Abstract
Data from the New Immigrant Survey were used to describe the home environments of 638 children ages birth to 3 years whose parents legally immigrated to the United States. Thirty-two indicators of home conditions were clustered into four domains: discipline and socioemotional in support, learning materials, enriching experiences, and family activities. Results revealed variation in how frequently infants from every country (Mexico, El Salvador, India, Philippines) and region (East Asia, Europe, Caribbean, Africa) studied experienced each home environmental condition. There were differences between countries and regions on many indicators as well as differences based on parents' level of education. The experiences documented for children of recent legal immigrants were similar to those documented for children of native-born families in other studies.
Providing a supportive home environment for infants can be challenging, especially for parents who no longer reside in their country of origin. Such families often lack the supportive structure of a network of kith and kin and may struggle with goals and ways of doing things that are quite unfamiliar. According to the 2010 census, one in four children living in America has at least one foreign-born parent (Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics, 2011), and that is likely an underestimate given the difficulty in accurately tracking unauthorized immigrants (Pew Hispanic Center, 2013). Little is known about the home environments of children of legal (much less illegal) immigrants in the United States even though a worldwide literature has pointed to the importance of children's experience at home (Bradley & Corwyn, 2005). In their policy-focused review of challenges that face those who wish to provide services to support infant mental health, Cohen, Oser, and Quigley (2012, April) specifically identified the special needs of immigrant families and made the observation that programs should “respond to their unique health and developmental needs (p. 8).” Although the literature on parenting among immigrants is growing, much remains undocumented regarding how different life at home is for U.S. children being reared by native-born and foreign-born parents. Most studies have focused on a small segment of the immigrant population living in a single location or on a narrow band of parenting practices, with very little on infancy. The lack of basic information about the experiences and surroundings of children of recent immigrants in their own home environments is problematic in a number of ways; but it poses particular challenges for service providers who might assist such families in the transition to parenthood. Programs such as Early Head Start and the Maternal, Infant, and Early Childhood Home Visitation Program in the United States come to mind as examples, but there is an established history of government-run home health visitation programs in many developed countries as well, almost all of which have immigrant families as participants (Bilukha et al., 2005).
Some adults enter the United States with a strong desire to fully assimilate into the American way of life whereas others come with a desire to maintain some of the values and ways of doing things from their countries of origin (Zhou, 1997). Formulating goals for childrearing and designing environments to support those goals become part of the larger panoply of actions newcomer parents engage in as part of their acculturation process (Berry, 2007). According to Nesteruk and Marks (2011), there is a general tendency for immigrants to adopt some of the values of the country of destination or to meld country-of-origin practices with those present in the country of destination. For example, Kim and Wong (2002) found that Chinese parents who immigrated to America professed more “Westernized” beliefs pertaining to parental control than did Chinese parents who immigrated to Taiwan. Even so, precisely how this plays into the routines of daily life, the physical affordances of the place of residence, and the interplay among family members is difficult to forecast. Moreover, how it affects interactions between parent and child also is a function of how parents cope with the transition to America, with the stresses that often entails (García-Coll & Magnusson, 1997; Pumariega, Rothe, & Pumariega, 2005). Indeed, a recent national study found high levels of aggravation among immigrant parents, leading to calls for “outreach efforts by clinicians, mental health professionals, school systems, and social workers… that will help improve the parenting experience of immigrant households (p. 2107)” (Yu & Singh, 2012). Kim and Hong (2007) argued for the importance of becoming more aware of distinct cultural proclivities of various immigrant communities for nurses, as nurses are often called upon to assist families with issues related to parenting.
The goal of this study is to more fully document some of the actions, objects, events, and conditions that research has suggested are implicated in children's competence and adaptive functioning as they are experienced by young children whose parents migrated to the United States. Research has shown that developmentally supportive environments include provisions for safety, a variety of stimulating materials and social encounters, warm and sensitive behavior on the part of major caregivers, the absence of harsh treatment, consistent organization of routines and physical features in a child's primary settings, and careful monitoring of children's behavior and circumstances (Bradley, 2009). In a review of world literature involving use of measures of the home environment, we found that children's development was associated with the variety of materials and opportunities for stimulation afforded them at home, the level of warmth and responsiveness displayed by parents, and the amount of physical discipline parents administered to them (Bradley & Corwyn, 2005). Even so, there are cultural variations regarding how important parents consider particular actions, objects, and activities for children (Fuller & García Coll, 2010). The New Immigrant Survey (NIS; Jasso, Massey, Rosenzweig, & Smith, 2000) affords an opportunity to more fully characterize the experiences and household conditions present for infants whose parents are legal immigrants and to determine whether those experiences connect to parental education in ways similar to those observed for others in the United States. This study documents the frequency with which infants whose parents legally migrated to the United States are exposed to various types of stimulating materials and enriching activities, the tendency of parents to provide various forms of socioemotional support during normal encounters with the child, how often the infant spends time with key family members, and the degree of safety in the home environment. We provide breakdowns according to countries and regions of origin and breakdowns by parents' level of education, as there is reason to believe that both may be implicated in what an infant experiences.
Culture, Modernization, and the Process of Immigration
According to the Migration Policy Institute (2013), immigration patterns into the United States during the final two decades of the 20th century and the first decade of the 21st century were considerably different from those seen in prior generations, especially regarding legal immigration. There has been a dramatic upsurge of immigrants from Latin America (other than Mexico), Asia, Africa, and the former Soviet Union (Capps, McCabe, & Fix, 2011; Greico, 2010; U.S. Census Bureau, 2013). The reasons for migration to the United States vary both within and across regions and countries.
Many factors determine what children experience at home; cultural beliefs, parental education, and economic conditions being among the most widely cited (Bradley & Corwyn, 2003; Conger & Donnellan, 2007; Rogoff, 2003). While there is an emerging consensus in countries throughout the world regarding the value of stimulation and education for children, there is less uniformity of opinion regarding other socialization goals. Parents from different societies have different goals and different means of achieving goals for their children (Gauvin, 2000; Rogoff, 2003). Even basic parenting tasks (e.g., soothing a fretful infant, developing communicative skills, promoting psychomotor skills) can take different forms or can be emphasized earlier in different societies. For example, mothers in some African societies use soothing language to calm a crying infant less often than is the case in many Western societies, preferring to comfort by means of strokes and caresses (Bradley, 2009; Kilbride & Kilbride, 1983). Likewise, there tends to be less verbal communication directed to infants and more emphasis on developing early psychomotor skills, the latter connected to the emphasis on “functionalism” (Adeyemi & Adeyinka, 2002; Werner, 1972). In many Asian societies, there is an emphasis on character development and living up to social expectations, sometimes leading to more assertive forms of discipline and greater monitoring of child behavior (Chao & Kanatsu, 2008; Lehrer, 1988; Tajima & Harachi, 2010), although it is not clear precisely how this might affect parenting of infants. Although such cultural proclivities might lead to more controlling behavior on the part of parents, it does not mean that parents who migrate from Asia would tend to be less warm. Values such as chiao shun and guan also could lead to using parenting practices generally considered “good” (e.g., praise) toward different ends than is typical for Western parents (i.e., as a means of reinforcing children's efforts to do the expected thing rather than acting in a self-directed manner) (Wang, Wiley, & Chiu, 2008). Somewhat by distinction, Asian families steeped in Buddhist values tend not to get personally involved in their children's academic efforts (García Coll et al., 2002), although it is not altogether clear how this might impact their early education efforts. In many African societies, respect of authority figures is generally considered important, often leading to lower expressions of warmth toward children and more frequent use of physical forms of discipline (Jackson, 2012). The strong sense of “familism” that characterizes most Latin American societies means that family members tend to engage in many joint activities (Estrada-Martinez, Padilla, Caldwell, & Schulz, 2011). Broad notions such as individualism-collectivism and autonomy-embeddedness have been used to characterize cultural tendencies that favor a more adult/authority versus a more child/autonomy set of values pertaining to parenting practices and the kinds of experiences afforded offspring (Kwak, 2003). Cultural proclivities like those described are likely to influence the experiences of infants whose parents migrate from particular countries. However, the connections are not always straightforward, as there are multiple dimensions of culture and variations within societies regarding how values are enacted (Estrada-Martinez et al., 2011; Warikoo & Carter, 2009).
Although cultural traditions help determine how parents care for children and organize the home environment, there is not always a tight connection between traditional cultural practices and parenting behavior, especially for parents who immigrate (Chao & Kanatsu, 2008). There has been movement toward “Western” styles of parenting in many countries, a trend that may be more pronounced in people who decide to immigrate to countries such as the United States (Camarota, 2012; Driscoll, Russell, & Crockett, 2008; Kwak, 2003; Tajiima & Harachi, 2010). However, if parents are anxious or feel uncertain about matters pertaining to their child's well-being, they may choose to maintain greater control over the children's activities and whereabouts (Driscoll et al., 2008). In effect, it remains unclear just how the movement toward “Western” styles may have influenced each of the varied household conditions and parenting practices deemed salient for infant development (Zhou, 1997).
Parental education appears to play a strong role in determining how parents approach childrearing (Walker et al., 2011). A worldwide literature has shown that parents with higher levels of education provide more social and material stimulation to their children, stimulation that includes a variety of potentially enriching activities and excursions (Bradley & Corwyn, 2005; Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & García Coll, 2001; Carneiro, Meghir, & Parey, 2013; Linver, Brooks-Gunn, & Kohen, 2002). Research has consistently shown that parents with higher levels of education also tend to manifest higher levels of social and emotional support for their offspring (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001; Carneiro et al., 2013; Yaman, Mesman, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Linting, 2010; Zhang, 2012). In a particularly revealing study, Mistry, Beisanz, Chien, Howes, and Benner (2008) found that family socioeconomic status (SES; based on a combination of maternal education and family income) predicted maternal language stimulation and maternal supportiveness for both native-born and immigrant mothers whose children were enrolled in Early Head Start. Better educated parents tend to have more “modern” beliefs about what children need and how they can be effective on their children's behalf (Palacios, Gonzalez, & Moreno, 1992). Studies done throughout the world have shown that maternal education is related to many decisions pertaining to children's care (Abuya, Ciera, & Kimani-Murage, 2012; Emina, Kandala, Inungu, & Ye, 2011; Hug & Tasnim, 2008; Miller & Rodgers, 2009), decisions about how best to interact with children and what kinds of opportunities they require to support their health and competence (Bradley, 2009).
As families are exposed to values and practices in the country of destination, there is a tendency for them to adopt some of those values or to meld country-of-origin values and practices with those prevalent in the country of destination (Nesteruk & Marks, 2011). As Kwak (2003) noted, there has been a discernible transformation in Chinese families toward the autonomy side of the autonomy-embeddedness continuum. However, studies of Japanese and Latin American immigrants have suggested that the transformation of parenting styles among immigrants is an uneven one, with a tendency to see behavioral adjustments in parenting practice prior to changes in parenting beliefs (Bornstein & Cote, 2001, 2004; Cote & Bornstein, 2001). Given the general movement toward autonomy-oriented values, if parents are anxious or feel uncertain about matters pertaining to their child's well-being, they may choose to maintain greater control over the children's activities and whereabouts (Driscoll et al., 2008). Factors such as parental education, neighborhood of residence, and family wealth help determine the patterns of parent–child interaction likely to occur and the kinds of materials and activities children with foreign-born parents are likely to be exposed to (García Coll et al., 2002).
In overview, cultural values, historical trends, parents' level of education, and the process of immigration blend in intricate ways to determine how immigrant parents in the United States enact the role of parenting, how they organize home life, in what activities they involve their children, and what materials they acquire to assist their children in the process of development. The notion of segmented assimilation suggests that adoption of American ways could very well be different depending on one's place of origin and the context of reception at destination (Zhou, 1997); but this also means that it is hard to predict just how often children with foreign-born parents will have access to particular materials or particular experiences often considered beneficial for development. In the very small number of studies that have compared parenting behaviors of immigrant parents to those of native-born parents, few significant differences have emerged in parenting behaviors such as reading to children, spanking them, or displaying warmth (Jackson, 2012). However, those studies generally have used relatively small samples, and analyses were done on only a small number of home conditions and parenting behaviors.
Using data from the NIS, our primary goal is to simply document how frequently infants from various countries and regions of the world are exposed to particular types of materials and experiences connected with their home life, with a view that such information can be helpful in guiding future research pertaining to environment-development relations for immigrant children and for working with immigrant families (Titzmann, 2013). To aid with interpretation, items are organized into three broad groupings that the literature has suggested are associated with children's development (Bradley, 2009): (a) socioemotional support, (b) household learning materials, and (c) activities and encounters at home. The groupings are broad, and some items do not neatly fit into the assigned category; but since no summary scores were calculated within categories, this small number of groupings seemed nonetheless useful. A similar approach was used in presenting data from a large U.S. sample (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001).
Methods
Sample
The NIS is a nationally representative, longitudinal study of new legal immigrants to the United States. It is based on representative samples of the administrative records compiled by the U.S. Immigration and Naturalization Service. The baseline survey was conducted in 2003 to 2004; the follow-up in 2007 to 2009 (Jasso, Massey, Rosenzweig, & Smith, 2005). A focal adult was the identified participant in the study, and initial interviews were completed on 8573 immigrants. Two children living with the focal adult were considered for inclusion on a selective basis. First preference was given to a child between 8 and 12 years old, second preference for children between 5 and 17 years, and last preference for children under age 5 years. Data from 638 children ages birth to 3 years were analyzed for purposes of this study, as those were the children on whom key markers of the home environment were available. The 638 children were clustered into four country (Mexico, El Salvador, India, Philippines) and four regional (East Asia, Europe, Caribbean, Africa) groups for purposes of analysis, as those were the only groups for which there was a sufficient sample size. Information was available on the education level of the focal adult. Breakdowns were made according to the focal adult's years of education (0–8, 9–13, ≥ 14). We used a similar approach in analyzing data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, a nationally representative survey of American families (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001).
Measures
Each focal adult was interviewed using a battery of assessments in 2003 to 2004. The interview included a series of structured questionnaires dealing with family demographics, pre-immigrant experience, employment, health, income, assets, and social variables (see nis.princeton.edu/nis_2003_questinnaires.html). When children were present in the home, an assessment of the home environment of the selected children was performed using items from the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment Inventory (HOME; Caldwell & Bradley, 2003). Specifically, the NIS used the HOME-short form (HOME-SF) that was implemented as part of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth study (HOME-SF–NLSY79; www.nlsinfo.org/content/cohorts/nlsy79-children/topical-guide/assessments/home-home-observation-measurement). There are specific age-related versions of the HOME-SF; each entails a series of standardized questions with multiple options. As part of the broader NIS battery, the focal adult also provided information on several other family events and materials present in the household that resembled the kinds of items contained in the HOME-SF. For purposes of the analyses presented herein, there were 23 items for which data were gathered using structured interview and eight items for which data were obtained using observation during the time of the visit. Abbreviated versions of each item are contained in Tables 1 and 2. Each of the items was dichotomized (0, 1) to correspond to the original scoring of the HOME Inventory. A total score was computed by summing scores from the 28 dichotomous items contained in standard HOME-SF, consistent with practice on the original HOME Inventory (Caldwell & Bradley, 2003).
Table 1. Percentage of Immigrant Families Receiving Credit for Home Conditions and Parenting Behaviors by Country/Region.
Country/Region | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||||||
Condition or Behavior | Mexico | El Salvador | East Asia | India | Philippinesa | Europe | Caribbean | Africa |
Household Learning Materials | ||||||||
Family gets daily newspaper | 38.9 | 16.7 | 73.3 | 51.7 | 71.4 | 62.2 | 29.5 | 43.6 |
Family gets magazines | 24.8 | 31.3 | 66.3 | 62.1 | 25.7 | 58.5 | 37.5 | 67.7 |
Home has encyclopedia | 22.8 | 29.2 | 31.4 | 65.5 | 25.7 | 58.5 | 32.1 | 43.6 |
Home has dictionary | 76.5 | 77.1 | 83.7 | 72.4 | 88.6 | 85.4 | 72.3 | 69.4 |
Home has computer | 45.6 | 60.4 | 66.3 | 77.6 | 97.1 | 69.5 | 69.6 | 80.7 |
Home has >50 books | 38.9 | 50.0 | 74.4 | 84.5 | 68.6 | 62.2 | 60.7 | 72.6 |
Home has VCR | 89.3 | 93.8 | 84.9 | 100.0 | 45.7 | 78.1 | 83.9 | 96.8 |
Home has calculator | 81.9 | 81.3 | 82.6 | 87.9 | 91.4 | 85.4 | 77.7 | 82.3 |
Home has DVD | 65.1 | 60.4 | 73.3 | 84.5 | 54.3 | 61.0 | 50.9 | 56.5 |
Socioemotional Support | ||||||||
Spanked ≤once last week | 96.0 | 91.7 | 95.4 | 89.7 | – | 92.7 | 89.3 | 98.4 |
Spoke to child | 79.7 | 76.9 | 81.3 | 76.9 | – | 72.7 | 95.0 | 89.5 |
Responded verbally to child | 88.2 | 76.9 | 90.6 | 92.3 | – | 90.9 | 85.0 | 89.5 |
Caressed or kissed child | 91.2 | 88.5 | 93.8 | 76.9 | – | 90.9 | 87.5 | 94.4 |
Did not hit or slap child | 98.6 | 100.0 | 87.5 | 100.0 | – | 100.0 | 90.2 | 89.5 |
Did not interfere with child | 63.8 | 73.1 | 75.0 | 100.0 | – | 100.0 | 80.5 | 79.0 |
Provided toy for child | 72.5 | 56.0 | 81.3 | 69.2 | – | 72.7 | 62.5 | 77.8 |
Kept child in view | 85.5 | 84.6 | 90.6 | 69.2 | – | 81.8 | 95.0 | 88.9 |
Activities & Encounters | ||||||||
Read to child 1+/week | 71.1 | 66.7 | 75.6 | 91.4 | 88.6 | 90.2 | 74.1 | 74.2 |
Taken to grocery 1+/week | 93.3 | 97.9 | 89.5 | 77.6 | 80.0 | 95.1 | 85.7 | 85.5 |
Child outside house 1+/week | 81.2 | 77.1 | 75.6 | 82.8 | 80.0 | 90.2 | 75.0 | 74.2 |
Teaches child about toys | 22.8 | 33.3 | 43.0 | 60.3 | 25.7 | 43.9 | 24.1 | 46.8 |
Child: | ||||||||
Watches TV <1 hr/daily | 55.7 | 54.2 | 60.5 | 63.8 | 42.9 | 86.6 | 58.0 | 61.3 |
Watches <1 hr/weekends | 66.4 | 66.7 | 64.0 | 67.2 | 51.4 | 84.2 | 61.6 | 54.8 |
Sees father daily | 89.3 | 89.6 | 95.4 | 93.1 | 88.6 | 92.7 | 83.0 | 91.9 |
Meals with mother & father twice/day | 41.6 | 39.6 | 55.8 | 37.9 | 65.7 | 41.5 | 39.3 | 59.7 |
TV on <5 hr/day | 55.0 | 64.6 | 66.3 | 63.8 | 42.9 | 75.6 | 56.3 | 46.8 |
Parent talks to child while doing daily chores | 86.6 | 87.5 | 87.2 | 87.9 | 91.4 | 95.1 | 94.6 | 87.1 |
Learning Materials for Children | ||||||||
3+ cuddly/role-play toys | 86.6 | 87.5 | 89.5 | 89.7 | 94.3 | 96.3 | 87.5 | 87.1 |
2+ push/pull toys | 75.2 | 75.0 | 79.1 | 84.5 | 68.6 | 84.2 | 69.6 | 71.0 |
Child has 3+ books | 71.1 | 77.1 | 75.6 | 84.5 | 91.4 | 82.9 | 69.6 | 58.1 |
Play environment safe | 84.8 | 83.3 | 83.3 | 82.4 | 75.0 | 73.7 | 83.7 | 77.8 |
For the Philippines, the number of cases for which data on the observation items were available was too small to include mean values in the table.
Table 2. Percentage of Immigrant Families Receiving Credit for Home Conditions and Parenting Behaviors by Parent Education.
Years of Education | |||
---|---|---|---|
|
|||
Condition or Behavior | 0–8 | 9–13 | ≥14 |
Household Learning Materials | |||
Family gets daily newspaper | 36.0 | 39.0 | 55.7 |
Family gets magazines | 30.3 | 34.7 | 59.1 |
Home has encyclopedia | 23.6 | 28.7 | 48.0 |
Home has dictionary | 67.4 | 75.3 | 83.1 |
Home has computer | 51.7 | 59.8 | 76.4 |
Home has >50 books | 40.5 | 55.8 | 71.6 |
Home has VCR | 86.5 | 87.7 | 84.5 |
Home has calculator | 75.3 | 82.5 | 85.8 |
Home has DVD | 50.6 | 61.8 | 68.6 |
Socioemotional Support | |||
Spanked ≤once last week | 91.0 | 94.0 | 93.9 |
Spoke to child | 82.1 | 81.7 | 83.5 |
Responded verbally to child | 86.8 | 83.7 | 91.1 |
Caressed or kissed child | 92.1 | 89.7 | 89.9 |
Did not hit or slap child | 97.4 | 94.9 | 92.5 |
Did not interfere with child | 74.4 | 74.5 | 76.3 |
Provided toy for child | 59.0 | 68.8 | 78.5 |
Kept child in view | 82.1 | 88.7 | 88.6 |
Activities and Encounters | |||
Read to child 1+/week | 58.4 | 74.9 | 86.2 |
Taken to grocery 1+/week | 91.0 | 90.4 | 87.5 |
Child outside house 1+/week | 67.4 | 79.7 | 83.1 |
Teaches child about toys | 21.4 | 30.3 | 43.6 |
Child: | |||
Watches TV <1 hr/daily | 50.6 | 63.4 | 62.5 |
Watches <1 hr/weekends | 58.4 | 68.1 | 65.5 |
Sees father daily | 93.3 | 88.1 | 90.9 |
Meals w mother & father twice/day | 55.1 | 43.8 | 44.9 |
TV on <5 hr/day | 59.6 | 57.4 | 61.2 |
Parent talks to child while doing daily chores | 92.1 | 87.3 | 90.9 |
Learning Materials for Children | |||
Child has 3+ books | 59.6 | 69.7 | 83.5 |
3+ cuddly/role-play toys | 85.4 | 87.3 | 92.2 |
2+ push/pull toys | 67.4 | 75.3 | 79.4 |
Play environment safe | 95.2 | 76.9 | 84.6 |
Given that the aim of this study was to document how frequently children are exposed to particular objects, actions, and events at home and not to make comparisons between families from particular countries or regions, no demographic adjustments were made to the data. Because of the very large amount of data collected from participants in the NIS, it was common to have missing data on some components in the NIS protocol. It was particularly common to have missing data on the observation items from the HOME-SF because those required that the target infant be awake and with the primary respondent during that part of the visit used to collect data from the NIS participants. By contrast, the amount of missing data on the interview items from the HOME-SF was generally modest. Accordingly, we decided to impute missing data for the interview items. We chose not to impute missing data for the observation items because the observation data were available on very small samples from some countries, leaving unclear how accurate the imputed values would be (Note that the content of observation items is quite different from the content of interview items.)
Multiple imputation is regarded as a particularly useful way of handling missing data for data like those used in this study. It was used to deal with missing data on all nonobservation HOME variables (Enders, 2010; Schafer & Graham, 2002). Multiple imputation is a regression-based procedure that generates multiple data sets, each with different estimates for missing values. Using the data-augmentation algorithm in the SAS MI procedure (SAS Version 9.3), we generated 20 imputed data sets. In total, the item-level imputation process included 23 of the 31 HOME variables, and three auxiliary variables (parent education, parent sex, parent country/region of origin). Missing data proportions on these 23 variables ranged between 1 and 48%. The HOME total score was computed from the 20 pooled data sets. Analyses were computed on each of the 20 data sets, and results were pooled to produce final estimates. Note that although the standard is to impute 20 or more data sets (Graham, Olchowski, & Gilreath, 2007), only a single imputed data set is necessary for statistical tests not requiring standard errors (Enders, 2010). Therefore, a single imputed data set was generated for all frequency analyses.
Results
Findings pertaining to the home environments are presented first by country/region of origin of the focal parent. They are then presented by level of education. Finally, there is a consideration of country/regional and educational differences on the HOME-SF total score.
Country/Regional Variation
There were marked variations in how frequently children had certain materials available or had certain experiences (e.g., 92% saw father every day, 75% had three or more children's books, 56% had a DVD player, and <7% had been spanked more than once during the prior week). Table 1 shows the percentage of households in the four countries and four regions who received credit (i.e., scored yes) for each of the 31 items analyzed.
Socioemotional support and discipline
Few children in any of the eight countries/regions had been spanked more than once during the past week (2–11%), with India and the Caribbean showing the highest percentage. Somewhat by contrast, there was close to a bimodal distribution regarding the likelihood that visitors would observe a child being struck while they were present. It very rarely happened in homes of immigrants from Mexico, El Salvador, India, or Europe whereas it happened about 10% of the time in households with immigrants from East Asia, the Caribbean, and Africa. There also were marked variations in how likely parents would interfere with the child's actions while the visitor was present, with parents from Mexico manifesting the highest likelihood (36%) and parents from India and Europe manifesting the lowest incidence (0%). Although the majority of parents from most regions demonstrated some form of physical affection to their children during the visit, there were notable variations across countries, with parents from India the least likely (77%) and parents from East Asia and Africa the most likely (>93%) to do so. There also was considerable variability regarding the likelihood that parents would spontaneously speak to the child (from 73% among European immigrants to 95% among immigrants from the Caribbean) and the likelihood that they would respond to a child's vocalization (from 77% among immigrants from El Salvador to 92% among families from India). Finally, there was considerable variation in how likely it was that the parents would provide the child a toy to play with during the visit (from 56% among immigrants from El Salvador to 81% among families from East Asia) and how consistently the parent kept the child in view during the visit (from 69% among Indian families to over 90% among families from East Asia, the Philippines, and the Caribbean).
Household learning materials
There were wide country/regional variations in the likelihood that homes would get a daily newspaper (17% El Salvador to 73% East Asia) or subscribe to a magazine (25% Mexico to 68% Africa). There also were wide variations in the likelihood that homes would have encyclopedias, dictionaries, VCRs, calculators, and DVD players. The majority of households of families from every country or region had at least 50 books, the primary exception being Mexico (39%). Computers also were fairly common, ranging from 46% in Mexican households to more than 80% among immigrants from Africa and the Philippines. Families from Mexico, El Salvador, and the Caribbean tended to have fewer printed forms of learning materials than was did families from other countries. The patterns for electronic forms were less clear.
Learning materials for children
More than 70% of children from all countries/regions had at least three developmentally appropriate books, the exception being children whose parents came from Africa. More than 85% of children from all societies had cuddly or role-playing toys, the highest percentage being children whose families migrated from Europe (96%). More than 68% also had some type of push or pull toy. In general, children's play environments were safe, with rates ranging from 74% for immigrants from Europe to nearly 85% of the households of Mexican immigrants.
Activities and encounters at home
Roughly 90% of children from every region except the Caribbean saw their father daily, and even 83% of those children did. More than 55% of children whose parents immigrated from East Asia, the Philippines, and Africa ate at least two meals a day with both mother and father, with roughly 40% of children whose parents immigrated from elsewhere doing so. In three fourths of European immigrant households, the television was on less than 5 hr per day. By contrast, having it on more than 5 hr per day was particularly common among immigrants from the Philippines and Africa (>50%). In the homes of Europeans, it was particularly rare for children to watch more than 1 hr of television on weekdays or weekends (∼15%). From one third to one half of children whose parents originated from other areas watched more than 1 hr per day. In more than 85% of households from every society, parents tended to speak to their children while doing ordinary household chores. There was more regional diversity in the likelihood parents would read to their children at least once a week, with rates being highest in households with immigrants from India and Europe (>90%) and lowest in households with immigrants from Mexico, El Salvador, the Caribbean, and Africa (<75%). Similarly, there was considerable diversity in the propensity of parents to teach children about a new toy versus to leave their children to investigate a new toy on their own. Parents from India were the most likely to spend time teaching children about a new toy (60%); by contrast, parents from Mexico, the Philippines, and the Caribbean were least likely (<26%). From 74 to 90% of children were taken outside for an excursion each week, with European immigrants showing the highest propensity to do so. Likewise, taking children to the grocery store at least once a week was common, with Indian-origin households having the lowest frequency (80%).
Variations by Years of Parent Education
Just as there were country/regional variations in the percentage of children who had each type of home experience examined, so were there differences by parents' level of education (see Table 2). Note that there was a small number of cases for which respondents reported 0 years of education. In most cases, 0 was recorded when the focal adult stated that he or she had received no formal education. In a few instances, it was recorded when the focal adult was unable to fully specify what education he or she had received. For purposes of analysis, we separated respondents into three broad education ranges: 0 to 8 years, 9 to 13 years, and 14 or more years.
Socioemotional support and discipline
Relatively few infants had received a spanking during the past week, with children of parents recorded as having 0 to 8 years of education being the most likely (∼9%). There also were few observed instances of children being slapped or hit during the home visit. Interestingly, the highest percentage of slapping or hitting was observed in homes of parents with the highest levels of education (∼7%). By contrast, about one fourth of parents in all three education groups disrupted or interfered with the child's activities during the visit. There was little variation in the likelihood that visitors would observe a parent speaking to a child or responding when a child spoke or vocalized by education group (84–91%). Roughly 90% of parents in all three education groups demonstrated some form of physical affection to the child during the visit. Parents in the lowest education group were slightly less likely to keep the child in view throughout the visit (82 vs. ∼89% in the other two groups). However, there was more variation in the likelihood that they would provide a toy for the child to play with during the visit (from 59% of parents with 0–8 years of education to almost 89% in the highest education group).
Household learning materials
There were variations in the likelihood that homes would get a daily newspaper (36% when parents had 0–8 years of education to 57% when parents had ≥ 14 years) or subscribe to a magazine (30% when parents reported 0–8 years of education to 59% for ≥ 14 years). There also were wide variations in the likelihood that homes would have encyclopedias, dictionaries, VCRs, calculators, and DVD players. In only about 40% of households where parents had low levels of education (<9 years) were 50 or more books observed. This compares to almost 72% of households where parents reported 14 or more years. Likewise, computers were much more common in households where parents had 14 or more years of education (>76%) versus households where parents had 0 to 8 years (48%).
Learning materials for children
There was marked variation in the likelihood that infants would have at least three developmentally appropriate books at home. The percentage was especially low in households where parents were recorded as having 0 to 8 years of education (60%). The percentage was about 70% in households with parents having between 9 and 13 years, and above 80% in the highest education group. There was considerably less variability in the likelihood that children would have three or more cuddly or role-play toys (85% in the lowest education group to 93% in the highest education group) or two or more push/pull toys (67% in the lowest education group to 79% in the ≥ 14 years of education group). For the most part, the play environments in most households appeared safe, with households in the group with 9 to 13 years being more often judged as unsafe (∼23%).
Activities and encounters at home
About 9 of every 10 children saw their fathers regularly in their households irrespective of parental education level. About one half ate at least two meals a day with both parents (a figure a little difficult to interpret given that some children were less than 1 year old). However, children whose parents had the lowest level of education were about 10% more likely to eat at least two meals with both parents than were children in the other two education groups. Around 90% of parents in all groups reported talking to the child while engaged in common everyday household routines. Over 86% of parents with 14 or more years of education reported reading to their children at least once a week, compared to only about 58% with less than 9 years of education. By contrast, less than 1 in 4 parents from the lowest education group reported spending time teaching their children how to use a new toy versus 43% of parents with 14 or more years of education. About 50% of children in the lowest education group watched 1 hr or less of television per day; closer to 60% watched 1 hr or less in the other two groups. The time watching television did not vary much from weekdays to weekend days. In roughly 60% of homes in all education groups, the television was reported as being on less than 5 hr per day. In roughly 90% of households, children were taken to the grocery store at least once a week, with little evidence that parental education was a factor. This pattern contrasts with that related to taking children outside for some type of outing each week. The parents with little education reported doing so with the least frequency (67%) whereas parents in the top two groups were more inclined to take children on an outing (∼80%). Reading to children at least once a week also was much more likely in the higher education groups (75–86%).
Variations on the HOME-SF Total Score
The overall mean scores for the HOME-SF did not very greatly from one country/region to another (18.4–21.7), with households from India, Europe, and East Asia having the highest means. As expected, there were variations in mean HOME-SF scores by parent level of education, ranging from 17.9 for homes where parents had less than 9 years of education to 20.9 for homes where parents had 14 or more years of education. As Table 3 shows, the correlation between parent level of education and the HOME-SF total score were nonsignificant for immigrants from six of the eight countries/regions examined, but significant associations emerged for East Asia, r = .31, p < .01, and Africa, r = .30, p < .05. The overall correlation between parental education and the total HOME-SF score was r = .29, p < .001. As an adjunct to these analyses, we looked at correlations between parental education and the subset of observation items designed to assess the social and emotional support provided by parents. The correlations were nonsignificant in every country as well as across the full sample.
Table 3. Correlations Between Parent Education and the HOME-SF Total Score by Region/Country.
Region/Country | Correlation | p | N |
---|---|---|---|
Mexico | 0.16 | 0.07 | 149 |
El Salvador | 0.24 | 0.11 | 48 |
East Asia | 0.31 | 0.01 | 86 |
India | 0.13 | 0.36 | 58 |
Philippines | 0.17 | 0.42 | 35 |
Europe | 0.22 | 0.10 | 82 |
Caribbean | 0.10 | 0.35 | 112 |
Africa | 0.30 | 0.02 | 62 |
Total Sample | 0.29 | <.001 | 638 |
Discussion
Using data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, and Garcia Coll (2001) found marked differences in what U.S. children experience at home as measured by the HOME-SF: the likelihood that certain learning materials would be available, the kinds of activities that they were involved in, the ways parents treated them, family routines, and physical affordances of the residence. When we analyzed data on the broad spectrum of U.S. children involved in the NLSY, we observed strong relations between variations in home experiences and several markers of children's development (Bradley, Corwyn, Burchinal, McAdoo, & Garia Coll, 2001). In the current study, using data from the NIS, we found variations in all these areas for children of legal U.S. immigrants. However, it is not easy to precisely characterize the experiences of children who have a foreign-born parent or how such experiences derive from immigrants' experiences in the early years of life based on data from the NIS because there appears to be interplay with many other aspects of the family context (Cardona, Nicholson, & Fox, 2000).
Because theory and research have suggested complexity in how various personal and contextual factors come together to influence parenting and because there were not large numbers of immigrants from most countries who had infants, we decided to analyze data only for the individual countries and world regions where there appeared to be sufficient cases to allow for meaningful estimates pertaining to the aspects of the environment assessed by the HOME-SF. It also led to our decision not to make statistical comparisons between regions and countries. Rather, we offer general observations regarding the findings in the hope that they will be useful for scholars and practitioners interested in immigrant families. It would seem that having information on general patterns of parenting behaviors and home conditions for various subgroups of immigrants could enable practitioners to more effectively engage immigrant parents in meeting the needs of their children. Despite limitations imposed by the data, the data from the NIS provide documentation of many aspects of home life for infants born to recent legal immigrants to the United States that are not found elsewhere. Thus, documenting these experiences should lead to a better understanding of the lives of recent immigrants and should help establish a foundation for several lines of inquiry pertaining to the development of immigrant children.
Data from the NIS showed that most children in immigrant households live with two parents; they live with two parents who either are from the same country of origin or with one foreign-born parent and one American-born parent. As stated earlier, it also is the case that different social, economic, political, and geographic circumstances tend to help determine why parents from particular countries migrate to the United States (Camarota, 2012) and the kind of reception that they are afforded upon arrival (García Coll & Marks, 2011). Moreover, as the findings indicate, children's experiences at home also reflect the parents' level of education; but education was more strongly implicated in household conditions for immigrants from some countries than from others. Parental level of education had little relation to the level of social and emotional support manifested to children during the visits used to collect data. Rather, educational status was more strongly connected to the types of material possessions that families had and the kinds of activities in which they engaged. Similarly, in a study on immigrant families whose children attended Early Head Start, Mistry et al. (2008) found that family SES was more strongly associated with maternal language stimulation than with maternal supportiveness, although the paths to both parenting behaviors was significant. Bradley and Corwyn (2003) found that household SES was more strongly implicated in the amount of stimulating materials and activities that U.S. children generally were exposed to than how warm and nurturing parents were, although there also were significant associations with the latter.
Material Possessions
Children live in a world dense with actions, objects, people, and events. Over time, these phenomena provide the ingredients and the structure that help determine developmental course (Bradley, 2009). It has long been recognized that children's environments are complex entities that include a diversity of resources arranged in a multiplicity of ways, with children gradually gaining more power (Heft, 1988). The homes of children whose families migrated from different regions varied widely on the likelihood that they would contain various forms of print materials. Households whose members came from Latin America were least likely to have magazines, newspapers, or encyclopedias than were households whose members migrated from other regions. Households where parents migrated from other countries and regions varied with respect to particulars (e.g., European and Indian households had the highest likelihood of containing an encyclopedia and African households were least likely to contain a dictionary whereas East Asian and Filipino households were most likely to get a newspaper). Latin American households also were less likely to have at least 50 books. Although the findings pertaining to written materials for adults in the household are not surprising, they should not be interpreted as suggesting simple cultural differences among immigrants, as immigrant groups differed by level of education and likely other factors that also have implications for using various forms of written materials. In addition, it is not fully clear what some of the differences might imply for infant development, particularly as access to information via electronic devices is rapidly changing the patterns of how adults obtain information.
Findings pertaining to the availability of electronic devices varied widely across world regions. For example, homes of immigrants from Mexico were the only ones where it was less than 50% likely that one would observe a computer whereas homes of immigrants from India and East Asia were the only ones where there was more than 70% likelihood that one would find a DVD player. More than 75% of homes from all countries except the Philippines had a VCR, with European homes the least likely (72%) and homes of Salvadoran, Indian, and African immigrants the most likely (>93%) to have one.
The likelihood that children would have at least 3 developmentally appropriate books mirrored the likelihood that the household would contain more than 50 books, but only to some degree. Specifically, households whose members arrived from East Asia, India, and Europe had high rates of both amounts. In some respects, these findings would seem to connect to those pertaining to enrollment of children in pre-kindergarten programs, as all suggest a commitment to learning and to education. Hernandez and Napierala (2012) reported that more than 60% of children whose parents immigrated from Western Europe, India, and Africa attended pre-kindergarten whereas less than 50% of parents who migrated from other regions enrolled their children in pre-kindergarten. On the other hand, perhaps because only three children's books were required to receive credit for the item, the pattern was not consistent across all regions. For example, although households of African immigrants were quite likely to have 50 or more books (64%), they were the least likely to have 3 or more children's books (57%). With respect to having access to other types of learning/play materials in the home, no obvious regional pattern emerged. Children in more than 85% of households from every country had at least three cuddly or role-play toys, and 70% of homes in every country had two or more push or pull toys. The difference in the two percentages likely reflects the age of children studied, with push and pull toys less likely in the first year of life.
Not surprisingly, when parents had higher levels of education (likely a stand-in for SES more broadly), it was more likely that the household would have books, computers, calculators, and other print materials. It also was more likely that infants would have more books and would be read to more often. The patterns obtained regarding parental education in the NIS also were like those obtained for household income in the NLSY (Bradley, Corwyn, Burchinal, et al., 2001) and household SES using data from the Early Head Start Research and Evaluation study (Mistry et al., 2008). Indeed, variations in parent SES for immigrants from different countries likely contributes to the regional/country differences in materials present in immigrant households. For example, from one fifth to one third of respondents who came from Mexico, El Salvador, and the Caribbean had less than a ninth-grade education. This contrasts to less than 5% from most other countries. Likewise, less than 20% of immigrants from Latin America had 14 or more years of education whereas from 54 to 83% of immigrants from India, East Asia, Europe, and Africa had 14 or more years of schooling. That does not mean that family SES was strongly associated with having all types of material possessions. For example, the likelihood that a child would have a role-play toy or a push/pull toy or that the family would have a VCR did not show as pronounced an SES (parental-education) pattern. In general, the findings corroborate the broader notion that better resourced families tend to invest in offspring well-being via providing them more opportunities for productive encounters with potentially enriching materials and events (Conger & Donnellan, 2007).
Child and Family Activities
Culture is often strongly implicated in the kinds of activities that people undertake as children and as adults (Rogoff, 2003), and there is evidence that immigrants bring certain cultural proclivities with them into the nation of destination (García Coll & Marks, 2011). The NIS contained relatively few indicators of child and adult activities, but for most there were indications of variation by region or country of origin—granted, we did not do statistical tests due to the limited group sizes available. For example, 9 of 10 children of European immigrants left the house for some type of outing at least once a week; this compares to only about 75% of children whose parents came from El Salvador, East Asia, the Caribbean, or Africa. The pattern of taking children to the grocery store every week was quite different, with families from El Salvador the most likely (98%) and families from India the least likely (78%) to do so. Children whose parents arrived from Europe also were least likely to watch more than 1 hr of television per day (∼15%), as compared to children whose parents came from the Philippines, Mexico, or El Salvador (∼40–50%). In addition, there was considerable variability in the likelihood that children would be with both mother and father during mealtime at least twice a day (>55% for East Asia, the Philippines, and Africa vs. ∼40% elsewhere). It is likely that some of the variation observed in family mealtimes reflects parents' working conditions. It is important to further delineate such issues because joint activities such as having meals together have been implicated in a variety of child developmental outcomes, including obesity (Rollins, BeLue, & Francis, 2010).
The extent to which regional variations observed in patterns of family activities and encounters may actually reflect family SES is not fully clear. Fewer of the family activities had the clear SES gradient present for most of the learning materials examined. For example, it was far more likely for children whose parents had less than a ninth-grade education to eat two meals together than for children of better educated parents to do so. Children of more highly educated parents also were less likely to watch more than 1 hr of television per day or to go every week to the grocery store with a parent. The small number of indicators in the NIS that addressed activities connected with home life make it difficult to draw conclusions about the broader connection of activity patterns to country of origin. For the most part, findings pertaining to parental education were similar to those observed in analyses of data from the NSLY (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001), suggesting that parent education and family SES likely play a similar role for new immigrant parents as they do for native-born parents regarding in-home and out-of-home activities involving children. They also likely reflect more “modern” views of parenting and what children require for optimal development (Palacios et al., 1992).
Interactions Between Parents and Children
Consistent, positive interaction with adult caregivers is considered the sine qua non of high-quality parenting. The vast majority of children studied lived with both parents and had daily involvement with their fathers. Previous data collected by the U.S. Census Bureau (2013) also showed that most children of immigrants live with both parents. Only in families from the Caribbean did the percent fall to less than 90%, but was still quite high (87%). The same pattern emerged with respect to parental education. This level of engagement with fathers exceeds what is typical for American families at large (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001; Yeung, Sandberg, Davis-Kean, & Hofferth, 2001).
There is broad agreement that children benefit from caregivers who are warm and responsive to their needs (Bretherton & Waters, 1985; Rohner, 1986). About 80% of parents from East Asia and Africa provided their child something to play with during the visit, but only about 60% of families from El Salvador and the Caribbean did so. It is hard to interpret these differences given the age of the children (i.e., some were <1 year old), but findings suggest that parent education likely played a role. Overall, findings are consistent with those reported by Cardona et al. (2000) for older children.
There were notable regional differences in the percentage of parents who spent time teaching their infants how to play with a new toy. In households of immigrants from Mexico, El Salvador, the Philippines, and the Caribbean, less than one third of parents showed children how to play with a new toy, choosing instead to allow the child to investigate the toy on his or her own. This contrasts to more than 60% of Indian parents. The same general pattern was observed with respect to reading to children: It was less likely among families who immigrated from Mexico, El Salvador, and the Caribbean than it was among immigrants from India, Africa, East Asia, or Europe. To what extent this reflects cultural proclivities is difficult to determine from the NIS, given that there was a pronounced difference in the likelihood that parents with 14 or more years of education (44%) would spend time teaching the child versus the likelihood that parents with less than 8 years of education (21%) would do so. Relatedly, parents with more than 14 years of education (86%) were more likely to read to their children every week as compared to parents with less than 9 years of education (58%). However, the observed regional/country differences are consistent with previously observed cultural proclivities among parents in America (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001), even with family income controlled. They also are consistent with findings from anthropologists pertaining to cultural differences in beliefs about children's developmental needs. For example, Latin American and Caribbean families generally view children as developing at a somewhat slower pace than do European parents, and they are less likely to engage in formal, structured patterns of teaching when children are young (Durbrow, 1999; Harkness & Super, 2002; Pachter & Dworkin, 1997). Interesting in this regard are findings reported by Jackson (2012); specifically, immigrant Latino mothers were about as likely to read to their 5-year-olds as were native-born Latino mothers. This latter finding corroborates arguments proffered by Fuller and García Coll (2010) about the problems connected with making broad generalizations concerning Latino families. Their point is that while a cultural proclivity to privilege parental actions aimed at helping children develop good manners and proper comportment (bien educado) could reduce the likelihood that parents would spend lots of time directly teaching skills needed for school success, many Latino parents blend old cultural traditions with more modern views in rearing their children. Thus, it becomes encumbent upon those working with Latino parents to find out what individual parents actually do.
Analysis of data from the NLSY and the Panel Study of Income Dynamics has revealed that the large majority of American parents express affection to their offspring quite often, although there were some small ethnic and SES variations (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001; Yeung et al., 2001). In this study, we observed variations in how likely an immigrant parent would express physical affection to a child, with lower rates observed in families from India, as expected (Chao & Kanatsu, 2008). Even so, the general level of affection was similar to that observed in American families overall. The findings are reminiscent of those reported by Jackson (2012), who also found that Black and Hispanic immigrant mothers were about as likely to get prenatal care and to breastfeed as were their native counterparts. The findings also are generally consistent with findings reported by Wang et al. (2008). In their study, Chinese immigrant parents manifested generally high levels of praise for their young children, rates that were similar to U.S. parents generally. There also were differences in the likelihood that parents from different countries/regions tended to speak to the child or to respond verbally when the child vocalized. Parents from Africa and the Caribbean were particularly likely to speak to their children during the visit (∼90–95% %) whereas parents from the Philippines and El Salvador were least likely to respond when the child vocalized. Parents from the Caribbean also were among the most likely to report speaking to the child while engaged in everyday tasks (95%), as were parents from Europe (95%). However, most parents from every region reported speaking to the child while doing routine tasks (>85%). To some extent, the proclivity of African immigrant parents to speak to their children is at odds with what has been observed in Africa itself (Adeyemi & Adeyinka, 2002), but this may be connected to the level of education of those who tend to immigrate and their intentions upon arrival (Nesteruk & Marks, 2011). One of the most remarkable findings in this study was how little parent education had to do with the quality of parents' social and emotional support manifested to children during the visit. Although there were small variations in the percentage of parents from the three education groups who spoke to the child, responded verbally to the child, showed physical affection to the child, hit the child, or interfered with a child's activities, the variations were small and there was no discernible pattern. When we correlated parent education to the scores that families received on an index composed of these behaviors, there was not a significant correlation in any region or across regions. It is not easy to compare these patterns to those observed in the NLSY given variations in study design and the relatively modest number of families in each subgroup analyzed, particularly since there were few, if any, immigrants from some countries who had less than 9 years of education. In the NLSY sample, there was a somewhat greater likelihood that high-SES parents will respond to infants' vocalizations (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001). Such a finding is consistent with the idea that more highly educated parents would have more “modern” values regarding engaging in dialogic communication with offspring (Palacios et al., 1992). Although findings using the NIS partially support such a proclivity (i.e., parents with ≥ 14 years of education showed the highest propensity to respond to a child's verbalization), current findings only partially support such a hypothesis (i.e., parents with <9 years of education were at least as likely as were parents with 9–13 years of education to respond verbally to their children). Broader cultural proclivities derived from country of origin or parental personality factors that were not measured may well loom larger than parental education, broadly speaking.
Even though somewhere around 30% of families in the United States report spanking children under age 3 years three or more times per week, only about 5% were observed hitting or slapping a child during visits made to obtain data for the NLSY (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001). The overall rates for spanking children reported by immigrants were a little lower than those reported in the NLSY, but the overall observed rates for hitting and slapping children during the visit were about the same. Such findings are consistent with those reported by Jackson (2012). Interestingly, there were some country/regional variations with respect to the latter indicator. Children from East Asia, the Caribbean, and Africa were about 10% more likely to be hit during the visit, a difference that coincides with cultural practices in the countries of origin (Bradley, 2009; Durbrow, 1999; Harkness & Super, 2002; Pachter & Dworkin, 1997). In these societies, respect for elders is highly valued, and there is little tolerance for interrupting adults. When Tajima and Harachi (2010) studied 11- to 15-year-old offspring whose parents were immigrants from Vietnam and Cambodia, they found that over 40% admitted to spanking their child at some point in the past. However, in their interviews with first-generation Korean American mothers, Kim and Hong (2007) found that spanking was generally considered a kind of last resort when trying to control undesirable behavior in children. The degree of acculturation was a factor in how frequently spanking occurred, but there was not a simple relation between acculturation and parenting beliefs observed.
Data from the NIS also showed that it was more common for parents from Africa, East Asia, and the Caribbean to interfere with a child's actions, but interference was common in households of families from Mexico and El Salvador as well. The latter speaks to some of the nuances connected to cultural influences on parental behavior pertaining to young children. Interestingly, there were limited variations in how children were disciplined or controlled as a function of parental level of education. This is in contrast to the findings that emerged in the NLSY, where children from poor families were more often hit, spanked, and interfered with— irrespective of race or ethnicity (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001). However, Cardona et al. (2000) found that highly educated Hispanic mothers actually used stricter control for their preschool-age children than did Hispanic mothers with lower levels of education. Their argument was that these mothers may have had more concerns about preparing their children to deal with the dominant culture. Unfortunately, there were insufficient cases in some educational subgroups in the NIS data set to allow for a more precise determination of how strongly country of origin was implicated in particular home experiences net of parent education.
The variations in country/regional tendencies with respect to communicating with a child, teaching a child, controlling a child, and expressing physical affection to a child speak to the complexities and nuances related to cultural influences on parenting (Fuller & García Coll, 2010; Harkness & Super, 2002; Rogoff, 2003). In some respects, the patterns that emerged resemble accounts of parenting practices done in societies of origin; in other respects, they mirror what is generally observed in American families. As García Coll and Marks (2011) noted, the blend can sometimes reflect the general tensions present during the process of immigration and acculturation. It is unfortunate that the NIS contains so little of the developmental outcomes for children since it would be useful to better understand how these blends of parenting practices and household affordances play into children's developmental course.
Data in the NIS, although useful for examining experiences of children of legal immigrants, does not capture the full array of children's experiences. First, the data set provides rather limited measurement of family routines, the kinds of regulations parents impose, how parents monitor their offspring, or parental efforts to teach their children or engage them in enriching activities. Second, data in the NIS do not make clear how variations in the experiences that are documented may matter for children's health, competence, or adaptive behavior. Third, immigrants from most countries did not represent the full geographic or SES distributions from their countries of origin—multiple selection factors are operative in migration. Thus, the education distribution from various countries in the NIS tended to be restricted (e.g., few highly educated parents from Mexico and El Salvador, few parents with low educational attainment from India, Europe, Africa, and the Philippines). This lack of good distribution could partially account for the observed low correlations between parent education and the total score on the HOME. Fourth, also note that legal immigrants are in various respects different from undocumented immigrants (much more often having higher levels of education and occupational status, and much less often facing the turmoil that surrounds being undocumented; Gonzales & Chavez, 2012). Thus, even though data from the NIS helps to fill a gap in knowledge about the experiences of children whose parents are immigrants, it does not tell the story of all immigrants. What is perhaps most revealing about the findings presented in this study is how much the home lives of children of legal immigrants resemble the home lives of children whose parents were born in America—at least as reported by their parents. It seems consistent with the contention by Nesteruk and Marks (2011) that immigrants tend to gravitate toward American ways as they acculturate to the United States, but the general move toward Western styles of parenting could be operative as well. Finally, it is important to reiterate that the purpose of this study was to document children's experiences in their home environments (essentially an exercise in descriptive science). As stated earlier, we did not statistically test for differences between regions or countries of origin, and we did not statistically test for differences in levels of parent education. Thus, even though our language suggests differences, any interpretations must be made with full understanding that it was not our intent to test any hypotheses pertaining to country of origin or parent education.
The current wave of immigrants entering the United States is huge, with little sign of abatement regarding legal immigration. However, like every prior wave of immigrants, the new wave contains a different mix than in times past. Knowing more about the lives of children whose parents have recently migrated to the United States should enable various agencies and groups to more effectively engage those families in their role as caregivers.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by Grant R21HD068721from the National Institute of Health and Grant R40MC25675 from the Health Resources and Services Administration, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Contributor Information
Robert H. Bradley, Family and Human Dynamics Research Institute, Arizona State University
Amy Pennar, T. Denny Sanford School of Social & Family Dynamics, Arizona State University.
Jennifer Glick, Center for Population Dynamics, T. Denny Sanford School of Social & Family Dynamics, Arizona State University.
References
- Abuya B, Ciera J, Kimani-Murage E. Effect of maternal education on child's nutritional status in the slums of Nairobi. BMC Pediatrics. 2012;12:80–89. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-12-80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adeyemi M, Adeyinka A. Some key issues in African traditional education. McGill Journal of Education. 2002;37:223–240. [Google Scholar]
- Berry JW. Acculturation strategies and adaptation. In: Lansford JE, Deater-Deckard K, Bornstein M, editors. Immigrant families in contemporary society. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 69–82. [Google Scholar]
- Bilukha O, Hahn R, Crosby A, Fullilove M, Liberman A, Moscicki E, et al. The effectiveness of early childhood home visitation in preventing violence: A systematic review. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2005;28:11–38. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein MH, Cote LR. Mother-infant interaction and acculturation: I. Behavioral comparisons of Japanese American and South American families. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2001;25:549–563. [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein MH, Cote LR. Mothers' parenting cognitions in cultures of origin, acculturating cultures, and cultures of destination. Child Development. 2004;75:221–235. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00665.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH. The home environment. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of cultural developmental science. New York: Psychology Press; 2009. pp. 505–530. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Age and ethnic variations in family process mediators of SES. In: Bornstein MH, Bradley RH, editors. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 161–188. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Caring for children around the world: A view from HOME. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2005;29:468–478. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, Burchinal M, McAdoo HP, García Coll C. Home environments of children in the United States, Part II: Relations with behavioral development through age thirteen. Child Development. 2001;72:1868–1886. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, García Coll C. The home environments of children in the United States, Part 1: Variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development. 2001;72:1844–1867. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bretherton I, Waters E. Growing points of attachment theory. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1985;50(1–2, Serial No. 209):3–35. [Google Scholar]
- Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment Inventory: Administration manual. Little Rock: University of Arkansas; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Camarota SA. Immigrants in the United States: A profile of America's foreign-born population. Center for Immigration Studies Report. 2012 Retrieved from http://www.cis.org/sites/articles/2012/immigrants-in-the-united-states-2012.pdf.
- Capps R, McCabe K, Fix M. New streams: Black African migration to the United States. Washington, DC: Migration Policy Institute; 2011. Retrieved from http://www.migrationpolicy.org. [Google Scholar]
- Cardona PG, Nicholson BC, Fox RA. Parenting among Hispanic and Anglo-American mothers with young children. Journal of Social Psychology. 2000;140:357–365. doi: 10.1080/00224540009600476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carneiro P, Meghir C, Parey M. Maternal education, home environments, and the development of children and adolescents. Journal of the European Economic Association. 2013;11:123–160. [Google Scholar]
- Chao R, Kanatsu A. Beyond socioeconomics: Explaining ethnic group differences in parenting through cultural and immigration processes. Applied Developmental Science. 2008;12:181–187. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J, Oser C, Quigley K. Making it happen, Overcoming barriers to providing infant–early childhood mental health. Zero to Three. 2012 Apr; Retrieved August 9, 2012, from http://www.zerotothree.org/public-policy/federal-policy/early-child-mental-health-final-singles.pdf.
- Conger RD, Donnellan M. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:175–199. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cote LR, Bornstein MH. Mother-infant interaction and acculturation: II. Behavioural coherence and correspondence in Japanese American and South American families. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2001;25:564–576. [Google Scholar]
- Driscoll A, Russell S, Crockett L. Parenting styles and youth well-being across immigrant generations. Journal of Family Issues. 2008;29:185–209. [Google Scholar]
- Durbrow EH. Cultural process in child competence: How rural Caribbean parents evaluate their children. In: Masten A, editor. Cultural processes in child development The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology. Vol. 29. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1999. pp. 97–122. [Google Scholar]
- Emina J, Kandala NB, Inungu J, Ye Y. Maternal education and child nutritional status in the Democratic Republic of Congo. Journal of Public Health and Epidemiology. 2011;3:576–592. [Google Scholar]
- Enders CK. Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Estrada-Martinez L, Padilla MB, Caldwell CH, Schulz AJ. Examining the influence of family environments on youth violence: A comparison of Mexican, Puerto Rican, Cuban, non-Latino Black, and non-Latino White adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2011;40:1039–1051. doi: 10.1007/s10964-010-9624-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Federal Interagency Forum on Child and Family Statistics. America's children: Key national indicators of well-being, 2011. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Fuller B, García Coll C. Learning from Latinos: Contexts, families, and child development in motion. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:559–565. doi: 10.1037/a0019412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- García Coll C, Akiba D, Palacios N, Bailey B, Silver R, DiMartino L, et al. Parental involvement in children's education. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2002;2:303–324. [Google Scholar]
- García Coll C, Magnusson K. The psychological experience of immigration. In: Booth A, Crouter A, Lansdale N, editors. Immigration and the family: Research and policy on US immigration. Mahwah, MJ: Erlbaum; 1997. pp. 91–132. [Google Scholar]
- García Coll C, Marks A. The immigrant paradox in children and adolescents: Is becoming American a developmental risk? Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Gauvin M. The social context of cognitive development. New York: Guilford Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Gonzales RG, Chavez LR. “Awakening to a Nightmare:” Abjectivity and illegality in the lives of undocumented 1.5-generation Latino immigrants in the United States. Current Anthropology. 2012;53:255–281. [Google Scholar]
- Graham JW, Olchowski AE, Gilreath TD. How many imputations are really needed? Some practical clarifications of multiple imputation theory. Prevention Science. 2007;8:206–213. doi: 10.1007/s11121-007-0070-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greico E. American Community Survey Reports, ACS-11. Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau; 2010. Race and Hispanic origin of the foreign-born population in the United States: 2007. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/prod/2010pubs/acs-11.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Harkness S, Super CM. Culture and parenting. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of parenting. 2nd. Vol. 2. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2002. pp. 253–280. [Google Scholar]
- Heft H. Affordances of children's environments. Childhood Environments. 1988;5:29–37. [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez DJ, Napierala JS. Children in immigrant families: Essential to America's future. New York: Foundation for Child Development; 2012. Retrieved April 5, 2014, from http://www.fce.us.org/sites/default/files/FINALChildreninImmigrantFamilies(2)_0.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Hug M, Tasnim T. Maternal education and child healthcare in Bangladesh. Maternal and Child Health. 2008;12:43–51. doi: 10.1007/s10995-007-0303-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson M. Parenting behavior, health, and cognitive development among children in Black immigrant families: Comparing the United States and the United Kingdom. Washington, DC: Migration Policy Institute; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Jasso G, Massey D, Rosenzweig M, Smith J. The New Immigrant Survey pilot (NIS-P): Overview and new findings about U.S. legal immigrants at admission. Demography. 2000;37:127–138. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jasso G, Massey D, Rosenzweig M, Smith J. The New Immigrant Survey 2003 (Round 1) Public release data. 2005 Mar; Retrieved March 7, 2008, from http://nis.princeton.edu.
- Kilbride P, Kilbride J. Socialization for high postive affect between mother and infant among the Baganda of Uganda. Ethos. 1983;11:232–245. [Google Scholar]
- Kim E, Hong S. First generation Korean American parents' perceptions of discipline. Journal of Professional Nursing. 2007;23:60–68. doi: 10.1016/j.profnurs.2006.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim SY, Wong VY. Assessing Asian and Asian American parenting: A review of the literature. In: Kurasaki KS, Okazaki S, Sue S, editors. Asian American mental health: Assessment theories and methods. New York: Kluwer; 2002. pp. 185–201. [Google Scholar]
- Kwak K. Adolescents and their parents: A review of intergenerational family relations for immigrant and non-immigrant families. Human Development. 2003;46:115–136. [Google Scholar]
- Lehrer B. The Korean American. New York: Chelsea House; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Linver MR, Brooks-Gunn J, Kohen D. Family processes as pathways from income to young children's development. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:719–734. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Migration Policy Institute. US historical immigration trends. 2013 Retrieved from http://www.migrationinformation.org/datahub/historictrends.cfm.
- Miller JE, Rodgers YV. Mother's education and children's nutritional status: New evidence from Cambodia. Asian Developmental Journal. 2009;26:131–165. [Google Scholar]
- Mistry RS, Biesanz JC, Chien N, Howes C, Benner AD. Socioeconomic status, parental investments, and the cognitive and behavioral outcomes of low-income children from immigrant and native households. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2008;23:193–212. [Google Scholar]
- Nesteruk O, Marks LE. Parenting in immigration: Experiences of mothers and fathers from Eastern Europe raising children in the United States. Journal of Comparative Family Studies. 2011;42:809–825. [Google Scholar]
- Pachter LM, Dworkin PW. Maternal expectations about normal child development in four cultural groups. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1997;151:1144–1150. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1997.02170480074011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palacios J, Gonzalez M, Moreno C. Stimulating the child in the zone of proximal development: The role of parents' ideas. In: Sigel L, McGillicuddy-DeLisi A, Goodnow J, editors. Parental belief systems. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1992. pp. 71–94. [Google Scholar]
- Pew Hispanic Center. A nation of immigrants: A portrait of the 40 million, including 11 million unauthorized. 2013 Retrieved from http://www.pewhispanic.org/files/01/statistical_portrait_jan_29.pdf.
- Pumariega AJ, Rothe E, Pumariega JB. Mental health of immigrants and refugees. Community Mental Health Journal. 2005;41:581–597. doi: 10.1007/s10597-005-6363-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogoff B. The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Rohner R. The warmth dimension. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Rollins BY, BeLue R, Francis LA. The beneficial effect of family meals on obesity differs by race, gender, and household education: The National Survey of Children's Health, 2003–2004. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2010;10:1335–1339. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:147–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tajima EA, Harachi TW. Parenting beliefs and physical discipline practices among Southeast Asian immigrants: Parenting in the context of cultural adaptation to the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2010;41:212–235. [Google Scholar]
- Titzmann PF. Immigrants or adolescents? The interplay of development and acculturation. APS Observer. 2013 Mar;26:32–34. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Census Bureau. Current population survey. 2013 Retrieved May 10, 2014, from http://www.sensus.gov/cps.
- Walker S, Wachs TD, Grantham-McGregor S, Black M, Nelson C, Hoffman S, et al. Inequality in early childhood: Risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet. 2011;378:1325–1328. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60555-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang YZ, Wiley AR, Chiu C. Independence-supportive praise versus interdependence praise. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2008;32:13–20. [Google Scholar]
- Warikoo N, Carter P. Cultural explanations for racial and ethnic stratification in academic achievement: A call for a new and improved theory. Review of Educational Research. 2009;79:366–394. [Google Scholar]
- Werner E. Infants around the world: Cross-cultural studies of psychomotor development from birth to two years. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 1972;3:12–34. [Google Scholar]
- Yaman A, Mesman J, van IJzendoorn M, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, LInting M. Parenting in an individualistic culture with a collectivisitic cultural background: The case of Turkish immigrant families with toddlers in the Netherlands. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2010;19:617–628. doi: 10.1007/s10826-009-9346-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeung WJ, Sandberg J, Davis-Kean P, Hofferth S. Children's time with their fathers in intact families. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2001;63:136–154. [Google Scholar]
- Yu SM, Singh GK. High parenting aggravation among US immigrant families. American Journal of Public Health. 2012;102:2102–2108. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang X. The effects of parental education and family income on mother–child relationships, father–child relationships, and family environments in the People's Republic of China. Family Process. 2012;51:483–497. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2011.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou M. Growing up American: The challenge confronting immigrant children and children of immigrants. Annual Review of Sociology. 1997;23:63–95. [Google Scholar]