Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Aug 25.
Published in final edited form as: Exp Aging Res. 2013;39(2):162–178. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2013.761547

Older Adults’ Hemodynamic Responses to an Acute Emotional Stressor: Short Report

Kathi L Heffner 1, Paul G Devereux 2, H Mei Ng 3, Amy R Borchardt 3, Karen S Quigley 4
PMCID: PMC4548932  NIHMSID: NIHMS711506  PMID: 23421637

Abstract

Background/Study Context

Vascular and myocardial activation can each increase blood pressure responses to stressors, but vascular responses are uniquely associated with negative affect, pernicious coping processes, and cardiovascular risk. These hemodynamic correlates of coping in response to acute stressors have not been well-characterized in older adults.

Methods

Adults 65 to 97 years of age (N = 74) either engaged in written disclosure about a distressing event (acute stressor) or wrote objectively about a neutral topic (control). Blood pressure, impedance cardiography, and affect measures were assessed at baseline and in response to writing. Moderating effects of age on affect, blood pressure, vascular, and myocardial responses to the acute stressor were tested using multiple linear regression models.

Results

Follow-up tests of age x writing group interactions indicated that the expected effects of written disclosure on systolic and diastolic blood pressure responses were diminished with increasing age. Regardless of age, compared to neutral writing, written disclosure increased negative affect and vascular responses, but not myocardial responses.

Conclusion

Blood pressure responses to an acute, emotionally evocative stressor were indistinguishable from blood pressure responses to a control condition among the eldest older adults in our sample. In contrast, characterizing the hemodynamic mechanisms of blood pressure responses revealed notable vascular effects of the acute, emotional stressor across a wide age range. Such characterization may be particularly useful for clarifying the psychophysiological pathways to older adults’ cardiovascular health.

Keywords: older adults, impedance cardiography, stress, reactivity, emotion, health

Introduction

Psychophysiological studies of cardiovascular reactivity can illuminate pathways through which stress and coping influence incidence and progression of cardiovascular (CV) disease. Research guided by biopsychosocial models of active and passive coping (Obrist, 1981) and challenge and threat (Blascovich, Mendes, Tomaka, Salomon, & Seery, 2003) suggests that both situational and individual differences in coping resources affect cardiovascular responding in distinct ways that may, in turn, differentially affect cardiovascular risk. Yet, there has been little attention paid to these psychophysiological pathways in older adults for whom the incidence of cardiovascular disease is substantial (Arnold et al., 2005; Driver, Djousse, Logroscino, Gaziano, & Kurth, 2008; Haan et al., 1996).

Passive coping situations, in which people have little or no control over outcomes, typically result in acute increases in blood pressure as a result of constriction of peripheral blood vessels (Hartley, Ginsburg, & Heffner, 1999; Obrist, 1981; Sherwood, Dolan, & Light, 1990). Vascular constriction is also observed when an individual perceives greater threat relative to coping resources, regardless of the whether or not control is afforded by the situation (Heffner, Ginsburg, & Hartley, 2002; Quigley, Barrett, & Weinstein, 2002; Tomaka, Blascovich, Kelsey, & Leitten, 1993). Moreover, vasoconstriction increases when people respond to stressors with increased negative affect (e.g., (Sinha, Lovallo, & Parsons, 1992). In contrast, controllable, active coping situations (Hartley et al., 1999; Obrist, 1981; Sherwood, Dolan et al., 1990), and perceptions of “challenge” – that is, perceiving that one has coping resources sufficient to meet the demand -- are associated with blood pressure increases due primarily to enhanced myocardial function (Heffner et al., 2002; Quigley et al., 2002; Tomaka et al., 1993). Heightened blood pressure reactivity to acute stressors itself predicts poor cardiovascular outcomes (Treiber et al., 2003), but evidence further suggests that heightened blood pressure due to vascular constriction has particularly negative cardiovascular health implications (Ottaviani, Shapiro, Goldstein, & Mills, 2007; Sherwood & Turner, 1995; Steptoe & Marmot, 2005).

The majority of findings characterizing these distinct psychophysiological processes are from studies of younger adults, and relatively few studies have evaluated vascular and myocardial mechanisms in older adults (e.g., Jennings et al., 1997; Pugh & Wei, 2001; Uchino, Uno, Holt-Lunstad, & Flinders, 1999). Given age-related changes in cardiovascular responses to acute stressors (Uchino, Birmingham, & Berg, 2010, for review), as well as changes in cognitive capacity and emotion regulation across the lifespan (e.g., Labouvie-Vief, 2003), the generalizability of these psychophysiological processes to older adults is unclear. It is therefore worthwhile to examine psychophysiological mechanisms of blood pressure responses across a wide age range of older adults.

Using data from a study of older adults’ coping and health, we examined the effects of an emotionally evocative stressor, written disclosure (Pennebaker, 2003), on blood pressure responses, and the vascular and myocardial mechanisms of those responses, in adults 65 years of age and older. Written disclosure heightens physiological arousal and negative affect (e.g., Epstein, Sloan, & Marx, 2005; Sloan & Marx, 2004), but studies have been limited to younger adults, and the mechanisms of blood pressure response during writing are unknown. We hypothesized that among older adults, written disclosure, compared to writing about neutral topics, would lead to larger increases in blood pressure primarily due to vascular constriction, indexed by total peripheral resistance (TPR; Sherwood, Dolan et al., 1990). In light of age-related increases in blood pressure reactivity to acute stress (Uchino et al., 2010), we also examined whether age moderated the effects of written disclosure on blood pressure, TPR and an index of myocardial activity, cardiac output (CO; Sherwood, Dolan et al., 1990) in older adults.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Seventy-four adults 65 years of age or older and without evidence of dementia (evaluated by a clock drawing task (CLOX1 scores > 10; Royall, Cordes, & Polk, 1998)) were recruited from the community for a study of stress and coping in older adulthood1. The resulting sample was 65 to 97 years of age (M = 75.89 years, SD = 7.91), the majority was female (74.7%), 93.3% were white, and non-Hispanic, 2.7% were American Indian or Alaskan Native, 1.3% were Hispanic or Latino, and 1.3% were Black or African American. Participants were paid $20 for participation in 4 study sessions comprising the larger study. The current study uses data collected during the first study session. The research was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Nevada, Reno and all participants gave their informed consent prior to inclusion in the study.

Self-Report Measures

Participants reported current health status, including presence of cardiovascular disease, history of cardiac events, hypertension, prescribed medication use and other chronic diseases. Positive and negative affect was measured using The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) prior to and after writing.

Cardiovascular Measures

Basal thoracic impedance (Z0), its first derivative (dZ/dt), and the electrocardiogram (ECG) were obtained using a BoMed NCCOM3-R7 impedance cardiograph (ICG; BoMed Medical Manufacturing, LTD, Irvine, CA) and an 8 disposable, pre-gelled, Ag/AgCl spot electrode arrangement in accordance with published guidelines (Sherwood, Allen et al., 1990). An internal processor in the BoMed derived and then output to disk heart rate (HR), stroke volume (SV), and CO (calculated as SV × HR) from the Z0, dZ/dt, and ECG analog signals recorded by the electrodes (see Hartley et al. (1999) for further details).

Systolic (SBP), diastolic (DBP) and mean arterial pressure (MAP; used in the derivation of TPR (where TPR = (MAP/CO)*80) were assessed every 2 minutes (Shapiro et al., 1996) with a Sentron automatic, oscillometric BP monitor (Bard Biomedical Division, Lombardi, IL) with the occlusion cuff placed over the brachial artery on the upper arm of the non-dominant hand.

Writing Task

The written disclosure paradigm used in this study was previously adapted for older adults (Klapow et al., 2001). Participants were asked to “write about your deepest thoughts and feelings about the most distressing experience in your life. The experience could be something that happened in the past or is happening right now.” Participants in the neutral topic group were instructed to write objectively about their plans for the day (Greenberg & Stone, 1992). All participants were asked to write for 15 minutes, to not worry about grammar or punctuation, and to repeat what they had already written if they ran out of things about which to write. The piece of paper containing the instructions was left with the participant during the writing task.

Procedures

Study sessions were conducted either at a university laboratory (62.7%) or in a private setting at senior housing centers; procedures were identical. After informed consent, the electrodes and the BP cuff were attached. During a 20-minute adaptation period, the participant completed questionnaires and then sat for a 5-minute rest period while baseline cardiovascular measures were recorded. She or he was then randomly assigned to the written disclosure or neutral topic condition, received the respective writing instructions, and wrote for 15 minutes. Physiological measures were recorded throughout the rest period and writing task, during which time the experimenter remained behind a portable curtain, out of sight of the participant.

Data Analysis

SBP, DBP, TPR, and CO values were averaged across the 5-minute baseline and across the 15-minute writing task to evaluate average reactivity across the task. Baseline differences were tested via ANOVA for each cardiovascular measure. Reactivity scores were calculated (task value – baseline value) for each measure and served as dependent variables in the analyses.

Demographic and health status differences by writing group were tested using chi-square or independent t-tests. Effects of writing on BP, TPR and CO reactivity, and moderation by age, were examined using multiple regression analyses separately for each dependent measure. Predictors were mean-centered. Our regression models predicting each cardiovascular reactivity measure included the corresponding baseline measure, age, writing group (disclosure versus neutral topic), and the age x writing group interaction term (α = .05).

Two-way interactions were evaluated using two methods. First, we conducted simple slopes analyses (Aiken & West, 1991; Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003) examining the effects of writing groups at the mean age of the sample (75.72 years), and one SD above (83.56 years) and below (67.91 years). Next, to more precisely examine age ranges for which written disclosure may affect reactivity, we determined regions of significance (Potthoff, 1964; Preacher, Curran, & Bauer, 2006; Rogosa, 1981) using web-based software (http://quantpsy.org/interact/index.html). This technique specified the range of the moderator (i.e., age) for which the slope of the physiological variable regressed onto writing condition was significantly different from zero (i.e., where the treatment had an effect). Significant main or moderated effects of writing were also followed by paired t-tests with Bonferroni adjustments to assess the difference between baseline and writing task levels of physiological measures within writing and/or age groups. Outcomes did not differ by data collection site; thus, data from both sites were combined.

Results

There were no differences between the writing groups on any demographic or health indicators (Table 1).

Table 1.

Sample Characteristics and Baseline Measures by Writing Group

Variable Written
disclosure
(n = 41)
Neutral topic
(n = 33)
p
Age [mean(SD)] 75.7 (7.6) 75.7 (8.3) .29
Female [n (%)] 31 (75.6) 24 (72.7) .80
Venue
 University laboratory [n (%)] 24 (58.5) 23 (69.7) .34
Anti-hypertensive medication use [n (%)] 16 (39.0) 9 (27.3) .33
 Adrenergic antagonist (beta or alpha) 5 (12.2%) 6 (18.2%) .53
Known cardiovascular disease [n (%)] 2 (4.9) 3 (9.1) .65
Other chronic condition [n (%)]a 23 (56.1) 18 (43.9) .92
Smoker [n (%)]b 4 (10.3) 5 (15.6) .72
Body Mass Index (kg/m2) [mean(SD)] 26.7 (5.37) 25.7 (4.4) .39
Baseline cardiovascular measure [mean(SD)]
 SBP (mmHg) 140.2 (18.1) 137.2 (20.1) .50
 DBP (mmHg) 74.2 (10.6) 73.2 (12.34) .74
 CO (l/min) 4.4 (1.1) 4.2 (0.9) .53
 TPR (dyne-s*cm5) 1865.6 (683.7) 1826.7 (683.7) .80
Positive affect [mean(SD)]c 33.5 (7.2) 32.1 (6.9) .39
Negative affect [mean(SD)]d 12.2 (4.1) 13.1 (4.0) .36

Note. SBP = systolic blood pressure. DBP = diastolic blood pressure. HR = heart rate. CO = cardiac output. TPR = total peripheral resistance.

a

Other chronic conditions included any endocrinological (e.g., diabetes), immunological (e.g., asthma, arthritis) or other chronic condition (e.g., age-related macular degeneration) self-identified by the participant that was not a disease of the cardiovascular system.

b

Data on smoking were missing from 1 neutral topic and 2 written disclosure participants.

c

Baseline positive affect scores ranged from 15 to 49.

d

Baseline negative affect scores ranged from 10 to 33.

