Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Emerg Adulthood. 2016 Feb 22;4(6):403–416. doi: 10.1177/2167696816631098

A Prospective Study of Adolescents’ Sexual Partnerships on Emerging Adults’ Relationship Satisfaction and Intimate Partner Aggression

Monica A Longmore , Wendy D Manning , Jennifer E Copp *, Peggy C Giordano
PMCID: PMC5440085  NIHMSID: NIHMS795615  PMID: 28546885

Abstract

We examined whether the influence of adolescents’ sexual partnerships, both dating and casual, carried over to affect emerging adults’ relationship satisfaction and experiences of intimate partner aggression. Analyses of longitudinal data from the Toledo Adolescent Relationships Study (n = 294) showed that net of control variables (delinquency, depression, family violence, relational and sociodemographic characteristics), adolescents’ number of dating, but not casual, sexual partners led to greater odds of intimate partner aggression during emerging adulthood. Further, relationship churning (breaking-up and getting back together) and sexual non-exclusivity during emerging adulthood mediated the influence of adolescents’ number of dating sexual partnerships on intimate partner aggression. The positive effect of dating sexual partnerships on intimate partner aggression was stronger for women compared with men. These findings confirm the long reach of adolescent experiences into emerging adulthood.

Keywords: intimate partner violence, relationship satisfaction, adolescent casual sex, adolescent dating


Prior research has explored romantic and sexual experiences that occur during the adolescent period, and theory and research focusing on emerging adulthood has emphasized the importance of intimate relationships for this phase of the life course. Limited research, however, has examined how these experiences may be connected. Much of the early research on adolescent sexuality has emphasized the problematic aspects of sexual involvement (e.g., Jessor & Jessor, 1977), but research has shown that sexual experience becomes increasingly normative across this developmental period (e.g., Joyner & Udry, 2000). Thus, it is important to explore whether variability in the context of these experiences aids our understanding of the character of relationships formed later on in emerging adulthood.

In the current study we focused on the number of sexual relationships adolescents reported, and further assessed the relationship context within which sexual activity occurred. Thus, we distinguished adolescents’ reports of casual sexual relationships, and sex that occurred within dating contexts. Some scholars have suggested that casual sexual liaisons may be particularly problematic, while others have focused more attention on dating relationships and have emphasized that dating a variety of partners may be important to relationship skill building. We examined the association between number of dating and casual sexual partners during adolescence and two dimensions of young adult relationships—relationship satisfaction, and the presence of serious conflicts (intimate partner aggression). These two outcomes are especially critical because (a) maintaining satisfying intimate relationships is a key developmental task during emerging adulthood (Arnett, 2004); and (b) the life stage of emerging adulthood is associated with particularly high rates of intimate partner aggression (Halpern, Spriggs, Martin, & Kupper, 2009; Johnson, Giordano, Manning, & Longmore, 2015).

Drawing on prospective, longitudinal data from a community sample, the Toledo Adolescent Relationships Study (TARS) (n = 294), we first examined whether the number of casual and dating sexual partners was related systematically to these features of emerging adult relationships, and subsequently explored potential mediators. Although it is somewhat intuitive to expect that reporting a greater number of partners, particularly dating partners, could have a positive effect on later relationship dynamics, an alternative hypothesis is that this pattern may reflect instability and greater odds of what has been called “relationship churning,” as well as an increased likelihood of infidelity (overlapping relationships). In turn, multiple break ups with the same partner (Vennum, Lindstrom, Monk, & Adams, 2014) and infidelity (Blow & Harnett, 2007) are associated with lower relationship satisfaction, and both dynamics are known risk factors for intimate partner aggression (Giordano, Copp, Longmore, & Manning, 2015; Halpern-Meekin, Manning, Giordano, & Longmore, 2013a).

Background

Adolescents’ interest in and formation of romantic relationships is important for building intimacy skills (e.g., Fincham, 2012; Furman & Simon, 1999; Furman & Collibee, 2014; Hartup, 1986; Young, Furman, & Laursen, 2011). Conceptually, Laursen and Jensen-Campbell (1999) conveyed this idea of relationship skill building in the following manner: “Brief romantic encounters provide adolescents with opportunities to practice exchange rules and refine personal resources prior to initiating relationships that entail commitment and reproduction” (p. 64). Furman and Simon (1999) also argued that romantic relationships allow adolescents to develop important skills that help them to understand partners’ motivations and behaviors. Yet, rather than highlighting the continuity between adolescence and emerging adulthood, most studies of adolescent development have considered romantic relationships as an end-point of research. Similarly, studies of emerging adulthood often have focused on relationships as constituting a set of experiences that uniquely influence and structure this life course stage. These depictions can lead to the premature conclusion that the relationships that define the life stage of emerging adulthood are unaffected by previous relationships and experiences.

Reviews (e.g., Collins, Welsh, & Furman, 2009; Furman & Rose, 2014) of the limited body of literature that empirically examined connections between adolescents’ and emerging adults’ intimate relationships, generally, have emphasized that adolescents’ dating plays a positive role for subsequent social and emotional development. Meier and Allen (2009, p. 309) argued, for example, that adolescents’ intimate relationships provide “developmental currency” for young adult relationship formation. Similarly, Giordano, Manning, Longmore, and Flanigan (2012) suggested that teen dating may enhance young adults’ relationship quality as these earlier experiences provide opportunities to learn ‘how to do’ romance, and these experiences may be especially important for young men’s transitions to emerging adulthood.

Although some scholars have emphasized the importance of adolescents’ romantic relationships for skill-building, nevertheless, other researchers have reported associations between adolescents’ dating and delinquency (e.g., Cui et al., 2012), substance use (e.g., DiClemente, Santelli, & Crosby, 2009; Furman & Collibee, 2014), depressive symptoms (e.g., Joyner & Udry, 2000; Mendle, Ferrero, Moore, & Harden, 2013), unprotected sexual intercourse (e.g., Tu, Lou, Gao, Li, & Zabin, 2012), and relationship violence (e.g., Halpern et al., 2009). These early experiences may influence the formation and qualities of subsequent young adult relationships. Emphasizing the notion of ‘timing,’ Furman and Collibee (2014) examined longitudinal data from a community-based sample and found that having a romantic partner was associated with adolescent risk behaviors, such as delinquency. In contrast, romantic involvement during young adulthood was associated with positive adjustment suggesting that precocious dating can be problematic for some individuals.

Additionally, some researchers (e.g., Cui et al., 2012; Sabia & Rees, 2012) have expanded their focus beyond whether adolescents are dating and have examined the number of dating partners as an indicator of risk behavior that may have negative consequences for the next phase in the life course, emerging adulthood. Cui et al. (2012), analyzing the Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health), found that the cumulative number of romantic relationships from adolescence to emerging adulthood was associated with antisocial behavior even after controlling for prior delinquency and sociodemographic characteristics. Similarly, Madsen and Collins (2011) examining the Minnesota Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children reported that a greater number of dating partners in mid-adolescence led to poorer quality romantic relationships in emerging adulthood as evidenced by shared negative affect among partners and a lack of smooth relationship functioning.

Implied in the number of dating partners, is number of sexual partners because in contemporary American society dating most often provides the context for sexual activity (Manning et al., 2006; O’Sullivan, Cheng, Harris, & Brooks-Gunn, 2007)). Moreover, the majority of American adolescents have dated. Drawing on the 1994–1995 Add Health sample, Carver, Joyner and Udry (2003) reported that 69% of male and 76% of female adolescents had dated someone by age 18. Similarly, based on the 2011 Monitoring the Future data, 66% of high school seniors reported that they had ever dated (Child Trends, 2013). Examining the second wave of the Add Health, O’Sullivan (2007) found that the normative pattern among adolescents was to date first, which would then lead to sexual intimacy. Nevertheless, not all dating partners become sexual partners. Based on the TARS data, estimates of the average number of dating partners with whom teens have had sex was two, although the range was 0–13, with male adolescents, on average, reporting more partners (Manning, Longmore, Copp, & Giordano, 2014). Thus, from these prior studies we concluded the following: (1) about three-quarters of teens date; (2) not all daters are sexually active with partners; and (3) among daters who are sexually active, on average, they are sexually active with two dating partners.

It is also important to consider that adolescents’ and young adults’ sexual encounters may occur outside of dating relationships. Based on the Add Health, for example, researchers (e.g., Ford, Sohn, & Lepkowski, 2001; Manning et al., 2005) have estimated that about half of sexually active teens have had sex with individuals with whom they were not dating, with young men more often than women reporting casual sexual experiences. Drawing on the National Survey of Family Growth (e.g., Abma, Martinez, & Copen, 2010), as well as the TARS data (e.g., Manning et al., 2014), researchers have reported that over one-third of sexually active adolescents have had more than one casual sexual partner. Further, Manning et al. (2006) reported that the majority of sexually active adolescents were involved in dating as well as casual sexual partnerships. In a recent review, Garcia, Reiber, Massey, and Merriwether (2012) concluded that casual sexual hookups would likely continue to be an important relational context for teens and young adults. As such, a goal of the current study was to examine whether adolescents’ cumulative number of casual as well as dating sexual relationships led to lower relationship satisfaction and higher odds of intimate partner aggression during emerging adulthood, and whether these associations differed for men and women.

Although studies have compared the influence of relationship context (i.e., dating versus casual sex) for adolescents’ depressive symptoms (e.g., Mendle et al. 2013), few studies have compared the relationship context of adolescents’ sexual activity for young adult outcomes. One exception is Raley et al.’s (2007) study, based on the Add Health, in which they found that adolescent sexual experience in ‘romantic’ or dating relationships was associated with greater odds of marrying or cohabiting, whereas casual sexual behavior increased the odds of cohabiting only. Yet whether the number of relationships and the underlying mechanisms by which adolescents’ cumulative sexual partnerships influenced future relationships were not examined.

Regarding mechanisms, we concur with Hartup (1986) who stated that adolescent relationships “serve as important templates or models that can be used in the construction of future relationships… [Thus] consequences of earlier relationships can frequently be detected in later ones” (p. 2). Vasilenko, Lefkowitz, and Welsh (2014) also stated that adolescents’ sexual behaviors influence subsequent relationships by providing “models of interactions within relationships and a context for practicing intimate behaviors” (p. 11). Vasilenko et al. further argued that it is important to examine relational processes that connect teens’ sexual behaviors to future relationship outcomes. In this vein, Furman and Collibee (2014) compared a life course approach, emphasizing normative experiences associated with age-appropriate social roles, and developmental task theory, emphasizing “soft” developmental stages. They concluded that precocious romantic involvement (even if normative) undermines subsequent healthy development. Thus, Furman and Collibee provide a provocative explanation for the association between adolescent romantic relationships and young adult risk outcomes by emphasizing the timing of dating. Yet, they did not address whether variation in the relationship context (casual versus dating) and frequency of adolescents’ sexual relationships influenced emerging adults, and whether there are gender differences. We suggest that subsequent patterns of (a) sexual non-exclusivity and (b) relationship instability may link teens’ sexual relationships with young adults’ relationship outcomes including satisfaction and intimate partner aggression. Examining these two potential mediating pathways will contribute to theoretical and empirical understandings of adolescents’ relationship context and emerging adults’ outcomes.

Potential Mediators: Young Adult Patterns of Non-Exclusivity and Relationship Instability

Although most young adults are in intimate relationships, often uncertainties characterize these relationships. For example, substantial numbers of emerging adults are in relationships in which one or both partners have been sexually non-exclusive. Examining the Add Health, Joyner, Manning, and Bogle (2013) found that 35% of young adults in dating and 28% in cohabiting relationships either were not, or their partners were not, sexually exclusive. Relevant to the current study, based on a variety of datasets, researchers (e.g., Bogaert & Sadava, 2002; Feldman & Cauffman, 1999; Maddox Shaw, Rhoades, Allen, Stanley, & Markman, 2013; McAlister, Pachana, & Jackson, 2005; Treas & Giesen, 2000) have found that multiple sexual partners during adolescence increased the odds of sexual non-exclusivity in adult relationships. Moreover, men were more likely to report being sexually non-exclusive (Giordano et al., 2015; Maddox et al. 2013; Schondelmyer, 2014). In other words, the cumulative number of sexual partnerships during adolescence may affect subsequent relationship functioning by its association with higher odds of sexually non-exclusive relationships during emerging adulthood, and effects may differ by gender. Further, sexual non-exclusivity among young adults is associated with self-reports of lower relationship satisfaction (Blow & Harnett, 2007) and higher odds of intimate partner violence (Giordano, Copp, Longmore, & Manning, 2015). Thus, a potential linking mechanism between number of teens’ relationships and emerging adults’ outcomes is a pattern of sexual non-exclusivity.

Additionally, emerging adult relationships are often unstable as evidenced by multiple break-ups with the same partner. More than 40% of young adults in the TARS sample, for example, experienced relationship churning or cycling, both breaking up and getting back together with their current, most recent, or ex-partners (Halpern-Meekin et al., 2013a, 2013b), and multiple partners during adolescence is associated with churning in emerging adulthood (Manning et al., 2014). Churning reflects lower relationship satisfaction. Vennum et al. (2014), examining retrospective data from cohabiting and married individuals, found that the frequency of cyclical or churning persisted into cohabitation and marriage, and led to lower relationship satisfaction. Churning is also associated with increased odds of experiencing intimate partner aggression (Halpern-Meekin et al., 2013a). Moreover, Treas and Giesen (2000) argued that access to many ex-partners provides individuals with more sexual opportunities, which is indicative of relationship churning as well as providing greater opportunities to be sexually non-exclusive.

Current Investigation

Although some scholars have stressed the developmental importance of adolescent dating for learning intimacy skills, and the increasing role of casual sexual activity, some researchers have concluded that many sexual partners may have negative consequences. Additionally, the influence of adolescents’ number of casual sexual partners on emerging adults’ relationships is not clear. Our goal was to determine whether, and how, the influence of adolescents’ sexual experiences might carry over into emerging adulthood. Our initial research question asked whether the cumulative frequency of involvement in adolescent sexual relationships was associated with lower relationship satisfaction and higher odds of intimate partner aggression during emerging adulthood.

Additionally, we asked whether the type of adolescent sexual relationship, dating versus casual, was differentially associated with either relationship satisfaction and/or intimate partner aggression, and whether these effects differed by gender. It is important to examine how precursors such as adolescents’ cumulative number of sexual relationships may influence relationship satisfaction and getting along with intimate partners without resorting to aggression because these outcomes involve skill sets with lifelong implications.

We hypothesized that the cumulative number of dating as well as casual sexual partnerships during adolescence would affect young adults’ reports of relationship satisfaction and intimate partner aggression through greater involvement in relationships characterized by patterns of sexual non-exclusivity and instability (churning). We expected that these patterns would mediate the effect of adolescents’ number of sexual partnerships on emerging adults’ relationship functioning. Thus, this mediation model suggests that the cumulative number of adolescents’ sex partners is associated with relational patterns, which in turn predispose individuals toward lower satisfaction and increased odds of intimate partner aggression. Yet, it is unclear whether the effects of casual, compared with dating, sexual relationships on the outcome variables differ. Thus, we compared the influence of these two contexts for sexual activity on the outcome variables.

We also examined whether the effects of teens’ casual and dating sexual partnerships, and emerging adults’ sexual non-exclusivity and relationship churning, differed for women and men. We build on research demonstrating that young men are more sexually permissive (Sprecher, Treger, & Sakaluk, 2013) as evidenced by reporting higher numbers of dating sexual partners, being more likely to engage in casual sex, and reporting higher numbers of casual sexual partners. Additionally, during emerging adulthood, men reported higher odds of sexual non-exclusivity (Manning et al., 2005) and relationship churning (Halpern-Meekin et al., 2013a; 2013b). Higher numbers of partners, both dating and casual, greater odds of sexual non-exclusivity and relationship churning are more normative for men compared with women, and perhaps less stigmatizing for men. Yet it is unclear whether the effects of these relationship experiences on the outcome variables would differ for women compared with men so we tested these conditional effects.

We included in our multivariate models antecedent variables found in prior studies to influence both relationship satisfaction and intimate partner aggression. Involvement in antisocial activities during emerging adulthood and prior depressive symptoms are associated with lower quality relationships (Longmore et al., 2014) and increased odds of intimate partner aggression (Johnson et al., 2014). Witnessing parental violence and/or experiencing coercive parenting while growing up is associated with lower relationship satisfaction (Kaura & Lohman, 2007), and increased odds that young adults’ experienced violence in their intimate relationships (Ehrensaft et al., 2003; Herrenkohl et al., 2004). Union status (e.g., dating, cohabiting, married) influenced both relationship satisfaction (Kamp Dush & Amato, 2005; Zimmer-Gembeck, Arnhold, & Connolly, 2014) and intimate partner aggression (Brown & Bulanda, 2008; Cui, Gordon, Ueno, & Fincham, 2013). Longer duration may indicate higher satisfaction as unsatisfying relationships likely have ended (Zimmer-Gembeck et al., 2014); yet, longer duration is associated with increased odds of intimate partner violence (Kenney & McLanahan, 2006). Teen parenthood is associated negatively with adults’ relationship satisfaction (Levenson, Cartensen, & Gottman, 1993), and positively with intimate partner aggression (Leaman & Gee, 2008). Thus, we controlled for these variables in our models predicting emerging adults’ relationship satisfaction and higher odds of intimate partner aggression from adolescents’ sexual partnerships.

Method

Data

The Toledo Adolescent Relationships Study (TARS) is a longitudinal data set that focuses on dating and sexual relationships from adolescence to emerging adulthood. We focused on an age subgroup of the data set: respondents who were in 7th grade in 2001 and followed to the fifth interview in 2011/12 when they were, on average, age 23. We drew the initial sample from a stratified, random sample of adolescents in Lucas County, Ohio, in the year 2000, devised by the National Opinion Research Center, which included over-samples of Black and Hispanic adolescents. We also interviewed a parent or caregiver separately. The geographic area of Lucas County is similar to U.S. Census data (2010) on the national population with regard to race and ethnicity, family income, and education. Because we interviewed outside of the school setting, respondents did not need to attend classes to be in the original study. These data were well suited for our research because they included in-depth measures of both adolescent and emerging adult sexual and dating relationships along with key control indicators measured in early adolescence.

Our analysis relied on adolescents’ reports of their cumulative number of sexual and dating partners at the fourth interview (2007–2008) when they were ages 18–19. The emerging adult experiences were from the fifth interview (2011–2012) when respondents were between ages 22 and 25 (on average 23). The broader age range for emerging adults was due to a time lapse between the fourth and fifth interviews of four to five years depending on the exact date of interview. Our analytic sample consisted of 294 respondents who provided information on a current or most recent relationship at the time of the fifth interview with 132 men and 162 women. The prospective, panel design permitted an assessment of intimate experiences based on self-reports at each interview rather than relying on a single retrospective report. Additionally, we included family and background measures asked in early adolescence. Thus, an asset of this study is that longitudinal, compared with cross-sectional, data minimizes retrospective bias.

Measures

Relationship satisfaction (Rust et al., 1986), assessed during emerging adulthood, average age 23 (fifth interview), included likert responses to the following nine items: (1) “I really appreciate his/her sense of humor”; (2) “He/she doesn’t seem to listen to me” (reverse coded); (3) “If he/she left me, life would not be worth living”; (4) “We both seem to like the same things”; (5) “I often have second thoughts about our relationship” (reverse coded); (6) “I enjoy just sitting and talking with him/her”; (7) “We become competitive when we have to make decisions” (reverse coded); (8)“I wish there was more warmth and affection between us” (reverse coded); and (9) “He/she is always correcting me” (reverse coded) (α = .76). Responses were (5) strongly agree to (1) strongly disagree.

Intimate partner aggression, measured during emerging adulthood, average age 23 (fifth interview), included responses to twelve items from the Revised Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS2) (Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1996). These included how often the respondent had done the following: (1) “thrown something at”; (2) “twisted arm or hair”; (3) “used a knife or gun”; (4) “punched or hit with something that could hurt”; (5) “choked”; (6) “slammed against a wall”; (7) “beat up”; (8) “burned or scalded on purpose”; (9) “kicked”; (10) “pushed, shoved, or grabbed”; (11) “slapped in the face or head with an open hand”; and (12) “hit” in reference to experiences with the current/most recent partner (α = .94). An identical set of questions assessed the respondent’s victimization experiences. Responses ranged from (1) never to (5) very often. We created a composite score by adding the number of different acts experienced, including both perpetration and victimization. Responses ranged from 0 to 20. To correct for skewness, we logged the score in multivariate analyses.

Number of sex partners. We measured the number of casual and dating sexual partnerships during late adolescence, ages 18–19 (fourth interview). The initial prompt stated: “When we refer to sex in the next questions, we mean vaginal sex. In your lifetime, how many sex partners have you had?” Number of casual sex partners referred to the question: “How many different people of the opposite sex have you had vaginal sex with that you weren’t really dating or going out with?” Number of dating sex partners was the difference between the total number of sex partners and the number of casual sex partners.

Relationship churning was a three category variable (fifth interview). Following Halpern-Meekin et al. (2013a; 2013b), we characterized respondents as churning if they broke up and got back together with their current or most recent partner or had sex with their ex-dating partner. The second category, stably together, included respondents in a current relationship who never broke up with this partner. The third category, stably apart, included respondents who reported on a prior relationship in which they only broke up once and did not get back together.

Sexual non-exclusivity referred to respondents’ self-reports of their own and/or their partners’ non-exclusivity and/or involvement in sexually non-exclusive relationships measured in emerging adulthood (fifth interview). The second category, sexually exclusive, indicated that neither partner had sex with someone else during the relationship.

Antisocial behavior was the mean of a ten-item self-reported scale (Elliott & Ageton, 1980) measured at the time of mid-adolescence (ages 15–16 at the third interview). It included the following items: (1) “drunk alcohol”; (2) “stolen (or tried to steal) things worth $5 or less”; (3) “stolen something worth more than $50”; (4) “carried a hidden weapon other than a plain pocket knife”; (5) “damaged or destroyed property on purpose”; (6) “attacked someone with the idea of seriously hurting him/her”; (7) “sold drugs”; (8) “been drunk in a public place”; (9) “broken into a building or vehicle”; and (10) “used drugs to get high” (α = .91). Responses ranged from (1) never to (9) more than once a day.

Depressive symptoms, measured in mid-adolescence (ages 15–16 at the third interview), were from the six-item version of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depressive Symptoms scale (CES-D) (Radloff, 1977). We asked how often each symptom occurred in the past seven days: (1) “could not get going”; (2) “could not shake off the blues”; (3) “had trouble keeping your mind on what you were doing”; (4) “felt lonely”; (5) “felt sad”; and (6) “had trouble getting to sleep or staying asleep” (α = .77). Responses ranged from (1) never to (7) six days a week.

Prior family violence. We included two measures of prior family violence. Witnessing parental violence was the respondent’s retrospective response, at the fifth interview, in which we asked, “How often did either one of your parents:” “throw something at the other”; “push, shove, or grab the other”; “slap the other in the face or head with an open hand”; and “hit the other” (α = .92). Due to the skewed nature of the responses, we dichotomized scores (1 = yes) to indicate any parental violence. Coercive parenting, measured in early adolescence (ages 12–13) asked the following: “When you and your parents disagree about things, how often do they push, slap, or hit you?” Responses ranged from 1 (never) to 6 (two or more times a week). Due to skewness, we dichotomized this variable to indicate any reports of coercive parenting (1 = yes).

Relationship indicators. We measured the relationship indicators at the time of the fifth interview. Union status, based on relationship histories, included dating (comparison group), cohabiting, and married. Current relationship indicated that respondents reported on their current versus most recent relationship. Relationship duration, measured in years, ranged from .08 (about a month) to 10 years. Teen parent referred to whether the respondent had children by late adolescence (18–19 years old at the fourth interview).

Sociodemographic indicators. We assessed the sociodemographic indicators at the time of the first interview in early adolescence (ages 12–13). Gender, a dichotomous variable, indicated whether the respondent was female. Age was the difference between date of birth and the fourth interview date. Race/ethnicity consisted of three self-reported categories: White (comparison group), Black, and Hispanic. Family structure during adolescence, from the respondent’s first interview asked, “During the past 12 months, who were you living with most of the time?” Respondents selected one of 25 categories, which we collapsed into four categories: two biological parents (comparison group), single parent, stepparents, or “other family” including living with other family members or foster care. Mother’s education, a proxy for social class background, was from the parent interview, and response categories included less than high school, high school graduate (comparison group), some college, or college or more.

Analytic Strategy

Table 1 presented a correlation matrix, including means/percentages for all variables in the multivariate analysis. We used these data to provide a descriptive portrait of the sample. We used ordinary least squares to model multivariate associations with relationship satisfaction and frequency of any intimate partner aggression, respectively. We showed results for relationship satisfaction in Table 2, and intimate partner aggression in Table 3. In the multivariate analyses, Model 1 included the association between number of casual sex partners and the dependent variable controlling for the known correlates including antisocial behavior, depression, family violence, relationship characteristics, and demographic background. In Model 2, we substituted number of dating for number of casual sex partners. Model 3 added the mediating variables, relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity, to Model 1. Likewise, Model 4 added relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity to Model 2. We performed nested F tests to examine model fit following the inclusion of additional parameters. We examined cross-product terms of adolescent dating sexual partnerships and gender in a model and casual sexual partnerships and dating in a separate model. Further, we interacted churning and gender as well as gender and sexual non-exclusivity in separate models. We presented significant interactions in Table 4.

Table 1.

Correlation Matrix for Study Variables with Means/Percentages for the Full Sample (n = 294)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1 Relationship
satisfaction
1.00
2 Intimate partner
aggression
−0.29*** 1.00
3 Number of
casual sexual
partners
−0.08 0.16** 1.00
4 Number of
dating sexual
partners
−0.15* 0.23*** 0.51*** 1.00
5 Relationship
churning
−0.31*** 0.29*** 0.06 0.17** 1.00
6 Stably broken
up
−0.09 −0.10 −0.04 −0.12* −0.34*** 1.00
7 Sexual non-
exclusivity
−0.37*** 0.29*** 0.18** 0.22*** 0.28*** −0.02 1.00
8 Delinquency −0.07 0.16** 0.33*** 0.31*** 0.05 −0.03 0.06 1.00
9 Depressive
symptoms
−0.19** 0.14* 0.12* 0.05 0.04 0.09 0.09 0.20*** 1.00
10 Witnessing
parental
violence
−0.15** 0.30*** 0.10 0.14* 0.07 0.00 0.12* 0.00 0.08 1.00
11 Coercive
parenting
0.01 0.13* 0.15* 0.08 0.10 −0.06 0.02 0.07 0.05 0.13* 1.00
12 Cohabiting −0.02 0.11 0.08 0.06 0.06 −0.19** −0.06 0.11 0.07 0.01 0.04 1.00
13 Married 0.01 0.05 0.05 0.06 0.02 −0.16** −0.03 0.01 −0.18** 0.04 −0.06 −0.24*** 1.00
14 Current
relationship
0.21*** −0.03 0.07 0.06 −0.03 −0.71*** −0.11 0.07 −0.09 0.00 0.04 0.23*** 0.20*** 1.00
15 Duration −0.13* 0.08 0.05 0.05 0.22*** −0.19** 0.05 0.04 −0.08 0.07 −0.07 0.15* 0.36*** 0.17**
16 Teen parent −0.17** 0.10 0.05 0.19** 0.22*** −0.09 0.11 0.05 0.09 −0.01 −0.02 −0.03 0.15** 0.04
17 Gender 0.07 0.10 −0.14* −0.04 0.06 −0.15 −0.01 −0.18** 0.11 0.09 0.06 −0.01 0.07 0.15
18 Age −0.05 0.04 0.18** 0.21*** 0.04 0.06 0.11 0.11 −0.06 0.09 −0.06 0.03 0.05 −0.06
19 Black −0.16** 0.10 0.16** 0.12* 0.16** −0.00 0.27*** −0.12* −0.02 0.12* 0.09 −0.07 −0.08 0.01
20 Hispanic −0.01 0.13* 0.06 0.05 0.17** 0.01 0.03 0.11 −0.06 0.09 0.08 0.03 0.01 −0.03
21 Other 0.03 −0.01 −0.02 −0.00 −0.00 −0.01 0.08 −0.07 0.12* 0.02 0.01 0.12* −0.06 0.05
22 Single parent −0.05 0.10 0.22*** 0.11 0.15** −0.01 0.07 0.02 −0.01 0.29** 0.09 0.07 0.01 0.04
23 Step-parent −0.09 −0.00 0.06 0.10 0.06 −0.08 0.04 0.14* 0.08 0.02 0.10 0.01 0.05 0.07
24 Other 0.00 0.20*** 0.06 0.13* −0.02 −0.06 0.09 0.02 0.00 0.05 0.08 0.02 −0.00 0.04
25 Less than high
school
−0.05 0.09 −0.01 0.17** 0.03 −0.05 0.07 −0.03 0.00 0.08 0.02 0.10 0.05 0.06
26 Some college −0.03 −0.01 0.03 0.03 0.04 −0.08 0.08 0.04 0.11 −0.01 −0.01 −0.03 0.00 0.08
27 College or
more
0.17** −0.14* −0.12* −0.18** −0.17** 0.08 −0.17** −0.07 −0.08 −0.12* −0.05 −0.13* −0.03 −0.10

Means/Percentages 3.48 1.84 1.43 2.04 38.78% 15.31% 28.91% 0.39 2.40 30.95% 23.47% 28.91% 12.24% 73.47%
15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27
1 Relationship
satisfaction
2 Intimate partner
aggression
3 Number of
casual sexual
partners
4 Number of
dating sexual
partners
5 Relationship
churning
6 Stably broken
up
7 Sexual non-
exclusivity
8 Delinquency
9 Depressive
symptoms
10 Witnessing
parental
violence
11 Coercive
parenting
12 Cohabiting
13 Married
14 Current
relationship
15 Duration 1.00
16 Teen parent 0.24*** 1.00
17 Gender 0.03 0.14** 1.00
18 Age 0.04 0.15** −0.09 1.00
19 Black −0.02 0.20*** 0.01 0.12* 1.00
20 Hispanic 0.07 0.06 0.01 0.06 −0.18** 1.00
21 Other −0.01 0.03 0.11 0.02 −0.09 −0.06 1.00
22 Single parent 0.09 0.19** −0.02 0.12* 0.18** −0.03 0.06 1.00
23 Step-parent 0.07 0.03 0.07 −0.03 −0.09 0.17** −0.00 −0.22*** 1.00
24 Other −0.00 0.06 0.15* 0.06 0.21*** −0.07 −0.07 −0.22 −0.16** 1.00
25 Less than high
school
0.02 0.19** 0.01 0.11 0.15* 0.15** −0.05 0.02 0.04 0.14* 1.00
26 Some college −0.01 0.01 0.01 −0.02 0.07 −0.09 −0.04 0.02 0.08 0.07 −0.22*** 1.00
27 College or
more
−0.10 −0.16** −0.10 −0.18** −0.16** −0.13* 0.01 −0.13* −0.02 −0.15* −0.16** −0.41*** 1.00

Means/Percentages 2.40 8.16% 55.10% 23.39 22.45% 10.20% 2.72% 23.47% 13.27% 13.95% 7.82% 36.05% 22.79%
*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

Table 2.

Coefficients for the OLS Regression of Adolescent Sex Experience and Young Adult Relationship Patterns on Relationship Satisfaction in Emerging Adulthood (n = 294)ab

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

Variable B SE B β B SE B β B SE B β B SE B β
Adolescent Sex Experience (interview 4)
  Number of casual sexual partners (lifetime) −0.002 0.02 −0.010 0.003 0.01 0.011
  Number of dating sexual partners (lifetime) −0.021 0.02 −0.083 −0.006 0.02 −0.025
Young Adult Relationship Patterns (interview 5)
  Relationship churning −0.271 0.08 −0.222** −0.270 0.08 −0.221**
    Stably broken up −0.168 0.14 −0.102 −0.173 0.14 −0.104
    (Stably together)
  Sexual non-exclusivity −0.334 0.08 −0.254*** −0.328 0.08 −0.250***
    (Sexually exclusive)
R2 .19 .19 .29 .29
F for change in R2 12.63*** 12.63***
a

Models include controls for antisocial behavior, prior depression, family violence, relationship characteristics, and sociodemographic characteristics

b

F statistic for change in R2 compares Model 1 to Model 3 and Model 2 to Model 4

p < .10;

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

Table 3.

Coefficients for the OLS Regression of Adolescent Sex Experience and Young Adult Relationship Patterns on Intimate Partner Aggression in Emerging Adulthood (n = 294)ab

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

Variable B SE B β B SE B β B SE B β B SE B β
Adolescent Sex Experience (interview 4)
  Number of casual sexual partners (lifetime) 0.018 0.02 0.047 0.011 0.02 0.031
  Number of dating sexual partners (lifetime) 0.047 0.02 0.121* 0.025 0.02 0.065
Young Adult Relationship Patterns (interview 5)
  Relationship churning 0.306 0.12 0.167* 0.296 0.12 0.161*
    Stably broken up −0.197 0.22 −0.079 −0.186 0.22 −0.075
    (Stably together)
  Sexual non-exclusivity 0.369 0.11 0.187** 0.359 0.11 0.182**
    (Sexually exclusive)
R2 .21 .22 .29 .30
F for change in R2 10.10*** 10.25***
a

Models include controls for antisocial behavior, prior depression, family violence, relationship characteristics, and sociodemographic characteristics

b

F statistic for change in R2 compares Model 1 to Model 3 and Model 2 to Model 4

p < .10;

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

Table 4.

Coefficients for the OLS Regression of Adolescent Sex Experience and Young Adult Relationship Patterns on Intimate Partner Aggression in Emerging Adulthood: Tests for the Moderating Effect of Gender (n = 294)a

Model 1 Model 2

Variable B SE B β B SE B β
Adolescent Sex Experience (interview 4)
  Number of casual sexual partners (lifetime) −0.028 0.03 −0.074 0.011 0.02 0.028
  Number of dating sexual partners (lifetime) 0.028 0.03 0.072 −0.030 0.03 −0.078
Young Adult Relationship Patterns (interview 5)
  Relationship churning 0.298 0.12 0.162* 0.266 0.12 0.145*
    Stably broken up −0.173 0.22 −0.070 −0.192 0.21 −0.078
    (Stably together)
  Sexual non-exclusivity 0.339 0.11 0.172** 0.320 0.11 0.162**
    (Sexually exclusive)
Number of casual sexual partners × gender 0.071 0.04 0.124
Number of dating sexual partners × gender 0.107 0.04 0.223*
R2 .30 .31
a

Models include controls for antisocial behavior, prior depression, family violence, relationship characteristics, and sociodemographic characteristics

p < .10;

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

Results

Among emerging adults, the average level of relationship satisfaction on the Rust et al. (1986) scale was 3.48 (range = 1–5) with a standard deviation of 0.60, indicating moderate levels of satisfaction across the sample as a whole (Table 1). Relationship satisfaction was not significantly different for men and women (not shown). About 25% of the sample reported any intimate partner aggression, which is consistent with estimates among young adults employing other data sets, such as the Add Health (e.g., Halpern et al., 2009). The percentages of men and women who reported any intimate partner aggression were not significantly different, although women reported slightly more frequent aggression (not shown). Thus, emerging adults on average reported moderate relationship satisfaction, but about one-fourth also reported relationship violence.

Regarding lifetime number of sexual partnerships, when respondents were ages 18–19, the average number of casual and dating sexual partners was 1.43 and 2.04 respectively. We note this is limited to vaginal sexual intercourse, and that the levels likely would be higher with a more expansive definition of sexual activity. Although male and female teens did not report a significant difference in number of dating sexual partners, male compared with female teens reported on average more casual sex partners (1.88 versus 1.17) (not shown).

Nearly two-fifths (39%) of emerging adults reported relationship churning, and a similar percentage of female (41%) compared with male (36%) respondents reported breaking up and getting back with their partners. A higher percentage of men (22%) compared with women (10%) reported being in stably broken up relationships, and similar percentages of men (43%) and women (48%) reported being in stable relationships (not shown). Nearly one-third, 29%, of emerging adults reported that they were in sexually non-exclusive relationships; the majority, 71%, reported that their current or most recent relationship was sexually exclusive. The percentage of men and women who reported being sexually exclusive was not significantly different (not shown).

Relationship Satisfaction

In Table 1 (correlation matrix) the cumulative number of dating, but not casual, sexual partners was associated with lower levels of relationship satisfaction during emerging adulthood. The bivariate correlation between number of casual sexual and number of dating sexual partners was .51. Consistent with expectations, relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity were associated with lower relationship satisfaction at the bivariate level.

In Table 2, model 1, we examined the cumulative frequency of adolescents’ casual sex partnerships on emerging adults’ relationship satisfaction controlling for antisocial behavior, depression, prior family violence, relationship characteristics, and sociodemographic characteristics. Similar to the bivariate results, teens’ number of casual sexual partners was not a significant influence on subsequent relationship satisfaction with the inclusion of the other correlates. In model 2 the frequency of dating sexual partners during adolescence was not associated with relationship satisfaction net of the control variables. Regarding our initial research questions, with the inclusion of the control variables, neither casual nor dating sexual relationships influenced subsequent relationship satisfaction during emerging adulthood.

In models 3 and 4 we added relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity. Relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity were associated with lower satisfaction. In models 3 and 4, as in models 1 and 2, numbers of casual and dating sexual partners during adolescence were not associated with relationship satisfaction in emerging adulthood, controlling for other factors. Thus, regarding the second research question, we found only partial support for mediation. Whereas many approaches to testing mediation hypotheses do not simultaneously examine multiple mediators or account for the effects of other covariates, an advantage to our approach is that we estimated the path coefficients in a multiple mediator model adjusting all paths for the potential influence of study covariates not proposed to be mediators in the models (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The association between frequency of casual sexual experience in adolescence was not associated with later relationship satisfaction at the bivariate level, and formal tests of mediation confirmed that relationship patterns did not mediate the effect of the number of casual sexual partners on relationship satisfaction. The effect of dating sexual experience, however, was partially mediated by churning (z = −1.82, p < .05) and sexual non-exclusivity (z = −2.36, p < .01). Furthermore, the observed relationship patterns (i.e., churning and sexual non-exclusivity) were associated with lower relationship satisfaction in emerging adulthood.

In supplemental analyses (not shown), we tested a series of interactions in separate models. We found that teens’ cumulative number of dating and casual sexual partners, and emerging adults’ patterns of relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity were related similarly to relationship satisfaction for men and women. Thus, with regard to our research questions about gender, the effects of dating and casual sexual partnerships, relationship churning, and sexual non-exclusivity on relationship satisfaction did not differ significantly between young men and women. With respect to some concerns that casual sex is especially detrimental for women, we did not find evidence to suggest that greater frequency of casual sexual experiences during adolescence had more adverse consequences for young women’s compared with men’s relationship satisfaction.

Intimate Partner Aggression

The bivariate information presented in Table 1 indicated that the cumulative numbers of adolescent casual and dating sexual relationships were each associated with higher odds of experiencing intimate partner aggression in emerging adulthood, and the effect was stronger for dating sexual partners. At the bivariate level, relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity were associated with greater odds of intimate partner aggression.

In Table 3 we presented multivariate results for the association between adolescent sexual partnerships, young adult relationship patterns, and any intimate partner aggression. In model 1 we examined the influence of adolescents’ casual sexual experience on the odds of intimate partner aggression. Net of the control variables, number of casual sexual experiences during adolescence was not a significant influence on the odds of intimate partner aggression during emerging adulthood. Results in model 2, in contrast, showed that number of dating sexual partners increased the odds of intimate partner aggression. Thus, with regard to our initial research questions, only number of dating sexual partners increased the odds of intimate partner aggression.

The next two models in Table 3 included the patterns of churning and sexual non-exclusivity. In model 3, the cumulative frequency of casual sexual partners during adolescence was not associated with the odds of experiencing intimate partner aggression during emerging adulthood with the inclusion of relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity, both of which were significantly associated with intimate partner aggression. Model 4 showed that with the inclusion of relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity, number of dating sexual partners during adolescence was no longer significantly associated with intimate partner aggression. Thus, the patterns of relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity developed in emerging adulthood explained some of the association between adolescent dating sexual experiences and intimate partner aggression.

Similar to the procedure outlined above, we estimated a multiple mediator model accounting for other key covariates. After adjusting all paths for the influence of the control variables, the direct path between casual sex partners and intimate partner aggression was attenuated. Net of controls, the number of dating sex partners continued to exert a positive influence on the odds of intimate partner aggression. Supplemental analyses revealed that patterns of relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity in emerging adulthood mediated the link between dating sexual partners and intimate partner aggression (specific indirect effects = 1.818 and 2.188 (p < .05), respectively).

In Table 4 we presented the significant interactions by gender. Consistent with the models predicting relationship satisfaction, the influence of relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity on intimate partner aggression was similar for men and women. Gender moderated the associations between adolescent sexual experiences and intimate partner aggression. Specifically, results in Model 1 indicated that the influence of number of casual sexual partners on the odds of intimate partner aggression was stronger for women. Further analyses indicated that although the association between casual sexual partners and the odds of intimate partner aggression is significantly different for men and women, the number of casual sexual partners is not associated with intimate partner aggression for either men or women. A similar pattern emerged for the cross-product of number of adolescent dating sexual partners with gender. That is, controlling for other factors, the association between number of adolescent dating sexual partners and intimate partner aggression was stronger for women. The number of dating partners was not associated with intimate partner aggression for men (b = −0.03, p > .10), but among women, the number of dating sexual partners was positively (b = 0.08, p < .05) associated with intimate partner aggression (results not shown). Thus, the number of adolescent sexual relationships was more strongly associated with physical aggression in emerging adulthood for women than men.

Discussion

We began this paper by reviewing prior research on adolescents’ sexual partnerships and their potential influences on emerging adult outcomes. The longitudinal framework of the current study enabled us to examine specific consequences of variations in the nature of these earlier sexual experiences for later satisfaction and involvement with relationships characterized by intimate partner aggression. One way that our work moved beyond prior studies was by distinguishing adolescents’ sexual experiences into those that occurred in dating and casual relationships. This distinction is important because young men compared with women report more casual sexual partnerships (Manning et al., 2014), and based on prior literature it is unclear whether casual sexual partnerships have greater negative implications for women (e.g., Fielder et al., 2014) or comparable effects for young men and women (e.g., Lyons et al., 2013). Additionally, there is a tendency to view dating sexual partners somewhat more positively as these experiences are thought to be associated with relationship skill-building (e.g., Raley et al., 2007) and model adult relationship progression. Initially, at the bivariate level, adolescent sexual experiences (i.e., number of casual sexual partners, number of dating sexual partners) were associated with lower levels of relationship satisfaction and higher odds of any intimate partner aggression during emerging adulthood. We found, however, that in the multivariate models that included other known correlates, the number of dating sexual partners, but not the number of casual sexual partners, was associated with increased odds of intimate partner aggression. Thus, it may be starting and ending dating sexual relationships that shape future relational trajectories rather than the traditionally, negatively, viewed casual sexual relationships.

We also considered the influence of emerging adults’ relational patterns as a mediating link between cumulative numbers of casual and dating sexual partnerships during adolescence and relationship functioning during emerging adulthood. Recognizing the complex dynamics of intimate relationships, these relational pathways included sexual non-exclusivity and relationship churning. Our findings contributed to the literature by emphasizing that although the cumulative frequency of dating sexual partnerships were associated with increased likelihood of experiencing physical violence in models that just included the control variables, the frequency of dating sexual partnerships was not significant in models that included sexual non-exclusivity and relationship churning. Specifically, the cumulative number of dating sexual partners influenced emerging adult relationship outcomes through relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity. The relationship context of sexual activity did not directly matter, but rather influenced relational patterns in emerging adulthood. Experiences with a greater number of dating sexual partners may be a form of “relationship baggage” that manifests itself in patterns of sexual non-exclusivity and relationship churning during emerging adulthood. This also suggests, however, that in those cases where these early sexual experiences do not connect to the churning pattern or later sexual non-exclusivity, it does not appear that such experiences inevitably foster detrimental adult relationship dynamics (i.e., less satisfying relationships or heightened risk for intimate partner aggression).

Based on prior work indicating that men are more sexually permissive, we examined whether adolescent sexual relationships would influence emerging adult relationship outcomes in different ways for men and women. It appears that the cumulative number of adolescent sexual partners (dating or casual) was similarly associated with relationship satisfaction for men and women. In contrast, adolescent sexual partnerships were associated more strongly with intimate partner aggression among women than men. Furthermore, among women it was the number of dating, and not casual, sexual partnerships that was associated with greater odds of intimate partner aggression, which is consistent with Klipfel et al.’s (2013) finding that nearly twice as many of their respondents reported some form of intimate partner aggression in dating compared with casual sexual relationships. Based on these findings we conclude that rather than being associated with relationship skill building, the cumulative number of dating sexual relationships can be associated with negative outcomes during emerging adulthood. In a set of three studies examining growth following the most stressful event they had encountered in the past year, the dissolution of a romantic relationship was the most common traumatic event cited by the sample (Park, Cohen, & Murch, 1996). Our conclusion is also consistent with Furman and Collibee’s view that precocious dating (or in our case a greater frequency of dating sexual relationships), although normative may not be developmentally appropriate; as such, there can be negative implications.

Although our work moves beyond prior studies, there are a few limitations. First, the data are drawn from one regional area (Lucas County, Ohio), although basic comparisons indicate that the large metropolitan area we focused on is similar to the U.S. as a whole on several basic demographic characteristics, including estimates of race and ethnicity, family status, income, and education. These results should be replicated with a nationally representative data source. Second, our analyses do not provide a comprehensive explanation for why the cumulative number of adolescents’ dating sexual relationships affects emerging adults’ relationships. That is, frequency of involvement captures little of the quality of dating relationships. For example, we did not examine whether violence occurred in these prior relationships. Moreover, there may be other mediating variables not examined and that would alter our conclusions about which variables most strongly mediate adolescents’ sexual activity. We also did not consider potential reciprocal relationships between the two mediators. Third, our indicator of casual sexual activity included prior sexual partners and friends; it is unclear whether effects would differ if we focused exclusively on casual sex with strangers. Bersamin et al. (2014), for example, found that sexual activity with individuals known less than a week had negative implications for emerging adults’ mental health. Fourth, researchers have begun to pay attention to the possibility that some observed differences in adolescents’ sexual activity reflect pre-existing differences (e.g., Mendle et al. 2013). Fifth, although we did include depressive symptoms (e.g., Sandberg-Thoma & Kamp Dush, 2014), we did not examine the extent to which a wider range of prior psychological characteristics might explain why teens, sexually active with dating partners, might fare worse in emerging adult relationships. Sixth, we focused on vaginal sexual intercourse, and to be sure, there are other sexual experiences that are relevant (anal and oral sex), especially with regard to casual sexual behavior as well as sexual non-exclusivity. A related issue is that the study was limited to heterosexual relationships. We expect a similar pattern of findings among same-sex couples, but do not have sufficient sample sizes to empirically examine this issue. Finally, we created our explanatory model within a specific social and historical context; as such, the precise risks associated with dating sexual relationships perhaps would differ at a different time.

Despite these limitations to our conclusions, the results of our analyses have implications for the broader theoretical literature on the implications of adolescents’ sexual behavior for emerging adults’ relationship satisfaction and use of physical aggression in intimate relationships. The current findings are potentially useful as they suggest that some conventional wisdom about sexual and dating experiences during adolescence may not provide an accurate portrait of some consequences of these experiences for emerging adulthood. Moreover, the high levels of relationship churning and sexual non-exclusivity among emerging adults require theories and approaches that acknowledge the links between adolescents’ and emerging adults’ experiences. Our findings suggest that prevention programs directed to address issues of teen sexuality and safe sex practices will need to confront that although dating experiences may be associated with positive meanings (e.g., Giordano et al., 2006), they are also associated with heightened risk later in the life course including intimate partner aggression. The longer window of assessment provided by longitudinal data showed that effects of casual sex were not consistent with an ominous portrait of negative sequelae of casual sexual experience. Yet, some associations (the link between number of sexual partners and any intimate partner aggression with partner) provide a more complex picture, highlighting the need for additional research that captures qualitative differences in the character and impact of these relationship experiences.

Acknowledgments

This research received support from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (HD036223, HD044206, and HD066087), the Department of Health and Human Services (5APRPA006009), the National Institute of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, U. S. Department of Justice (Award Nos. 2009-IJ-CX-0503 and 2010-MU-MU-0031), and the Center for Family and Demographic Research, Bowling Green State University, which has core funding from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R24HD050959). The opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the official views of the Department of Justice or National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

  1. Abma J, Martinez G, Copen C. Teenagers in the United States: Sexual activity, contraceptive use, and childbearing, National Survey of Family Growth 2006–2008. Vital Health Statistics. 2010;23(30):1–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bersamin M, Zamboanga B, Schwartz S, Donnellan M, Hudson M, Weisskirch R, Kim S, Agocha V, Whitbourne S, Caraway S. Risky business: Is there an association between casual sex and mental health among young emerging adults? Journal of Sex Research. 2014;51:43–51. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2013.772088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blow A, Harnett K. Infidelity in committed relationships II: A substantive review. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy. 2007;31:217–233. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.2005.tb01556.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bogaert A, Sadava S. Adult attachment and sexual behavior. Personal Relationships. 2002;9(2):191–204. [Google Scholar]
  6. Booth A, Crouter A. The new population problem: Why families in developed countries are shrinking and what it means. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown S, Bulanda J. Relationship violence in early adulthood: a comparison of daters, cohabitors, and marrieds. Social Science Research. 2008;37:73–87. [Google Scholar]
  8. Carver K, Joyner K, Udry R. National Estimates of Adolescent Romantic Relationships. In: Florsheim P, editor. Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory, research, and practical implications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 23–56. [Google Scholar]
  9. Child Trends. Dating: Indicators on children and youth. 2013 Retrieved from http://www.childtrends.org/wp-content/uploads/2012/05/73_Dating.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  10. Collins WA, Welsh DP, Furman W. Adolescent romantic relationships. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:631–652. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cui M, Gordon M, Ueno K, Fincham F. The continuation of intimate partner violence from adolescence to emerging adulthood. Journal of Marriage & Family. 2013;75:300–313. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cui M, Ueno K, Fincham F, Donnellan M, Wickrama K. The association between romantic relationships and delinquency in adolescence and emerging adulthood. Personal Relationships. 2012;19:354–366. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01366.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. DiClemente RJ, Santelli SJ, Crosby RA. Adolescent health: Understanding and preventing risk behaviors. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ehrensaft M, Cohen P, Brown J, Smailes E, Chen H, Johnson J. Intergenerational transmission of partner violence: A 20-year prospective study. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 2003;71:741–753. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.71.4.741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Elliott D, Ageton S. Reconciling race and class differences in self-reported and official estimates of delinquency. American Sociological Review. 1980;45(1):95–110. [Google Scholar]
  16. Fergus S, Zimmerman M. Adolescent resilience: A framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annual Review of Public Health. 2005;26:399–419. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Feldman S, Cauffman E. Your cheatin’ heart: Attitudes, behaviors, and correlates of sexual betrayal in late adolescents. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 1999;9(3):227–252. [Google Scholar]
  18. Fielder R, Carey M. Predictors and consequences of sexual “hook-ups” among college students: A short-term prospective study. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2010;39(5):1105–1119. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9448-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fielder R, Walsh J, Carey K, Carey M. Sexual hookups and adverse health outcomes: A longitudinal study of first-year college women. Journal of Sex Research. 2014;1:131–144. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2013.848255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fincham FD. Challenges in charting the course of romantic relationships in adolescence and emerging adulthood. In: Booth A, editor. Early adulthood in a family context. Myrtle Beach, SC: Springer Press; 2012. pp. 165–172. [Google Scholar]
  21. Finer LB, Philbin JM. Sexual initiation, contraceptive use, and pregnancy among young adolescents. Pediatrics. 2013;131(5):886–891. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-3495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ford K, Sohn W, Lepkowski J. Characteristics of adolescents’ sexual partners and their association with use of condoms and other contraceptive methods. Family Planning Perspectives. 2001;33(3):100–105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Furman W, Rose A. Friendships, romantic relationships, and other dyadic peer relationships in childhood and adolescence: A unified relational perspective. In: Lerner R, editor. The handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th Edition), Vol. 3: Social and emotional development. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  24. Furman W, Simon V. Cognitive representations of romantic relationships. In: Furman W, Brown B, Feiring C, editors. The development of romantic relationships in adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1999. pp. 75–98. [Google Scholar]
  25. Garcia J, Reiber C, Massey S, Merriwether A. Sexual hookup culture: A review. Review of General Psychology. 2012;16:161–176. doi: 10.1037/a0027911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Giordano P, Copp J, Longmore M, Manning W. Contested domains, verbal ‘amplifiers,’ and intimate partner violence in young adulthood. Social Forces. 2015 doi: 10.1093/sf/sov055. forthcoming. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Giordano P, Longmore M, Manning W. Gender and the meanings of adolescent romantic relationships: A focus on boys. American Sociological Review. 2006;71:260–287. [Google Scholar]
  28. Giordano P, Manning W, Longmore M, Flanigan C. Developmental shifts in the character of romantic and sexual relationships from adolescence to emerging adulthood. In: Booth A, Brown S, Landale N, Manning W, McHale S, editors. National symposium on family issues, Vol. 2: Early adulthood in a family context. New York: Springer; 2012. pp. 133–164. [Google Scholar]
  29. Halpern C, Spriggs A, Martin S, Kupper L. Patterns of intimate partner aggression victimization from adolescence to emerging adulthood in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;45:508–516. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Halpern-Meekin S, Manning W, Giordano P, Longmore M. Relationship churning, physical violence, and verbal abuse in young adult relationships. Journal of Marriage & Family. 2013a;75:2–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01029.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Halpern-Meekin S, Manning W, Giordano P, Longmore M. Relationship churning in emerging adulthood: On/off relationships and sex with an ex. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2013b;28:166–188. doi: 10.1177/0743558412464524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hartup W. On relationships and development. In: Hartup WW, Rubin Z, editors. Relationships and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1986. pp. 1–26. [Google Scholar]
  33. Herrenkohl T, Mason W, Kostgerman R, Legnua K, Hawkins J, Abbott R. Pathways from physical childhood abuse to partner violence in emerging adulthood. Violence & Victims. 2004;19:123–136. doi: 10.1891/vivi.19.2.123.64099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Johnson W, Giordano P, Longmore M, Manning W. The influence of intimate partner violence on trajectories of depressive symptoms from adolescence to emerging adulthood. Journal of Health & Social Behavior. 2014;55(1):39–55. doi: 10.1177/0022146513520430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Joyner K, Manning W, Bogle R. Working Paper 2013-002. Bowling Green, OH: Center for Family and Demographic Research, Bowling Green State University; 2013. The stability and qualities of same-sex and different-sex couples in emerging adulthood. [Google Scholar]
  36. Joyner K, Udry J. You don’t bring me anything but down: Adolescent romance and depression. Journal of Health & Social Behavior. 2000;41:369–391. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kamp Dush C, Amato P. Consequences of relationship status and quality for subjective well-being. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships. 2005;22(5):607–627. [Google Scholar]
  38. Kaura S, Lohman B. Dating violence victimization, relationship satisfaction, mental health problems, and acceptability of violence: A comparison of men and women. Journal of Family Violence. 2007;22:367–381. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kenney C, McLanahan S. Why are cohabiting relationships more violent than marriages? Demography. 2006;43:127–140. doi: 10.1353/dem.2006.0007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Klipfel K, Clazton S, van Dulmen M. Interpersonal aggression victimizait6on within casual sexual relationships and experiences. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2014;29(3):557–569. doi: 10.1177/0886260513505207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Laursen B, Jensen-Campbell L. The nature and functions of social exchange in adolescent romantic relationships. In: Furman W, Brown B, Feiring C, editors. The development of romantic relationships in adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1999. pp. 50–74. [Google Scholar]
  42. Leaman S, Gee C. Intimate partner violence among adolescent and young adult mothers. Journal of Family Violence. 2008;23:519–528. [Google Scholar]
  43. Levenson R, Cartensen L, Gottman J. Gender and satisfaction. Psychology & Aging. 1993;8(2):301–313. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.8.2.301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Longmore M, Manning W, Giordano P, Copp J. Poor relationship quality, intimate partner aggression, and depressive symptoms during emerging adulthood. Social Science Research. 2014;48:77–89. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2014.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lyons H, Manning W, Giordano P, Longmore M. Predictors of heterosexual casual sex among young adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2013;42:585–593. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-0051-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Maddox Shaw A, Rhoades G, Allen E, Stanley S, Markman H. Predictors of extradyadic sexual involvement in unmarried opposite-sex relationships. The Journal of Sex Research. 2013;50(6):598–610. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2012.666816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Madsen S, Collins W. The salience of adolescent romantic experiences for romantic relationship qualities in emerging adulthood. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2011;21:789–801. [Google Scholar]
  48. Manning W, Giordano P, Longmore M. Hooking up: The relationship contexts of ‘non-relationship’ sex. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2006;21:459–483. [Google Scholar]
  49. Manning W, Longmore M, Giordano P. Adolescents’ involvement in non-romantic sexual activity. Social Science Research. 2005;34(2):384–407. [Google Scholar]
  50. Manning W, Longmore M, Copp J, Giordano P. The complexities of adolescent dating and sexual relationships: Fluidity, meaning(s), and implications for young adults’ well-being. In: Lefkowitz ES, Vasilenko SA, editors. New directions for child & adolescent development: Positive and negative outcomes of sexual behaviors. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2014. pp. 53–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. McAlister A, Pachana N, Jackson C. Predictors of young dating adults’ inclination to engage in extradyadic sexual activities: A multi-perspective study. British Journal of Psychology. 2005;96(3):331–350. doi: 10.1348/000712605X47936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Meier A, Allen G. Romantic relationships from adolescence to emerging adulthood: Evidence from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Sociological Quarterly. 2009;50:308–335. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.2009.01142.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mendle J, Ferrero J, Moore S, Harden K. Depression and adolescents’ sexual activity in romantic and non-romantic relationship contexts: a genetically-informative sibling comparison. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2013;122(1):51–63. doi: 10.1037/a0029816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. O’Sullivan L, Cheng M, Harris K, Brooks-Gunn J. I wanna hold your hand: The progression of social, romantic and sexual events in adolescent relationships. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2007;39:100–107. doi: 10.1363/3910007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Preacher K, Hayes A. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods. 2008;40:879–891. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.3.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Radloff L. The CES-D scale: A self-reported depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  57. Raley K, Crissey S, Muller C. Of sex and romance: Adolescent relationships in the transition to adulthood. Journal of Marriage & Family. 2007;69:1210–1226. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00442.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rhoades G, Kamp Dush C, Atkins D, Stanley S, Markman M. Breaking up is hard to do: The impact of unmarried relationship dissolution on mental health and life satisfaction. Journal of Family Psychology. 2011;25:366–374. doi: 10.1037/a0023627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rust J, Bennum I, Crowe M, Golombok S. The Golombok Rust Inventory of Marital State (GRIMS) Sexual & Marital Therapy. 1986;1(1):55–60. [Google Scholar]
  60. Sabia J, Rees D. Does the number of sexual partners affect educational attainment? Evidence from female respondents to the Add Health. Journal of Population Economics. 2012;25:89–118. [Google Scholar]
  61. Sandberg-Thoma S, Kamp Dush C. Indicators of adolescent depression and relationship progression in emerging adulthood. Journal of Marriage & Family. 2014;76:191–206. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schondelmyer E. Changes in risk behavior: Partnerships and age; Paper presented at the annual Add Health Users conference; Bethesda, MD. 2014. [Google Scholar]
  63. Sprecher S, Treger S, Sakaluk J. Premarital sexual standards and sociosexuality: Gender, ethnicity, and cohort differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2013;42:1395–14054. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0145-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Straus M, Hamby S, Boney-McCoy S, Sugarman D. The revised conflict tactics scales (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues. 1996;17:283–316. [Google Scholar]
  65. Treas J, Giesen D. Sexual infidelity among married and cohabiting Americans. Journal of Marriage & Family. 2000;62(1):48–60. [Google Scholar]
  66. Tu X, Lou C, Gao E, Li N, Zabin L. The relationship between sexual behavior and nonsexual risk behaviors among unmarried youth in three Asian cities. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2012;50:S75–S82. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. U.S. Census Bureau. Statistical abstract of the United States. 129th. Washington, DC: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  68. Vasilenko S, Lefkowitz E, Maggs J. Short-term positive and negative consequences of sex based on daily reports among college students. Journal of Sex Research. 2012;49:558–569. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2011.589101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Vasilenko S, Lefkowitz, Welsh D. Is sexual behavior healthy for adolescents? A conceptual framework for research on adolescent sexual behavior and physical, mental, and social health. In: Lefkowitz ES, Vasilenko SA, editors. New directions for child & adolescent development: Positive and negative outcomes of sexual behaviors. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2014. pp. 3–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Vennum A, Lindstrom R, Monk K, Adams R. “It’s complicated”: The continuity and correlates of cycling in cohabiting and marital relationships. Journal of Social & Personal Relationships. 2014;31:410–430. [Google Scholar]
  71. Young BJ, Furman W, Laursen B. Models of change and continuity in romantic experiences. In: Fincham FD, editor. Romantic relationships in emerging adulthood. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2011. pp. 44–66. [Google Scholar]
  72. Zimmer-Gembeck M, Arnhold V, Connolly J. Intercorrelations of intimacy and identity dating goals with relationship behaviors and satisfaction among heterosexual couples. Social Science. 2014;3:44–59. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES