Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2016 Aug 2;10:367. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00367

On the Effect of Sex on Prefrontal and Cerebellar Neurometabolites in Healthy Adults: An MRS Study

Dominique Endres 1,*,, Ludger Tebartz van Elst 1,, Bernd Feige 1, Stephan Backenecker 1, Kathrin Nickel 1, Anna Bubl 2, Thomas Lange 3, Irina Mader 4, Simon Maier 1,, Evgeniy Perlov 1,
PMCID: PMC4969301  PMID: 27531975

Abstract

In neuropsychiatric research, the aspects of sex have received increasing attention over the past decade. With regard to the neurometabolic differences in the prefrontal cortex and the cerebellum of both men and women, we performed a magnetic resonance spectroscopic (MRS) study of a large group of healthy subjects. For neurometabolic measurements, we used single-voxel proton MRS. The voxels of interest (VOI) were placed in the pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (pACC) and the left cerebellar hemisphere. Absolute quantification of creatine (Cre), total choline (t-Cho), glutamate and glutamine (Glx), N-acetylaspartate, and myo-inositol (mI) was performed. Thirty-three automatically matched ACCs and 31 cerebellar male–female pairs were statistically analyzed. We found no significant neurometabolic differences in the pACC region (Wilks' lambda: p = 0.657). In the left cerebellar region, we detected significant variations between the male and female groups (p = 0.001). Specifically, we detected significantly higher Cre (p = 0.005) and t-Cho (p = 0.000) levels in men. Additionally, males tended to have higher Glx and mI concentrations. This is the first study to report neurometabolic sex differences in the cerebellum. The effects of sexual hormones might have influenced our findings. Our data indicates the importance of adjusting for the confounding effects of sex in MRS studies.

Keywords: MR spectroscopy, sex, choline, creatine, cerebellum

Introduction

In neuropsychiatric research, aspects of sex have received increasing attention over the past decade. There are interesting sex distribution ratios in populations with neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorders (ASD), which are more common in boys than in girls (Biederman and Faraone, 2005; Tebartz van Elst et al., 2013). In adulthood, women are more frequently affected by depression and anxiety (Karger, 2014). Knowledge about the physiology of sex-related differences is the basis for understanding the pathophysiology of neuropsychiatric sex-related differences. Morphometric studies have shown that males' brains are 9–12% larger than females, although the relevance of this is unclear (Lenroot and Giedd, 2010). Connection studies have illustrated higher intra-lobe (within one hemisphere) neural communication in male brains and higher inter-lobe (between the left and right hemispheres) neural communication in female brains (Ingalhalikar et al., 2014). On a functional level, there are differences in the functional organization of language in the brain; during phonological tasks, brain activation in males is lateralized to the left frontal lobe, while female brains experience more diffuse activation involving the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus (Shaywitz et al., 1995).

Sex vs. gender

In biopsychological research, the terms “sex” and “gender” are often used synonymously (Schuurs and Verheul, 1990). However, in a strict sense, “sex” refers to the biological state of being male or female according to an individual's genetic code, while “gender” focuses on the social role an individual plays in society, that is, whether a person feels and acts according to the expected roles of a female or male. We do not address gender issues in this sense, but rather, investigate the possible cerebral differences in the neurometabolite levels of healthy female and male individuals. Therefore, we use the terms “sex” or “sex-related” to report our findings, not to be confused with the colloquial use of “sex” for sexual intercourse, which is of no issue in this paper.

Neurometabolite signals in females and males

Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) is a unique non-invasive method for measuring the homeostatic levels of various neurometabolites in the brain. Chemical shift imaging (CSI) allows researchers to analyze metabolite ratios in greater regions and perform secondary selection of voxels of interest (VOI). Single-voxel spectroscopy enables absolute quantification of neurometabolites in the predetermined VOI. The established proton MRS (1H-MRS) allows researchers to measure creatine (Cre) levels, the total choline (t-Cho) signal, the combined glutamate (Glu), and glutamine (Gln) signal (Glx), N-acetylaspertate (NAA), and myo-Inositol (mI) (Ross and Bluml, 2001). Cre serves a marker of brain energy metabolism. Due to its constancy over time, it is also used as a reference substance in metabolite ratios (Malhi et al., 2002). The t-Cho signal consists mainly of phosphorylcholine and glycerylphosphorylcholine, and it is a marker of cell membrane turnover (Gujar et al., 2005). Glu is a major excitatory neurotransmitter (Novotny et al., 2003). Gln is the precursor and stored form of Glu in astrocytes (Govindaraju et al., 2000). NAA is a marker of neuronal and axonal integrity. The mI signal is a glial marker and is part of the phosphatidylinositol second messenger system (Ross and Bluml, 2001).

Previous studies analyzing neurometabolic differences between the sexes have mostly found differences in the t-Cho and Glx levels of males and females. The t-Cho levels in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) of males were found to be higher than those of females (Hädel et al., 2013). However, the neurometabolites of the left cingulum showed no difference between sexes (Grachev and Apkarian, 2000). Two small studies found that cerebellar neurometabolites did not differ between sexes (Pouwels and Frahm, 1998; Safriel et al., 2005). Higher concentrations of Glu were found in left female hippocampi (Hädel et al., 2013). An age-related Glu decrease in the basal ganglia was found in males, but not females (Sailasuta et al., 2008; Chang et al., 2009). Table 1 provides an overview of other previous studies investigating our topic.

Table 1.

Previous MRS studies on healthy subjects.

Study N (male/female) Methods Age Localization Findings
Pouwels and Frahm, 1998 17M: 17F 2 T, SVS, 1H-MRS (STEAM) 18–30 years Parietal white/gray matter mI ↑in females parietal white matter Cre ↔, t-Cho ↔, Glu ↔, NAA ↔
Frontal white/gray matter; occipital white/gray matter thalamus; vermis cerebelli, left cerebellar hemisphere Cre ↔, t-Cho ↔, Glu ↔, NAA ↔, mI ↔
Grachev and Apkarian, 2000 19M: 19F 1.5 T, SVS, 1H-MRS (STEAM) 19–31 years Left orbital frontal cortex Glc ↑ in females
Left sensorimotor cortex NAA ↑ in females
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex Lactate ↑ in males
Left thalamus, left cingulum, left insula t-Cho ↔, Glu ↔, NAA, mI ↔, Glc ↔, GABA ↔, lactate ↔
Zhong et al., 2002 22M: 17F 4 T, SVS, 1H-MRS (PRESS) 20–88 years Frontal white matter t-Cho ↑ in males, Cre ↔, NAA ↔, mI ↔
Thalami, basal ganglia Cre ↔, t-Cho ↔, NAA ↔, mI ↔
Pfleiderer et al., 2004 29M: 23F (DLPFC), 40M: 22F (ACC) 1.5 T, SVS, 1H-MRS (PRESS) 20–75 years Left/right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, left anterior cingulate cortex Cre↔, t-Cho↔, Glx↔, NAA↔
NAA in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and left anterior cingulate cortex positively correlated with IQ in women.
Safriel et al., 2005 32M: 40F 1.5 T, SVS, 1H-MRS (PRESS) 20–44 years Gray matter in the cortical frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobe, basal ganglia, thalamus; subcortical white matter in the parietal and frontal lobes; pons, cerebellum (all regions could not be studied in each subject) NAA/Cre ↔, t-Cho/Cre ↔ (in all 10 regions)
Sailasuta et al., 2008; Chang et al., 2009* 39M: 23F 3 T, SVS, 1H-MRS (PRESS) 21–71 years Parietal gray matter t-Cho ↑ in males, Cre ↔, Glu ↔, NAA ↔
Basal ganglia NA ↑ in males, age-dependent Glu ↓ in males, Cre ↔, t-Cho ↔, Glu ↔
Frontal gray/white matter Cre ↔, t-Cho ↔, Glu ↔, NA(A) ↔
Doelken et al., 2009 13M: 16F 3 T, multivoxel technique, 1H-MRS Average age: 29 years Hippocampi, basal ganglia, insula cortex, cingulum, and precuneus; gray and white matter from the frontal and parietal lobes NAA ↔, Glx ↔, t-Cho ↔, mI ↔, Cre ↔
Ostojic et al., 2011 24M: 26F 1.5 T, multivoxel technique, 1H-MRS 30–58 years White matter bilateral anterior, in the middle and in posterior regions; gray matter of the anterior, middle and posterior regions Cho/Cre ↑and NAA/Cre ↑ in the right frontal parafalcine cortex, NAA/Cho ↔ (in all regions)
Hädel et al., 2013 59M: 59F 3 T, SVS, 1H-MRS (PRESS) 19–55 years Left hippocampus Glu ↑ in females Cre ↔, t-Cho ↔, NAA ↔
Anterior cingulate cortex t-Cho ↑in males, Cre ↔, Glu ↔, NAA ↔
*

Chang et al. (2009) reported corrected results from Sailasuta et al. (2008). Studies measuring the ACC or cerebellar regions are in bold type. Abbreviations: M, male; F, female; T, Tesla; SVS, single voxel spectroscopy; 1H-MRS, proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy; STEAM, stimulated echo acquisition method; PRESS, point-resolved spectroscopy; Cre, creatine; t-Cho, phosphorylcholine + glycerylphosphorylcholine; Glx, glutamate + glutamine; NAA, N-acetylaspartate; mI, myo-Inositol; NA, N-acetyl; Glc, glucose; IQ, intelligence quotient. ↑, increased; ↓, decreased; ↔, no metabolite differences between groups.

Rationale of our study

The aim of our study was to analyze neurometabolic sex-related differences in a large sample of healthy adult subjects. The study was part of a large clinical trial funded by the German Federal Ministry of Science and Education (ADHD-NET: 01GV0605, 01GV0606) (Tebartz van Elst et al., 2014; van Elst et al., 2014; Endres et al., 2015; Maier et al., 2015; Philipsen et al., 2015). In this project, we performed structural imaging (Riedel et al., 2014; Maier et al., 2015) and analyzed neurometabolic signals in the pACC and left cerebellum of adult patients with ADHD and ASD (Tebartz van Elst et al., 2014; van Elst et al., 2014; Endres et al., 2015). The voxels were selected based on our earlier finding that glutamatergic alterations occur in these regions (Perlov et al., 2007, 2009, 2010). The ACC is an important region for neuropsychiatric research because it integrates information from other areas of the brain into the fronto-striato-thalamo-frontal circuits (Bush et al., 2000; Tebartz van Elst and Perlov, 2013), therefore contributing to emotional self-control, focused problem solving, error detection, and adaptive responses to changing conditions (Allman et al., 2001). The cerebellum has attracted increasing attention in the field of neuropsychiatry in recent years. Current models view the cerebellum as “smoother” and as a place for internal models of mental functions (Schmahmann, 2004; Ito, 2008). As stated in our trial protocol, which is published on the Internet, we initially planned to analyze a control group of 50 subjects (Philipsen, 2008). During the data generation process, we decided to increase the size of the control group to better answer our fundamental research questions about the effects of sex and age on both regions. Overall, 119 healthy controls were eligible for the MRS study. After the selection procedure, we included 82 pACC and 78 cerebellar high-quality spectra of well-investigated healthy controls. Having measured such a large sample of control subjects, we were able to perform a strict matching procedure for age and IQ levels. Earlier studies found that men had higher t-Cho concentrations in the pACC and that there were no differences in the left cerebellar hemispheres of males and females (Pouwels and Frahm, 1998; Pfleiderer et al., 2004; Hädel et al., 2013). Because a limited amount of data was available from studies with less restrictive methods and different methodological approaches, our statistical analyses were exploratory.

Participants and methods

Approval from the local ethics committee was obtained before starting the study (Faculty of Medicine, Freiburg University, 217/06). The study was part of a larger, government-funded project and was registered by Current Controlled Trials (ISRCTN54096201). Some data from this large multi-center study have already been published in previous papers (Endres et al., 2015; Maier et al., 2015; Philipsen et al., 2015). All participants gave written consent before undergoing scanning.

Recruitment of subjects and matching procedure

Healthy participants were recruited via announcements on the campus of the University Medical Center Freiburg. Overall, 119 healthy controls were eligible for the MRS study. After performing a strict selection procedure, we were able to assess 82 pACC and 78 cerebellar spectra with a balanced ratio of males and females for the cerebellar VOI (39 males to 39 females) and a nearly balanced ratio for the pACC region (40 males to 42 females). The details of the selection procedure have already been published (Endres et al., 2015). Participants with current neurological or psychiatric diseases or who had consumed psychotropic drugs over an extended period of time were excluded. Participants with psychiatric axis I disorders were excluded based on the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Sheehan et al., 1998). Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Hautzinger, 2006), and cognitive deficits were assessed using the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scale (CAARS) (Conners, 1999). BDI scores higher than 18 and CAARS t-scores higher than 65 were pre-defined as exclusion criteria. Premorbid verbal IQ was assessed using the multiple-choice vocabulary intelligence test (Lehrl et al., 1995). After our initial recruitment efforts, the male and female groups were not exactly matched in terms of the possibly influential factors of age (Kaiser et al., 2005) and premorbid verbal IQ (Jung et al., 1999). Therefore, we performed an automatic matching procedure for these factors (Kaller et al., 2014; Tebartz van Elst et al., 2014; van Elst et al., 2014), tolerating age differences of ≤5 years and IQ differences of ≤10 points between individual male–female pairs. This has resulted in optimal matching for 33 pACC and 31 cerebellum male and female controls.

MRI data acquisition

All measurements were obtained using a 3 Tesla whole-body scanner (Siemens MAGNETOM Trio, a TIM System; Erlangen, Germany) with a 12-channel head coil. First, a T1-weighted three-dimensional imaging data set was recorded using a magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo sequence with the following parameters: field of view = 256 × 256 mm2, repetition time = 2200 ms, echo time = 4.11 ms, flip angle = 12°, and voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3. For spectroscopic measurements, the VOIs were located in the pACC (16 × 25 × 20 mm) and the left cerebellar hemisphere (20 × 20 × 20 mm) (Figure 1). For 1H MRS, a point-resolved spectroscopy sequence with a repetition time of 3000 ms and an echo time of 30 ms (number of averages = 96) was used. For absolute quantification of metabolites, we acquired a non-water-suppressed reference spectrum.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Voxel localization in the pregenual ACC and the left cerebellar hemisphere and typical MRS-spectra. Abbreviations: mI, myo-inositol; t-Cho, phosphocholine and glycerophosphorylcholine; Cre, creatine; Glx, glutamate and glutamine; NAA, N-acetylaspartate; ppm, parts per million.

Spectroscopic analysis

MRS analysis was performed following a methodical procedure developed in earlier studies (Tebartz van Elst et al., 2014; van Elst et al., 2014; Endres et al., 2015). The well-known linear combination of the model spectra algorithm was applied for the investigator-independent spectral analysis (Provencher, 1993, 2001), while the absolute quantification of the neurometabolites was estimated using an internal water signal reference (Helms, 2008). Only those metabolites with Cramér–Rao lower bounds of <20% for the main metabolites were included in the statistical analyses (http://s-provencher.com/pub/LCModel/manual/manual.pdf). Each VOI was segmented into gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) using the unified segmentation approach developed by Ashburner and Friston (2005), which was implemented using Statistical Parametric Mapping, Version 8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8/). The metabolic concentrations in each voxel were corrected for the partial volumes of the GM, WM, and CSF based on the segmentation processes.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, Version 22. Group comparisons for parametric variables (age, IQ, nicotine consumption, and psychometric scores) were performed through independent sample t-tests. A p < 0.05 served as the criterion for significance. Metabolite concentrations in the male and female groups were compared using a multivariate analysis of covariance with a general linear model. Sex was set as a fixed factor in this analysis, while the metabolite concentrations of Cre, t-Cho, Glx, NAA, and mI were set as dependent variables. To investigate the overall effect of all five metabolite concentrations on the groups, a multivariate Wilks' lambda test was performed. The level of significance for overall and single-group comparisons was corrected for multiple tests using the Bonferroni approach. We measured two regions, so a p < 0.025 was chosen as the criterion for significance. To test whether there was a dimensional relationship between metabolite signals that differ between groups and factors such as IQ, subclinical depressiveness, ADHD symptoms, and nicotine consumption, we performed a correlation analysis of these variables using an exploratory Pearson correlation coefficient. A p < 0.025 was chosen as the criterion for significance in the correlation analyses because we measured two regions.

Results

Demographic and psychometric information

The demographic and psychometric data are summarized in Table 2. Overall, the psychometric scores measuring the depressive symptoms (BDI), attention impairment (CAARS), and hyperactivity (CAARS) were not significantly different between the sexes. The possibly confounding variable of nicotine consumption (cigarettes per day) was balanced between the groups.

Table 2.

Demographic and psychometric data.

Male (n = 33) Female (n = 33) p-value Male (n = 31) Female (n = 31) p-value
Mean ± SD Mean ± SD Mean ± SD Mean ± SD
Pregenual anterior cigulate cortex Left cerebellar hemisphere
Age 35.76 ± 9.523 (range: 23–58) 35.48 ± 9.884 (range: 22–58) 0.909 36.48 ± 9.359 (range: 23–58) 36.29 ± 9.399 (range: 22–58) 0.936
IQ (Lehrl et al., 1995) 120.64 ± 16.695 120.67 ± 16.635 0.994 120.19 ± 16.672 120.61 ± 16.233 0.920
BDI (Hautzinger, 2006) 2.3045 ± 3.23478 2.3061 ± 3.59217 0.999 2.4210 ± 3.30358 2.3581 ± 3.67440 0.944
CAARS-Total (Conners, 1999) 38.18 ± 9.831 34.42 ± 6.906 0.077 38.03 ± 9.820 33.97 ± 5.523 0.050
Nicotine 1.0909 ± 2.87623 1.6364 ± 4.51953 0.561 1.1613 ± 2.95631 1.2903 ± 3.95132 0.885

Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; IQ, intelligence quotient (measured by MWTB = “Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenz-Test”); BDI, Beck depression inventory; CAARS, Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scales. Nicotine: Consumption in cigarettes per day.

MRS results

Table 3 summarizes the spectroscopic results. Upon comparing the male and female groups, we found no significant differences in the pACC VOI (Wilks' lambda: p = 0.657). In the left cerebellar region, we found significant differences between the groups (Wilks' lambda: p = 0.001). In the male group, Cre and t-Cho levels were significantly increased. In addition, men tended to have higher Glx and mI concentrations, although this was no longer significant after performing the Bonferroni correction (Figure 2). The voxel composition (GM, WM, and CSF) did not differ significantly between the groups.

Table 3.

Spectroscopic findings from the pregenual anterior cingulate cortex and the cerebellum.

Male (n = 33) Female (n = 33) Statistics MANCOVA Male (n = 31) Female (n = 31) Statistics MANCOVA
Mean ± SD Mean ± SD (Wilks'-lambda: p = 0.675) Mean ± SD Mean ± SD (Wilks'-lambda: p = 0.001)
Pregenual anterior cingulate cortex Left cerebellar hemisphere
Cre 8.7765 ± 1.35235 8.6416 ± 1.23539 F = 0.179, p = 0.674 9.5325 ± 0.82719 8.9359 ± 0.77770 F = 8.559, p = 0.005*
t-Cho 2.3039 ± 0.38403 2.1825 ± 0.40183 F = 1.573, p = 0.214 2.3769 ± 0.22968 2.1114 ± 0.22831 F = 20.834, p = 0.000*
Glx 15.9069 ± 2.27305 15.6276 ± 1.95646 F = 0.286, p = 0.594 11.2101 ± 1.06483 10.5301 ± 1.38934 F = 4.678, p = 0.035**
NAA 11.2808 ± 1.31831 11.2864 ± 1.20675 F = 0.000, p = 0.986 9.0551 ± 0.68609 8.9793 ± 0.72651 F = 0.179, p = 0.674
mI 6.0527 ± 0.94888 6.0200 ± 0.96456 F = 0.019, p = 0.890 5.2363 ± 0.92073 4.7326 ± 0.84443 F = 5.038, p = 0.029**

Abbreviations: SD, standard deviation; Cre, creatine; t-Cho, phosphorylcholine + glycerylphosphorylcholine; Glx, glutamate + glutamine; NAA, N-acetylaspartate; mI, myo-Inositol.

*

Significant after Bonferroni correction;

**

not significant after Bonferroni correction.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Cerebellar metabolite concentrations presented as scatterplots. Abbreviations: Cre, creatine; t- Cho, phosphorylcholine + glycerylphosphorylcholine; Glx, glutamate + glutamine; NAA, N-acetylaspartate; mI, myo-Inositol.

Dimensional analysis

We found no correlation between the cerebellar metabolite signals that differed between groups (Cre and t-Cho signals) and psychometric scores in the control groups. In the pACC of male subjects, we found a significant correlation between t-Cho signals and BDI scores (r = 0.423; p = 0.014; N = 33). No significant correlations were found in the female group.

Discussion

The main finding of this study is that Cre and t-Cho signals in the left cerebellar hemisphere are higher in males than in females. We found no differences between sexes in the pACC.

Sample selection and spectroscopic imaging

The healthy subjects were thoroughly tested. Through a battery of psychometric tests, psychiatric axis I and II disorders were excluded. A history of neurological disorders, drug consumption, or neurological or psychiatric medications also led to exclusion. We tried to create age- and IQ-matched male and female groups using an automatic matching procedure (Kaller et al., 2014). Methodologically, the well-established single voxel 1H-MRS was used. Cerebellar brain size differs between the sexes (Raz et al., 1998, 2001), as does the distribution of Cre and t-Cho concentrations in the WM and GM (Ross and Bluml, 2001). Therefore, we corrected for the GM, WM, and CSF content of each measured VOI. In doing so, we excluded false positive results caused by different VOI compositions.

Comparison to previous studies

As noted in the introduction, three studies have investigated the cingulate region, and two studies have examined the cerebellum with respect to sex effects. Hädel et al. (2013) examined 118 healthy controls in the ACC and, like us, found no difference between sexes in terms of Cre, Glx, NAA, and mI concentrations, although they found increased anterior cingulate t-Cho concentrations in males. Another study also found higher anterior cingulate t-Cho-signals in males (Pfleiderer et al., 2004). In the third study investigating the cingulate region, no metabolic differences between sexes were found in the left cingulum (Grachev and Apkarian, 2000). In our study, the t-Cho concentration in the pACC showed a slight tendency to be higher in the male group (2.3039 in males vs. 2.1825 in females; p = 0.214). However, this finding was not significant, similar to the findings of Pfleiderer et al. (2004). The difference in the findings of the present study and those of Hädel et al. might be due to different matching procedures. Hädel et al. (2013) did not create age- and IQ-matched groups; although they used age as a covariate, they did not correct for education or intelligence. A recent study, however, has shown that t-Cho is correlated with IQ (Jung et al., 1999). Thus, IQ should be corrected for, as it is a confounding effect.

To our knowledge, cerebellar neurometabolites were analyzed in only two previous studies. The first study found no differences between sexes in a sample of 34 healthy controls (17 males and 17 females) using a different single-voxel spectroscopy sequence (stimulated echo acquisition mode) (Pouwels and Frahm, 1998). The second study comparing small groups of 10 male and 10 female subjects showed no alterations in NAA/Cre and t-Cho/Cre ratios in either group (Safriel et al., 2005). Increased t-Cho concentrations have been found in various regions of males' brains, including the parietal GM, frontal WM, and ACC (Zhong et al., 2002; Chang et al., 2009; Hädel et al., 2013). In our exploratory study, we detected a correlation between t-Cho scores and depression scores in the male group. Therefore, the t-Cho alterations found in earlier studies might have been influenced by depression. To our knowledge, differences in Cre concentrations have not been described in earlier studies.

Neurochemical perspective

Neurochemically, we found significantly higher Cre and t-Cho concentrations in males' cerebella. The Cre peak, which includes creatine and phosphocreatine, reflects total cellular Cre stores. Regulation takes place via (1) enzyme equilibrium between Cre and phosphocreatine, which plays a central role in energetic adenosine triphosphate synthesis; (2) the biosynthetic pathway through liver and kidney enzymes, where Cre is synthesized; and (3) osmotic forces with increased hyperosmolar states and decreased hypoosmolar states (Ross and Bluml, 2001). Despite this complex regulation, Cre signals remain relatively constant over time and therefore are frequently used as a reference substance in metabolite ratios (Malhi et al., 2002). In this respect, our finding that Cre alterations in the cerebellum depend on one's sex is crucial for MRS research in cerebellar regions because it could explain the strong influence of sex on metabolite ratios (with Cre as the denominator).

The t-Cho signal consists of phosphorylcholine and glycerylphosphorylcholine and, to a smaller degree, the neurotransmitter acetylcholine. In 1H-MRS, the t-Cho signal serves as a marker of membrane turnover because it includes precursors and degradation products of cell membrane phospholipids (Hajek and Dezortova, 2008). Many focal, inflammatory, and hereditary diseases lead to an increase in the t-Cho signal. Cell membrane turnover is an energy-dependent process and thus might be linked to Cre energy metabolism.

Pathophysiological interpretation

What are the reasons for different t-Cho and Cre concentrations in the left cerebellar hemisphere between sexes?

Obviously, the influence of the sex hormones might explain cerebellar neurometabolic variations between the sexes (Rasgon et al., 2001; Rapkin et al., 2011). Ovarian steroid hormones (i.e., primary excitatory estrogens and primary inhibiting progesterone) are widespread in the brain and have modulating effects on brain function (Majewska, 1987; Rasgon et al., 2001). In an earlier study on female subjects, t-Cho/Cre ratios in the parietal WM were found to significantly differ from the mid-follicular stage to the late luteal phase of the menstrual cycle; however, Cre concentrations and the role of the cerebellum were not analyzed (Rasgon et al., 2001). Neurometabolic studies using positron emission tomography with [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose showed menstrual cycle-dependent changes in the cerebellum. There was also an increase in cerebellar activity from the follicular phase to the late luteal phase of the menstrual cycle (Rapkin et al., 2011). In patients with premenstrual dysphoric disorder, these changes were correlated with mood changes (Rapkin et al., 2011). Based on these findings, one might speculate that our finding that cerebellar neurometabolic profiles differ between sexes indicates not structural but ovarian steroid-related changes in neuronal function.

Role of the cerebellum

The cerebellum has received increased attention in neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric research in the past decade. Schmahmann and Sherman defined cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome, which is characterized by disturbances in executive functions, impaired spatial cognition, personality and affect changes, and linguistic difficulties (Schmahmann and Sherman, 1998). The dysmetria of thought theory and the internal modeling machine hypothesis provide explanations for these findings. The dysmetria of thought theory describes the cerebellum as a “smoother” vehicle for motor and mental functions (Schmahmann, 2004), while the internal modeling machine hypothesis views mental activities as controlled by internal models in the cerebellum (Ito, 2008). One might speculate that our finding that cerebellar neurometabolites differ between sexes could be associated with disease distribution (e.g., ADHD, ASD, depression) among the sexes. Findings of this nature are of interest to the public, and further studies are needed to gain a more precise understanding of this topic. Future studies focusing on the differing roles of the cerebellum in males and females should employ hormonal testing, multimodal structural and neurometabolic imaging, and parallel motor and neuropsychological testing.

Conclusion

In our study, we found no significant differences in the pACCs and distinct differences in the left cerebellar neurometabolites of males and females. The finding that Cre is altered in the cerebellum is important for MRS research because it could explain the strong influence of sex on Cre-dependent metabolite ratios. MRS studies should analyze sex-balanced groups in a standardized manner or should otherwise correct for sex. Further multimodal studies analyzing the role of the cerebellum in males and females should be performed.

Author contributions

DE and LTvE conducted the data analysis and wrote the paper. DE, LTvE, SM, and EP organized the study. BF and SB supported the data analysis. TL and IM gave technical support. All authors were crucially involved in the theoretical discussion and performing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

Parts of the study were funded by the German Federal Ministry of Science and Education (BMBF; ADHD-NET: 01GV0605, 01GV0606).

Conflict of interest statement

DE, BF, SB, KN, AB, TL, SM, and EP declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. LTvE: Advisory boards, lectures, or travel grants within the last three years: Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Novartis, Shire, UCB, GSK, Servier, Janssen, and Cyberonics. IM: Lecture fees from Bracco Imaging Deutschland GmbH, Germany; Roche Pharma AG, Germany; UCB Pharma GmbH, Germany.

References

  1. Allman J. M., Hakeem A., Erwin J. M., Nimchinsky E., Hof P. (2001). The anterior cingulate cortex. The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 935, 107–117. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03476.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ashburner J., Friston K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. Neuroimage 26, 839–851. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Biederman J., Faraone S. V. (2005). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 366, 237–248. 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66915-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bush G., Luu P., Posner M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed). 4, 215–222. 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chang L., Jiang C. S., Ernst T. (2009). Effects of age and sex on brain glutamate and other metabolites. Magn. Reson. Imaging 27, 142–145. 10.1016/j.mri.2008.06.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Conners C. K. (1999). Conners' Adult ADHD Rating Scales (CAARS). North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems. [Google Scholar]
  7. Doelken M. T., Mennecke A., Stadlbauer A., Kloska S., Struffert T., Engelhorn T., et al. (2009). Multi-voxel magnetic resonance spectroscopy of cerebral metabolites in healthy adults at 3 Tesla. Acad. Radiol. 16, 1493–1501. 10.1016/j.acra.2009.07.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Endres D., Perlov E., Maier S., Feige B., Nickel K., Goll P., et al. (2015). Normal neurochemistry in the prefrontal and cerebellar brain of adults with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:242. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Govindaraju V., Young K., Maudsley A. A. (2000). Proton NMR chemical shifts and coupling constants for brain metabolites. NMR Biomed. 13, 129–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Grachev I., Apkarian A. V. (2000). Chemical heterogeneity of the living human brain: a proton MR spectroscopy study on the effects of sex, age, and brain region. Neuroimage 11(5 Pt 1), 554–563. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0557 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gujar S. K., Maheshwari S., Björkman-Burtscher I., Sundgren P. C. (2005). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J. Neuroophthalmol. 25, 217–226. 10.1097/01.wno.0000177307.21081.81 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hädel S., Wirth C., Rapp M., Gallinat J., Schubert F. (2013). Effects of age and sex on the concentrations of glutamate and glutamine in the human brain. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 38, 1480–1487. 10.1002/jmri.24123 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hajek M., Dezortova M. (2008). Introduction to clinical in vivo MR spectroscopy. Eur. J. Radiol. 67, 185–193. 10.1016/j.ejrad.2008.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hautzinger M. (2006). Das Beck Depressionsinventar II. Deutsche Bearbeitung und Handbuch zum BDI II. Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services. [Google Scholar]
  15. Helms G. (2008). The principles of quantification applied to in vivo proton MR spectroscopy. Eur. J. Radiol. 67, 218–229. 10.1016/j.ejrad.2008.02.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ingalhalikar M., Smith A., Parker D., Satterthwaite T. D., Elliott M. A., Ruparel K., et al. (2014). Sex differences in the structural connectome of the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 823–828. 10.1073/pnas.1316909110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ito M. (2008). Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 304–313. 10.1038/nrn2332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Jung R. E., Brooks W. M., Yeo R. A., Chiulli S. J., Weers D. C., Sibbitt W. L. (1999). Biochemical markers of intelligence: a proton MR spectroscopy study of normal human brain. Proc. Biol. Sci. 266, 1375–1379. 10.1098/rspb.1999.0790 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kaiser L. G., Schuff N., Cashdollar N., Weiner M. W. (2005). Age-related glutamate and glutamine concentration changes in normal human brain: 1H MR spectroscopy study at 4 T. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 665–672. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kaller C. P., Loosli S. V., Rahm B., Gössel A., Schieting S., Hornig T., et al. (2014). Working memory in schizophrenia: behavioral and neural evidence for reduced susceptibility to item-specific proactive interference. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 486–494. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Karger A. (2014). Geschlechtsspezifische Aspekte bei depressiven Erkrankungen. Bundesgesundheitsblatt 57, 1092–1098. 10.1007/s00103-014-2019-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lehrl S., Triebig G., Fischer B. (1995). Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence. Acta Neurol. Scand. 91, 335–345. 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1995.tb07018.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lenroot R. K., Giedd J. N. (2010). Sex differences in the adolescent brain. Brain Cogn. 72, 46–55. 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Maier S., Perlov E., Graf E., Dieter E., Sobanski E., Rump M., et al. (2015). Discrete global but no focal gray matter volume reductions in unmedicated adult patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. [Epub ahead of print]. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Majewska M. D. (1987). Steroids and brain activity. Essential dialogue between body and mind. Biochem. Pharmacol. 36, 3781–3788. 10.1016/0006-2952(87)90437-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Malhi G. S., Valenzuela M., Wen W., Sachdev P. (2002). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy and its applications in psychiatry. Aust. N.Z. J. Psychiatry 36, 31–43. 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2002.00992.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Novotny E. J., Fulbright R. K., Pearl P. L., Gibson K. M., Rothman D. L. (2003). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy of neurotransmitters in human brain. Ann. Neurol. 54(Suppl. 6), S25–S31. 10.1002/ana.10697 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ostojic J., Kozic D., Lucic M., Konstantinovic J., Covickovic-Sternic N., Pavlovic A., et al. (2011). Multivoxel MRS: right frontal parafalcine cortex - area of neurobiochemical gender differentiation? Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 32, 683–687. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Perlov E., Philipsen A., Hesslinger B., Buechert M., Ahrendts J., Feige B., et al. (2007). Reduced cingulate glutamate/glutamine-to-creatine ratios in adult patients with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder–a magnet resonance spectroscopy study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 41, 934–941. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Perlov E., Philipsen A., Matthies S., Drieling T., Maier S., Bubl E., et al. (2009). Spectroscopic findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: review and meta-analysis. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 10, 355–365. 10.1080/15622970802176032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Perlov E., Tebarzt van Elst L., Buechert M., Maier S., Matthies S., Ebert D., et al. (2010). H1-MR-spectroscopy of cerebellum in adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 44, 938–943. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.02.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Pfleiderer B., Ohrmann P., Suslow T., Wolgast M., Gerlach A. L., Heindel W., et al. (2004). N-acetylaspartate levels of left frontal cortex are associated with verbal intelligence in women but not in men: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Neuroscience 123, 1053–1058. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Philipsen A. (2008). P R Ü F P L A N: Vergleich einer strukturierten störungsspezifischen Gruppenpsychotherapie plus Placebo oder Methylphenidat versus einer psychiatrischen Beratung plus Placebo oder Methylphenidat bei ADHS im Erwachsenenalter – eine erste randomisierte Multizenter-Studie (BMBF-ADHD-C1). Available online at: http://www.uniklinik-freiburg.de/fileadmin/mediapool/07_kliniken/psy_psychiatrie/pdf/forschung/Pr%C3%BCfplan_Protokollversion_5_-Amendment-4_17.12.08.pdf
  34. Philipsen A., Jans T., Graf E., Matthies S., Borel P., Colla M., et al. (2015). Effects of group psychotherapy, individual counseling, methylphenidate, and placebo in the treatment of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 1199–1210. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Pouwels P. J., Frahm J. (1998). Regional metabolite concentrations in human brain as determined by quantitative localized proton MRS. Magn. Reson. Med. 39, 53–60. 10.1002/mrm.1910390110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Provencher S. W. (1993). Estimation of metabolite concentrations from localized in vivo proton NMR spectra. Magn. Reson. Med. 30, 672–679. 10.1002/mrm.1910300604 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Provencher S. W. (2001). Automatic quantitation of localized in vivo 1H spectra with LCModel. NMR Biomed. 14, 260–264. 10.1002/nbm.698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rapkin A. J., Berman S. M., Mandelkern M. A., Silverman D. H. S., Morgan M., London E. D. (2011). Neuroimaging evidence of cerebellar involvement in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 374–380. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rasgon N. L., Thomas M. A., Guze B. H., Fairbanks L. A., Yue K., Curran J. G., et al. (2001). Menstrual cycle-related brain metabolite changes using 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy in premenopausal women: a pilot study. Psychiatry Res. 106, 47–57. 10.1016/S0925-4927(00)00085-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Raz N., Dupuis J. H., Briggs S. D., McGavran C., Acker J. D. (1998). Differential effects of age and sex on the cerebellar hemispheres and the vermis: a prospective MR study. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 19, 65–71. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Raz N., Gunning-Dixon F., Head D., Williamson A., Acker J. D. (2001). Age and sex differences in the cerebellum and the ventral pons: a prospective MR study of healthy adults. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 22, 1161–1167. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Riedel A., Maier S., Ulbrich M., Biscaldi M., Ebert D., Fangmeier T., et al. (2014). No significant brain volume decreases or increases in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder and above average intelligence: a voxel-based morphometric study. Psychiatry Res. 223, 67–74. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.05.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ross B., Bluml S. (2001). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the human brain. Anat. Rec. 265, 54–84. 10.1002/ar.1058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Safriel Y., Pol-Rodriguez M., Novotny E. J., Rothman D. L., Fulbright R. K. (2005). Reference values for long echo time MR spectroscopy in healthy adults. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 26, 1439–1445. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Sailasuta N., Ernst T., Chang L. (2008). Regional variations and the effects of age and gender on glutamate concentrations in the human brain. Magn. Reson. Imaging 26, 667–675. 10.1016/j.mri.2007.06.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schmahmann J. D. (2004). Disorders of the cerebellum: ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 16, 367–378. 10.1176/jnp.16.3.367 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schmahmann J. D., Sherman J. C. (1998). The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain 121(Pt 4), 561–579. 10.1093/brain/121.4.561 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Schuurs A. H., Verheul H. A. (1990). Effects of gender and sex steroids on the immune response. J. Steroid Biochem. 35, 157–172. 10.1016/0022-4731(90)90270-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Shaywitz B. A., Shaywitz S. E., Pugh K. R., Constable R. T., Skudlarski P., Fulbright R. K., et al. (1995). Sex differences in the functional organization of the brain for language. Nature 373, 607–609. 10.1038/373607a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sheehan D. V., Lecrubier Y., Sheehan K. H., Amorim P., Janavs J., Weiller E., et al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59(Suppl. 20), 22–33, quiz 34-57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tebartz van Elst L., Maier S., Fangmeier T., Endres D., Mueller G. T., Nickel K., et al. (2014). Disturbed cingulate glutamate metabolism in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: evidence in support of the excitatory/inhibitory imbalance hypothesis. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 1314–1325. 10.1038/mp.2014.62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Tebartz van Elst L., Perlov E. (2013). Epilepsie und Psyche: Psychische Störungen bei Epilepsie - epileptische Phänomene in der Psychiatrie. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer. [Google Scholar]
  53. Tebartz van Elst L., Pick M., Biscaldi M., Fangmeier T., Riedel A. (2013). High-functioning autism spectrum disorder as a basic disorder in adult psychiatry and psychotherapy: psychopathological presentation, clinical relevance and therapeutic concepts. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 263(Suppl. 2), S189–S196. 10.1007/s00406-013-0459-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. van Elst L. T., Maier S., Fangmeier T., Endres D., Mueller G. T., Nickel K., et al. (2014). Magnetic resonance spectroscopy comparing adults with high functioning autism and above average IQ. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 1251. 10.1038/mp.2014.160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Zhong K., Chang L., Carasig D., Zimmerman L., Ernst T. (2002). Gender and age effects in human white matter on MR spectroscopy at 4 Tesla, in Poster 2492 (Honolulu, HI: ). [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES