |
rs6566674 | 68662791 | T | 0.20 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 1.26 | 1.01 | 1.57 |
rs8098624 | 68669199 | T | 0.30 | 0.24 | 0.002 | 1.35 | 1.12 | 1.63 |
rs8098760 | 68669970 | T | 0.11 | 0.08 | 0.02 | 1.42 | 1.06 | 1.90 |
rs17086492 | 68674050 | C | 0.14 | 0.10 | 0.001 | 1.52 | 1.17 | 1.98 |
rs1109070 | 68674559 | C | 0.21 | 0.16 | 0.01 | 1.35 | 1.09 | 1.68 |
based on NCBI 36.
minor allele frequency.
Fisher's exact test.
Lower (L) and upper (U) 95% confidence intervals.
Table 2. Allele frequencies of the eight SNPs of NETO1.
SNP | Positiona | Minor allele | Replication (N = 1882) | Meta analysis (N = 2990) | |||||||||
Caseb | Controlb | p-valuec | OR | L95d | U95d | p-valuec | OR | L95d | U95d | BDpe | |||
rs9962470 | 68616086 | A | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.24 | 0.79 | 0.53 | 1.18 | 0.126 | 0.79 | 0.59 | 1.07 | 0.99 |
rs17086334 | 68626752 | C | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.87 | 0.98 | 0.77 | 1.25 | 0.289 | 1.11 | 0.92 | 1.33 | 0.09 |
rs17795324 | 68654033 | G | 0.29 | 0.30 | 0.61 | 0.96 | 0.84 | 1.11 | 0.028 | 0.88 | 0.79 | 0.99 | 0.04 |
rs6566674 | 68662791 | T | 0.17 | 0.19 | 0.12 | 0.88 | 0.74 | 1.04 | 0.926 | 1.01 | 0.88 | 1.15 | 0.01 |
rs8098624 | 68669199 | T | 0.27 | 0.29 | 0.43 | 0.94 | 0.82 | 1.09 | 0.213 | 1.08 | 0.96 | 1.21 | 0.003 |
rs8098760 | 68669970 | T | 0.10 | 0.08 | 0.18 | 1.17 | 0.93 | 1.47 | 0.017 | 1.24 | 1.04 | 1.49 | 0.26 |
rs17086492 | 68674050 | C | 0.12 | 0.11 | 0.22 | 1.14 | 0.93 | 1.39 | 0.003 | 1.28 | 1.09 | 1.50 | 0.09 |
rs1109070 | 68674559 | C | 0.16 | 0.19 | 0.01 | 0.78 | 0.65 | 0.93 | 0.718 | 0.98 | 0.85 | 1.12 | 0.0001 |
based on NCBI 36.
minor allele frequency.
Fisher's exact test.
Lower (L) and upper (U) 95% confidence intervals.
p-value of Breslow-Day test.
Discussion
In this study, we investigated the association between eight SNPs within NETO1 and schizophrenia in the Japanese population. We detected associations between NETO1 and schizophrenia in the meta-analysis, however, as the JGWAS dataset was included in the meta-analysis, evidence for association might be overestimated. In order to address this issue we tested the association between candidate SNPs from our meta-analysis and cognitive performance measured by the CPT and WCST. This analysis was of interest for us as Neto1-null mice show deficits in LTP and learning and memory [7], and if the three SNPs (rs17795324, rs8098760, rs17086492) were genuinely associated with schizophrenia, then carriers of risk alleles would likely have deficits in cognitive processing assessed by CPT and WCST [6]. However, we could not detect any association between these three SNPs (rs17795324, rs8098760, rs17086492) and the psychological tests we applied. We also performed meta-analysis (Method S1) of rs6566674 (the SNP that was included both in our replication sample set and another Japanese GWAS [17]), however we did not detect evidence for an association with schizophrenia. These results suggest that NETO1 is not associated with schizophrenia in the Japanese population.
We detected four SNPs, which showed heterogeneous association (Breslow-Day test; rs17795324: P = 0.04, rs6566674: P = 0.01, rs8098624: P = 0.003, rs1109070: P = 0.0001) in the meta-analysis. These high heterogeneities may be derived from flip-flop phenomenon, that is, associations of opposite alleles at the same biallelic locus with the same disease [18]. Although the flip-flop phenomenon may represent a genuine genetic association (i.e., genuinely different LD architectures across populations with different ancestral origins), it may also be an artifact due to sampling variation that leads to variability in observed LD patterns.
Several caveats should be considered when interpreting the results of our study. First, in terms of sample size, the replication dataset may not have sufficient statistical power to detect associations between SNPs with low genotype relative risk (GRR) and schizophrenia. In other words, our sample has statistical power greater than 0.8 for the detection of association signals at nominal statistical significance, of the polymorphism with a minor allele frequency of 0.1, when the GRR is 1.30. Therefore, the possibility of association between schizophrenia and common SNPs with GRR<1.30 cannot be excluded. Furthermore, the JGWAS may not have sufficient power to detect associations between SNPs with low GRR and schizophrenia. Therefore, other relevant common variants in the NETO1 region, which the JGWAS cannot identify, may exist. In addition, association between NETO1 and schizophrenia may be specific only for the patients with early onset age. However, as in the current study, the frequency of the early onset schizophrenia patients was low (less than 5%). The power to detect such an association is inadequate. Therefore, the effect of onset age on the association between NETO1 and schizophrenia remains to be investigated in further studies.
The second caveat is that our study design was based on the common disease, common variant hypothesis, based on which we applied a minor allele frequency threshold (>5%) and selected eight SNPs for follow-up. In the best case scenario, common variants detected in GWAS can explain only part of the heritability in cases of schizophrenia (∼30%) [19] and missense or nonsense mutations on the one side and structural variations (i.e., copy number variants (CNVs)) on the other side are likely to contribute to the increased susceptibility [20]. Recently, the concept of synthetic associations has been suggested, though some there are some objections [21]. Uncommon or rare genetic variants can easily create synthetic associations that are credited to common variants. This possibility requires careful consideration in the interpretation and follow up of GWAS signals [22].
The third caveat in our association study is that cases and controls in replication samples were not matched in age. In other words, although highly unlikely, the controls may develop schizophrenia at some point in life, as they were significantly younger than cases.
The fourth caveat is related to the validity of cognitive function analysis. The premise that NETO1 was associated with cognitive function in humans had been derived from results of a knock-out mice study [7]. Neto1-null mice showed impaired spatial learning measured by the Morris water maze task, the delayed matching-to-place version of the Morris water maze task and displaced-object tasks. In the current study, we investigated executive function (WCST) and vigilance/attention (CPT-IP), however, the results of these cognitive tests might not represent similar cognitive dysfunctions that were shown in the Neto1-null mouse study. It may be useful to examine different domains of cognitive impairment associated with NETO1 in schizophrenic patients using broader cognitive assessment tools.
In conclusion, we were not able to detect evidence for an association between NETO1 and schizophrenia in the Japanese population. Common variants within the NETO1 locus may not increase the genetic risk for schizophrenia in the Japanese population. Additionally, common variants investigated in the current study did not affect cognitive performance, as measured by the CPT and the WCST.
Materials and Methods
Participants
This study was approved by the Ethics Committees of the Nagoya University Graduate School of Medicine and Fujita Health University, and written informed consent was obtained from each participant. Patients were included in the study if they (1) met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia, (2) were physically healthy and (3) had no mood disorders, substance abuse, neurodevelopmental disorders, epilepsy or known mental retardation. A general characterization and psychiatric assessment of subjects is available elsewhere [23]. Controls were selected from the general population. Control subjects had no history of mental disorders, based on questionnaire responses from the subjects themselves during the sample inclusion step, and based on an unstructured diagnostic interview done by an experienced psychiatrist during the blood collection step. The JGWAS sample was comprised of 575 patients with schizophrenia (43.5±14.8 years (mean±s.d.), male 50%) and 564 healthy controls with no personal or family history of psychiatric illness (44.0±14.4 years (mean±s.d.), male 49.8%). All subjects were unrelated, living in the central area of the Honshu island of Japan and self-identified as members of the Japanese population. Subjects of replication samples consisted of 963 schizophrenic patients (47.7±0.5 years (mean±s.d.), male 55.2%) and 919 healthy controls (45.0±0.5 years (mean±s.d.), male 51.0%). The JGWAS and replication samples were collected independently at each university hospital.
Genotyping and data analysis
Based on the JGWAS results we initially selected SNPs with probability values, p<0.05 and allelic frequencies, MAF>0.05 within the NETO1 locus. Then we identified redundant SNPs based on the linkage disequilibrium or LD pattern within the interrogated region. Specifically, if the correlation coefficient between two loci (r2) was 0.8 or higher, only one of the two loci was selected for the association study [24]. The correlation coefficient between two loci (r2) was calculated using Haploview v.4.1 based on the HapMap database (release no. 24, population: Japanese in Tokyo). Finally, we selected seven nonredundant SNPs within the NETO1 locus. Moreover, one common polymorphism (rs9962470), which showed a low p-value (p = 0.000154) in a previous GWA study [14], was included for genotyping. All eight SNPs are intronic polymorphisms. DNA was extracted from peripheral blood according to a standard protocol [23]. Genotyping was performed using a fluorescence-based allelic discrimination assay (Taqman, Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA). Power was calculated according to the methods of Skol et al. [25].
To exclude low-quality DNA samples or genotyping probes, data sets were filtered on the basis of SNP genotype call rate (more than 90%) or checked deviation from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) in the control sample. Subjects whose percentage of missing genotypes was >30% or who had evidence of possible DNA contamination were excluded from subsequent analyses.
To reduce the total number of tests, eight associated markers were selected based on the JGWAS results. Next, conditional on the findings of the JGWAS, which used a less stringent nominal level, a meta-analysis was done involving the confirmation sample using the replication data and data from the JGWAS. In the replication sample, Fisher's exact test was used to compare allele frequencies between patients and control subjects. The significance level was set at p<0.05. In the replication sample set, log likelihood ratio tests for assessing haplotype-wise association between schizophrenia and a combination of tagging SNPs was performed using UNPHASED software v3.04. The rare haplotype frequency threshold was set at 5% [26]. In this meta-analysis, p-values were generated by a Cochran–Mantel–Haenszel stratified analysis, and the Breslow–Day test was performed for evaluations of heterogeneous associations as implemented in gPLINK v.2.050 [27].
Neurocognitive assessment
1. CPT
We used the Continuous Performance Test–Identical Pairs Version Release 4.0 (NewCPT.exe, Copyright 1982–2004 by Barbara A. Cornblatt, All Rights Reserved). The size of PC monitor used for the test was 10.4 inches as each letter was at least 2.2×1.5 cm [28]. Stimuli were flashed on the screen at a constant rate of 1 per second, with a stimulus “on” time of 50 ms. Stimuli were four-digit numbers and were presented 150 times. In each 150-trial condition, 30 of the trials (20%) were target trials and required a response. Target trials were those on which the second of a pair of two identical stimuli appeared [28]. The outcome measure was a mean, d′.
2. WCST
The WCST [29] mainly assesses executive function including cognitive flexibility in response to feedback. We used a modified and computerized version of the test: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Keio Version) (KWCST) [30]. The outcome measures were numbers of categories achieved (CA), total errors (TE), and perseverative errors of Milner (PEM) and Nelson types (PEN) in the first trial. We selected outcomes in the WCST, following a prior study, which used KWCST as a measure of cognitive function [31].
(1) CA: This is the number of categories for which six consecutive correct responses are achieved (eight is the maximum number of categories which can be achieved), and is the sum measure of the level of conceptual shifts in the KWCST.
(2) PEN: This is the number of incorrect responses in the same category as the immediately preceding incorrect response (maximum of 47 perseverative errors) [32].
(3) PEM: This is the number of incorrect responses in the same category as the immediately preceding correct response after the category changes.
(4) TE: This is the total number of incorrect responses [33].
3. Clinical information
Chlorpromazine (CPZ) equivalent doses were calculated based on the report by Inada [34], [35]. The Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS) was used to evaluate patients [36].
4. Analysis of cognitive performance
From the sample used in the current study, we made a subset of randomly selected participants older than 18 years of age for analysis of cognitive performance. Cognitive data analysis was done for the participants who completed both WCST and CPT-IP. We checked the effect of three SNPs on cognitive performance measured by the Continuous Performance Test and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (107 schizophrenic patients, 104 Healthy controls). IBM SPSS statistical software, version 19 was used for all analyses. We compared age, education, CPZ equivalent doses, age at onset, duration of illness, positive scale, negative scale and General Psychopathology Scale between schizophrenia cases and control subjects using a two-tailed t-test and Welch's t-test. We compared sex between case and control groups using Fisher's exact test. Next, we compared d′ in the CPT and CA, PEM, PEN, TE in the WCST between the case and control groups using a two-tailed t-test and Welch's test (Table S4).
Supporting Information
Meta-analysis.
(DOC)
Haplotype analysis of the eight SNPs of NETO1.
(DOC)
Allele frequencies of the eight SNPs of NETO1 in males.
(DOC)
Allele frequencies of the eight SNPs of NETO1 in females.
(DOC)
Cognitive performance of three SNPs in NETO1.
(DOC)
Acknowledgments
We sincerely thank the patients and healthy volunteers for their participation in this study. We express our gratitude to Ryoko Ishihara, Ph.D., Hiromi Noma, Saori Yamashita, Ph.D., and Mami Yoshida for their technical assistance.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: Funding for this study was provided by research grants from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan; the Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare of Japan; Grant-in-Aid for “Integrated Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders” carried out under the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan; Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on Innovative Areas (Comprehensive Brain Science Network) from the Ministry of Education, Science, Sports and Culture of Japan; The Academic Frontier Project for Private Universities, Comparative Cognitive Science Institutes; and the Core Research for Evolutional Science and Technology. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Steinpreis R. The behavioral and neurochemical effects of phencyclidine in humans and animals: some implications for modeling psychosis. Behav Brain Res. 1996;74:45–55. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00162-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kristiansen LV, Huerta I, Beneyto M, Meador-Woodruff JH. NMDA receptors and schizophrenia. Current opinion in pharmacology. 2007;7:48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2006.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Stephan KE, Baldeweg T, Friston KJ. Synaptic plasticity and dysconnection in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:929–939. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Belforte J, Zsiros V, Sklar E, Jiang Z, Yu G, et al. Postnatal NMDA receptor ablation in corticolimbic interneurons confers schizophrenia-like phenotypes. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:76–83. doi: 10.1038/nn.2447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Stohr H, Berger C, Frohlich S, Weber BH. A novel gene encoding a putative transmembrane protein with two extracellular CUB domains and a low-density lipoprotein class A module: isolation of alternatively spliced isoforms in retina and brain. Gene. 2002;286:223–231. doi: 10.1016/s0378-1119(02)00438-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Li F, Tsien JZ. Memory and the NMDA receptors. N Engl J Med. 2009;361:302–303. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcibr0902052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ng D, Pitcher GM, Szilard RK, Sertie A, Kanisek M, et al. Neto1 is a novel CUB-domain NMDA receptor-interacting protein required for synaptic plasticity and learning. PLoS Biol. 2009;7:e41. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tang TT, Yang F, Chen BS, Lu Y, Ji Y, et al. Dysbindin regulates hippocampal LTP by controlling NMDA receptor surface expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:21395–21400. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0910499106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gao XM, Sakai K, Roberts RC, Conley RR, Dean B, et al. Ionotropic glutamate receptors and expression of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor subunits in subregions of human hippocampus: effects of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1141–1149. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.7.1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Straub C, Hunt DL, Yamasaki M, Kim KS, Watanabe M, et al. Distinct functions of kainate receptors in the brain are determined by the auxiliary subunit Neto1. Nature neuroscience. 2011 doi: 10.1038/nn.2837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Copits BA, Robbins JS, Frausto S, Swanson GT. Synaptic Targeting and Functional Modulation of GluK1 Kainate Receptors by the Auxiliary Neuropilin and Tolloid-Like (NETO) Proteins. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31:7334–7340. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0100-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.O'Donnell L, Soileau B, Heard P, Carter E, Sebold C, et al. Genetic determinants of autism in individuals with deletions of 18q. Hum Genet. 2010;128:155–164. doi: 10.1007/s00439-010-0839-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Li H, Wetten S, Li L, St Jean PL, Upmanyu R, et al. Candidate single-nucleotide polymorphisms from a genomewide association study of Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2008;65:45–53. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2007.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Shi J, Levinson DF, Duan J, Sanders AR, Zheng Y, et al. Common variants on chromosome 6p22.1 are associated with schizophrenia. Nature. 2009;460:753–757. doi: 10.1038/nature08192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ikeda M, Aleksic B, Kinoshita Y, Okochi T, Kawashima K, et al. Genome-wide association study of schizophrenia in a Japanese population. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:472–478. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Shi G, Boerwinkle E, Morrison AC, Gu CC, Chakravarti A, et al. Mining gold dust under the genome wide significance level: a two-stage approach to analysis of GWAS. Genetic epidemiology. 2011;35:111–118. doi: 10.1002/gepi.20556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yamada K, Iwayama Y, Hattori E, Iwamoto K, Toyota T, et al. Genome-wide association study of schizophrenia in Japanese population. PloS one. 2011;6:e20468. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lin PI, Vance JM, Pericak-Vance MA, Martin ER. No gene is an island: the flip-flop phenomenon. American journal of human genetics. 2007;80:531–538. doi: 10.1086/512133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM, O'Donovan MC, et al. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. 2009;460:748–752. doi: 10.1038/nature08185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Manolio TA, Collins FS, Cox NJ, Goldstein DB, Hindorff LA, et al. Finding the missing heritability of complex diseases. Nature. 2009;461:747–753. doi: 10.1038/nature08494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Anderson CA, Soranzo N, Zeggini E, Barrett JC. Synthetic associations are unlikely to account for many common disease genome-wide association signals. PLoS Biol. 2011;9:e1000580. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dickson SP, Wang K, Krantz I, Hakonarson H, Goldstein DB. Rare variants create synthetic genome-wide associations. PLoS Biol. 2010;8:e1000294. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Aleksic B, Kushima I, Ito Y, Nakamura Y, Ujike H, et al. Genetic association study of KREMEN1 and DKK1 and schizophrenia in a Japanese population. Schizophrenia research. 2010;118:113–117. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Barrett J, Fry B, Maller J, Daly M. Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics. 2005;21:263–265. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bth457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Skol AD, Scott LJ, Abecasis GR, Boehnke M. Joint analysis is more efficient than replication-based analysis for two-stage genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet. 2006;38:209–213. doi: 10.1038/ng1706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Dudbridge F. Likelihood-based association analysis for nuclear families and unrelated subjects with missing genotype data. Human heredity. 2008;66:87–98. doi: 10.1159/000119108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559–575. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cornblatt BA, Risch NJ, Faris G, Friedman D, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L. The Continuous Performance Test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP): I. New findings about sustained attention in normal families. Psychiatry Res. 1988;26:223–238. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(88)90076-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Heaton RK. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Manual) Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kashima H, Handa T, Kato M, Sakuma K, Yokoyama N, et al. Neuropsychological investigation on chronic schizophrenia. Aspects of its frontal functions. In: Takahashi R, Flor-Henry P, Gruzelier J, Niwa S, editors. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hori H, Noguchi H, Hashimoto R, Nakabayashi T, Omori M, et al. Antipsychotic medication and cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006;86:138–146. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Suzuki H, Gen K, Inoue Y. An unblinded comparison of the clinical and cognitive effects of switching from first-generation antipsychotics to aripiprazole, perospirone or olanzapine in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Heaton RK, Chelune GL, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual: revised and expanded. Oclessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resourses; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Inagaki A, Inada T. Dose equivalence of psychotropic drugs. 2008. pp. 887–890. Part XXI: dose equivalence of novel antipsychotics: Blonanserin. Rinsho Seishin Yakuri (Japanese Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology)
- 35.Inagaki A, Inada T. Dose equivalence of psychotropic drugs. 2010. pp. 1349–1353. Part XXII: dose equivalence of depot antipsychotics III: risperidon long-acting injection. Rinsho Seishin Yakuri (Japanese Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology)
- 36.Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Meta-analysis.
(DOC)
Haplotype analysis of the eight SNPs of NETO1.
(DOC)
Allele frequencies of the eight SNPs of NETO1 in males.
(DOC)
Allele frequencies of the eight SNPs of NETO1 in females.
(DOC)
Cognitive performance of three SNPs in NETO1.
(DOC)