Blood Pressure Reactivity to Writing

In the regression models predicting SBP and DBP reactivity, after controlling for baseline blood pressure, the age x writing group interaction term was significant for DBP reactivity and approached significance for SBP reactivity (Table 2).

Table 2.

Multiple Regression Analyses for SBP, DBP, CO, and TPR Reactivity to Writing

SBP reactivitya
DBP reactivityb
CO reactivityc
TPR reactivityd
B B B B
Variable (SE) β p (SE) β p (SE) β p (SE) β p
Baseline −.18
(.07)
−.27 .02 −.16
(.07)
−.26 .02 −.21
(.05)
−.42 .00
Age .76
(.24)
.48 .00 .40
(.14)
.44 .01 .02
(.01)
.28 .09 6.70
(6.07)
.17 .27
Writing group 2.15
(2.67)
.09 .42 2.42
(1.52)
.17 .12 −.24
(.13)
−.21 .07 148.88
(66.25)
.24 .03
Age x writing
group
−.67
(.35)
−.30 .06 −.46
(.20)
−.36 .02 −.01
(.02)
−.12 .47 −10.18
(8.52)
−.18 .24

Note. SBP = systolic blood pressure. DBP = diastolic blood pressure. CO = cardiac output. TPR = total peripheral resistance.

a

Full model: F(4, 73) = 4.32, p = .004, R2 = .20.

b

Full model: F(4, 73) = 4.83, p = .09, R2 = .09.

c

Full model: F(3, 73) = 2.24, p = .004, R2 = .20.

d

Full model: F(4, 73) = 5.73, p = .000, R2 = .25.

Simple slopes analyses indicated that at the selected value of 1 SD below the mean age of the sample, i.e., 67.9 years), writing about distressing events result in higher DBP (B = 6.01; p = .01) and SBP reactivity (B = 7.39; p = .05) compared to writing about neutral topics. At the mean sample age (75.7 years) and 1 SD above it (83.6 years), the simple slope of writing group regressed onto DBP and SBP reactivity was not significant (all p’s > .12). The region of significance analysis for DBP resulted in 95% region of significance with a lower bound of 74.6 years and an upper bound beyond the range of the sample (121.1 years), implying that the effect of written disclosure on DBP reactivity was statistically significant at ages up to 74.6 years. For SBP, the 95% region of significance had a lower bound outside the lowest age in the sample (−28.4) and an upper bound of 68 years of age, suggesting that SBP effects of written disclosure were also greatest among the older adults in the lower age range of the sample.

Figures 1A and 1B depict the interaction of age and writing condition on SBP and DBP reactivity; for illustration, 75 years of age was used as the cut-off to distinguish age groups based on findings from the region of significance analysis for DBP. Paired t-tests indicated that 3 of the 4 groups (that is, groups categorized as 75 years of age or older versus those younger than 75 years of age by writing group) had a significant increase in DBP from baseline to writing after adjustment for multiple comparisons; the change in DBP from baseline to writing was on average non-significant among older adults below 75 years of age who wrote about neutral topics. All groups showed significant SBP increases from baseline, although the neutral topic writers below 75 years of age had, on average, the smallest increase.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Baseline and age-adjusted diastolic blood pressure (A) and systolic blood pressure (B) reactivity by writing group and age category. ** p < .01 and * p < .05; indicates significant change from baseline within group based on paired t-tests with Bonferroni adjustment comparing the baseline to the writing task.

Descriptive statistics for the blood pressure measures at baseline and during writing, within age (below 75 years of age or 75 years and older) and writing groups, are presented in Table 3, along with potential confounders (i.e., gender, anti-hypertensive use, BMI), none of which differed either between or within writing groups for either age group (all p’s > .20). It should be noted that although the effect of written disclosure on BP was on average diminished among adults in the older age ranges in our sample, there was overlap of the distribution of BP reactivity scores for the age categories used for illustration, as well as greater dispersion within the older age category (e.g., neutral topic writing, DBP change: below 75 years of age, M = 4.3 mmHg, SD = 5.6; ≥ 75 years of age, M = 10.9, SD = 9.1). This suggests variability in the extent to which written disclosure effects diminished among adults at or above 75 years of age.

Table 3.

Descriptive Statistics for Cardiovascular Measures and Potential Confounds by Age and Writing Groups

Below 75 years of age
75 years of age or older
Written
disclosure
(n = 18)
Neutral topic
(n = 15)
Written
disclosure
(n = 23)
Neutral topic
(n = 18)
Female [n (%)] 11 (61.1) 12 (80.0) 12 (66.7) 20 (87.0)
Anti-hypertensive use [n (%)] 6 (33.3) 4 (26.7) 10 (43.5) 5 (27.8)
Adrenergic antagonist [n (%)] 4 (22.2) 4 (26.7) 1 (4.3) 2 (11.1)
BMI (kg/m2) [mean(SD)] 28.6 (5.6) 26.2 (3.8) 25.2 (4.8) 25.2 (4.8)
SBP (mmHg) [mean(SD)]
 Baseline 135.1 (16.1) 138.6 (15.5) 144.2 (19.0) 136.1 (23.7)
 Writing 154.8 (19.8) 148.5 (14.7) 161.5 (17.6) 158.2 (23.9)
DBP (mmHg) [mean(SD)]
 Baseline 74.3 (11.5) 76.2 (9.7) 74.0 (10.1) 70.8 (14.0)
 Writing 85.1 (13.4) 80.5 (7.3) 83.9 (11.1) 81.6 (12.8)
CO (liters/min) [mean(SD)]
 Baseline 4.4 (1.0) 4.5 (1.0) 4.4 (1.3) 3.9 (0.6)
 Writing 4.3 (0.9) 4.4 (1.1) 4.4 (1.4) 4.2 (0.8)
TPR (dyn-s*cm-5) [mean(SD)]
 Baseline 1825.9 (659.3) 1798.1 (610.0) 1896.7 (715.3) 1882.0 (593.0)
 Writing 2115 (576.3) 1867.7 (448.6) 2087.0 (728.7 2071.2 (540.3)

Note. BMI = body mass index. SBP = systolic blood pressure. DBP = diastolic blood pressure. CO = cardiac output. TPR = total peripheral resistance.

Adrenergic antagonists include both beta and alpha blockers.

Myocardial and Vascular Influences on Blood Pressure Reactivity

In subsequent regression models, we evaluated the effects of writing group and age on CO and TPR reactivity (Table 2). Written disclosure was a significant predictor of TPR reactivity during writing, and approached significance as a predictor of CO reactivity; the age x writing group interaction was not significant for CO or TPR reactivity. Specifically, older adults writing about distressing events had greater increases in TPR and minimal changes in CO compared to older adults writing about neutral topics. Figures 2A and 2B illustrate the effect of writing group on TPR and CO reactivity.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Baseline and age-adjusted cardiac output (A), and total peripheral resistance (B) reactivity by writing group, and for exploratory purposes, cardiac output (C), and total peripheral resistance (D) reactivity by writing group and age category. ** p < .01 and * p < .05; indicates significant change from baseline within group based on paired t-tests with Bonferroni adjustment comparing the baseline to the writing task.

Although the age by writing group interaction was not significant for TPR and CO, given the findings from the simple slopes analyses, we explored whether potential trends in TPR and CO responses within the age and writing groups could help explain age-moderated effects of written disclosure on blood pressure reactivity (descriptive data presented in Table 3). Before adjustment for multiple comparisons, paired t-tests indicated that CO increased significantly for adults at or above 75 years of age writing about neutral topics (M = .47 l/min, SD = .70; t(17) = −2.86, p = .01), though after Bonferroni adjustment, the difference was no longer significant. CO reactivity was minimal for all other groups (Figure 2C). As shown in Figure 2D, TPR reactivity was statistically significant for both age groups writing about distressing events, both before and after Bonferroni adjustment, but not for either age group writing about neutral topics.

Positive and Negative Affect Following Writing

Regressing age, writing group, and the age x writing group interaction onto negative affect change (post-writing negative affect minus pre-writing negative affect) indicated a significant effect of condition (B = 5.61; p = .000; full model: F (3, 72) = 5.81, p = .001, R2 = .20); older adults who wrote about distressing events had a greater increase in negative affect than those writing about neutral topics (Figure 3A). Paired t-tests also indicated that mean negative affect was lower post-writing compared to pre-writing in the neutral writing group (Figure 3A). This effect was relatively minimal, however, and negative affect did not differ at baseline across the 2 writing conditions. The age x condition interaction term was not significant (B = −.12; p = .48), suggesting no moderating effects of age. Neither age (B = .20; p = .18), condition (B = −2.04; p = .21), nor their interaction (B = −.22; p = .29) related to positive affect change pre- to post-writing (full model: F (3, 73) = 1.21, p = .31, R2 = .05; Figure 3B).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Mean negative (A) and positive (B) affect at baseline and post-writing; *p < .05, ** p < .01.

Discussion

Writing about distressing events was associated with a significant increase in TPR and negative affect across a wide age range of older individuals. On the contrary, the effect of written disclosure on DBP and SBP measures diminished with increasing age, with older adults at higher age ranges showing on average comparable increases in blood pressure during writing regardless of writing topic. Together, these findings suggest that the mechanisms underlying blood pressure responses during writing (e.g., increases in vasoconstriction versus myocardial activation) vary depending on the content of writing and the accompanying emotional response. In the absence of impedance cardiography-derived measures of TPR, the blood pressure findings for the oldest adults in the sample would be difficult to interpret. These data, in keeping with prior data (Kline et al., 2002), show that examining the hemodynamic mechanisms underlying BP reactivity is important in fully characterizing acute psychophysiological responses to stress. Limiting assessment to changes in physiological endpoints, without considering the mechanisms underlying such changes, can obscure our interpretations of individuals’ cognitive and affective states (Berntson, Cacioppo, & Quigley, 1993). Thus, the findings here underscore the need to use more fine-grained assessment and analysis of cardiovascular function in order to elucidate psychophysiological pathways to older adults’ cardiovascular health outcomes.

These findings also support the use of written disclosure about distressing events (e.g., Pennebaker, 2003) as an emotionally evocative psychological stressor for older adults. Our data suggest that the affective quality of the task did not differ across this wide age range despite the age-moderated effects on blood pressure reactivity to written disclosure. Further, the significantly larger increase in vasoconstriction and enhanced negative affect elicited by writing about distressing events as compared to neutral topics suggests the possibility of greater psychological threat (Heffner et al., 2002; Quigley et al., 2002; Tomaka et al., 1993) and/or passive coping (Hartley et al., 1999; Heffner et al., 2002; Sherwood, Dolan et al., 1990) during a single session of written disclosure. In light of age-related changes in emotion regulation (Carstensen, Fung, & Charles, 2003; Labouvie-Vief, 2003), tasks like written disclosure that explicitly engage and strongly activate cognitive-emotional processes may provide especially rich information about the psychophysiological correlates of stress and coping across the lifespan relative to other experimental stimuli which may not be as emotionally meaningful for older adults (Uchino et al., 2010).

The increase in blood pressure during neutral writing among adults 75 and older is noteworthy and may have been due to the observed increases in CO (although non-significant after Bonferroni correction) coupled with a small and non-significant increase in TPR. We examined whether the number of words written could potentially explain the CO increase, but there were no main or interactive effects of age and writing group on word count (data not shown). We also did not find an association between age and CO reactivity in our sample, in accord with prior studies (Pugh & Wei, 2001; Uchino et al., 2010). TPR responses to written disclosure (or to writing in general) also did not change as a function of age across the range of ages tested here. The current study includes a sample of adults 65 to 97 years of age, with a mean sample age slightly over 75 years, with 56% of participants over 75 years of age. Although arguably a strength of the study, this age distribution renders comparisons to prior studies difficult, where most have compared young adults to older adults, and the upper age range of older adults is typically limited to 75 years of age. Also, the relatively homogeneous sample of predominantly Caucasian females limits the generalizability of these findings.

The population of adults 65 years of age and older is rapidly expanding and, as noted, cardiovascular disease incidence over 65 is sizeable (Arnold et al., 2005; Driver et al., 2008; Haan et al., 1996). Thus, there is a growing need for methodologies and measures that can be used to characterize the myocardial and vascular concomitants of psychological and emotional coping with life stressors in older adults. Attention to the myocardial and vascular mechanisms of blood pressure responses to acute emotional stressors, using tasks such as written emotional disclosure, likely will lead to a better understanding of the potential health effects of stressor exposures in older adulthood.

Acknowledgments

This study was partially supported by National Institute on Aging grants 1R03AG019908 and R24 AG031089-01, a VA Health Services Research & Development grant (IIR-02-296), and by a grant from the University of Nevada Reno, Junior Faculty Research Grant Fund. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NIA, NIH, the Department of Veterans Affairs, or the University of Nevada, Reno. We wish to thank the research participants and directors of the senior residences who made this research possible. We also thank Brenda Gosser for her invaluable help with data collection and management.

Footnotes

1

The CLOX1 was introduced after the initiation of the larger study; thirty participants in the current sample did not complete the CLOX1.

References

  1. Aiken LS, West SG. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnold AM, Psaty BM, Kuller LH, Burke GL, Manolio TA, Fried LP, et al. Incidence of cardiovascular disease in older Americans: The cardiovascular health study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2005;53(2):211–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53105.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berntson GG, Cacioppo JT, Quigley KS. Cardiac psychophysiology and autonomic space in humans: empirical perspectives and conceptual implications. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;114(2):296–322. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.2.296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Tomaka J, Salomon K, Seery M. The robust nature of the biopsychosocial model challenge and threat: A reply to Wright and Kirby. Personality & Social Psychology Review. 2003;7(3):234–243. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0703_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Carstensen LL, Fung HH, Charles ST. Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion. 2003;27(2):103–123. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. 3rd Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; Mahwah, NJ: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  7. Driver JA, Djousse L, Logroscino G, Gaziano JM, Kurth T. Incidence of cardiovascular disease and cancer in advanced age: Prospective cohort study. British Medical Journal. 2008;337:a2467. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a2467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Epstein EM, Sloan DM, Marx BP. Getting to the heart of the matter: Written disclosure, gender, and heart rate. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67(3):413–419. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000160474.82170.7b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Greenberg MA, Stone AA. Emotional disclosure about traumas and its relation to health: Effects of previous disclosure and trauma severity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;63(1):75–84. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.63.1.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Haan MN, Selby JV, Rice DP, Quesenberry CP, Jr., Schofield KA, Liu J, et al. Trends in cardiovascular disease incidence and survival in the elderly. Annals of Epidemiology. 1996;6(4):348–356. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(96)00054-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hartley TR, Ginsburg GP, Heffner K. Self presentation and cardiovascular reactivity. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1999;32(1):75–88. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(99)00003-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Heffner KL, Ginsburg GP, Hartley TR. Appraisals and impression management opportunities: Person and situation influences on cardiovascular reactivity. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2002;44(2):165–175. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00200-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Jennings JR, Kamarck TW, Manuck SB, Everson SA, Kaplan GA, Salonen JT. Aging or disease? Cardiovascular reactivity in Finnish men over the middle years. Psychology and Aging. 1997;12(2):225–238. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Klapow JC, Schmidt SM, Taylor LA, Roller P, Li Q, Calhoun JW, et al. Symptom management in older primary care patients: feasibility of an experimental, written self-disclosure protocol. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2001;134:905–911. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-134-9_part_2-200105011-00015. 9 Pt 2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kline KA, Saab PG, Llabre MM, Spitzer SB, Evans JD, McDonald PA, et al. Hemodynamic response patterns: responder type differences in reactivity and recovery. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(6):739–746. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3960739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Labouvie-Vief G. Dynamic integration: affect, cognition, and the self in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2003;12(6):201–206. [Google Scholar]
  17. Obrist PA. Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspective. Plenum; New York: 1981. [Google Scholar]
  18. Ottaviani C, Shapiro D, Goldstein IB, Mills PJ. Vascular profile, delayed recovery, inflammatory process, and ambulatory blood pressure: laboratory-to-life generalizability. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2007;66(1):56–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Pennebaker JW. The social, linguistic and health consequences of emotional disclosure. In: Suls J, Wallston KA, editors. Social psychological foundations of health and illness. Blackwell; Malden, MA: 2003. pp. 288–313. [Google Scholar]
  20. Potthoff RF. On the Johnson-Neyman technique and some extensions thereof. Psychometrika. 1964;29:241–256. [Google Scholar]
  21. Preacher KJ, Curran PJ, Bauer DJ. Computational tools for probing interaction effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 2006;31(4):437–448. [Google Scholar]
  22. Pugh KG, Wei JY. Clinical implications of physiological changes in the aging heart. Drugs and Aging. 2001;18(4):263–276. doi: 10.2165/00002512-200118040-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Quigley KS, Barrett LF, Weinstein S. Cardiovascular patterns associated with threat and challenge appraisals: a within-subjects analysis. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(3):292–302. doi: 10.1017/s0048577201393046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Rogosa D. On the relationship between the Johnson-Neyman region of significance and statistical tests of parallel within group regressions. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 1981;41:73–84. [Google Scholar]
  25. Royall DR, Cordes JA, Polk M. CLOX: An executive clock drawing task. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 1998;64(5):588–594. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.64.5.588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Shapiro D, Jamner LD, Lane JD, Light KC, Myrtek M, Sawada Y, et al. Blood pressure publication guidelines. Psychophysiology. 1996;33(1):1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb02103.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Sherwood A, Allen MT, Fahrenberg J, Kelsey RM, Lovallo WR, van Doornen LJ. Methodological guidelines for impedance cardiography. Psychophysiology. 1990;27(1):1–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb02171.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Sherwood A, Dolan CA, Light KC. Hemodynamics of blood pressure responses during active and passive coping. Psychophysiology. 1990;27(6):656–668. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb03189.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Sherwood A, Turner JR. Hemodynamic responses during psychological stress: implications for studying disease processes. Int J Behav Med. 1995;2(3):193–218. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0203_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Sinha R, Lovallo WR, Parsons OA. Cardiovascular differentiation of emotions. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1992;54(4):422–435. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199207000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Sloan DM, Marx BP. A closer examination of the structured written disclosure procedure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72(2):165–175. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Steptoe A, Marmot M. Impaired cardiovascular recovery following stress predicts 3-year increases in blood pressure. Journal of Hypertension. 2005;23(3):529–536. doi: 10.1097/01.hjh.0000160208.66405.a8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Tomaka J, Blascovich J, Kelsey RM, Leitten CL. Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 1993;65(2):248–260. [Google Scholar]
  34. Treiber FA, Kamarck T, Schneiderman N, Sheffield D, Kapuku G, Taylor T. Cardiovascular reactivity and development of preclinical and clinical disease states. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2003;65(1):46–62. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200301000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Uchino BN, Birmingham W, Berg CA. Are older adults less or more physiologically reactive? A meta-analysis of age-related differences in cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory tasks. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2010;65B(2):154–162. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Uchino BN, Uno D, Holt-Lunstad J, Flinders JB. Age-related differences in cardiovascular reactivity during acute psychological stress in men and women. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 1999;54(6):339–346. doi: 10.1093/geronb/54b.6.p339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 1988;54(6):1063–1070. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES