Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2012 Jul 15;140(1-3):83–86. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.06.033

Comparison of Putative Intermediate Phenotypes in Schizophrenia Patients With and Without Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Examining Evidence for the Schizo-Obsessive Subtype

Anna R Docherty a, Michael J Coleman b, Xiawei Tu c, Curtis K Deutsch d, Nancy R Mendell c, Deborah L Levy b
PMCID: PMC3423524  NIHMSID: NIHMS391168  PMID: 22796151

Abstract

Obsessive-compulsive symptoms or obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) are estimated to occur in up to 30% of patients with schizophrenia. Whether this subgroup of patients is cognitively, affectively, or physiologically distinct remains unclear. 204 schizophrenia patients, 15 who also met criteria for a diagnosis of OCD, and 147 healthy controls were examined on several intermediate phenotypes. The patient groups did not differ from each other except that the co-morbid group exhibited an elevated rate of eye-tracking dysfunction. Results suggest that OCD-co-morbid patients did not comprise a distinct subgroup based on the measures studied here, although systematic assessment of larger cohorts is warranted.

Keywords: schizophrenia, OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder, intermediate phenotype, subtype

1. Introduction

Obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS) or obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) occur in approximately 30% of patients with schizophrenia (Byerly et al., 2005; Tibbo et al., 2000)--a much higher prevalence than is observed in the general population (1.2-2.4%; Foa et al., 1995). OCS are also increased in the psychosis prodrome (Niendam et al., 2008), are associated with poorer prognosis (Fenton & McGlashan, 1986), and may reflect reduced functional connectivity in basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex (Harrison et al., 2009).

Although some evidence suggests that OCD-co-morbid schizophrenia is a possible subtype of schizophrenia (e.g., Aoyama et al. 2000; Iida, et al. 1998; Levine et al. 1998), the clinical relationship between schizophrenia and OCD is poorly understood (Bottas et al. 2005). The two most common designs are to use categorical diagnostic criteria to establish co-morbidity, or to evaluate severity of OCS using symptom scales and checklists. Indeed, the frequency of OCS in schizophrenia seems to vary as a function of how extensive the OCS screening methods were (Byerly et al. 2005; Nolfe et al 2010).

The use of intermediate phenotypes may help to identify schizophrenia subtypes with distinct underlying pathophysiologies. In the present study, we examined neurocognitive phenotypes associated with genetic liability to schizophrenia and severity of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia patients with and without co-morbid OCD in order to determine whether any of these measures distinguished between the schizophrenia subgroups. These phenotypes include eye tracking dysfunction (Levy et al. 1993; Levy et al. 2010a, thought disorder (Holzman et al. 2005; Levy et al. 2010b), and craniofacial dysmorphology (Deutsch et al. 2000; Deutsch et al. 2012).

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 189 patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, 15 schizophrenia-OCD-co-morbid patients, and 147 healthy comparison subjects participated. Normal controls did not meet criteria for a diagnosis of psychosis (lifetime), an Axis II schizophrenia-related personality disorder, or nonpsychotic bipolar disorder, and did not have a family history of psychosis, psychiatric hospitalization, or suicide. Information concerning demographics, medications, and global level of functioning is presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic Characteristics, Global Assessment of Functioning, and Antipsychotic Medication: Schizophrenia Patients (with and without OCD) and Controls.

SZ SZ-OCD NC
N 189 15 147
Age (Years) 47.2 (0.0) 41.3 (8.0) 39.9 (14.9)
Gender (% Female) 45 50 56
Race (% Caucasian) 91 100 85
SES 2.5 (1.0) 2.5 (1.3) 2.3 (.9)
Est. VIQ 98.5 (22.1)* 100.7 (10.2) 105.8 (11.2)
Years Education 13.9 (2.3)* 15.0 (2.8) 15.0 (2.3)
GAS 37.7 (10.0) 34.1 (4.8) 75.7 (11.8)
Antipsychotics
Typical only 53 1
Atypical only 129 13
Typical and Atypical 23 0

SZ = schizophrenia patients, SZ-OCD = schizophrenia patients with co-morbid obsessivecompulsive disorder diagnosis, NC = normal controls; SES = socio-economic status based on a revised version of the Hollingshead Scale (1965), Est VIQ = estimated verbal IQ based on the vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised (WAIS-R) (Wechsler, 1981), GAS = global assessment of functioning score. Mean (standard deviation)

*

p < 0.001, comparison with control participant group

2.2. Diagnostic Assessment

Axis I disorders were assessed in patients and controls using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID, First et al., 2002). Schizotypal, schizoid, and paranoid personality disorders were assessed in controls using the Structured Interview for Schizotypal Symptoms (Version 1.5; Kendler, 1989). An experienced clinician administered the interviews. An independent group of senior diagnosticians reviewed the interview materials and all available hospital records and assigned consensus DSM-IV (APA, 1994) Axis I and Axis II (nonpsychotic individuals only) diagnoses based on best-estimate methods (Leckman et al., 1982). These subject groups are part of a larger sample that also includes individuals who meet criteria for a diagnosis of bipolar disorder with psychotic features and relatives of the three subject groups. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS; Overall & Gorham, 1962) was used to assess symptom severity in the patient groups. The Global Assessment of Functioning score (GAF; DSM-IV, 1994) was used to estimate level of functioning in all subjects. Chlorpromazine equivalents were calculated using widely adopted methods (Davis, 1974; Woods, 2003).

2.3. Craniofacial Dysmorphology

Quantitative dysmorphology methods were used to assess craniofacial asymmetries arising along an embryologically derived region implicated in schizophrenia maldevelopment: the junction of two embryonic primordia, the frontonasal and the maxillary prominence derivatives. Using the methods of Farkas and Deutsch (1996), standard anthropometric techniques were used to measure these regions, including the orbits, surrounding architecture, and alar conformation. Higher scores indicate more abnormal measurements. This quantitative measure has been shown to be elevated among probands with schizophrenia and their unaffected first-degree relatives, but not among those with bipolar affective disorder (Deutsch et al., 2012).

2.4. Thought Disorder

Thought disorder was assessed using the Thought Disorder Index (TDI; Johnston & Holzman, 1979; Solovay et al, 1986). Verbatim transcripts of responses to ten cards of the Rorschach were scored by two experienced raters. Deviant verbalizations and combinatory thinking, two components of the total score, were also calculated. Higher scores indicate greater deviance. Each record was also assigned a TDI “diagnosis,” indicating whether the thought disorder that was present was characterized by features consistent with schizophrenia, affective illness, or neither.

2.5. Eye Tracking Dysfunction

A target subtending 1.25 degrees of visual angle moved horizontally in a 0.4 Hz sine wave for 30 seconds. The subject was instructed to follow the target as carefully as possible. Eye movements were recorded using infrared reflectometry. Eye position tracings were assigned a qualitative rating (normal/abnormal) and were scored for the following quantitative variables using an interactive software program:

  • Closed-loop peak gain - the ratio of eye velocity to target velocity during the period of peak target velocity;

  • Frequency of catch-up saccades – frequency of saccades in the same direction as the target that re-positioned the eyes on the target when eye velocity lagged behind that of the target;

  • Frequency of compensatory saccades - sum of the frequency of catch-up and back-up saccades; and

  • Frequency of intrusive saccades - the sum of the frequency of square-wave jerks and anticipatory saccades.

The interview, diagnostic review, collection and processing of phenotype measures, and the clinical ratings were carried out blind to group membership.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

Shapiro-Wilk tests of normality were used to examine the distributions of the dependent measures within groups. Group comparisons of continuously distributed demographic, clinical, and intermediate phenotype variables were performed using Wilcoxon tests. This nonparametric test was used instead of the usual 2-sample t-test, because it is essentially as powerful as the t-test, but does not require that the variable be normally distributed within groups and is therefore a conservative choice. Chi square tests were used to assess group comparisons on qualitative variables. An α level of 0.01 was used as the minimum requirement for statistical significance.

3. Results

Demographic characteristics of the schizophrenia, SZ-OCD, and control groups are presented in Tables 1 and 2. OCD and non-OCD patient groups did not differ in medication dosage in chlorpromazine equivalence units (t[13] = 1.05, P = 0.31). Non-OCD schizophrenia patients had significantly fewer years of formal education (Z = 4.40, P < 0.001) and a significantly lower estimated verbal IQ than controls (Z = 4.77, P < 0.001); SZ-OCD patients did not differ from controls in year of education (Z = 1.47, P = 0.14) or estimated verbal IQ (Z = 0.27, P = 0.79). No other significant differences between groups on the demographic variables were observed. Tables 2 and 3 present data comparing the two schizophrenia groups on demographic and clinical variables. The two subgroups of patients did not differ significantly on any measures of symptom severity (all Ps >0.01).

Table 2. Comparisons of SZ and SZ-OCD Groups on Demographic Variables.

SZ Mean (SD) SZ-OCD Mean (SD) p d
Age 38.5 (9.6) 41.3 (8.0) ns −0.32
SES 2.5 (1.0) 2.5 (1.3) ns 0.08
Race ns −0.13
Handedness ns 0.02
Est. VIQ 98.5 (22.1) 100.7 (10.2) ns 0.10
Education 13.9 (2.3) 14.9 (2.8) ns 0.44
GAS 38 (10) 34 (5) ns 0.36
CPZ 617 (490) 945 (1076) ns 0.39

d = Cohen’s effect size for group contrast

SES = socioeconomic status, Est VIQ = estimated verbal IQ, GAS = global assessment of functioning score, CPZ = chlorpromazine equivalent dose.

Table 3. Comparisons of SZ and SZ-OCD Groups on Measures of Symptom Severity based on the BPRS.

SZ (M) SZ-OCD (M) p d
Somaticism 2.603 1.648 0.04 −0.50
Anxiety 3.027 1.814 0.06 0.54
Emotional Withdrawal 3.627 1.704 0.22 0.30
Disorganization 3.107 1.898 0.28 −0.32
Guilt 2.509 1.744 0.31 −0.21
Tension 2.201 1.593 0.04 0.55
Mannerism 2.342 1.707 0.53 −0.16
Grandiose 2.156 1.717 0.55 −0.17
Depressed Mood 2.719 1.609 0.96 −0.00
Hostility 1.862 1.648 0.28 0.29
Suspiciousness 3.924 1.483 0.04 0.50
Hallucinations 2.955 1.995 0.71 0.12
Motor 2.182 2.251 0.52 −0.18
Uncooperativeness 1.741 1.420 0.46 −0.13
Unusual Thought
Content
4.121 1.334 0.34 0.28
Blunted Affect 3.831 2.130 0.20 0.33
Excitement 1.629 1.267 0.72 0.07
Disorientation 1.277 1.026 0.32 −0.27
BPRS Total 47.759 14.788 0.49 0.16
BPRS Factor Scores
BPRS Affect 10.857 4.421 0.94 −0.05
BPRS Positive 12.339 5.920 0.90 −0.01
BPRS Negative 9.640 4.187 0.36 0.21
BPRS Resistance 7.527 3.320 0.11 0.38
BPRS Activation 6.156 3.562 0.18 0.20

d = Cohen’s effect size for group contrast

Participants with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, with or without OCD, differed from control participants on all phenotype measures, with the exception of frequency of intrusive saccades. Participants with schizophrenia exhibited significantly more deviance involving the frontonasal-maxillary junction, more thought disorder, and worse performance on all quantitative indices of eye-tracking.

SZ-OCD patients showed a significantly increased rate of ETD (72%) compared with non-OCD schizophrenia patients (44%; X2 = 6.3, P < 0.01; Controls = 12%). However, the SZ and SZ-OCD groups did not differ on any of the quantitative eye-tracking measures. Patients with and without OCD did not differ in frequency of SZ-related TD (χ2 = 0.003, P > 0.96), quantitative thought disorder measures, or frontonasal maxillary junction scores.

4. Discussion

SZ and SZ-OCD patients closely resembled each other on clinical variables and on the intermediate phenotype measures used in this study, suggesting that SZ-OCD is not a distinct subtype. The one exception to this pattern was the significantly higher rate of eye-tracking dysfunction (ETD), assessed qualitatively, in schizophrenia patients with co-morbid OCD.

Although schizophrenia has been systematically associated with abnormalities in pursuit initiation and maintenance (for a review, see Levy et al., 2010), findings on oculomotor function in OCD have been much less consistent. In a recent review of eye movement studies in OCD, the authors concluded that pursuit deficits in this clinical population “are much less robust and systematic” than in schizophrenia, and speculated that this difference may be due to the greater involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia vs. orbitofrontal prefrontal cortex in OCD (Jaafari et al., 2011).

The superiority of the qualitative rating of eye tracking in distinguishing OCD-co-morbid schizophrenia patients from non-OCD patients is consistent with other reports that this measure discriminates patients and relatives from controls better than specific quantitative measures (e.g., Friedman et al. 1995; Keefe et al. 1997; Levy et al. 2000). A recent meta-analysis of eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia quantified the results of studies that used specific quantitative measures of pursuit performance and global measures (O’Driscoll and Callahan, 2008). They found that effect sizes for global variables were uniformly large, and that the largest effect size was obtained for qualitative ratings (d =1.55). Global measures are less physiologically informative than specific measures, however, underscoring the need to systematically compare schizophrenia patients with and without OCD and non-schizophrenic-OCD patients using eye movement tasks that can help to determine whether the specific processes that underlie eye tracking dysfunction differ among these groups.

Limitations

The sample size of the OCD-co-morbid schizophrenia patients was small, which may have reduced power to detect differences between this group and the non-OCD-co-morbid group. Also, we compared the two groups of schizophrenia patients on a select group of measures that may not be sensitive to subgroup distinctions. Thus, we cannot conclusively rule out the possibility that OCD-co-morbid schizophrenia is a subtype. Similarly, the OCD co-morbid subgroup was identified on the basis of meeting diagnostic criteria for both disorders. Although this is a particularly conservative criterion for co-morbidity, it is possible that detailed screening of OCS (i.e., Y-BOCS scores) would have yielded significant associations between number or severity of OCS and the intermediate phenotypes studied here.

Table 4. Schizophrenia Group Comparisons: Craniofacial Dysmorphology, Thought Disorder Variables, and Quantitative Eye Tracking Dysfunction.

Group N Mean (SD) P d
Craniofacial
Dysmorphology
SZ 176 11.49 (5.7) 0.76 −0.15
SZ-OCD 11 10.64 (5.9)
Thought Disorder

Combinatory
Thinking
SZ 194 3.46 (3.1) 0.45 0.08
SZ-OCD 14 3.71 (2.3)

Deviant
Verbalizations
SZ 194 5.51 (5.2) 0.18 −0.40
SZ-OCD 14 3.43 (2.7)

Thought Disorder
Index Score
SZ 194 22.86 (22.7) 0.83 −0.15
SZ-OCD 14 19.52 (15.6)
Eye-Tracking

Catch-Up Saccade
Frequency (/min)
SZ 187 82.33 (44.6) 0.45 −0.25
SZ-OCD 14 71.34 (29.2)

Compensatory
Saccade Frequency
(/min)
SZ 187 93.54 (45.6) 0.67 −0.18
SZ-OCD 14 85.38 (27.5)

Intrusive Saccade
Frequency (/min)
SZ 187 14.61 (19.4) 0.23 0.20
SZ-OCD 14 18.49 (17.4)

Gain SZ 183 0.80 (0.2) 0.49 −0.11
SZ-OCD 14 0.78 (0.1)

d = Cohen’s effect size for group contrast

SZ = schizophrenia patients, SZ-OCD = schizophrenia patients with co-morbid obsessive-compulsive disorder diagnosis.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Anne Gibbs for recruitment, Alison Shell for helpful comments, and the participants.

Role of the Funding Source

This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH071523, the Brain and Behavior Foundation, the Essel Foundation, and the Sidney R. Baer, Jr. Foundation. These funding sources did not contribute in any way to the conceptualization of the study, to the analyses of data, or to any other aspect of the preparation of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributors

Author DL designed the study and wrote the protocol. Authors AD, DL, and CD managed the literature searches and analyses. Authors AD, XL, NM, and DL undertook the statistical analyses, and author AD wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed APA; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Byerly M, Goodman W, Acholonu W, Bugno R, Rush AJ. Obsessive compulsive symptoms in schizophrenia: frequency and clinical features. Schizophr. Res. 2005;76(2):309–316. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Davis JM. Dose equivalence of the antipsychotic drugs. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1974;11:65–69. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(74)90071-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Deutsch CK, Hobbs K, Price SFR, Gordon-Vaughn K. Skewing of the brain midline in schizophrenia. NeuroReport. 2000;11:3985–3988. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200012180-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Deutsch CK, Levy DL, Price SFR, Bodkin JA, Boling L, Coleman MJ, Johnson F, Lerbinger J, Matthysse S, Holzman PS. Quantitative measures of craniofacial dysmorphology in a family study of schizophrenia and bipolar illness. 2012 doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv014. In revision. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Farkas LG, Deutsch CK. Anthropometric determination of craniofacial morphology. Am. J. Med. Genet. 1996;65(1):1–4. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320650102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Fenton WS, McGlashan TH. The prognostic significance of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1986;143(4):437–441. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.4.437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. (SCID-I/P) Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  9. Foa EB, Kozak MJ, Goodman WK, Hollander E, Jenike MA, Rasmussen SA. DSM-IV field trial: obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1995;152(1):90–96. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.1.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Friedman L, Kenny JT, Jesberger JA, Choy MM, Meltzer HY. Relationship between smooth pursuit eye-tracking and cognitive performance in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 1995;37(4):265–72. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00170-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gooding DC, Basso MA. The tell-tale tasks: a review of saccadic research in psychiatric patient populations. Brain and Cognit. 2008;68(3):371–390. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, Pujol J, Ortiz H, López-Solà M, Hernández-Ribas R, Deus J, Alonso P, Yücel M, Pantelis C, Menchon JM, Cardoner N. Altered corticostriatal functional connectivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2009;66(11):1189–1200. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hollingshead A. Two-factor index of social position. Yale Station; New Haven, CT: 1965. [Google Scholar]
  14. Holzman PS, Levy DL, Johnston MH. The use of the Rorschach technique for assessing formal thought disorder. In: Bornstein RF, Masling JM, editors. Scoring the Rorschach: Seven Validated Systems. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, New Jersey: 2005. pp. 55–95. [Google Scholar]
  15. Jaafari N, Rigalleau F, Rachid F, Delamillieure P, Millet B, Olie JP, et al. A critical review of the contribution of eye movement recordings to the neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2011;124:87–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01721.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Keefe RS, Silverman JM, Mohs RC, Siever LJ, Harvey PD, Friedman L, Roitman SE, DuPre RL, Smith CJ, Schmeidler J, Davis KL. Eye-tracking, attention, and schizotypal symptoms in nonpsychotic relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen. Psychiatry. 1997;54(2):169–76. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830140081014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kendler KS. Structured Interview for Schizotypal Symptoms (SISS, version 1.5) Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia; Richmond, Virginia: 1989. [Google Scholar]
  18. Leckman J, Sholomskas D, Thompson WD, Belanger A, Weissman MM. Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1982;39(8):879–883. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290080001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lee MJ, Shin YB, Sunwoo YK, Jung SH, Kim WH, Kang MH, et al. Comparative analysis of cognitive function in schizophrenia with and without obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Investigation. 2009;6(4):286–93. doi: 10.4306/pi.2009.6.4.286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Levy DL, Lajonchere CM, Dorogusker B, Min D, Lee S, Tartaglini A, Lieberman JA, Mendell NR. Quantitative characterization of eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2000;42(3):171–85. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00122-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Levy DL, Holzman PS, Matthysse S, Mendell NR. Eye tracking dysfunction and schizophrenia: A critical perspective. Schizophr. Bull. 1993;19:461–536. doi: 10.1093/schbul/19.3.461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Levy DL, Sereno AB, Gooding DC, O’Driscoll G. Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia: Characterization and pathophysiology. In: Swerdlow NR, editor. Behavioral Neurobiology of Schizophrenia and its Treatment. Springer-Verlag; Berlin Heidelberg: 2010a. pp. 311–347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Levy DL, Coleman MJ, Sung H, Ji F, Matthysse S, Mendell NR, Titone D. The genetic basis of thought disorder and language and communication disturbances in schizophrenia. J. Neurolinguistics. 2010b;23:176–192. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Niendam TA, Berzak J, Cannon TD, Bearden CE. Obsessive compulsive symptoms in the psychosis prodrome: correlates of clinical and functional outcome. Schizophr. Res. 2009;108:170–175. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. O’Driscoll GA, Callahan BL. Smooth pursuit in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review of research since 1993. Brain Cogn. 2008;68(3):359–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Overall JE, Gorham DR. Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psych. Rep. 1962;10:799–812. [Google Scholar]
  27. Pallanti S, Castellini G, Chamberlain SR, Quercioli L, Zaccara G, Fineberg NA. Cognitive event-related potentials differentiate schizophrenia with obsessive compulsive disorder (schizo-OCD) from OCD and schizophrenia without OC symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2009;170:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Poyurovsky M, Fuchs C, Wiezman A. Obsessive-compulsive disorder in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1999;156(12):1998–2000. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.12.1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Solovay MR, Shenton ME, Gasperetti C, Coleman M, Kestnbaum E, Carpenter IX, Holzman PS. Scoring manual for the Thought Disorder Index (revised version) Schizophr. Bull. 1986;12:483–496. doi: 10.1093/schbul/12.3.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Tibbo P, Kroetsch M, Chue P, Warneke L. Obsessive-compulsive disorder in schizophrenia. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2000;34(2):139–146. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(99)00048-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised. Psychological Corporation; New York: 1981. [Google Scholar]
  32. Woods SW. Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2003;64(6):663–667. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v64n0607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES