Abstract
Starting perceptual training at easy levels before progressing to difficult levels generally produces better learning outcomes than constantly difficult training. However, little is known about how “easy” these initial levels should be in order to yield easy-to-hard effects. We compared five levels of initial training block difficulty varying from very easy to hard in two auditory discrimination learning tasks, a frequency modulation rate discrimination (Experiment 1) and a frequency range discrimination (Experiment 2). The degree of difficulty was based on individualized pre-training ~71% correct discrimination thresholds. Both experiments revealed a sweet-spot for easy-to-hard effects. Conditions where initial blocks were either too easy or too difficult produced less benefit than blocks of intermediate difficulty. Results challenge assumptions that sequencing effects in learning are related to attentional-spotlighting of task-relevant dimensions. Rather, they support incremental learning models that account for easy-to-hard effects. Further, the results have implications for how perceptual training regimens should be designed to maximize the benefits of rehabilitative perceptual training.
Keywords: Fading, Progressive Training, Transfer Along a Continuum, Adaptive Training
Introduction
Learning can impact the way we experience the world by changing how we process the features of stimuli that reside in it (Goldstone, 1998). Examples of this perceptual learning include how non-native accents become easier to understand after exposure (Van Engen & Peelle, 2014) and how training to become a wine expert allows one to notice details that a novice misses (James, 1890). Learning is generally more effective if training starts off at an easy level of difficulty before moving to hard perceptual problems. For instance, both rats and humans learn a difficult auditory identification task better if they start with easy trials than if they complete difficult trials throughout training (Liu, Mercado, Church, & Orduña, 2008). This phenomenon has been referred to as the easy-to-hard effect. It occurs in several modalities and species (for review, see Wisniewski, Radell, Church, & Mercado, 2017).
One account of easy-to-hard effects is that initial easy trials serve to direct learners’ attention to relevant dimensions. Once the “attentional spotlight” is placed on the most relevant dimension, learners perceive that dimension more minutely. Pashler and Mozer (2013) found easy-to-hard effects with participants categorizing face-like “demon” stimuli by horn height. If participants were told that horn height was critical, the easy-to-hard effect disappeared. A single exposure that directs attention to aspects of an image needed to perceive an object (e.g., a face masked by noise) can also yield stable and long-lasting changes in perception (Ahissar & Hochstein, 2004). Such “Eureka” effects have been used as support for theories that explain perceptual learning with attentional-spotlighting mechanisms (Ahissar & Hochstein, 2004). Formal models that allow “attentional stretching” of dimensions can account for many instances of easy-to-hard effects as well as other effects of stimulus sequencing (e.g., Carvalho & Goldstone, 2015; Krushcke, 1992; Sutherland & Mackintosh, 1971).
Incremental associative and/or representational modification-based learning processes may additionally drive easy-to-hard effects. The classic gradient interaction theory of Spence (1937) proposes that positive excitatory gradients of generalization develop around reinforced stimuli, while negative inhibitory gradients surround non-reinforced stimuli. An individual’s ability to discriminate is governed by the summation of these gradients. If the gradients are too overlapping (as when a reinforced S+ stimulus is difficult to discriminate from a non-reinforced S−), they will mostly cancel each other out and learning will proceed slowly. If the gradients are more separated, but still overlapping, their summation will produce a stronger difference between S+ and S− that will generalize from the easy to the hard version of the task (also see McLaren & Mackintosh, 2002). Non-associative representational modification-based models account for easy-to-hard effects by taking into consideration how stimulus sequencing affects the plasticity of stimulus representations. When two stimuli are very similar, they will compete to change the same representational space (e.g., neurons coding for the same frequencies of sound). In this case, learning proceeds slowly. When two stimuli are easier to discriminate, competition is reduced, thus a more accurate representation of each stimulus can be developed and later refined on the hard version of the task (Saksida, 1999).
The debate between attentional-spotlighting and incremental learning theories continues. In one recent study, people were trained to discriminate auditory frequency modulation (FM) rates in two different frequency ranges (300 – 600 Hz or 3000 – 6000 Hz; Wisniewski, et al., 2017). In one frequency range, difficulty faded from easy-to-hard. In the other, discriminations remained constantly difficult. Even though the same dimension (FM rate) was relevant in both frequency ranges, participants performed better in the range that received easy-to-hard training. However, other work has failed to find easy-to-hard benefits when attentional-spotlighting effects are kept to a minimum (Pashler & Mozer, 2013). Those authors concluded that easy-to-hard effects should only manifest in relatively high-level perceptual category learning tasks where the learner must first figure out which dimensions are relevant. In some cases, simple exposure to stimuli in an easy-to-hard progression can benefit learning, suggesting that an attention independent non-associative learning process is at play (Church, Mercado, Wisniewski, & Liu, 2013; Sanjuán, Nelson, & Alonso, 2014). In a similar vein, enhancement of auditory-evoked potentials (AEPs) is larger after easy-to-hard compared to constantly difficult training (Orduña, Liu, Church, Eddins, & Mercado, 2012). This is the case even when participants are asked to ignore sounds during AEP collection (Orduña et al., 2012). However, it could be argued that exposure to easily discriminable contrasts serves as a cue for directing attention to specific dimensions even when there is no task involved, and that participants are paying attention to stimuli during passive AEP collection despite being instructed not to do so.
Here, we pit yet-to-be tested predictions of these two classes of theory against each other in a relatively low-level auditory task. Incremental learning processes are constrained by the degree to which initial trials are “easy”. If initial trials contain very distinct non-overlapping representations, generalization from those easy trials to harder trials will be minimal. For instance, gradients of excitation and inhibition will be too disimilar to yield strong gradient interaction at the points of the hard S+/S− discrimination. From the view of non-associative representational modification accounts, extremely easy trials make it less likely that representations will be refined that can be usefully modified on a harder version of the task. On the contrary, attentional-spotlighting accounts predict that easy trials should facilitate performance as long as the discrimination-relevant dimension is made obvious.
Experiment 1
Five groups of individuals were trained to discriminate rates of frequency modulation (FM). Groups were either trained constantly at their predetermined 71% correct threshold (hard - H) or faded from easier contrasts to their threshold (easy-to-hard - EH). There were four different EH conditions differing in the degree to which initial easy trials were easy: EH1 (most difficult) - EH4 (easiest). If easy-to-hard effects are exclusively related to learning the discrimination-relevant dimension, then there should be a monotonic trend of increasing post-training performance from H to EH4 or no effect of training due to a “Eureka” experience in the thresholding phase. However, incremental theories predict that there should be a sweet spot where fading benefits are observed somewhere between the H and EH4 conditions.
Methods
We designated data collection and analysis plans prior to running experiments. The a priori plans, an annotated post experiment file, and the raw data can be downloaded from www.alclaboratory.com/opendata.
Participants
Seventy individuals at Kansas State University participated in exchange for course credit. All signed a consent form approved by the local IRB and reported normal hearing. Fourteen participants were randomly assigned to each of the conditions.
Stimuli and Apparatus
Single upward directed FM sweeps (1.5 – 3 kHz) served as stimuli (see Figure 1a for spectrograms of select stimuli). FM sweeps were rendered online in MATLAB 2018b (Mathworks, Natick, MA) along with experimental procedures. A 5 octaves/s sweep served as a standard “Slow” rate. All other rates were faster.
Figure 1.
(a) Spectrograms of select FM sweep stimuli at rates from 5 octaves/s to 18 octaves/s. (b) Depiction of FM rates that would be presented under each condition for a participant with a threshold of .5 octaves/s.
Neutral valence silent videos were downloaded from videos.pexels.com under a creative commons zero license. A random video was selected after each block and played during a forced 30 s break. This served to mitigate overwhelming listeners with constant auditory stimulation.
Participants sat in sound attenuating booths (WhisperRoom, Knoxville, TN) and heard stimuli over Sennheiser HD-280 closed back headphones (Sennheiser, Germany) connected to Focusrite Scarlett external sound cards (Focusrite, United Kingdom). Stimuli were presented at ~81 dB SPL. Responses were made on custom keypads (P.I., Engineering, Williamston, MI).
Procedures
A two-interval two-alternative forced-choice (2i-2afc) task was used to identify ~71% correct thresholds in an initial 60 trial block. On each trial, two FM sounds were played, separated by a 500 ms inter-stimulus interval. One was always the standard. The other was faster (order selected at random on each trial). Participants’ task was to indicate which sound was slower. The FM rate difference started at 4 octaves/s on trial 1. That is, the “slow” rate was 5 octaves/s, and the “fast” was 9 octaves/s. Using a one-up, two-down procedure (Levitt, 1970), the difference was made larger by dividing by 0.9 after every incorrect response and was made smaller by multiplying by 0.9 after every two consecutive correct responses. The mean octaves/s difference on the last ten trials of the block was taken as a participant’s threshold.
Participants then proceeded to 1i-2afc training. Participants were asked to indicate whether a single FM sweep was “fast” or “slow”. Participants were informed that 50% of sounds would be “fast” and 50% would be “slow”. Responses were not registered until the sound completed playing. Feedback of correctness was presented after responding. If a response was not made within a 5-s window following sound offset, a missing response was recorded. These trials were not included in the analysis. A pseudorandom trial order was used such that the same stimulus could not occur more than 4 times consecutively.
Group H completed 4 blocks of training, all at threshold. Group EH1 had the 1st block of training at 2.25 times threshold, and the 2nd training block at 1.625 times threshold. First and second block training levels for groups EH2, EH3, and EH4 were 3.5 and 2.25, 6 and 3.5, and 11 and 6, respectively. Training blocks 3 and 4 were at threshold for EH groups 1–4, just as the H group. Figure 1 shows an example of the progressions of FM rates that would be used for training blocks 1–4 for a hypothetical individual with a 0.5 octaves/s threshold under each of the conditions. Training blocks were all 32 trials long.
After training, all participants completed an 80-trial test at threshold. The task was identical to training except that no feedback was given after responding. Test performance served as the critical variable for our hypotheses.
Results and Discussion
The mean difference threshold across all individuals was .56 octaves/s (SD=.27). There were no significant differences in thresholds across conditions, F<2.
A’ was used as a measure of accuracy in training and testing, computed using equation 1 when hit rate (H) was greater than the false alarm rate (F), and equation 2 otherwise.
(1) |
(2) |
Figure 2a shows A’ for the training blocks. Unsurprisingly, the initial 2 training blocks showed different accuracies across the conditions. The H condition showed the lowest performance as the first two blocks were at ~71% thresholds. For the EH groups, there was higher accuracy for EH4 compared to EH1, with the others were in between. Accuracy in the 3rd and 4th blocks of training was comparable across conditions.
Figure 2.
(a) Accuracy (A′) across training blocks for all groups in Experiment 1. Error bars show standard error of the mean (SEM). (b) Accuracy (A′) in the test for all groups. Error bars show SEM. (c) Linear and quadratic contrasts. Error bars show 90% confidence intervals according to a null hypothesis distribution generated using a permutation procedure (see text).
The lowest level of test performance was observed for the H condition, while the highest performance was observed for the EH2 condition (Figure 2b). Planned orthogonal linear (attentional spotlighting prediction; coefficients = −2, −1, 0, 1, 2) and quadratic contrasts (incremental prediction; coefficients = 2, −1, - 2, −1, 2) were computed for the trends in means across conditions H to EH4. These contrasts were assessed for significance using a nonparametric permutation-based procedure. For 1000 iterations, condition labels were randomly shuffled and a linear (ψlinear) and quadratic (ψquadratic) contrast statistic was computed. These permuted ψ values created a null hypothesis distribution. A p-value was considered to be the proportion of ψ values in the distribution that exceeded that of the actual data (α=.05).
Figure 2c depicts the observed linear and quadratic contrast statistics along with 90% confidence intervals for 1-sided hypotheses (derived from obtained null hypothesis distribution). The linear contrast was not significant, p=.448. The quadratic contrast was, p=.029, demonstrating that intermediate levels of difficulty for initial easy trials led to better test accuracy than levels that were too difficult (H condition) or too easy (EH4 condition).
Experiment 2
Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1 except that stimuli varied on the dimension of frequency range rather than FM rate. The purpose was to test whether or not Experiment 1 effects could be replicated for a different acoustic dimension.
Methods
With one exception, the data collection and analysis plans were identical to Experiment 1. Instead of using FM rate difference threshold as a drop criterion, a threshold of 2 semitones frequency range difference was used.
Participants
Seventy-three individuals enrolled in courses at Kansas State University participated in exchange for credit. All signed a consent form approved by Kansas State University’s IRB. All participants self-reported normal hearing. Fourteen participants were initially randomly assigned to each of the conditions. Three participants were dropped due to high pitch discrimination thresholds (>2 semitones) and were replaced.
Stimuli and Apparatus
A standard “low” pitched stimulus was identical to the “slow” stimulus in Experiment 1 (5 octaves/s; 1.5 – 3 kHz frequency range). All other stimuli spanned a higher frequency range.
Procedures
The thresholding task was similar to Experiment 1 except that the participant’s task was to indicate which sound was lower in pitch. Frequency range difference started at 2.16 semitones. The standard “low” sound swept from 1.5 to 3 kHz, while the “high” sound swept from 1.7 kHz to 3.4 kHz. The difference was made larger by dividing the difference in Hz by 0.9 after every incorrect response and made smaller by multiplying by 0.9 after every two consecutive correct responses. The mean difference on the last ten trials of the block was taken as a participant’s ~71% correct threshold.
Training and testing procedures were the same as Experiment 1, except that the stimulus differences were based off thresholds for the frequency range of FM sweeps rather than rate. Also, the task was to indicate whether a sound was “high” or “low” rather than “fast” or “slow”.
Results and Discussion
The mean threshold across participants was .49 semitones (SD=.38). There were no significant differences across conditions, F<2. As in Experiment 1, the EH4 group performed best and the H group performed worst for the first two blocks of training (see Figure 3a). The other EH groups were in between. There also appeared to be a trend in blocks 3 and 4 for higher accuracy in the EH1–3 groups compared to the others. This is consistent with a nonmonotonic trend in accuracy across degree of initial block easiness. We turn next to the test data, as this data was critical to evaluating the competing hypotheses.
Figure 3.
(a) Accuracy (A′) across training blocks for all groups. In Experiment 2 Error bars show standard error of the mean (SEM). (b) Accuracy (A′) in the test for all groups. Error bars show SEM. (c) Linear and quadratic contrasts. Error bars show 90% confidence intervals according to a null hypothesis distribution generated using a permutation procedure (see text).
Figure 3b shows A’ in the test. Much like Experiment 1, the data show a quadratic trend from H to EH4 such that conditions where initial trials were too easy or too difficult showed lower accuracy than conditions with intermediate levels of difficulty for the first two blocks. The same statistics used in Experiment 1 were employed in the analysis of Experiment 2 (Figure 3c). The linear trend was not significant, p=.311. The quadratic contrast was significant, p=.008, as predicted by incremental theories of learning.
General Discussion
In two experiments, we pitted predictions of attentional-spotlighting and incremental learning theories against each other in the context of easy-to-hard effects. We found a sweet spot for easy-to-hard effects in auditory learning such that training protocols where initial blocks are too easy or too difficult produce less benefit than blocks of intermediate difficulty. This result was observed for two different acoustic dimensions, was predicted by incremental accounts of learning, and runs counter to predictions of attentional-spotlighting. It is also worth noting that discrimination thresholds on the relevant dimension for every listener were collected before training. Listeners were able to reach reasonable thresholds, suggesting they had knowledge of the relevant dimension before ever starting training. Easy-to-hard effects observed as a result of training are thus unlikely to come from a “Eureka” experience (Ahissar & Hochstein, 2004) or dimensional discovery (Pashler & Mozer, 2013).
It appears that attentional spotlighting is not a sufficient explanation of easy-to-hard effects on its own. Others have come to similar conclusions based on recent work in category learning. Lee and Livesey (2018) trained participants to categorize circles that varied in color and size such that paying attention to a single dimension was encouraged. Afterwards, participants were tested either in a condition where they could consistently apply a rule based on the relevant dimension (e.g., small circles are Category A, but large circles are Category B), or in a condition where such a rule led to inconsistent performance. Participants tested in the rule-consistent condition showed generalization patterns that were indicative of rule use on the relevant dimension (e.g., size). However, the inconsistent group showed generalization patterns more consistent with incremental learning processes (also, see Wisniewski, Radell, Guillette, Sturdy, & Mercado, 2012; Wisniewski, Church, & Mercado, 2014).
The recent work of Lee and Livesey (2018) combined with the results reported here suggest an interesting possibility for why attentional spotlighting has remained such a favored account of easy-to-hard effects: strong attentional spotlighting effects have the potential to mask effects of other learning processes. Easy-to-hard effects unlikely to be related to attentional spotlighting are generally smaller (e.g., Cohen’s d = 0.90 for Experiment 1; Liu et al., 2008) compared to the effect of knowing the relevant dimension(s) (e.g., Cohen’s d = 2.58 for Experiment 5; Pashler & Mozer, 2013). At the same time, selection of suboptimal “easy” levels may lead to weak easy-to-hard effects generated by incremental learning processes, making this discrepancy even larger. Many models account for sequencing effects through adjustments to a parameter that creates “attentional stretching” of a dimension by reducing stimulus similarity (Sequential Attention Theory; Carvalho & Goldstone, 2015; ALCOVE; Kruschke, 1992; Analyzer Theory; Sutherland & Mackintosh, 1971). Often, other models (e.g., Saksida, 1999; McLaren & Mackintosh, 2002) or model components that do not allude to attention are ignored or quickly abandoned as explanations of sequencing. We do not protest the idea that attentional spotlighting contributes to sequencing effects in learning, only to the notion that attentional spotlighting is a sufficient explanation on its own. Models that take into consideration attentional states, along with incremental learning processes, will do a better job of simulating easy-to-hard effects and perceptual learning in general.
Largely based on the assumption that initial easy trials help learners discover relevant dimensions, adaptive training procedures that start at very easy levels are used extensively in perceptual learning studies. Some have suggested that the most effective way to train individuals should be to use initial easy trials until an individual discovers the relevant dimension, then switch to constantly difficult trials that allow for most accurate tweaking of category boundaries (e.g., Pashler & Mozer, 2013). This is somewhat in line with how adaptive procedures work, where the majority of trials are at an individual’s asymptotic performance. The current data highlights the need for empirically-based selection of perceptual training regimens. Though comparisons of different adaptive procedures sometimes show little to no effects of varying conditions (Amitay et al., 2006), future work should compare the effectiveness of adaptive training procedures to those of fixed levels of progression selected to optimize learning (e.g., by designing initial trials to be in between too easy and too difficult). Since incremental learning processes are also impacted by sequencing, it is further important to examine stimulus sequencing effects under conditions of exposure (cf. Church et al., 2013; Sanjuan et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2010), latent learning between blocks or sessions (Molloy, Moore, Sohoglu, & Amitay, 2012), and when performance depends on generalizing across training-irrelevant dimensions and/or stimulus characteristics (Wisniewski, Liu, Church, & Mercado, 2014; Wisniewski, Mantell, & Pfordresher, 2013). It will also be necessary to examine whether initial trial ease affects learning in other modalities the same way.
Consistent with incremental based learning theory explanations of easy-to-hard effects, we found a sweet spot for initial trial ease in two different auditory tasks. Considering incremental learning as well as attentional-spotlighting explanations of sequencing effects in training will help advance theory, as well as lead to more effective training procedures.
Open Practices Statement
A pre-experiment plan, annotated post-experiment file, and the raw data are available at www.alclaboratory.com/opendata.
Acknowledgments
Kelsey Wheeler helped with data collection. This project was supported by a grant from the National Institute of General Medical Science GM113109 of the National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This Author Accepted Manuscript is a PDF file of a an unedited peer-reviewed manuscript that has been accepted for publication but has not been copyedited or corrected. The official version of record that is published in the journal is kept up to date and so may therefore differ from this version.
References
- Ahissar M, & Hochstein S (2004). The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(10), 457–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amitay S, Irwin A, Hawkey DJ, Cowan JA, & Moore DR (2006). A comparison of adaptive procedures for rapid and reliable threshold assessment and training in naive listeners. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(3), 1616–1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carvalho PF, & Goldstone RL (2015). The benefits of interleaved and blocked study: Different tasks benefit from different schedules of study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(1), 281–288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Church BA, Mercado E III, Wisniewski MG, & Liu EH (2013). Temporal dynamics in auditory perceptual learning: Impact of sequencing and incidental learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(1), 270–276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James WJ (1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York, NY: Dover. [Google Scholar]
- Kruschke JK (1992). ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychological Review, 99(1), 22–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee JC, & Livesey EJ (2018). Rule-based generalization and peak shift in the presence of simple relational rules. PloS One, 13(9), e0203805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levitt H (1970). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 467–477. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaren IPL, & Mackintosh NJ (2002). Associative learning and elemental representation: II. Generalization and discrimination. Animal Learning & Behavior, 30(3), 177–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaren IPL, & Suret M (2000). Transfer along a continuum: Differentiation or association? In Gleitman LR & Joshi AK (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Second Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 340–345), Philadelphia, PA. [Google Scholar]
- McLaren IPL, Kaye H, & Mackintosh NJ (1989). An associative theory of the representation of stimuli: Applications to perceptual learning and latent inhibition In Morris RGM (Ed.), Parallel distributed processing: Implications for psychology and neurobiology (pp. 102–130). New York, NY: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Molloy K, Moore DR, Sohoglu E, & Amitay S (2012). Less is more: latent learning is maximized by shorter training sessions in auditory perceptual learning. PloS One, 7(5), e36929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orduña I, Liu EH, Church BA, Eddins AC, & Mercado E III (2012). Evoked-potential changes following discrimination learning involving complex sounds. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123(4), 711–719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pashler H, & Mozer MC (2013). When does fading enhance perceptual category learning?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(4), 1162–1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saksida LM (1999). Effects of similarity and experience on discrimination learning: a nonassociative connectionist model of perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 25(3), 308–323. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanjuan MC, Nelson JB, & Alonso G (2014). An easy-to-hard effect after nonreinforced preexposure in a sweetness discrimination. Learning & Behavior, 42(3), 209–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spence KW (1937). The differential response in animals to stimuli varying within a single dimension. Psychological Review, 44, 430–444. [Google Scholar]
- Spiering BJ, & Ashby FG (2008). Response processes in information-integration category learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90(2), 330–338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutherland NS, & Mackintosh NJ (1971). Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Van Engen KJ, & Peelle JE (2014). Listening effort and accented speech. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisniewski MG, Church BA, & Mercado E (2009). Learning-related shifts in generalization gradients for complex sounds. Learning & Behavior, 37(4), 325–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisniewski MG, Church BA, & Mercado E III (2014). Individual differences during acquisition predict shifts in generalization. Behavioural Processes, 104, 26–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisniewski MG, Radell ML, Guillette LM, Sturdy CB, & Mercado E (2012). Predicting shifts in generalization gradients with perceptrons. Learning & Behavior, 40(2), 128–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisniewski MG, Mantell JT, & Pfordresher PQ (2013). Transfer effects in the vocal imitation of speech and song. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 23(2), 82–99. [Google Scholar]
- Wisniewski MG, Liu EH, Church BA, & Mercado E III (2014). Learning to discriminate frequency modulation rate can benefit and worsen pitch acuity. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 135(2), EL55–EL60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisniewski MG, Radell ML, Church BA, & Mercado E III (2017). Benefits of fading in perceptual learning are driven by more than dimensional attention. PloS One, 12(7), e0180959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright BA, Sabin AT, Zhang Y, Marrone N, & Fitzgerald MB (2010). Enhancing perceptual learning by combining practice with periods of additional sensory stimulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 12868–12877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]