The topic of debate is whether fear influences vestibular-evoked balance responses. Opposing arguments from Reynolds et al. (2015) rely on the reported lack of changes in the early (<800 ms) response to square-wave electrical vestibular stimulation and decreased later (>800 ms) responses. The evidence drawn from the lack of early response change is unconvincing due to an increased likelihood of type II error caused by a small sample size and poor resolution resulting from the following factors: (i) use of centre of mass position and velocity estimates to detect vestibular-evoked responses in leg muscles, especially as tandem-stance requires multijoint control; (ii) the limited number of stimulations; and (iii) small early responses due to low (1 mA) stimulus intensity (Ali et al. 2003). Likewise, interpretation of late responses is confounded by the potential influence of fear on reactions to non-vestibular sensory feedback related to the vestibular perturbation (Day & Guerraz, 2007) and conscious strategies (Reynolds, 2010) within this time period.
We disagree with Reynolds et al. (2015) that fear-related increases in gain and coupling of ground reaction forces (GRFs) to stochastic vestibular stimulation (SVS) are irrelevant to balance. Early responses are clearly distinguishable using GRFs (Marsden et al. 2002), with less mechanical filtering of the neuromuscular response affecting GRFs than kinematics (Forbes et al. 2015). In our study, SVS was engineered to highlight changes in early balance responses to vestibular stimulation that are not confounded by later perturbation-induced sensorimotor responses (Horslen et al. 2014). The significant SVS gain changes observed between 2 and 16 Hz coincides with natural head frequencies recorded during gait and whole-body perturbations (for review, see Forbes et al. 2015). After establishing threat-related effects on early vestibular-evoked balance responses, we confirmed these effects in GRFs and EMG from posturally engaged muscles using an SVS stimulus that included the 0–2 Hz bandwidth and with vestibular-evoked myogenic potentials (see Horslen et al. 2015).
The notion that we have confused statistical and functional significance is false; we view evidence of a true effect, beyond chance or bias, as a necessary prerequisite to considering functional significance. Furthermore, functional significance should not be discounted because of small absolute changes, which would be expected given the small virtual rotational velocity induced by the stimulus (Peters et al. 2015) and are consistent with magnitudes reported elsewhere (Reynolds, 2010; Mian & Day, 2014). In contrast, we believe that the relative change in gain of early vestibular-evoked responses provides more meaningful insight into functional significance. Thus, fear-related increases in gain (81–231%) are non-trivial and functionally significant, particularly when translated to real-life head accelerations.
Call for comments
Readers are invited to give their views on this and the accompanying CrossTalk articles in this issue by submitting a brief (250 word) comment. Comments may be submitted up to 6 weeks after publication of the article, at which point the discussion will close and the CrossTalk authors will be invited to submit a ‘Last Word’. Please email your comment to jphysiol@physoc.org.
Additional information
Competing interests
None declared.
Funding
Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) to M. G. Carpenter (application #326910).
References
- Ali AS, Rowen KA. Iles JF. Vestibular actions on back and lower limb muscles during postural tasks in man. J Physiol. 2003;546:615–624. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.030031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day BL. Guerraz M. Feedforward versus feedback modulation of human vestibular-evoked balance responses by visual self-motion information. J Physiol. 2007;582:153–161. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.132092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbes PA, Siegmund GP, Schouten AC. Blouin JS. Task, muscle and frequency dependent vestibular control of posture. Front Integr Neurosci. 2015;8:94. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horslen BC, Dakin CJ, Inglis JT, Blouin JS. Carpenter MG. CrossTalk proposal: Fear of falling does influence vestibular-evoked balance responses. J Physiol. 2015;000:000–000. doi: 10.1113/JP270269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horslen BC, Dakin CJ, Inglis JT, Blouin JS. Carpenter MG. Modulation of human vestibular reflexes with increased postural threat. J Physiol. 2014;592:3671–3685. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.270744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mian OS. Day BL. Violation of the craniocentricity principle for vestibularly evoked balance responses under conditions of anisotropic stability. J Neurosci. 2014;34:7696–7703. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0733-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsden JF, Castellote J. Day BL. Bipedal distribution of human vestibular-evoked postural responses during asymmetrical standing. J Physiol. 2002;542:323–331. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.019513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters RM, Rasman BG, Inglis JT. Blouin JS. The gain and phase of perceived virtual rotation evoked by electrical vestibular stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 2015 doi: 10.1152/jn.00114.2015. (in press) DOI: 10.1152/jn.00114.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds RF. The effects of voluntary sway control on the early and late components of the vestibular-evoked postural response. Exp Brain Res. 2010;201:133–139. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2017-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds RF, Osler CJ, Tersteeg MCA. Loram ID. CrossTalk opposing view: Fear of falling does not influence vestibular-evoked balance responses. J Physiol. 2015;000:000–000. doi: 10.1113/JP270444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]