Skip to main content
GeroScience logoLink to GeroScience
. 2017 May 27;39(3):305–329. doi: 10.1007/s11357-017-9977-7

Loss of gait control assessed by cognitive-motor dual-tasks: pros and cons in detecting people at risk of developing Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases

Maroua Belghali 1, Nathalie Chastan 1,2, Fabien Cignetti 3, Damien Davenne 1, Leslie M Decker 1,
PMCID: PMC5505895  PMID: 28551877

Abstract

Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases are age-related progressive neurodegenerative diseases of increasing prevalence worldwide. In the absence of curative therapy, current research is interested in prevention, by identifying subtle signs of early-stage neurodegeneration. Today, the field of behavioral neuroscience has emerged as one of the most promising areas of research on this topic. Recently, it has been shown that the exacerbation of gait disorders under dual-task conditions (i.e., simultaneous performance of cognitive and motor tasks) could be a characteristic feature of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases. The cognitive-motor dual-task paradigm during walking allows to assess whether (i) executive attention is abnormally impaired in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease or (ii) compensation strategies are used in order to preserve gait function when the basal ganglia system is altered in prodromal Parkinson’s disease. This review aims at (i) identifying patterns of dual-task-related gait changes that are specific to Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases, respectively, (ii) demonstrating that these changes could potentially be used as prediagnostic markers for disease onset, (iii) reviewing pros and cons of existing dual-task studies, and (iv) proposing future directions for clinical research.

Keywords: Dual-task, Gait control, Attention, Executive function, Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, Prediagnostic phase, Prodromal phase, Clinical phase

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and Parkinson’s disease (PD) are a major public health concern as the world’s population ages. Unfortunately, AD and PD are up-to-date diagnosed in the advanced stage. In the absence of curative therapy, current research is interested in prevention by identifying subtle signs of early-stage neurodegeneration. Crucially, early diagnosis would provide a critical opportunity for disease-modulating interventions targeting modifiable risk factors for AD or PD (Barnes and Yaffe 2011), or even neuroprotective therapies, which both would help delay, slow, or even prevent disease progression (i.e., more cases would remain in the mild stage rather than degrade to moderate or severe stages; Petersen 2009). These risk factors are modifiable by making lifestyle changes (e.g., greater participation in physically and intellectually stimulating activities, social engagement, balanced diet with a high proportion of unsaturated fatty acids) and treating long-term health conditions (e.g., excessively high or low blood pressure, insufficient and/or fragmented sleep, diabetes, and midlife obesity).

From these clinical perspectives, increasing evidence supports the idea that the neuropathological process underlying AD and PD begin long before the onset of clinical symptoms as currently defined. This prodromal phase has opened new lines of research to identify the earliest markers that confirm the beginning of the disease. Behavioral neuroscience has emerged as one of the most promising areas of research on this topic. Recently, it has been shown that gait control is already affected in the prodromal phases of AD and PD (Postuma et al. 2012; Montero-Odasso et al. 2014), and thus may have the potential to serve as a new prodromal marker of these diseases. Prodromal markers represent “signatures” of an ongoing pathological process before the presence of typical symptoms allowing the clinical diagnosis (Heinzel et al. 2016). The loss of gait control is commonly evaluated by dual-task walking paradigms, during which the subject performs a cognitive (attention-demanding) task while walking, to assess any modifications compared to the reference (i.e., single-task condition), in either the cognitive or the walking subtasks (“dual-task cost”; Woollacott and Shumway-Cook 2002; McIsaac et al. 2015). At the onset of mild cognitive impairment (MCI, which may in some cases represent the prodromal phase of AD), this method has been widely used to assess whether or not executive attention functions (i.e., refers to higher cognitive processes used to allocate attention among different tasks performed simultaneously) are abnormally impaired. Such cognitive dysfunctions (i.e., predominantly inhibitory control) are considered today as a predictor of AD (Balota et al. 2010; Harrington et al. 2013; Seo et al. 2016). In the prodromal phase of PD, the dual-task paradigm is used to exhaust compensation strategies aimed at preserving the motor function when the basal ganglia system is altered (Chen et al. 2013; Lerche et al. 2014). Collectively, previous evidence that dual-task conditions significantly affect patients in the prodromal phases of AD and PD with respect to healthy controls might offer a rationale for the use of this paradigm as a screening tool for the risk of developing AD and PD. This background leads to the heart of this review. Thus, our purposes were fourfold (i) to identify patterns of dual-task-related gait changes that are specific to AD and PD, respectively, (ii) to demonstrate that these changes could potentially be used as prodromal markers for clinical disease onset, (iii) to review pros and cons of existing dual-task studies, and (iv) to propose future directions for clinical research.

Changes in gait control under single-task condition in Alzheimer’s disease

Clinical phase of Alzheimer’s disease

AD, the primary cause of irreversible dementia among elderly people, is a progressive neurodegenerative disease characterized by the presence of amyloid beta plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (Ballard et al. 2011; Ittner and Götz 2011) together with synaptic reduction, neuronal death, and atrophy (Braak and Braak 1991; Terry et al. 1991). In addition to these classical neuropathological markers, some studies have shown a disease burden in the motor cortex equivalent to the entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, and the association areas of the frontal and parietal lobes (Suvà et al. 1999). Behaviorally, patients with early AD have mainly severe episodic and working memories impairments, deficits in language, and semantic knowledge. Also, they fail tasks requiring relatively complex brain communication, such as under dual-task conditions; these tasks depending substantially on executive attention control (Nordahl et al. 2006; Mayda et al. 2011). Interestingly, cognitive impairment in AD, specifically executive attention control deficits, and the hyperexcitability of the motor cortex affect gait (van Iersel et al. 2004; Sheridan and Hausdorff 2007; Beauchet et al. 2008; Coelho et al. 2012). Overall, AD patients present a lower gait speed (Verghese et al. 2007; Rucco et al. 2017), shorter stride length (Coelho et al. 2012; Rucco et al. 2017) and cadence (Sheridan and Hausdorff 2007), and greater stride time variability (Beauchet et al. 2008, 2014). Stride length variability has rarely been studied but appear to be increased compared to healthy older adults (van Iersel et al. 2004; Coelho et al. 2012). According to Beauchet et al. (2008), stride time variability is a fine marker of gait control, thus, highlighting that gait in AD should not be considered as a simple automatic motor behavior, but one requiring “high-level cognitive functions,” particularly executive attention control (Allali et al. 2007).

Prodromal phase of Alzheimer’s disease

Converging evidence suggests that executive attention control, predominantly inhibitory control, changes before episodic memory impairment, which is the clinical hallmark of early AD (Harrington et al. 2013). Importantly, such changes could predict AD (Balota et al. 2010). Neuropathological findings suggest that extracellular amyloid could appear first in the basal isocortex including the entorhinal cortex and only later in the hippocampus (Harrington et al. 2013). As mentioned earlier, deficits in executive attention control may be responsible for gait disorders. Interestingly, this association also exists in the prodromal phase of AD and may predict older adults at risk of developing dementia. In fact, longitudinal studies that examined baseline gait disorders and the risk of dementia have shown that gait slowing appears 12 years before the onset of MCI and precedes a decline in cognitive function (Buracchio et al. 2010; Mielke et al. 2013). The Oregon Brain Aging study supports this view and found that a longer time to complete a 30-ft (15-m) walk at baseline was an independent predictor of cognitive impairment over a 6-year period of follow-up (Marquis et al. 2002). In addition to these findings, the 5-year longitudinal study conducted by Verghese et al. (2007) showed that changes in both rhythm (i.e., cadence, swing time, stance time, and double support time) and variability (i.e., stride length variability and swing time variability) domains of gait predicted decline in both executive attention control and episodic memory. The latter two were in turn shown to be associated with an increased risk for cognitive decline and dementia (Marquis et al. 2002; Verghese et al. 2007). Recently, during a median follow-up of 2.7 years, Taniguchi et al. (2012) found that shorter stride length was an independent predictor of cognitive decline in a general population of older adults and could be a better predictor than gait slowing.

Collectively, the abovementioned findings are consistent with the view that the loss of cognitive and motor functions observed in the prodromal phase of AD may share common underlying neuropathological mechanisms, namely, an executive attention control deficit. According to Buchman and Bennett (2011), the fact that several non-cognitive symptoms, such as gait disorders, predict the subsequent development of AD, suggests that non-cognitive behaviors may serve as phenotypic markers of prodromal AD. Likewise, Beauchet et al. (2014) suggested that there might be a motor phenotype of cognitive performance decline, which could be used to improve the prediction of dementia, particularly in AD. Currently, such a view is mainly explored using dual-task-walking paradigms. This approach is very advantageous compared to others, because it reveals subtle age- and disease-related abnormalities in gait control, which are masked by internal factors such as compensation mechanisms underlying cognitive reserve, and thus difficult to detect using either conventional neuropsychological tests or single-task walking assessment (Muir et al. 2012; Perrochon and Kemoun 2014; Perrochon et al. 2013, 2015).

Changes in gait control under single-task condition in Parkinson’s disease

Clinical phase of Parkinson’s disease

PD is a chronic neurodegenerative disease caused by progressive and profound loss of nigrostriatal dopaminergic neurons projecting to the striatum and the presence of Lewy bodies in the central, peripheral, and autonomic nervous systems (Jellinger 2012). Clinically, PD is characterized by the emergence of at least two cardinal motor features, including bradykinesia (i.e., slowness of movement) and either rigidity, tremor, gait disorders, or postural instability (Tolosa et al. 2006; Massano and Bhatia 2012). Overall, gait disorders appear in early in PD. They are characterized by (i) decreases in gait speed, stride length, and arm swing amplitude (Lewek et al. 2010; Volpe et al. 2017) and (ii) increases in cadence, double support time, arm swing amplitude, swing time asymmetry (Yogev et al. 2007), and variability in stride time and swing time (Baltadjieva et al. 2006; Mirek et al. 2007; Svehlík et al. 2009; Lewek et al. 2010; Volpe et al. 2017). From a behavioral point of view, the gait showing results from the inability of PD patients to generate sufficient stride length and arm swing amplitude (i.e., gait hypokinesia) even though cadence control remains intact (Morris et al. 1994; Hausdorff 2009), whereas the increased variability in temporal parameters from an alteration of the internal cueing mechanism needed to walk in a rhythmic fashion (Sheridan and Flowers 1990; Morris et al. 1994; Ebersbach et al. 1999; Schaafsma et al. 2003). From a neuropathological point of view, the causes of these gait disorders may result from (i) dopaminergic striatal deficit (Wu et al. 2015), (ii) excessive inhibition of the mesopontine locomotion center by the basal ganglia (Sterling et al. 2015), and (iii) cortical cholinergic denervation (Bohnen et al. 2013; Müller et al. 2015).

Prodromal phase of Parkinson’s disease

Positron emission tomography data revealed that clinical symptoms of Parkinsonism could develop after 70–80% of striatal dopamine is depleted, corresponding to 30–50% cell death of dopaminergic neurons (Fearnley and Lees 1991; Stoessl 2007; Postuma et al. 2012). The duration of this phase prior to diagnosis of PD is still in debate (Gaenslen et al. 2011) and has been estimated to last many years, with an average of 5 years (Morrish et al. 1998; Postuma et al. 2012), or even decades (Marek and Jennings 2009). To date, several motor and non-motor symptoms characterize this prodromal phase. While prodromal non-motor symptoms (e.g., mood disorders and olfaction) are largely explored, motor symptoms, on the other hand, remain poorly studied. However, because PD is currently diagnosed by its motor features, it is reasonable to speculate that subtle changes in the motor function will be present before the appearance of the cardinal motor signs required for diagnosis (Mirelman et al. 2016). In line with this view, Postuma et al. (2012) suggested that the stage when idiopathic rapid eye movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder (i.e., characterized by vivid, often-fearful dreams, and loss of normal muscle atonia during rapid eye movement sleep) (Olson et al. 2000) is diagnosed may provide a window to better understand prodromal motor symptoms. Accordingly, motor skills of 78 patients with idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder, as assessed by the Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale, the Purdue Pegboard Test, the Alternate Tap Test, and the Timed Up and Go Test, became abnormal ∼4.5 years before the diagnosis of PD (Postuma et al. 2012). Specifically, voice and face akinesia seem to be the first signs to appear (estimated prodromal interval: 9.8 years), followed by rigidity (4.4 years), gait abnormalities (4.4 years), and limb bradykinesia (4.2 years). As regards gait changes, these findings suggest that patients with early PD should present subtle gait disorders (Panyakaew and Bhidayasiri 2013; Mirelman et al. 2016). Yet, they are not detectable under undisturbed walking conditions (i.e., self-selected comfortable speed under single-task condition) in the prodromal phase. This is perhaps due to satisfactory motor system mechanisms compensating the slowly progressive nigrostriatal dopamine depletion within and outside the basal ganglia (Buhmann et al. 2005; Maetzler and Hausdorff 2012; Bezard and Fernagut 2014). Unfortunately, the exact compensatory mechanisms underlying gait disorders in the prodromal phase of PD remain unclear. Previous studies examining manual motor performance proposed the following mechanisms: (i) an increased activity in lateral premotor and parietal areas as well as in the anterior cingulate cortex to counterbalance defective striatofrontal motor circuits (Samuel et al. 1997; Sabatini et al. 2000; Buhmann et al. 2005), (ii) an increase in corticostriatal excitatory input to the basal ganglia (Wu et al. 2015), and (iii) the use of extrastriatal areas, such as executive attention control and more “‘voluntary” brain activity efforts to execute movements usually performed automatically in healthy people (Wu et al. 2015).

Although the above studies did not examine gait, the findings are consistent with the possibility of changes in the neural networks underlying control of gross motor skills such as gait, and perhaps also in the recruitment of compensatory processes (or cognitive strategy use), which might themselves depend on the individual’s cognitive reserve (Stern 2009). The challenge remains about how to capture these gait disorders despite compensatory capabilities? An intriguing possibility, which has recently been envisioned by very few studies (Lerche et al. 2014; Mirelman et al. 2011, 2016), is that dual-task walking may serve as a new, sensitive prodromal marker of PD.

Changes in gait control under dual-task condition in Alzheimer’s disease

Clinical phase of Alzheimer’s disease

In the clinical phase of AD, all studies showed that dual-task-related gait changes are larger in AD patients than in healthy controls. Such disorders include decreases in gait speed (Camicioli et al. 1997; Sheridan et al. 2003; Cocchini et al. 2004; Pettersson et al. 2007; Maquet et al. 2010; Coelho et al. 2012; Muir et al. 2012; Rucco et al. 2017), cadence (Coelho et al. 2012; Rucco et al. 2017), stride length, and increases in stride width (Rucco et al. 2017), double support time (Muir et al. 2012; Rucco et al. 2017), swing time, stance time (Rucco et al. 2017), stride time (Muir et al. 2012), stride length asymmetry (Maquet et al. 2010), stride length variability (Rucco et al. 2017), stride time variability (Sheridan et al. 2003; Muir et al. 2012), stance time variability, swing time variability, and stride width variability (Rucco et al. 2017). In most studies, the authors analyzed dual-task-related changes in gait but not in cognitive performance. The few studies that also did the latter showed a decline in the two domains (Cocchini et al. 2004; Coelho et al. 2012; Ijmker and Lamoth 2012). From a neuropathological point of view, dual-task-related gait changes in AD could reflect metabolic changes in cells of the associative cortices and hippocampal complex (Rolls and Grabenhorst 2008). The study of Marshall et al. (2007) suggests that the reduction in glucose consumption and perfusion in the cingulate gyrus and orbitofrontal cortex may also explain the difficulty of patients with AD to perform dual-task walking.

Prodromal phase of Alzheimer’s disease

In the prodromal phase of AD, dual-task-related gait changes are larger in MCI patients than in healthy controls, and greater dual-task complexity further worsens gait performance. The gait disorders reported in MCI patients include (i) decreases in gait speed (Pettersson et al. 2007; Maquet et al. 2010; Montero-Odasso et al. 2009a; Montero-Odasso et al. 2012; Muir et al. 2012; Doi et al. 2014; Tseng et al. 2014; Tarnanas et al. 2015) and stride length (Montero-Odasso et al. 2009b; Maquet et al. 2010) and (ii) increases in stride time (Muir et al. 2012), swing time (Boripuntakul et al. 2014), stride length variability (Boripuntakul et al. 2014), double support time variability (Montero-Odasso et al. 2009b), and stride time variability (Camicioli et al. 2006; Allali et al. 2007; Montero-Odasso et al. 2009b; Muir et al. 2012; Ijmker and Lamoth 2012; Montero-Odasso et al. 2014). From a neuropathological point of view, dual-task-related gait changes were interpreted as resulting from impairments in working memory and executive attention control (Maquet et al. 2010), both altering the ability of patients to execute more than one task at a time. Another interpretation proposed by Montero-Odasso et al. (2009a) is the limitation of attentional resources (the “central capacity-sharing model of dual-task performance”; Tombu and Jolicoeur 2003), which could account for dual-task interference in gait among people with MCI. Hence, it is likely that both explanations of dual-task interference during gait are relevant.

In contradiction with the above results, two studies (Pettersson et al. 2005; Nascimbeni et al. 2015) have reported no dual-task walking effect. This counterintuitive finding is likely due to either the gait assessment method (i.e., use of clinical performance-based measures, such as Timed Up and Go Test, Tinetti Gait Test, instead of instrumental gait analysis), the choice of the secondary cognitive task (i.e., type and complexity), the selection criteria for the control group (e.g., in Pettersson et al. 2005: people complaining of subjective cognitive impairment), the large heterogeneity of the MCI group, and/or the low sample size. Furthermore, it is reasonable to assume that part of these MCI patients will not evolve to dementia, but rather remain cognitively stable or even revert to normal during follow-up. It is hypothesized that dual-task walking could have the potential to improve the power of discrimination between MCI converters and non-converters. In a longitudinal study, comparing MCI patients who developed AD to those who remained stable, Gillain et al. (2016) provided first evidence supporting this view. Specifically, the authors showed that MCI converters deteriorated their gait speed and gait variability under dual-task conditions more than non-converters. Other longitudinal studies recruiting both healthy older adults and MCI patients are needed to confirm these findings. Besides, such studies are important to determine the threshold of change under dual-task walking that would differentiate healthy compensation mechanisms from pathological ones.

Table 1 summarizes the findings of dual-task-related gait changes in AD and MCI patients.

Table 1.

Summary of dual-task testing protocols in the prodromal and clinical phases of Alzheimer’s disease

Studies Populations Cognitive tasks Gait tasks Instruments Cognitive parameters Gait parameters Dual-task cost analysis Dual-task findings
Camicioli et al. 1997 EAD (n = 15)
HOA (n = 43)
Verbal fluency task Walking 30 ft at usual pace Not indicated Not measured Time to walk 30 ft
Cadence
Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ time to walk 30 ft in EAD compared to HOA
Sheridan et al. 2003 EAD (n = 28) Forward digit span task Walking 50 ft at usual pace Foot switches placed inside the shoe (i.e., at the ankle) Not measured Gait speed,
Stride time variability
Wilcoxon signed-rank test ↑ stride time variability,
↓ gait speed
Cocchini et al. 2004 EAD (n = 15)
HOA (n = 15)
Verbal fluency task
Digit span task
Walking 11 m at usual pace Not indicated Number of words verbally enumerated
Percentage of digits correctly recalled
The distance covered by each participant in 30 s Not indicated - When task difficulty was not titrated, EAD walked a shorter distance than HOA
- When task difficulty was titrated, group difference disappeared
- Decreased cognitive performance in EAD compared to HOA
Pettersson et al. 2005 NCI (n = 33)
MCI (n = 59)
EAD (n = 22)
HOA (n = 26)
Verbal fluency task Walking 10 m at usual pace Not indicated Not measured Time to walk 10 m Two-way factorial ANCOVA ↓ time to walk 10 m in EAD compared to HOA
Camicioli et al. 2006 EAD (n = 42) Forward digit span task Walking 15 m at usual and fast paces GAITRite® system Not measured Gait speed
Cadence
Stride time
Stride length
Swing time
Double support time
Repeated measures ANOVA ↑ stride time,
↑ swing time,
↑ swing time variability,
↓ cadence
Pettersson et al. 2007 AD (n = 6)
MCI (n = 6)
HOA (n = 25)
Verbal fluency task Walking 10 m at usual pace Stopwatch Not measured Gait speed Difference in mean between single- and dual-task walking ↓ gait speed in EAD compared to MCI and HOA
Allali et al. 2007 AD, VaD,
mixed AD/VaD
(n = 16)
Forward and backward counting Walking 10 m at usual pace GAITRite® system Number of figures verbally enumerated Stride time
Stride time variability
Dual-task costa for each parameter ↑ stride time variability
- No difference in cognitive performance
Montero-Odasso et al. 2009a MCI (n = 55) Serial substraction task
Verbal fluency task
Walking 10 m at usual pace Stopwatch Not measured Gait speed Difference in time between single and dual-task walking ↓ gait speed
Montero-Odasso et al. 2009b MCI (n = 11) Serial substraction task (from 100) Walking 6 m at usual pace GAITRite® system Not measured Gait speed
Stride length
Stride time
Double support time
Gait variability for all parameters
Paired t test ↓ gait speed,
↑ stride time variability,
↑ stride time,
↑ double support time
Maquet et al. 2010 MCI (n = 14)
MAD (n = 6)
HOA (n = 14)
Serial substraction task Walking 45 m at usual pace Locometrix®
Electrical photocells
Not measured Gait speed
Stride frequency
Stride length
Stride symmetry
Number of stops
Difference in mean between single- and dual-task walking ↓ gait speed in MAD and MCI compared to HOA
Ijmker and Lamoth 2012 AD and FTD (n = 15)
YOA (n = 12)
HOA (n = 14)
Phonemic fluency task (letters: “R,” “G,” and “P”) Walking for 3 min at a self-selected pace up and down a 10 m long course Ambulant accelerometer Number of words verbally enumerated Gait speed
Stride time
Stride time variability
Wilcoxon signed-rank test ↑ stride time variability and
- Decreased cognitive performance in FTD and AD compared to YOA and HOA
Muir et al. 2012 AD (n = 23)
MCI (n = 29)
HOA (n = 22)
Verbal fluency task
Serial substraction task (by ones and sevens)
Walking 6 m at usual pace GAITRite® system Not measured Gait speed
Stride time variability
Dual-task costb for each parameter ↑ stride time,
↑ stride time variability,
↓ gait speed in AD and MCI compared to HOA
Coelho et al. 2012 MAD (n = 23) Serial substraction task (from 20) Walking 8 m at usual pace Digital camera
Passive marker to the fifth right metatarsus
Number of errors Cadence
Stride length
Gait speed
Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ cadence,
↓ gait speed,
↓ stride length
- MAD made more errors
Montero-Odasso et al. 2012 MCI (n = 43)
HOA (n = 25)
Serial substraction task (from 100)
Semantic fluency task
Walking 6 m at usual pace GAITRite® system Not measured Gait speed
Stride time
Stride time variability
Repeated measures ANOVA ↑ stride time variability,
↓ gait speed in MCI compared to HOA (all the more with increasing task complexity)
Boripuntakul et al. 2014 MCI (n = 30)
HOA (n = 30)
Serial substraction task (by threes from 20 or 50) Gait initiation (first and second stride)
Walking at usual pace
GAITRite® system Not measured Swing time
Stride time
Stride length
Stride width
Gait variability for all parameters
Repeated measures ANOVA ↑ swing time,
↑ stride length variability for the first and second stride gait initiation in MCI compared to HOA
Tseng et al. 2014 a-MCI (n = 16)
HOA (n = 10)
Phonemic fluency task (letter “C”)
5-digit backward span task
Serial substraction task (by sevens)
3-item delayed recall task
Walking 10 m at usual pace Not indicated 1 point for each word correctly recited
1 point for each digit in the right order
1 point for each correct subtraction
Gait speed Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ gait speed in a-MCI compared to HOA
- Decreased cognitive performance in a-MCI compared to HOA
Montero-Odasso et al., 2014 a-MCI (n = 42)
na-MCI (n = 22)
HOA (n = 35)
- Serial substraction task (by ones from 100)
- Verbal fluency task
- Walking at usual pace - GAITRite® system - Not measured - Stride time variability
- Gait speed
- Dual-task cost2 for each parameter ↑ stride time variability,
↓ gait speed in a-MCI compared to HOA and na-MCI
Doi et al. 2014 a-MCI (n = 191)
na-MCI (n = 198)
HOA (n = 35)
Serial substraction task (from 100) Walking 11 m at usual pace Horizontal walkway Not measured Gait speed Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ gait speed in both a-MCI and na-MCI compared to HOA
Nascimbeni et al. 2015 MCI (n = 13)
HOA (n = 10)
Serial substraction task (by ones from 378 or 283)
Verbal fluency task
Short story recall task
Walking at usual pace back and forth over a distance of 12 m Footswitches based on force sensitive resistors placed on the first and fifth metatarsal heads and the posterior part of heel
Stopwatch
Number of correctly produced digits
Number of correctly produced items
Number of correctly recalled items
Stride time
Stride time variability
Double support time
Gait speed
Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ gait speed,
↑ stride time,
↑ stride time variability,
↑ double support time in both groups
- Performance decreased on serial substraction and improved on short story recall in both groups
Tarnanas et al. 2015 EAD (n = 86)
Na-MCI (n = 65)
HOA (n = 76)
Serial substraction task (by ones from 100)
Verbal fluency task
Walking at usual pace over a distance of 10 m GAITRite® system Not measured Gait speed
Stride time variability
Difference in mean between single- and dual-task walking ↓ gait speed,
↑ stride time variability in EAD and na-MCI compared to HOA
Gillain et al. 2016 MCI (n = 13) Serial substraction task (from 50) Walking at usual pace Locometrix® Not measured Gait speed
Stride time variability
Stride length
Cadence
Difference in mean between single- and dual-task walking ↓ gait speed,
↑ stride time variability in MCI who developed AD
Rucco et al. 2017 AD (n = 22)
FTD (n = 23)
HOA (n = 20)
Serial substraction task (by sevens from 100) Walking at usual pace over a distance of 10 m Stereo-photogrammetric system Not measured Gait speed
Cadence
Stride length
Stride time
Stride width
Stance time
Swing time
Double support time
Gait variability for all parameters
Difference in mean between single- and dual-task walking ↓ gait speed,
↓ cadence,
↑ double support time,
↑ stance time variability,
↑ swing time variability,
↑ stride length variability,
↑ stride width variability in AD and FTD compared to
HOA

NCI subjects with no cognitive impairment, EAD early Alzheimer’s disease, AD Alzheimer’s disease, MAD mild Alzheimer’s disease, FTD frontal temporal dementia, MCI mild cognitive impairment, a-MCI amnesic mild cognitive impairment, na-MCI non-amnesic mild cognitive impairment, HOA healthy older adults, YOA younger older adults, VaD vascular dementia, ↑ increased, ↓ decreased

aGait DTC = [(dual-task gait performance − single-task gait performance)/(dual-task gait performance + single-task gait performance)/2] × 100

bDTC = [(single-task value − dual-task value)/single-task value] × 100

Changes in gait control under dual-task condition in Parkinson’s disease

Clinical phase of Parkinson’s disease

In the clinical phase of PD, dual-task-related gait changes are larger in PD patients than in healthy controls. Such disorders include (i) decreases in gait speed (Fuller et al. 2013; Wild et al. 2013; Stegemöller et al. 2014; Strouwen et al. 2016), stride length (O’Shea et al. 2002; Rochester et al. 2004; Plotnik et al. 2011; Tseng et al. 2012; Panyakaew and Bhidayasiri 2013; Kelly and Shumway-Cook 2014; Stegemöller et al. 2014), bilateral gait coordination (i.e., the generation of left-right antiphase stepping), and swing time (Plotnik et al. 2009; 2011; Tseng et al. 2012; Vervoort et al. 2016) and (ii) increases in stride time (Panyakaew and Bhidayasiri 2013; Stegemöller et al. 2014), cadence (Rochester et al. 2014), stance time (Wild et al. 2013), stride width (Stegemöller et al. 2014), stride length asymmetry (Rochester et al. 2014; Vervoort et al. 2016), swing time variability (Stegemöller et al. 2014), stride time variability, and stride length variability (Hausdorff et al. 2003; Yogev et al. 2007; Plotnik et al. 2009, 2011; Yogev-Seligmann et al. 2012; Rochester et al. 2014; Stegemöller et al. 2014). Decreases in arm swing amplitude and increases in both arm swing asymmetry and arm swing variability have also been documented (Mirelman et al. 2016). The few studies that have examined dual-task-related changes in both gait and cognitive performance showed a decline in the two domains (O’Shea et al. 2002; Yogev et al. 2005).

In contradiction with these findings, two studies (Kelly and Shumway-Cook 2014; Rochester et al. 2014) found that certain aspects of gait, namely gait speed, stride length, stride length variability, and stride time variability, were unexpectedly unaffected under dual-task conditions. This may be due to the use of an auditory Stroop task (Kelly and Shumway-Cook 2014) and an auditory Wechsler Forward Digit Span task (Rochester et al. 2014) as secondary tasks. Indeed, the auditory modality may provide beneficial cueing effects on gait rhythm through compensatory cerebellar-parieto-premotor pathways or by facilitating task prioritization and/or executive attention control (Rochester et al. 2008). Overall, PD patients were less able than healthy controls to use a “posture first” strategy (i.e., prioritizing walking over other concurrent tasks). In fact, some patients may give inappropriate prioritization to the concurrent attention-demanding task, thus, sacrificing attention resources needed for gait by using a “posture second” strategy (Bloem et al. 2006). Evidence for this strategy use was observed in several studies in which explicit instructions regarding task prioritization were not given (Yogev-Seligmann et al. 2010). Hence, one could posit that the auditory cueing provided by the cognitive task improve walking automaticity, and thus reduce the attentional demands of gait control.

From a neuropathological point of view, theoretical mechanisms that could explain dual-task-related gait changes in clinical PD are (i) a reduced movement automaticity (i.e., the ability to perform a skilled movement without conscious or executive control or attention directed toward the movement) and the need for increased reliance on cognitive resources to control gait because of basal ganglia dysfunction (Wu et al. 2004; Poldrack et al. 2005), (ii) dopamine-mediated dysfunction of the basal ganglia (Middleton and Strick 2000), and (iii) presence of non-dopaminergic pathology, such as serotonin, norepinephrine (noradrenaline), or acetylcholine (Leverenz et al. 2009). Recently, Nieuwhof et al. (2016) suggest that altered functioning of the prefrontal cortex may also contribute to difficulties in dual-task walking. These mechanisms are not mutually exclusive but might overlap with one another.

Prodromal phase of Parkinson’s disease

There exist three studies that have examined dual-task-related gait changes in the prodromal phase of PD (Lerche et al. 2014; Mirelman et al. 2011, 2016). All of them have reported dual-task interference during gait. Specifically, two cross-sectional studies conducted by Mirelman et al. (2011, 2016) have shown decreases in arm swing amplitude and axial rotation smoothens and increases in arm swing variability, arm swing asymmetry (Mirelman et al. 2016), and stride time variability (Mirelman et al. 2011) in LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers without a clinical diagnosis of PD. The third study by Lerche et al. (2014) has demonstrated dual-task decrement in gait speed in patients with mild parkinsonian signs to the same extent as non-demented patients with PD. From a neuropathological point of view, the authors suggested that impairment of movement automaticity because of early basal ganglia dysfunction and cognitive inflexibility might explain these changes in gait under dual-task conditions.

Table 2 summarizes the findings of dual-task-related gait changes in patients with PD, mild parkinsonian signs, and LRRK2-G2019S mutation carriers without a clinical diagnosis of PD.

Table 2.

Summary of dual-task testing protocols in the prodromal and clinical phases of Parkinson’s disease

Studies Populations Cognitive tasks Gait tasks Instruments Cognitive parameters Gait parameters Dual-task cost analysis Dual-task findings
O’Shea et al. 2002 PD (n = 15)
HOA (n = 15)
Coin transference
Serial substraction task (by threes from 125 or 250)
Walking 10 m at usual pace Footswitches based on force sensitive resistors placed on the hallux and the first, second, and fourth metatarsal heads Not measured Gait speed
Stride length
Cadence
Double support time
Paired t test ↑ double support time,
↓ stride length,
↓ gait speed,
↓ cadence in PD (with either a motor or cognitive secondary task)
Hausdorff et al. 2003 PD (n = 10) Serial substraction task (by threes from 200, 193, and 186) Walking 20 m at usual pace Footswitches based on force sensitive resistors Not measured Stride time
Stride time variability
Wilcoxon signed-rank test ↑ stride time variability
Yogev et al. 2005 PD (n = 30)
HOA (n = 28)
Serial substraction task (by sevens from 500)
Listening through ear phones to a text
Walking 20 m at usual pace Footswitches based on force sensitive resistors Not measured Stride time
Swing time
Stride time
variability
Repeated measures ANOVA ↑ stride time variability in PD compared to HOA
Plotnik et al. 2009 PD (n = 21)
HOA (n = 13)
Serial substraction task (by sevens) Walking at usual pace for 2 min Footswitches based on force sensitive resistors Not measured Gait speed
Stride time variability
Swing time variability
Stride time asymmetry
Left-right stepping coordination
Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ left-right stepping coordination,
↑ swing time variability,
↑ stride time variability in PD compared to HOA
Plotnik et al. 2011 PD (n = 30) Serial substraction task (by threes and sevens) Walking 80 m at usual pace Footswitches based on force sensitive resistors Not measured Gait speed
Stride length
Stride time variability
Swing time variability
Swing time asymmetry
Left-right stepping coordination
Paired t test ↓ left-right stepping coordination,
↓ stride length,
↓ gait speed,
↑ swing time asymmetry,
↑ swing time variability
- Gait changes are more pronounced with concomitant serial subtraction task by sevens
Mirelman et al. 2011 LRRK2 (n = 25)
HOA (n = 27)
Serial substraction task (by sevens) Walking 20 m at usual and maximal pace Accelerometer Not measured Gait speed
Stride time
Stride length
Stride time variability
Paired t test ↑ stride time variability in LRRK2 compared to HOA at both paces
Tseng et al. 2012 PD (n = 30)
HOA (n = 28)
Serial substraction task (from 100) Walking forward and backward 6 m at usual pace GAITRite® system Not measured Gait speed
Stride length
- Stride time
Double support time
Gait variability for each
Paired t test ↓ gait speed,
↓ swing time,
↓ stride length,
in both walking directions in PD compared to HOA
Yogev-Seligmann et al. 2012 PD (n = 18)
HOA (n = 15)
Verbal fluency task Walking 30 m at usual pace Ambulatory recorder
Footswitches based on force sensitive resistors
Not measured Stride time
Swing time
Stride time variability
Gait speed
Swing time variability
Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ gait speed,
↑ stride time variability,
↑ swing time variability,
in both groups
Fuller et al. 2013 PD (n = 154) Phonemic fluency (letters: “F”, “A”, and “S”) Walking 15 m at usual pace Not indicated Number of words verbally enumerated Gait speed Hierarchical regression ↓ gait speed
- Decreased cognitive performance
Panyakaew and Bhidayasiri 2013 PD (n = 21)
HOA (n = 21)
Serial substraction task (by threes from 200) Walking 7.82 m at usual pace GAITRite® system Not measured Gait speed
Cadence
Stride length
Stride time
Swing time
Double support time
Stride width
Gait variability for each parameter
Paired t test ↑ cadence,
↑ stride width,
↓ swing time,
↑ stride time in PD compared to HOA
Wild et al. 2013 PD (n = 18)
HOA (n = 18)
Serial substraction task (by sevens from 500)
Text comprehension task
Phoneme counting task
Walking 9 m at usual pace Kinemetric with a fixed camera Total correct serial subtractions
Percent of correct responses (text comprehension)
Percent of total phonemes
Stride length
Cadence
Swing time
Stance time
Gait DTC formulaa ↑ cadence,
↓ swing time,
↓ stance time in PD compared to HOA
- Decreased cognitive performance in PD in all conditions compared to HOA
Yogev-Seligmann et al. 2012 PD (n = 18)
YA (n = 21)
HOA (n = 15)
Verbal fluency task Walking 30 m at usual pace Ambulatory recorder
Footswitches based on force sensitive resistors
Accelerometer located on posterior distal part of the shin above the malleoli
Number of words verbally enumerated Gait speed
Stride time
Stride time variability
Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ gait speed,
↑ stride time,
↑ stride time variability in PD compared to YA and HOA
Rochester et al. 2014 PD (n = 44)
HOA (n = 55)
Working memory task (participants listened to strings of digits and repeated them back) alking for 2 min at usual pace GAITRite® system Error rate Gait speed
Stride length
Stride time
Stride width
variability
Stride time variability
Stride width variability
Stance time asymmetry
Gait DTC formulab ↓ gait speed,
↑ stride length,
↑ stride time,
↑ stride width
Variability,
↑ stride time variability,
↑ stride width variability,
↑ stance time asymmetry in both HOA and PD
Kelly and Shumway-Cook 2014 PD (n = 11)
HOA (n = 12)
Auditory Stroop task Usual-base and narrow-base walking over a distance of 8.8 m Motion capture system (Qualisys) Response latency
Response accuracy
Stride length
Stride width
Cadence
Stride width variability
Stride time variability
Center of mass
Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ gait speed in PD compared to HOA
- Decreased cognitive performance in PD compared to HOA
Lerche et al. 2014 PD (n = 27)
MPS (n = 73)
HOA (n = 892)
Box-checking task
Serial substraction task (by sevens)
Walking 20 m at usual and maximal paces Accelerometer Not measured Gait speed Gait DTC formulac ↓ gait speed in both conditions
Stegemöller et al. 2014 PD (n = 35) Serial substraction task (by threes) Walking 12 m at usual pace Motion capture system Not measured Gait speed
Stride time
Stride length
Stride width
Swing time
Variability for each parameter
Repeated measures ANOVA ↓ gait speed,
↑ stride length,
↑ stride time,
↑ stride width,
↑ variability for each parameter
Strouwen et al. 2016 PD (n = 121) Serial substraction task
Auditory Stroop task
Mobile phone task
Walking 10 m at usual pace GAITRite® system Reaction time
Number of errors
Gait speed Regression models ↓ gait speed
- A strong correlation between gait speed and cognitive flexibility
Vervoort et al. 2016 PD (n = 73)
HOA (n = 20)
Auditory Stroop task Walking 6 m at usual pace Motion capture system (Vicon) Reaction time (average and variability) Gait speed
Stride length
Stride width
Stride time
Stance time
Swing time
Double support time
Gait variability for each gait parameter
MANOVA ↑ stance time,
↑ stride length asymmetry,
↓ swing time in PD compared to HOA
- Decreased cognitive performance in PD compared to HOA
- A strong correlation between gait speed and cognitive flexibility
Mirelman et al. 2016 PD (n = 194)
LRRK2 (n = 122)
HOA (n = 64)
Serial substraction task (by threes) Walking 15 m at usual pace Lightweight body-fixed sensor
Velcro straps
Arm swing amplitude
Arm swing variability
Arm swing asymmetry
Stride time variability
Axial rotation smoothness
Paired t test ↑ stride time variability,
↑ arm swing variability,
↑ arm swing asymmetry,
↓ axial rotation smoothness in LRRK2 compared to HOA

HOA healthy older adults, YA younger adults, PD Parkinson’s disease, LRRK2 carriers of the LRRK2-G2019S mutation, MPS mild Parkinsonian signs in older adults, IRBD patients with REM idiopathic behavioral disorder, ↑ increased, ↓ decreased

aGait DTC = [(single-task gait performance − dual-task gait performance)/single-task gait performance × 100

bGait DTC = dual-task gait performance − single-task gait performance

cGait DTC = (1 − dual-task speed/single-task speed) × 100

Summary of methodological issues of dual-task studies

Currently, a comparison between dual-task studies in the clinical and prodromal phases of both AD and PD is made difficult because of the variety of all aspects of the protocols. Cognitive tasks used include the following: auditory Stroop task; serial subtraction by ones, threes, and sevens from a given number (e.g., 100 and 283); working memory task (i.e., listening to strings of digits while walking and repeating them back and letter 2-back task); short story recall memory task; 3-item delayed recall episodic memory task; and semantic fluency (i.e., naming animals and reciting male or female names) or phonemic fluency (i.e., letters F, A, S, R, G, P, and C) tasks. Gait tasks used include walking at a comfortable, self-selected pace or at a maximum pace over varying distances (6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 15, 12, 25, and 45 m), walking backward, or walking 3 min). Instruments that have been used to assess both gait parameters (gait speed, stride time, stride length, stride height, stride width, double support time, stride frequency, symmetry, regularity, and gait variability) and cognitive performance (number of correct responses, reaction time, error rate, and accuracy) include treadmill, GAITRite®, stopwatch, Locometrix®, and motion capture system. Other sources of variability are the choice of the clinical population (MCI subtypes, different stages of AD: from moderate to severe), sample size, medication, and statistical approach (i.e., repeated ANOVA, paired Student t test, specific dual-task cost formulas). Tables 1 and 2 summarize the different dual-task testing protocols and findings in both the clinical and prodromal phases of AD and PD.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first review that provides a comprehensive overview of dual-task-related gait changes that are specific to AD and PD (Table 3).

Table 3.

Dual-task-related gait changes in the prodromal and clinical phases of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases, including six domains of spatiotemporal gait parameters inspired from Hollman et al. 2011 and Lord et al. 2013 (pace, rhythm, phases, asymmetry, variability, and postural control), and arm swing parameters inspired from Mirelman et al. 2016 (amplitude, variability, and asymmetry)

Gait domains Clinical phase Prodromal phase
AD PD AD PD
Pace ↓ gait speed
↓ stride length
↑ stride time variability
↑ swing time variability
↑ stance time variability
↓ gait speed
↓ stride length
↑ stride time variability
↑ swing time variability
↓ gait speed
↓ stride length
↑ stride time variability
↑ double support time variability
↓ gait speed
↑ stride time variability
Rhythm ↓ cadence
↑ swing time
↑ stance time
↑ stride time
↑ cadence
↓ swing time
↑ stance time
↑ stride time
↑ swing time
↑ stride time
Phases ↑ double support time ↑ double support time ↑ double support time
Asymmetry ↑ stride time asymmetry
Variability ↑ stride length variability
↑ stride width variability
↑ stride length variability
↑ stride width variability
↑ stride length variability
Postural control ↑ stride width
↑ stride length asymmetry
↑ stride width
↑ stride length asymmetry
Arm swing parameters ↓ arm swing amplitude
↑ arm swing asymmetry
↑ arm swing variability
↓ arm swing amplitude
↑ arm swing asymmetry
↑ arm swing variability

AD Alzheimer’s disease, PD Parkinson’s disease, ↑ increased, ↓ decreased

Dual-task-related gait changes in AD at the clinical stage

The pace (i.e., gait speed, stride length, stride time variability, swing time variability, stance time variability, and double support time), rhythm (i.e., stride time, stance time, swing time, and cadence), variability (stride length variability and stride width variability), and postural control (i.e., stride width and stride length asymmetry) domains of gait showed to be consistently sensitive to dual-task interference in AD. Unfortunately, the exact neuropathological mechanisms underlying changes in each domain of gait are still unknown. Yet, based on neuropsychological studies, changes in the pace domain could be related to impairments in either executive attention control (Sheridan et al. 2003; Maquet et al. 2010; Coelho et al. 2012) or global cognitive functions (Coelho et al. 2012). Based on the study of Nakamura et al. (1996), decreased regional cerebral blood flow in the frontal lobe may also contribute to changes in the pace domain (particularly, stride time variability). Other studies suggested that atrophy in the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex, which are two key regions responsible for the sequential ordering of movement (Smith and Milner 1981) may explain changes in both the variability and rhythm domains of gait under dual-task conditions (Morris et al. 2016). From a clinical point of view, dual-task paradigms have been used to distinguish fallers from non-fallers among AD patients. For example, Reelick et al. (2011) showed that only stride length variability discriminate significantly between the faller groups in the dual-task, and not the single-task, condition. Furthermore, if clinicians are seeking further information on attention and executive functions of their patients, dual-task gait paradigms might be of value to explore the attentional requirement of balance during locomotion (Amboni et al. 2013; Menant et al. 2014).

Dual-task-related gait changes in PD at the clinical stage

The rhythm (i.e., cadence, stride time, and stance time), asymmetry (i.e., stride time asymmetry), postural control (i.e., stride width and stride length asymmetry), pace (i.e., gait speed, stride length, stride time variability, swing time variability, and double support time variability), and variability (i.e., stride length variability and stride width variability) domains of gait, but also arm swing parameters (i.e., arm swing amplitude, arm swing variability, and arm swing asymmetry) showed to be consistently sensitive to dual-task interference in PD. Biomechanically, gait slowing is mainly due to reduced stride length. The reason for reduced stride length has not yet been fully elucidated, although it has been suggested that gait hypokinesia could reflect a difficulty in activating the locomotor control system (i.e., inadequate contribution to cortical motor set by the basal ganglia (Hausdorff 2009). PD patients typically increased their cadence to compensate for reduced stride length (Morris et al. 1994). Concerning the other pace parameters, some studies showed that increases in stride time variability and swing time variability are likely due to the inability to generate “internal cues” in the supplementary motor areas, a necessary step in the creation of a normal sequenced movement (Yogev et al. 2005) According to Blin et al. (1990), deficits in muscle strength, of which the exact neuropathological mechanisms may come from central activation impairments (Stevens-Lapsley et al. 2012), may also explain changes in the pace domain. Beyond motor explanation, Yogev et al. (2005) suggest that deficits in executive attention control may also contribute to these changes among PD patients.

Decreased arm swing amplitude of the more affected body side is frequently reported in the literature. Such parameter seems to be related to both rigidity (Kwon et al. 2014) and dopamine-mediated dysfunction of the basal ganglia (Wu et al. 2015). Also, it mechanically induces a reduction in both bilateral arm coordination and arm swing symmetry (Lewek et al. 2010; Huang et al. 2012) as well as greater arm swing variability (Mirelman et al. 2016). From a neuropathological point of view, these changes in arm swing parameters appear to be linked to the asymmetric process of nigrostriatal dopaminergic denervation (Lewek et al. 2010; Huang et al. 2012). In an effort to clarify the origin of dual-task-related gait changes in PD, Lord et al. (2010) have suggested that some gait domains are levodopa-sensitive and others are levodopa-resistant. In support of this view, Hausdorff (2009) showed that temporal gait variability is independent of dopaminergic pathways, and that both spatial gait variability and arm swing parameters are dependent of dopaminergic pathways (Lord et al. 2010). Hence, such findings suggest that the latter have potential utility to predict gait dysfunction related to PD (Lord et al. 2010). As for AD, dual-task-related gait changes in the clinical phase of PD is clinically relevant to detect patients at risk of falls. Also, some studies have shown that dual-task walking performance was associated with gait quality (Kelly et al. 2012), freezing (Spildooren et al. 2010), disease severity, and disability (Fuller et al. 2013).

Dual-task-related gait changes: comparison between AD and PD at the clinical stage

Even though the neuropathological origin of dual-task-related gait changes is different between the two diseases, the corresponding gait patterns display some similarities, particularly in the pace, rhythm, postural control, and variability domains of gait. Mechanistically, similar dual-task-related gait changes between AD and PD are likely due to a limited attentional capacity (serial bottleneck theory; Pashler 1984, 1994) resulting from more brain damage that prevents patients to share their attentional resources and perform both tasks simultaneously (capacity sharing theory; Tombu and Jolicoeur 2003). In addition, dysfunction in executive attention, anxiety, and depression, which are known to have negative effects on cognition and motor performance, may also be another common factor (Hausdorff et al. 2003; Beurskens and Bock 2012). Despite this commonality, gait parameters between the two patient groups have never been directly compared. Thus, the question arises as to whether the dual-task walking paradigm is sensitive enough to characterize specific spatiotemporal gait profiles of AD and PD, respectively. Such a question has not yet been addressed in the literature. Nonetheless, our review suggests that the rhythm (i.e., cadence), pace (i.e., stride length and stride time variability), and variability (i.e., stride length variability) domains of gait as well as arm swing parameters (i.e., arm swing amplitude, arm swing asymmetry, and arm swing variability) could differentiate these two diseases. Specifically, we suggest that cadence and arm swing parameters would be more altered in PD, whereas stride length, stride time variability, and stride length variability would be more altered in AD (Table 3). Further studies comparing patients with AD, PD, and healthy controls are needed to confirm this proposal.

The second result that clearly emerges from this review is that dual-task-related gait changes could potentially be used as prodromal markers for clinical disease onset.

Added value of the dual-task paradigm in the prediction of AD and PD

Both PD and AD share a long prediagnostic phase in which gait control under dual-task conditions is already altered. The dual-task walking paradigm showed benefit either (i) to explore the involvement of cortical level in gait control (Beauchet et al. 2014), thus, allowing to assess whether or not executive attention control is abnormally impaired, as in prodromal AD or (ii) to exhaust compensation strategies aimed at preserving motor function when the basal ganglia system is altered, as in prodromal PD. Taking together, it is understandable that dual-task walking may be a useful tool to reveal deficits unseen during single-task walking, and therefore be a particularly sensitive predictor of AD and PD. Given that the neuropathological profile of PD differs from AD, the question arises as to whether the dual-task walking paradigm is sensitive enough to identify disease-specific gait patterns during the prediagnostic phase. To our knowledge, this question has not been addressed in the literature. Currently, decreased gait speed is one of the most reported gait parameters in the literature for both diseases. Despite the relevance of such a finding, gait slowing could not be considered as a specific parameter for either disease, as it also occurs in healthy aging. From a neuropathological point of view, gait slowing may be primarily due to neuropathological mechanisms that are common to normal aging and neurodegenerative diseases, especially increased prion-inflammatory makers and vascular burden (Valkanova and Ebmeier 2017). In the next sections, we identified dual-task-related changes in other parameters than gait speed that could be specific to AD and PD at the prodromal stage.

Dual-task-related gait changes in AD at the prodromal stage

Patients with various subtypes of MCI decrease their speed under dual-task conditions (Pettersson et al. 2007; Maquet et al. 2010; Montero-Odasso et al. 2009a, 2012; Muir et al. 2012; Doi et al. 2014; Tseng et al. 2014; Tarnanas et al. 2015). This gait slowing may result from reduced stride length and increased double support time (Bahureksa et al. 2017) and is mostly due to cerebrovascular disease (Annweiler et al. 2013). Besides, the rhythm (i.e., stride time, stance time, and cadence), pace (i.e., stride length, stride time variability, and double support time variability), and variability (i.e., stride length variability) domains of gait showed to be consistently sensitive to dual-task interference. Notably, in a recent meta-analysis including 14 studies, Bahureksa et al. (2017) found that dual-task walking highlights the sensitivity of the latter gait parameters for discriminating between MCI subtypes and healthy controls. Furthermore, it has been shown that dual-task-related gait changes could differentiate between non-amnestic and amnestic MCI (Montero-Odasso et al. 2014; Valkanova and Ebmeier 2017). Specifically, the pace domain (i.e., gait speed and stride length) was more affected in non-amnestic MCI, whereas the rhythm (i.e., cadence, swing time, stance time, and double support time) and variability (i.e., stride length variability and swing time variability) domains were worse in amnestic MCI (Verghese et al. 2008). Of note, Verghese et al. (2008) used another model to study the gait domains compared to the one used in our review (inspired from Hollman et al. 2011 and Lord et al. 2013).

From a neuropathological point of view, imaging studies using magnetic resonance spectroscopy in MCI patients showed that dual-task-related gait changes are associated with altered neurochemistry (i.e., lower N-acetyl aspartate/creatine) and lower volume of both the hippocampal and primary motor cortex (Annweiler et al. 2013). Specifically, stride length variability was correlated negatively with the hippocampal neurochemistry, whereas stride time variability was correlated negatively with the volume and neurochemistry of the primary motor cortex (Annweiler et al. 2013). Mechanistically, such findings suggest that dual-task-related gait changes in MCI patients likely result from impaired frontal-hippocampal circuits that are important in spatial orientation and navigation (Montero-Odasso et al. 2014). Furthermore, these results highlight the possible involvement of primary motor cortex changes in the onset of high-level gait disorders during the prodromal stage of AD (Annweiler et al. 2013). In addition to imaging data, neuropsychological studies suggest that reduced performance in episodic memory (Montero-Odasso et al. 2014), working memory, and executive attention control (Maquet et al. 2010) may alter the ability of MCI patients to execute more than one task at a time. Particularly, reduced performances in both episodic memory and executive attention control are strongly associated with stride time variability (Maquet et al. 2010; Beauchet et al. 2014; Montero-Odasso et al. 2014). Furthermore, the meta-analysis of Beauchet et al. (2014) showed that higher stride time variability was related to both MCI and dementia. Therefore, dual-task-related changes in stride time variability could be a motor signature of MCI, and thus be potentially used as a marker of cognitive decline before and during dementia, including AD (Beauchet et al. 2014). From a neuropathological point of view, such results suggest that dual-task-related gait changes in MCI do not only result from impaired frontal-hippocampal circuits but also from fronto-striatal circuits that are important in executive attention control (Montero-Odasso et al. 2014). Hence, it is likely that both explanations are relevant to explain dual-task interference in prodromal AD. Concerning the other gait domains found in the literature, the specific underlying mechanisms are unknown. Yet, other neuropsychological studies suggest that the pace domain of gait (i.e., stride length) is mainly associated with decreased performance in both visuospatial abilities and executive attention control (Maquet et al. 2010). The recent meta-analysis of Morris et al. (2016) further suggested that the variability (i.e., stride length variability) and rhythm (i.e., stride time, stance time, and cadence) domains of gait could be associated with global cognition.

Dual-task-related gait changes in PD at the prodromal stage

LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers without a clinical diagnosis of PD (i.e., likely representing a prodromal stage of PD) decrease their speed under dual-task conditions (Mirelman et al. 2011, 2016). This gait slowing results from reduced stride length, which itself is mainly due to co-contraction of proximal muscles. Besides, stride time variability and arm swing parameters (i.e., arm swing amplitude, arm swing variability, and arm swing asymmetry) are both impaired. According to Mirelman et al. (2016), the poorer performance of the mutation carriers may be consistent with initial abnormalities in the central gait network. Notably, only arm swing parameters under dual-task conditions discriminate between LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers and healthy controls. Such findings confirm the sensitivity of dual-task walking to unmasked compensation strategies aimed at preserving the motor (gait) function when the basal ganglia system is altered. Overall, these mechanisms depend on brain regions relatively unaffected by dopamine depletion, such as the anterior striatum (Brück et al. 2006; Mounayar et al. 2007). Using fMRI in early-stage PD, Helmich et al. (2010) showed a shift in cortico-striatal connectivity from severely affected striatal regions (posterior putamen) to less affected striatal regions (anterior putamen). If these changes reflect compensation, they should also occur in the prodromal phase of PD, when a functional reorganization of cerebral circuits prevents overt clinical symptoms (Palop et al. 2006). Accordingly, asymptomatic LRRK2 G2019S mutation carriers show a reorganization of cortico-striatal circuits that mirrors findings in idiopathic PD (Helmich et al. 2015).

Future directions for clinical research

The findings of this review suggest that dual-task-related gait changes can be detected in early stages of AD and PD and can, therefore, be a marker of these diseases. However, before its use in clinical practice, some methodological concerns need clarification. First, the majority of findings reported in this review come from transversal studies. Therefore, longitudinal studies characterizing the continuum of gait profile of AD and PD from complaints to the clinical expression of the disease are needed not only to confirm findings from transversal studies but also to identify a critical threshold for dual-task-related gait changes which would allow discriminating healthy populations from pathological ones. To our knowledge, this threshold is unknown in the literature. Methodologically, in order to propose a reliable threshold, it is imperative to take into account factors modulating the capacity to cope with a concurrent cognitive load while walking, among which, the most important are age, gender, stress, processing speed (Sleimen-Malkoun et al. 2013), and cognitive reserve (i.e., the background cognitive capacity that a subject brings to a given task; Stern 2009). Applying these methodological recommendations for the conduct of future research would likely enhance the reliability of dual-task gait assessment and its large-scale application in clinical practice.

Another matter that requires further refinement and clarification is the choice of the secondary cognitive task. Although the serial subtraction task (substracting by sevens) has been reported with the highest sensitivity to challenge dual-task walking performance (Bahureksa et al. 2017), it remains non-specific to neurodegenerative process underlying AD and PD. Furthermore, this cognitive task depends on arithmetic skills, which are very heterogeneous between individuals. In our view, a way to make better use of dual-task walking paradigms is to select a secondary cognitive task that both interferes with gait control and challenges the underlying neuropathological processes (in favor of the bottleneck model) of each disease. As such, a possible solution comes from neuropsychological findings that have recently demonstrated that patients with prodromal AD are particularly deficient in tasks requiring inhibition (i.e., Stroop task; Balota et al. 2010) and patients in prodromal PD in tasks involving cognitive flexibility (i.e., task set switching; Cools et al. 2001; Lerche et al. 2014). Importantly, these executive function components are known to interfere with gait (Maquet et al. 2010; Morris et al. 2016) and begin to decrease early in the disease course. Moreover, cognitive flexibility and inhibition depend on dopamine function in the basal ganglia (Berry et al. 2016) and entorhinal cortex (Velayudhan et al. 2013), respectively. Thus, our proposition, requiring more investigation, could open new avenues to use gait as a robust marker for prediagnostic AD and PD.

In conclusion, a growing body of evidence shows that dual-task-related gait changes may be a potential marker that could increase the likelihood of early detection of AD and PD. Longitudinal studies and new dual-task walking paradigms based on the neuropathological profile of each disease are needed to better understand the dual-task interference origins and the interaction between brain damage and gait control, notably by comparing patients from complaints to disease expression (preclinical, prodromal, and clinical) from those who will remain stable.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the French National Institute of Health and Medical Research (INSERM) and the Regional Council of Basse-Normandie.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Allali G, Kressig RW, Assal F, et al. Changes in gait while backward counting in demented older adults with frontal lobe dysfunction. Gait Posture. 2007;26:572–576. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amboni M, Barone P, Hausdorff JM. Cognitive contributions to gait and falls: evidence and implications. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2013;28:1520–1533. doi: 10.1002/mds.25674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Annweiler C, Beauchet O, Bartha R, et al. Motor cortex and gait in mild cognitive impairment: a magnetic resonance spectroscopy and volumetric imaging study. Brain J Neurol. 2013;136:859–871. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bahureksa L, Najafi B, Saleh A, et al. The impact of mild cognitive impairment on gait and balance: a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies using instrumented assessment. Gerontology. 2017;63:67–83. doi: 10.1159/000445831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ballard C, Gauthier S, Corbett A, et al. Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet. 2011;377:1019–1031. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61349-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Balota DA, Tse C-S, Hutchison KA, et al. Predicting conversion to dementia of the Alzheimer’s type in a healthy control sample: the power of errors in Stroop color naming. Psychol Aging. 2010;25:208–218. doi: 10.1037/a0017474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baltadjieva R, Giladi N, Gruendlinger L, et al. Marked alterations in the gait timing and rhythmicity of patients with de novo Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:1815–1820. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05033.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barnes DE, Yaffe K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10:819–828. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70072-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beauchet O, Allali G, Berrut G, et al. Gait analysis in demented subjects: interests and perspectives. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2008;4:155–160. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S2070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Beauchet O, Allali G, Montero-Odasso M, et al. Motor phenotype of decline in cognitive performance among community-dwellers without dementia: population-based study and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2014;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Berry AS, Shah VD, Baker SL, et al. Aging affects dopaminergic neural mechanisms of cognitive flexibility. J Neurosci. 2016;36:12559–12569. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0626-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beurskens R, Bock O. Age-related deficits of dual-task walking: a review. Neural Plast. 2012;2012:131608. doi: 10.1155/2012/131608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bezard E, Fernagut P-O. Premotor parkinsonism models. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2014;20(Suppl 1):S17–S19. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8020(13)70007-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blin O, Ferrandez AM, Serratrice G. Quantitative analysis of gait in Parkinson patients: increased variability of stride length. J Neurol Sci. 1990;98:91–97. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(90)90184-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bloem BR, Grimbergen YAM, van Dijk JG, Munneke M. The “posture second” strategy: a review of wrong priorities in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci. 2006;248:196–204. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2006.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bohnen NI, Frey KA, Studenski S, et al. Gait speed in Parkinson disease correlates with cholinergic degeneration. Neurology. 2013;81:1611–1616. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182a9f558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Boripuntakul S, Lord SR, Brodie MA, et al. Spatial variability during gait initiation while dual tasking is increased in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. J Nutr Health Aging. 2014;18:307–312. doi: 10.1007/s12603-013-0390-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1991;82:239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brück A, Aalto S, Nurmi E, et al. Striatal subregional 6-[18F]fluoro-L-dopa uptake in early Parkinson’s disease: a two-year follow-up study. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2006;21:958–963. doi: 10.1002/mds.20855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Loss of motor function in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Expert Rev Neurother. 2011;11:665–676. doi: 10.1586/ern.11.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Buhmann C, Binkofski F, Klein C, et al. Motor reorganization in asymptomatic carriers of a single mutant Parkin allele: a human model for presymptomatic parkinsonism. Brain J Neurol. 2005;128:2281–2290. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Buracchio T, Dodge HH, Howieson D, et al. The trajectory of gait speed preceding mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2010;67:980–986. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Camicioli R, Bouchard T, Licis L. Dual-tasks and walking fast: relationship to extra-pyramidal signs in advanced Alzheimer disease. J Neurol Sci. 2006;248:205–209. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2006.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Camicioli R, Howieson D, Lehman S, Kaye J. Talking while walking: the effect of a dual task in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1997;48:955–958. doi: 10.1212/WNL.48.4.955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Chen H, Burton EA, Ross GW, et al. Research on the premotor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: clinical and etiological implications. Environ Health Perspect. 2013;121:1245–1252. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1306967. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cocchini G, Della Sala S, Logie RH, et al. Dual task effects of walking when talking in Alzheimer’s disease. Rev Neurol (Paris) 2004;160:74–80. doi: 10.1016/S0035-3787(04)70951-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Coelho FG, Stella F, de Andrade LP, et al. Gait and risk of falls associated with frontal cognitive functions at different stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2012;19:644–656. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2012.661398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cools R, Barker RA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Mechanisms of cognitive set flexibility in Parkinson’s disease. Brain J Neurol. 2001;124:2503–2512. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.12.2503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Doi T, Shimada H, Makizako H, et al. Cognitive function and gait speed under normal and dual-task walking among older adults with mild cognitive impairment. BMC Neurol. 2014;14:67. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-14-67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ebersbach G, Heijmenberg M, Kindermann L, et al. Interference of rhythmic constraint on gait in healthy subjects and patients with early Parkinson’s disease: evidence for impaired locomotor pattern generation in early Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 1999;14:619–625. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(199907)14:4<619::AID-MDS1011>3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fearnley JM, Lees AJ. Ageing and Parkinson’s disease: substantia nigra regional selectivity. Brain J Neurol. 1991;114(Pt 5):2283–2301. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.5.2283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fuller RL, Van Winkle EP, Anderson KE, et al. Dual task performance in Parkinson’s disease: a sensitive predictor of impairment and disability. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2013;19:325–328. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gaenslen A, Swid I, Liepelt-Scarfone I, et al. The patients’ perception of prodromal symptoms before the initial diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2011;26:653–658. doi: 10.1002/mds.23499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gillain S, Dramé M, Lekeu F, et al. Gait speed or gait variability, which one to use as a marker of risk to develop Alzheimer disease? A pilot study. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2016;28:249–255. doi: 10.1007/s40520-015-0392-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Harrington MG, Chiang J, Pogoda JM, et al. Executive function changes before memory in preclinical Alzheimer’s pathology: a prospective, cross-sectional, case control study. PLoS One. 2013;8:e79378. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hausdorff JM. Gait dynamics in Parkinson’s disease: common and distinct behavior among stride length, gait variability, and fractal-like scaling. Chaos Woodbury N. 2009;19:26113. doi: 10.1063/1.3147408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hausdorff JM, Balash J, Giladi N. Effects of cognitive challenge on gait variability in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2003;16:53–58. doi: 10.1177/0891988702250580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Heinzel S, Roeben B, Ben-Shlomo Y et al (2016) Prodromal markers in Parkinson’s disease: limitations in longitudinal studies and lessons learned. Front Aging Neurosci. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2016.00147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  39. Helmich RC, Derikx LC, Bakker M, et al. Spatial remapping of cortico-striatal connectivity in Parkinson’s disease. Cereb Cortex N Y N. 2010;20:1175–1186. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Helmich RC, Thaler A, van Nuenen BFL, et al. Reorganization of corticostriatal circuits in healthy G2019S LRRK2 carriers. Neurology. 2015;84:399–406. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hollman JH, McDade EM, Petersen RC. Normative spatiotemporal gait parameters in older adults. Gait Posture. 2011;34:111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Huang X, Mahoney JM, Lewis MM, et al. Both coordination and symmetry of arm swing are reduced in Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture. 2012;35:373–377. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.10.180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Ijmker T, Lamoth CJC. Gait and cognition: the relationship between gait stability and variability with executive function in persons with and without dementia. Gait Posture. 2012;35:126–130. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ittner LM, Götz J. Amyloid-β and tau—a toxic pas de deux in Alzheimer’s disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:65–72. doi: 10.1038/nrn2967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Jellinger KA. Neuropathology of sporadic Parkinson’s disease: evaluation and changes of concepts. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2012;27:8–30. doi: 10.1002/mds.23795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kelly VE, Eusterbrock AJ, Shumway-Cook A. A review of dual-task walking deficits in people with Parkinson’s disease: motor and cognitive contributions, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Park Dis. 2012;2012:918719. doi: 10.1155/2012/918719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kelly VE, Shumway-Cook A (2014) The ability of people with Parkinson’s disease to modify dual-task performance in response to instructions during simple and complex walking tasks. Exp Brain Res. doi:10.1007/s00221-013-3737-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  48. Kwon K-Y, Kim M, Lee S-M, et al. Is reduced arm and leg swing in Parkinson’s disease associated with rigidity or bradykinesia? J Neurol Sci. 2014;341:32–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2014.03.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lerche S, Hobert M, Brockmann K, et al. Mild parkinsonian signs in the elderly—is there an association with PD? Crossectional findings in 992 individuals. PLoS One. 2014;9:e92878. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Leverenz JB, Quinn JF, Zabetian C, et al. Cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson disease. Curr Top Med Chem. 2009;9:903–912. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lewek MD, Poole R, Johnson J, et al. Arm swing magnitude and asymmetry during gait in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture. 2010;31:256–260. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lord S, Galna B, Verghese J, et al. Independent domains of gait in older adults and associated motor and nonmotor attributes: validation of a factor analysis approach. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:820–827. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Lord S, Rochester L, Hetherington V, et al. Executive dysfunction and attention contribute to gait interference in “off” state Parkinson’s Disease. Gait Posture. 2010;31:169–174. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2009.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Maetzler W, Hausdorff JM. Motor signs in the prodromal phase of Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2012;27:627–633. doi: 10.1002/mds.24973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Maquet D, Lekeu F, Warzee E, et al. Gait analysis in elderly adult patients with mild cognitive impairment and patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease: simple versus dual task: a preliminary report. Clin Physiol Funct Imaging. 2010;30:51–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-097X.2009.00903.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Marek K, Jennings D. Can we image premotor Parkinson disease? Neurology. 2009;72:S21–S26. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318198df97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Marquis S, Moore MM, Howieson DB, et al. Independent predictors of cognitive decline in healthy elderly persons. Arch Neurol. 2002;59:601–606. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.4.601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Marshall GA, Monserratt L, Harwood D, et al. Positron emission tomography metabolic correlates of apathy in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:1015–1020. doi: 10.1001/archneur.64.7.1015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Massano J, Bhatia KP. Clinical approach to Parkinson’s disease: features, diagnosis, and principles of management. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. 2012;2:a008870. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a008870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Mayda ABV, Westphal A, Carter CS, DeCarli C. Late life cognitive control deficits are accentuated by white matter disease burden. Brain J Neurol. 2011;134:1673–1683. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. McIsaac TL, Lamberg EM, Muratori LM. Building a framework for a dual task taxonomy. Biomed Res Int. 2015;2015:591475. doi: 10.1155/2015/591475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Menant JC, Schoene D, Sarofim M, Lord SR. Single and dual task tests of gait speed are equivalent in the prediction of falls in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2014;16:83–104. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2000;31:236–250. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00040-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mielke MM, Roberts RO, Savica R, et al. Assessing the temporal relationship between cognition and gait: slow gait predicts cognitive decline in the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:929–937. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Mirek E, Rudzińska M, Szczudlik A. The assessment of gait disorders in patients with Parkinson’s disease using the three-dimensional motion analysis system Vicon. Neurol Neurochir Pol. 2007;41:128–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Mirelman A, Bernad-Elazari H, Thaler A, et al. Arm swing as a potential new prodromal marker of Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2016;31:1527–1534. doi: 10.1002/mds.26720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Mirelman A, Gurevich T, Giladi N, et al. Gait alterations in healthy carriers of the LRRK2 G2019S mutation. Ann Neurol. 2011;69:193–197. doi: 10.1002/ana.22165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Montero-Odasso M, Bergman H, Phillips NA, et al. Dual-tasking and gait in people with mild cognitive impairment. The effect of working memory. BMC Geriatr. 2009;9:41. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-9-41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Montero-Odasso M, Casas A, Hansen KT, et al. Quantitative gait analysis under dual-task in older people with mild cognitive impairment: a reliability study. J Neuroengineering Rehabil. 2009;6:35. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-6-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Montero-Odasso M, Muir SW, Speechley M. Dual-task complexity affects gait in people with mild cognitive impairment: the interplay between gait variability, dual tasking, and risk of falls. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2012;93:293–299. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.08.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Montero-Odasso M, Oteng-Amoako A, Speechley M, et al. The motor signature of mild cognitive impairment: results from the gait and brain study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69:1415–1421. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Morris ME, Iansek R, Matyas TA, Summers JJ. The pathogenesis of gait hypokinesia in Parkinson’s disease. Brain J Neurol. 1994;117(Pt 5):1169–1181. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.5.1169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Morris R, Lord S, Bunce J, et al. Gait and cognition: mapping the global and discrete relationships in ageing and neurodegenerative disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;64:326–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Morrish PK, Rakshi JS, Bailey DL, et al. Measuring the rate of progression and estimating the preclinical period of Parkinson’s disease with [18F]dopa PET. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1998;64:314–319. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.64.3.314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Mounayar S, Boulet S, Tandé D, et al. A new model to study compensatory mechanisms in MPTP-treated monkeys exhibiting recovery. Brain J Neurol. 2007;130:2898–2914. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Muir SW, Speechley M, Wells J, et al. Gait assessment in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: the effect of dual-task challenges across the cognitive spectrum. Gait Posture. 2012;35:96–100. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2011.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Müller MLTM, Bohnen NI, Kotagal V, et al. Clinical markers for identifying cholinergic deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2015;30:269–273. doi: 10.1002/mds.26061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Nakamura T, Meguro K, Sasaki H. Relationship between falls and stride length variability in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Gerontology. 1996;42:108–113. doi: 10.1159/000213780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Nascimbeni A, Caruso S, Salatino A, et al. Dual task-related gait changes in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Funct Neurol. 2015;30:59–65. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Nieuwhof F, Reelick MF, Maidan I, et al. Measuring prefrontal cortical activity during dual task walking in patients with Parkinson’s disease: feasibility of using a new portable fNIRS device. Pilot Feasibility Stud. 2016;2:59. doi: 10.1186/s40814-016-0099-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Nordahl CW, Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP, et al. White matter changes compromise prefrontal cortex function in healthy elderly individuals. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:418–429. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.3.418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Olson EJ, Boeve BF, Silber MH. Rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder: demographic, clinical and laboratory findings in 93 cases. Brain J Neurol. 2000;123(Pt 2):331–339. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. O’Shea S, Morris ME, Iansek R. Dual task interference during gait in people with Parkinson disease: effects of motor versus cognitive secondary tasks. Phys Ther. 2002;82:888–897. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Palop JJ, Chin J, Mucke L. A network dysfunction perspective on neurodegenerative diseases. Nature. 2006;443:768–773. doi: 10.1038/nature05289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Panyakaew P, Bhidayasiri R. The spectrum of preclinical gait disorders in early Parkinson’s disease: subclinical gait abnormalities and compensatory mechanisms revealed with dual tasking. J Neural Transm. 2013;120:1665–1672. doi: 10.1007/s00702-013-1051-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Pashler H. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1984;10:358–377. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pashler H. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol Bull. 1994;116:220–244. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Perrochon A, Kemoun G. The Walking Trail-Making Test is an early detection tool for mild cognitive impairment. Clin Interv Aging. 2014;9:111–119. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S53645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Perrochon A, Kemoun G, Watelain E, et al. The “stroop walking task”: an innovative dual-task for the early detection of executive function impairment. Neurophysiol Clin Clin Neurophysiol. 2015;45:181–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2015.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Perrochon A, Kemoun G, Watelain E, Berthoz A. Walking Stroop carpet: an innovative dual-task concept for detecting cognitive impairment. Clin Interv Aging. 2013;8:317–328. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S38667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Petersen RC. Early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: is MCI too late? Curr Alzheimer Res. 2009;6:324–330. doi: 10.2174/156720509788929237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Pettersson AF, Olsson E, Wahlund L-O. Motor function in subjects with mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;19:299–304. doi: 10.1159/000084555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Pettersson AF, Olsson E, Wahlund L-O. Effect of divided attention on gait in subjects with and without cognitive impairment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2007;20:58–62. doi: 10.1177/0891988706293528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Plotnik M, Giladi N, Dagan Y, Hausdorff JM. Postural instability and fall risk in Parkinson’s disease: impaired dual tasking, pacing, and bilateral coordination of gait during the “ON” medication state. Exp Brain Res. 2011;210:529–538. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2551-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Plotnik M, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Bilateral coordination of gait and Parkinson’s disease: the effects of dual tasking. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80:347–350. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.157362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Poldrack RA, Sabb FW, Foerde K, et al. The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. J Neurosci. 2005;25:5356–5364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3880-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Postuma RB, Aarsland D, Barone P, et al. Identifying prodromal Parkinson’s disease: pre-motor disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2012;27:617–626. doi: 10.1002/mds.24996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Reelick MF, Kessels RPC, Faes MC, et al. Increased intra-individual variability in stride length and reaction time in recurrent older fallers. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2011;23:393–399. doi: 10.1007/BF03337764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Rochester L, Galna B, Lord S, Burn D. The nature of dual-task interference during gait in incident Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience. 2014;265:83–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.01.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Rochester L, Hetherington V, Jones D, et al. Attending to the task: interference effects of functional tasks on walking in Parkinson’s disease and the roles of cognition, depression, fatigue, and balance. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2004;85:1578–1585. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.01.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Rochester L, Nieuwboer A, Baker K, et al. Walking speed during single and dual tasks in Parkinson’s disease: which characteristics are important? Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2008;23:2312–2318. doi: 10.1002/mds.22219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Rolls ET, Grabenhorst F. The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: from affect to decision-making. Prog Neurobiol. 2008;86:216–244. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Rucco R, Agosti V, Jacini F, et al. Spatio-temporal and kinematic gait analysis in patients with Frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease through 3D motion capture. Gait Posture. 2017;52:312–317. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Sabatini U, Boulanouar K, Fabre N, et al. Cortical motor reorganization in akinetic patients with Parkinson’s disease: a functional MRI study. Brain J Neurol. 2000;123(Pt 2):394–403. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Samuel M, Ceballos-Baumann AO, Blin J, et al. Evidence for lateral premotor and parietal overactivity in Parkinson’s disease during sequential and bimanual movements. A PET study Brain J Neurol. 1997;120(Pt 6):963–976. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.6.963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Schaafsma JD, Giladi N, Balash Y, et al. Gait dynamics in Parkinson’s disease: relationship to Parkinsonian features, falls and response to levodopa. J Neurol Sci. 2003;212:47–53. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(03)00104-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Seo EH, Kim H, Lee KH, Choo IH. Altered executive function in pre-mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis JAD. 2016;54:933–940. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Sheridan MR, Flowers KA. Movement variability and bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease. Brain J Neurol. 1990;113(Pt 4):1149–1161. doi: 10.1093/brain/113.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Sheridan PL, Hausdorff JM. The role of higher-level cognitive function in gait: executive dysfunction contributes to fall risk in Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24:125–137. doi: 10.1159/000105126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Sheridan PL, Solomont J, Kowall N, Hausdorff JM. Influence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1633–1637. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51516.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Sleimen-Malkoun R, Temprado J-J, Berton E. Age-related dedifferentiation of cognitive and motor slowing: insight from the comparison of Hick-Hyman and Fitts’ laws. Front Aging Neurosci. 2013;5:62. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Smith ML, Milner B. The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial location. Neuropsychologia. 1981;19:781–793. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(81)90090-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Desloovere K, et al. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: the impact of dual-tasking and turning. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2010;25:2563–2570. doi: 10.1002/mds.23327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Stegemöller EL, Wilson JP, Hazamy A, et al. Associations between cognitive and gait performance during single- and dual-task walking in people with Parkinson disease. Phys Ther. 2014;94:757–766. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20130251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Sterling NW, Cusumano JP, Shaham N, et al. Dopaminergic modulation of arm swing during gait among Parkinson’s disease patients. J Park Dis. 2015;5:141–150. doi: 10.3233/JPD-140447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Stevens-Lapsley J, Kluger BM, Schenkman M. Quadriceps muscle weakness, activation deficits, and fatigue with Parkinson disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2012;26:533–541. doi: 10.1177/1545968311425925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Stoessl AJ. Positron emission tomography in premotor Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2007;13:S421–S424. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8020(08)70041-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Strouwen C, Molenaar EALM, Keus SHJ, et al. Are factors related to dual-task performance in people with Parkinson’s disease dependent on the type of dual task? Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2016;23:23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Suvà D, Favre I, Kraftsik R, et al. Primary motor cortex involvement in Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1999;58:1125–1134. doi: 10.1097/00005072-199911000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Svehlík M, Zwick EB, Steinwender G, et al. Gait analysis in patients with Parkinson’s disease off dopaminergic therapy. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2009;90:1880–1886. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2009.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Taniguchi Y, Yoshida H, Fujiwara Y, et al. A prospective study of gait performance and subsequent cognitive decline in a general population of older Japanese. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2012;67:796–803. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Tarnanas I, Papagiannopoulos S, Kazis D, et al. Reliability of a novel serious game using dual-task gait profiles to early characterize aMCI. Front Aging Neurosci. 2015;7:50. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Terry RD, Masliah E, Salmon DP, et al. Physical basis of cognitive alterations in Alzheimer’s disease: synapse loss is the major correlate of cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol. 1991;30:572–580. doi: 10.1002/ana.410300410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Tolosa E, Wenning G, Poewe W. The diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5:75–86. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70285-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Tombu M, Jolicoeur P. A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2003;29:3–18. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Tseng BY, Cullum CM, Zhang R. Older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment exhibit exacerbated gait slowing under dual-task challenges. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2014;11:494–500. doi: 10.2174/1567205011666140505110828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Tseng I-J, Jeng C, Yuan R-Y. Comparisons of forward and backward gait between poorer and better attention capabilities in early Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture. 2012;36:367–371. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.03.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Valkanova V, Ebmeier KP. What can gait tell us about dementia? Review of epidemiological and neuropsychological evidence. Gait Posture. 2017;53:215–223. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. van Iersel MB, Hoefsloot W, Munneke M, et al. Systematic review of quantitative clinical gait analysis in patients with dementia. Z Gerontol Geriatr. 2004;37:27–32. doi: 10.1007/s00391-004-0176-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Velayudhan L, Proitsi P, Westman E, et al. Entorhinal cortex thickness predicts cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis JAD. 2013;33:755–766. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2012-121408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Verghese J, Robbins M, Holtzer R, et al. Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment syndromes. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1244–1251. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01758.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, et al. Quantitative gait dysfunction and risk of cognitive decline and dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78:929–935. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.106914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Vervoort G, Heremans E, Bengevoord A, et al. Dual-task-related neural connectivity changes in patients with Parkinson’ disease. Neuroscience. 2016;317:36–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.12.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Volpe D, Pavan D, Morris M, et al. Underwater gait analysis in Parkinson’s disease. Gait Posture. 2017;52:87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Wild LB, de Lima DB, Balardin JB, et al. Characterization of cognitive and motor performance during dual-tasking in healthy older adults and patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol. 2013;260:580–589. doi: 10.1007/s00415-012-6683-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture. 2002;16:1–14. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00156-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Wu T, Hallett M, Chan P. Motor automaticity in Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol Dis. 2015;82:226–234. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2015.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Wu T, Kansaku K, Hallett M. How self-initiated memorized movements become automatic: a functional MRI study. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91:1690–1698. doi: 10.1152/jn.01052.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, et al. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:1248–1256. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04298.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Yogev G, Plotnik M, Peretz C, et al. Gait asymmetry in patients with Parkinson’s disease and elderly fallers: when does the bilateral coordination of gait require attention? Exp Brain Res. 2007;177:336–346. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0676-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. Do we always prioritize balance when walking? Towards an integrated model of task prioritization. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2012;27:765–770. doi: 10.1002/mds.24963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Yogev-Seligmann G, Rotem-Galili Y, Mirelman A, et al. How does explicit prioritization alter walking during dual-task performance? Effects of age and sex on gait speed and variability. Phys Ther. 2010;90:177–186. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from GeroScience are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES