Skip to main content
Food Science & Nutrition logoLink to Food Science & Nutrition
. 2019 Aug 9;7(9):3042–3051. doi: 10.1002/fsn3.1162

The relationship between dietary micronutrients intake and cognition test performance among school‐aged children in government‐owned primary schools in Kumasi metropolis, Ghana

Reginald Adjetey Annan 1,, Charles Apprey 1, Odeafo Asamoah‐Boakye 1, Satoru Okonogi 2, Taro Yamauchi 3, Takeshi Sakurai 2
PMCID: PMC6766589  PMID: 31572597

Abstract

Nutrients are critical for optimal brain development, and good nutritional status is associated with cognitive development and improvement. The relationship between micronutrients intake and cognition in Ghanaian school‐aged children has not been studied. The study investigated dietary intakes of micronutrients and cognition test performance of school‐aged children. A cross‐sectional study was undertaken among 438 school children, aged 9–13 years from ten randomly selected basic schools in Kumasi, Ghana. Socio‐demographic data were obtained from a structured questionnaire. Dietary intakes of iron, zinc, vitamin B6, folate, vitamin B12, and vitamin A were determined from repeated 24‐hr dietary recall data from 351 children, while cognition test was performed using a Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM), a 36‐question test. Among 351 children, 156 (44.4%) had inadequate zinc intake, whereas 96 (27.4%) had inadequate iron intake. More than 1 in 2 children had inadequate vitamin A intake while 55.8% and 53.0% had inadequate vitamin B12 and folate intakes, respectively. More school‐aged boys (66.3%) than girls (46.8%) had inadequate vitamin B12 intake (χ 2 = 13.393, p < .001), while for iron, folate, vitamin B6, zinc, and vitamin A, the differences were not significant. Mean RCPM test score differed significantly between school type (p < .001), but did not differ between the different ages, and between children with adequate and inadequate iron, zinc, vitamin B12, vitamin B6, and vitamin A intakes, except for folate intake (p = .050). Weak positive significant associations were observed between RCPM test score and zinc and folate intakes (p = .050). Dietary micronutrient intakes were inadequate in majority of these children, which put them at risk of weakened immune system and poor health, but did not show significant associations with RCPM performance. Further studies using other forms of cognition tests may help confirm our findings, and provide the impetus for the necessary interventions.

Keywords: cognition tests, dietary intakes, micronutrients, school‐aged children

1. INTRODUCTION

Proper brain function is a requirement for efficient cognitive function, and disruption of the brain reduces its efficiency (Bellisle, 2004). Good nutrition is necessary (Bryan, Osendarp, Hughes, & Baghurst, 2004), for brain function and cognitive performance, and thus should be adequately provided at all times (Bellisle, 2004). Poor nutritional status can indeed adversely affect brain function and cognitive performance (Bellisle, 2004). The proportion of children who do not obtain their full developmental potential is considerably large, and efforts to identify the specific causes of poor developmental outcomes are relevant (Peet et al., 2015). Micronutrient deficiencies are common among many developing countries, particularly among children, partly because of their higher physiological requirements and lower consumption of nutrient‐rich foods (Thankachan et al., 2013). The consumption of monotonous diets that are low in animal products and rich in phytates is the potential causes of deficiencies in iron, zinc, vitamin A, and vitamin B12 (Thankachan et al., 2013). New estimates show that nearly 250 million children under 5 years from low‐ and middle‐income countries are at risk of not reaching their developmental potential, partly due to poor nutrition during pregnancy and the first 2 years of life (Black et al., 2017). Therefore, poor micronutrients intake and nutrition in general, causing poor cognitive development, are among the reasons for many children not reaching their developmental potential (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mean cognitive test score between gender, and level of micronutrients intake. Comparison of mean cognitive score is all not significant, except for gender (p < .001) and folate intake (p = .05)

Cognition is the ability to assimilate and process information received from different sources and to convert them into knowledge (Monti, Moulton, & Cohen, 2015). Cognitive processes include learning, attention, memory, language, reasoning, and decision‐making (Monti et al., 2015). Children especially of school age, require proper cognition for higher and excellent academic performance (Yehuda, Rabinovitzm, & Mostofskym, 2006). Most cognitive processes and activities are highly associated with brain function, physiology, and structure (Gomez‐Pinilla, 2008), and thus, its development may be affected by nutrition, physical activity, and social and economic status (Lemaire et al., 2010). During primary school education, deficiencies of vitamin A, iron, vitamin B12, vitamin B6, zinc, and folate among children can cause increased morbidity and negatively affect classroom attention and cognitive performance and thus devastate optimal schooling and academic performance (Fiorentino et al., 2017).

Vitamin B12 is an important micronutrient found only in animal food sources and is required for complete brain development and cognition (Rathod, Kale, & Joshi, 2016). Several studies conducted in developing countries have reported a high prevalence of vitamin B12 deficiency in children and attributed to inadequate vitamin B12 intake (Finkelstein, Layden, & Stover, 2015; Swaminathan, Thomas, & Kurpad, 2015; Venkatramanan, Armata, Struppm, & Finkelstein, 2016). Vitamin B12 deficiency is associated with poor cognitive development and growth in children (Strand et al., 2013; Moore et al., 2012; Van de Rest et al., 2012), and vitamin B12 status in infancy have been associated with development and performance on neuropsychological tests after 5 years (Kvestad et al., 2017).

Zinc deficiency may affect cognitive development by causing changes in neuropsychological behavior, and motor development, through its interactions with other nutrients (Black, 2003). Interventional studies on effects of zinc supplementation and cognitive performance in schoolchildren have produced conflicting results. Randomized controlled trials in Chinese and Mexican‐American children showed that zinc supplementation improved neuropsychological performance and reasoning, when compared with controls (Penland et al., 1999; Sanstead et al., 1998). In Canada, a randomized controlled trial found no significant effect of zinc supplementation on cognitive development in school‐aged children, compared with controls (Gibson et al., 1989).

Childhood iron deficiency anemia has been associated with delayed cognitive and motor development, and has negative effects on academic performance and educational fulfillment (Luo et al., 2017). In school‐aged children, cognitive function is impaired by iron deficiency with anemia (Grantham‐McGregor & Ani, 2001). A randomized cross‐over study by Sorensen et al. (2015) in Denmark found that school girls aged 8–11 years with few iron stores had a poorer overall “school performance” and poorer reading performance compared with girls with larger iron stores.

Vitamin A also regulates majority of the neurodevelopmental pathways that can plausibly influence cognition (Ali et al., 2017). A study by Buckley et al. (2013) in rural Nepal found that vitamin A supplementation from preconception through postpartum did not improve cognition development of children at 10–13 years. However, in Indonesia, provision of vitamin A supplement to infants showed slight improvement in motor development scores after 3 years (Humphrey et al., 1998).

The potential effect of adequate consumption of micronutrients on cognitive performance in school‐aged children is of special relevance. However, little is known regarding micronutrients intake and cognitive function in Ghanaian school children, though studies have shown high prevalence of micronutrients deficiencies in children. The above literature serves as backdrop for the aim of this study, which sought to assess the relationship between dietary intake of micronutrients and cognitive test performance of school‐aged children in Kumasi metropolis.

2. MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.1. Study design and participants

A cross‐sectional study design was adopted for this research. The study recruited 438 school‐aged children between ages 9 and 13 years, living and attending basic schools within the Kumasi metropolis. Participants were recruited from ten (10) randomly selected basic government schools in the Kumasi Metropolis. In each school, all school children within the required ages in primary five were recruited for the study.

2.2. Study area

The Kumasi Metropolis is one of the twenty‐seven (27) districts in the Ashanti region, with Kumasi as the district’s capital. The Kumasi Metropolis covers 254 square kilometers and encompasses 10 sub‐metropolitan areas: Manhyia, Tafo, Suame, Asokwa, Oforikrom, Asawasi, Bantama, Kwadaso, Nhyiaso, and Subin. It is located in South‐central Ghana. Kumasi metropolis is an important educational center with private‐owned and government‐owned primary and junior high schools, together referred to as basic schools. The metropolis has 203 government‐owned primary schools, and all were eligible for the study.

2.3. Sample size and sampling procedure

Statistically, 10 schools were determined appropriate for the study and each of the schools was randomly selected from all government‐owned primary schools in the Metropolis, such that each school had an equal chance of being selected. However, a convenience sample size of 500 was used for this study and 50 pupils were to be selected from each school. In each school, all children in primary 5, who were within the ages 9–13 years were chosen for the study. All children who were therefore present on the day of data collection were included in this study. Since some of the schools had less than 50 in the chosen class, the eventual number was less than 500. Sick and physically challenged pupils were excluded from the study, since anthropometrics and physical fitness tests had to be performed and those in this category were not fit for those procedures.

2.4. Data collection

Data were collected on their dietary micronutrients intake and cognition level. Socio‐demographic data such as age, gender, and socioeconomic status of parent/guardians were obtained. Data were collected by researchers and other trained field assistants. Data were collected between September 2016 and May 2017.

2.4.1. Assessment of dietary intake

A 24‐hr triplicate recall on two weekdays and a weekend was used to collect dietary intakes of schoolchildren. Household food models of food items were used to identify quantities of foods eaten by participants. The weights (in grams) of foods consumed by participants were determined from the handy measures, and the composition of nutrients in meals was analyzed with the Nutrient Analysis Template, University of Ghana, Food Science, and Nutrition Department (2010). The dietary intakes of micronutrients (iron, zinc, vitamin B6, folate vitamin B12, and vitamin A) were compared with Dietary Reference Intake (2000 and 2001) by Food and Nutrition Board, Institute of Medicine for recommended dietary allowances Zinc (2005 dietary reference; National Academy of Sciences & Food & Nutrition Board, Institute of Medicine, 2005). Some participants were absent at the time of collecting dietary intakes, so 351 participants reported their intakes.

2.4.2. Cognition assessment test

The Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM) test was used to assess the cognitive level of the schoolchildren. The cognition test was performed in a quiet environment, and the test procedures were explained to participants. The test contains three sets of twelve problems (36 colored questions), which measures fluid intelligence by problem‐solving and abstract reasoning by analogy, and has been used extensively as a culturally fair test of intelligence (Raven, 2000). The tests involved progressively geometrical designs and patterns with a missing piece, and each question has six to eight options to pick from and fill the missing piece. The cognitive tests were administered by well‐trained research assistants. The children were given a booklet containing the test and answer sheets to select the correct answer for each question. This was explained to the pupil prior to the test.

2.5. Data analysis

Data were analyzed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences version 25 (SPSS IBM Inc). Micronutrients intake values were converted into categories of adequacy or inadequacy. Data are expressed as the mean value ± standard deviation (SD) for continuous variables. Chi‐square cross tabulation was performed to find associations between adequate or inadequate micronutrient intake by gender and age group. The independent t test and ANOVA were used to compare mean cognition test scores by age group, gender, and micronutrients intake status and by school, while partial correlations (adjusted for dietary carbohydrate and protein intakes) were performed to test association between dietary micronutrients intake and cognition test scores. All tests were two‐tailed, and p‐values ≤.05 were considered statistically significant.

3. RESULTS

Table 1 presents socio‐demographic, average daily micronutrients intake of school‐aged children between 9 and 13 years. Among 438 study participants, 213 (48.6%) were males and 225 (51.4%) were females. Majority of the participants were at ages 11 (32.4%) and 12 (30.4%) years old. Majority of participants (61.3%, 55.8%, and 53.0%) had inadequate intake of vitamin A, vitamin B12, and folate, respectively. Also, of 351 children, 156 (44.4%) had inadequate zinc intake, whereas 96 (27.4%) had inadequate iron intake. Majority of the schoolchildren (63.8%) passed the percent RCPM test score, scoring above average percent (50%; Table 1) (Figure 1).

Table 1.

Socio‐demographic, micronutrients intake and percent cognition test score of school‐age children (9–13 years)

Variable Frequency Percentage (%)
Gender N = 438  
Male 213 48.6
Female 225 51.4
Age group (years)
9 32 7.3
10 93 21.2
11 142 32.4
12 133 30.4
13 38 8.7
Micronutrients intake (Per day) N = 351  
Dietary iron
Adequate intake, 8 mg 255 72.6
Inadequate intake, less than 8 mg 96 27.4
Dietary zinc
Adequate, 8 mg 195 55.6
Inadequate, less than 8 mg 156 44.4
Dietary vitamin B12
Adequate, 1.8 µg 155 44.2
Inadequate, less than 1.8 µg 196 55.8
Dietary vitamin B6
Adequate, 1.0 mg 276 78.6
Inadequate, less than 1.0 mg 75 21.4
Dietary folate
Adequate, 300 µg 165 47.0
Inadequate, less than 300 µg 186 53.0
Dietary vitamin A
Adequate, 2,000 IU 136 38.7
Inadequate, less than 2,000 IU 215 61.3
Percentage cognition test (%)
Poor cognition, less than 50 127 36.2
Good cognition, 50–69.9 126 35.9
Excellent cognition, 70–100 98 27.9

Based on Dietary Reference Intake (2005) by Food and Nutrition Board, Institute of Medicine.

Table 2 compares proportions of children who had adequate or inadequate micronutrients intake by gender and age. Proportion of male pupils with inadequate vitamin B12 intake was higher (66.3%) than female pupils (46.8%, p < .001). There were no significant differences between dietary micronutrients intake and age of participants. However, the 10‐year‐old pupils had the highest proportion of inadequate zinc (48.7%) and vitamin B12 (59.0%) intakes, whereas inadequate vitamin B6 (30.8%), folate (69.2%), and vitamin A (73.1%) were highest among the 9‐year‐old pupils. For dietary iron, vitamin B6, and folate, the female pupils (29.3%, 23.4%, 54.8%) had higher inadequate intakes compared with male pupils, respectively (25.2%, 19.0%, 50.9%), though none of the differences were statistically significant. Male pupils (64.4%) had a higher proportion of inadequate vitamin A intake compared with female pupils (58.5%, p = .273; Table 2).

Table 2.

Distribution of micronutrients intake by gender and age of study population

Micronutrients intake (Per day) Gender χ 2 p‐Value Age group (years)
N (%)
χ 2 p‐Value
Male, N (%)
N = 163
Female, N (%)
N = 188
9 10 11 12 13
Dietary iron
Adequate, 8 mg 122 (74.8) 133 (70.7) 0.739 .403a 19 (73.1) 59 (75.6) 82 (68.3) 75 (76.5) 20 (69.0) 2.420 .659b
Inadequate, less than 8 mg 41 (25.2) 55 (29.3)     7 (26.9) 19 (24.4) 38 (31.7) 23 (23.5) 9 (31.0)    
Dietary zinc
Adequate, 8 mg 91 (55.8) 104 (55.3) 0.009 1.000a 15 (57.7) 40 (51.3) 68 (56.7) 55 (56.1) 17 (58.6) 0.808 .937b
Inadequate, less than 8 mg 72 (44.2) 84 (44.7)     11 (42.3) 38 (48.7) 52 (43.3) 43 (43.9) 43 (43.9)    
Dietary vitamin B12
Adequate, 1.8 µg 55 (33.7) 100 (53.2) 13.393 <.001a 12 (46.2) 32 (41.0) 55 (45.8) 41 (41.8) 15 (51.7) 1.376 .848b
Inadequate, less than 1.8 µg 108 (66.3) 88 (46.8)     14 (53.8) 46 (59.0) 65 (54.2) 57 (58.2) 14 (48.3)    
Dietary vitamin B6
Adequate, 1.0 mg 132 (81.0) 144 (76.6) 1.000 .193a 18 (69.2) 65 (83.3) 93 (77.5) 78 (79.6) 22 (75.9) 2.671 .614b
Inadequate, less than 1.0 mg 31 (19.0) 44 (23.4)     8 (30.8) 13 (16.7) 27 (22.5) 20 (20.4) 7 (24.1)    
Dietary folate
Adequate, 300 µg 80 (49.1) 85 (45.2) 0.524 .269a 8 (30.8) 35 (44.9) 60 (50.0) 47 (48.0) 15 (51.7) 3.621 .460b
Inadequate, less than 300 µg 83 (50.9) 103 (54.8)     18 (69.2) 43 (55.1) 60 (50.0) 51 (52.0) 14 (48.3)    
Dietary vitamin A
Adequate, 2,000 IU 58 (35.6) 78 (41.5) 1.283 .273a 7 (26.9) 39 (50.0) 46 (38.3) 35 (35.7) 9 (31.0) 6.808 .146b
Inadequate, less than 2,000 IU 105 (64.4) 110 (58.5)     19 (73.1) 39 (50.0) 74 (61.7) 63 (64.3) 20 (69.0)    

Total = 351, N—frequency, %—percentage, p‐value is significant at p ≤ .05.

a

Fisher's exact p‐values.

b

chi‐square p‐values.

Table 3 presents proportions of children with adequate versus inadequate micronutrients intake by school type and also compares proportion of children with poor, good, or excellent cognition score by school type. The proportions with adequate or inadequate micronutrients intake vary by school type for all the micronutrients: dietary iron (p < .001), zinc (p = .001), vitamin B12 (p = .001), vitamin B6 (p = .025), folate (p = .017), and vitamin A (p = .001). Similarly, proportions of children with poor, good, or excellent cognition score varied by school type (p = .003).

Table 3.

Distribution of micronutrients intake and cognition test by school type of study population

Variable micronutrients intake School type N (%) p‐Value
School A School B School C School D School E School F School G School H School I School J
Dietary iron
Adequate 48 (94.1) 19 (76.0) 33 (70.2) 24 (61.5) 27 (75.0) 12 (42.9) 37 (80.4) 21 (65.6) 14 (66.7) 20 (76.9) <.001
Inadequate 3 (5.9) 6 (24.0) 14 (29.8) 15 (38.5) 9 (25.0) 16 (57.1) 9 (19.6) 11 (34.4) 7 (33.3) 6 (23.1)  
Dietary zinc
Adequate 36 (70.6) 17 (68.0) 29 (61.7) 25 (64.1) 18 (50.0) 7 (25.0) 28 (60.9) 9 (28.1) 12 (57.1) 14 (53.8) .001
Inadequate 15 (29.4) 8 (32.0) 18 (38.3) 14 (35.9) 18 (50.0) 21 (75.0) 18 (39.1) 23 (71.9) 9 (42.9) 12 (46.2)  
Dietary vitamin B12
Adequate 28 (54.9) 13 (52.0) 18 (38.3) 18 (46.2) 23 (63.9) 4 (14.3) 24 (52.2) 7 (21.9) 7 (33.3) 13 (50.0) .001
Inadequate 23 (45.1) 12 (48.0) 29 (61.7) 21 (53.8) 13 (36.1) 24 (85.7) 22 (47.8) 25 (78.1) 14 (66.7) 13 (50.0)  
Dietary vitamin B6
Adequate 47 (92.2) 22 (88.0) 34 (72.3) 28 (71.8) 24 (66.7) 20 (71.4) 40 (87.0) 26 (81.3) 13 (61.9) 22 (84.6) .025
Inadequate 4 (7.8) 3 (12.0) 13 (27.7) 11 (28.2) 12 (33.3) 8 (28.6) 6 (13.0) 6 (18.8) 8 (38.1) 4 (15.4)  
Dietary folate
Adequate 36 (70.6) 7 (28.0) 24 (51.1) 16 (41.0) 16 (44.4) 10 (35.7) 24 (52.2) 11 (34.4) 9 (42.9) 12 (46.2) .017
Inadequate 15 (29.4) 18 (72.0) 23 (48.9) 23 (59.0) 20 (55.6) 18 (64.3) 22 (47.8) 21 (65.6) 12 (57.1) 14 (53.8)  
Dietary vitamin A
Adequate intake 30 (58.8) 9 (36.0) 19 (40.4) 12 (30.8) 12 (33.3) 3 (10.7) 25 (54.3) 14 (43.8) 4 (19.0) 8 (30.8) .001
Inadequate intake 21 (41.2) 16 (64.0) 28 (59.6) 27 (69.2) 24 (66.7) 25 (89.3) 21 (45.7) 18 (56.3) 17 (81.0) 18 (69.2)  
Cognition test percent (%)
Poor 11 (21.6) 15 (60.0) 13 (27.7) 15 (38.5) 18 (50.0) 5 (17.9) 9 (19.6) 17 (53.1) 10 (47.6) 14 (53.8) .003
Good 20 (39.2) 5 (20.0) 21 (44.7) 16 (41.0) 9 (25.0) 12 (42.9) 19 (41.3) 10 (31.3) 8 (38.1) 6 (23.1)  
Excellent 20 (39.2) 5 (20.0) 13 (27.7) 8 (20.5) 9 (25.0) 11 (39.2) 18 (39.1) 5 (15.6) 3 (14.3) 6 (23.1)  

Total = 351, N—frequency, %—percentage, poor—<50%, good—50%–69.9%, excellent—70%–100%, chi‐square p‐value is significant at p ≤ .05.

Table 4 presents cognition test outcome by gender, school type, and micronutrients intake. The average cognitive test score varied by gender (boys had a higher average) and school type (the highest score was 24.3 while the lowest was 16.3 points), and not by micronutrients intake (except for folate) and age. Children with adequate folate intake had 2.7 points in cognition score than those with inadequate folate intake (p = .050). However, mean cognition scores did not vary between children with adequate or inadequate intakes for iron, vitamin A, B12, B6 and zinc.

Table 4.

Unadjusted mean comparison of cognition test scores by gender, school type, and micronutrients intake

Variables Total number Cognition Mean ± SD (SEM) p‐value
N = 351 RCPM score
Total RCPM test score 351 20.6 ± 7.7 (0.4)  
Gender
Male 163 22.2 ± 7.6(0.6) <.001
Female 188 19.0 ± 7.8(0.6)  
Age group (years)
9 26 18.9 ± 8.8 (1.7) .231
10 78 19.7 ± 6.9 (0.8)  
11 120 21.8 ± 8.2 (0.7)  
12 98 19.9 ± 7.8 (0.8)  
13 29 20.5 ± 7.9 (1.5)  
School type
School A 51 22.7 ± 5.9 (0.8)a <.001
School B 25 18.4 ± 8.0 (1.6)  
School C 47 20.9 ± 6.7 (0.9)  
School D 39 19.4 ± 8.5 (1.4)  
School E 36 19.1 ± 9.0 (1.5)  
School F 28 24.3 ± 7.1 (1.3)b  
School G 46 23.8 ± 6.2 (0.9)c  
School H 32 16.8 ± 8.5 (1.5)a,b,c  
School I 21 18.3 ± 7.0 (1.5)  
School J 26 18.5 ± 7.7 (0.4)  
Micronutrients intake (Per day)
Dietary iron
Adequate 255 20.4 ± 7.8 (0.5) .666
Inadequate 96 20.8 ± 8.2 (0.8)  
Dietary zinc
Adequate 195 20.4 ± 7.3 (0.5) .812
Inadequate 156 20.6 ± 8.5 (0.7)  
Dietary vitamin B12
Adequate 155 20.3 ± 7.7 (0.6) .723
Inadequate 196 20.6 ± 8.1 (0.6)  
Dietary vitamin B6
Adequate 276 20.3 ± 7.7 (0.5) .386
Inadequate 75 21.2 ± 8.4 (1.0)  
Dietary folate
Adequate 165 21.4 ± 7.7 (0.6) .050
Inadequate 186 19.7 ± 8.0 (0.6)  
Dietary vitamin A
Adequate intake 136 20.5 ± 7.9 (0.7) .965
Inadequate intake 215 20.5 ± 7.9 (0.5)  

Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation (standard error mean), and p‐value is significant at p ≤ .05. Post hoc analysis showed significant mean differences between school types with same alphabets (a, p‐value = .016, b, p‐value = .004, c, p‐value = .002).

Independent t test.

ANOVA.

In Table 5, we compare cognition scores by number of micronutrients that intakes were adequate for. Three groups were created, those with 0–2 nutrients adequate, those with 3–4 adequacy, and those with 5–6 nutrients adequate. Between these three groups, the mean cognition scores were not different (p = .753), although those with 5–6 nutrients adequate scored a slightly higher cognition score. Similar observations were showed when the children were grouped into those with 0–3 versus those with 4–6 nutrients adequate.

Table 5.

Unadjusted mean comparison of combined adequacy for 6 micronutrients and cognition test score

Combined 6 micronutrients adequacy N = 351 Cognition p‐Value
RCPM score
Combined nutrients adequacy 1
0–2 Nutrients adequacy 111 20.5 ± 8.3 (0.8)a .753
3–4 Nutrients adequacy 121 20.1 ± 7.7 (0.7)  
5–6 Nutrients adequacy 115 20.9 ± 7.7 (0.7)  
Combined nutrient adequacy 2
0–3 Nutrients adequacy 165 20.3 ± 8.1 (0.6)b .745
4–6 Nutrients adequacy 186 20.6 ± 7.7 (0.6)  
Combined nutrients adequacy 3
All nutrients inadequacy 46 19.9 ± 8.3 (1.2) .970
All nutrients adequacy 52 20.4 ± 6.9 (0.9)  
1–5 nutrients adequacy 253 20.2 ± 8.1 (0.5)  

Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation (standard error mean), and p‐value is significant at p ≤ .05.

a

ANOVA.

b

Independent t test analysis.

There was weak, nearly significant positive correlation between cognition test score and dietary zinc and folate intakes (p = .05). However, intakes of the other micronutrients had no significant association with cognition test scores (Table 6).

Table 6.

Pearson's correlation between cognition test score and micronutrients intake

Micronutrients intake (Per day) RCPM test score
r (p‐Value)
Dietary iron .097 (.080)
Dietary zinc .106 (.050)
Dietary vitamin B12 .098 (.076)
Dietary vitamin B6 .096 (.082)
Dietary folate .104 (.050)
Dietary vitamin A .048 (.387)

Adjusted for dietary carbohydrate and protein intakes, p‐value is significant at p ≤ .05 (two‐tailed).

4. DISCUSSION

The present study reports the relationship between dietary intake of micronutrients and cognition test performance of school‐aged children. There were more female participants than male participants indicating that gender disparity in school enrollment among children in the Metropolis is probably not an issue any more.

Micronutrient deficiencies in children from developing countries remain a public health issue as they are persistently common. Deficiencies of micronutrients can be attributed to inadequate dietary intake, low bioavailability of micronutrients, and anti‐nutritional inhibitors. Deficiencies in micronutrients increase risk of diseases and infection by weakening the immune system and further depleting nutrients stores (Katona and Katona‐Apte, 2008). Many studies have pointed out that deficiencies in vitamin A and iron are among the causes of anemia, infection, low immunity, morbidity, and child mortality (Beard, 2001; Ramakrishan, Aburto, McCabe, & Martorell, 2004). This study reported poor micronutrients intake among the schoolchildren, compared with their RDAs. Majority of participants (61.3%) had inadequate vitamin A intake while inadequate vitamin B12 (55.8%) and folate (53.0%) were reported in more than 5 in 10 schoolchildren. These imply that a large number of these children were likely to be micronutrients deficient at different degrees and their consequences, including poor health and neurodevelopmental outcomes. Previous studies in Ghana (Alicke et al., 2017; Egbi, Alatiah, Ayi, & Steiner‐Asiedu, 2017) had reported high prevalence of vitamin A deficiency (93.6%, 36.6%) in children aged 6–12 years. The story seems not to change. On the other hand, inadequate dietary iron intake was not as high in this study, with less than a third of the children having inadequate iron intakes. The inadequacies in micronutrients intake observed in this population are worrying as these children would soon be adolescents and their needs would be much higher. Therefore, interventions are needed to avert problems that may arise especially for the girl children as they step into puberty, especially with regards to iron deficiency.

There were no statistical differences between dietary micronutrients intake and the different ages. However, the 10‐year‐old schoolchildren recorded the highest proportion with inadequate zinc and vitamin B12, while the nine‐year‐olds had the highest proportions with vitamin B6, folate, and vitamin A, implying that the lower ages of this physiologic group are likely to have poorer intakes. Additionally, there were significant variations in micronutrients intake between the schools. The school with the highest proportions with adequate micronutrients intake showed close to the best cognition test scores compared with the other schools, implying that consumption of adequate micronutrients by school was related to better cognition test performance. However, a couple of schools that did not fall within the best of micronutrient intake also were recorded among the best cognitive performance. This suggests that micronutrients intake alone cannot be used to explain the cognition test performance.

Designed for children aged 5 through 13 years of age, the elderly, and mentally and physically impaired individuals, the RCPM test contains 36 progressively geometrical designs and patterns with a missing piece, listed in order of difficulty. It measures the test taker’s reasoning ability, meaning making, and general intelligence (Raven, 1936). Twelve questions each are in set A and B, and another 12 questions in set AB, inserted between A and B. Our study revealed that majority of the schoolchildren (63.8%) passed the percent RCPM test score, scoring above average percent (50%), and the overall mean RCPM test score was above average (20.6 ± 7.7). As no national data exist for this average to be compared, it is not clear whether the average score by the children in this study is a good average or not, apart from the fact that close to two‐thirds of the children scored over 50%.

Although in this study, the boys had a better RCPM test score than the girls, mean cognition test score did not differ by age, and whether the children had adequate or inadequate iron, zinc, vitamin B6, vitamin B12, and vitamin A intake, except for folate. For folate, the children with adequate intake had a higher mean RCPM test score than those with inadequate intake, implying that folate intake, but not the other micronutrients, was related to the cognition test score. Evidence pointing to micronutrients intake and cognition is not consistent. A study by Boeke et al. (2013) in USA found no association between dietary vitamin B12 and cognitive outcomes. However, other studies, including Gewa et al., (2009) in Kenya and Ahmadi, Sohrabi, and Eftekhari (2009) in Iran, found that higher dietary vitamin B12 intake among schoolchildren was associated with improved cognitive outcomes. Clearly, further studies are required to elucidate these relationships, and as more studies on this subject are conducted, better understanding will be reached.

4.1. Limitations

The study assessed dietary intakes of participants but not serum levels, which may reflect the status of these nutrients better. It is also important to assess the overall patterns of intake rather than the specific nutrients. The dietary intakes were reported by schoolchildren directly, and therefore, over‐ or under‐estimation of portion sizes is possible. The study however incorporated the use of visual portion estimates like household handy measures during the data collection to help participants recall food portions consumed and reduce bias. The authors also recognize that the 24‐hr recall may not be the most reliable method for this age group. There was no comparison of income and other socioeconomic variables between the children, but it was expected that these children were from similar backgrounds by the fact that they were from government‐owned primary schools.

5. CONCLUSIONS

A large proportion of school‐aged children 9–13 years, attending government primary schools in Kumasi Metropolis, had inadequate dietary micronutrients intake (with the poorest intakes within ages 9 and 10) and therefore may be at risk of deficiencies and their devastating consequences, such as weakened immune system, poor health, increased risk for infectious diseases, and delayed physical and neurodevelopmental. This is the first in the population we studied that tried to find the relationship between micronutrients intake and RCPM scores, and although with the exception of folate, most of the nutrients were not significantly related to this particular cognition parameter, and further studies are needed using other cognition markers for a better understanding of this relationship. It may also be important to look at overall dietary pattern rather than specific nutrients for further studies. The findings however still call for interventions to promote adequate intake of nutrients, especially micronutrients in school‐aged children. Similar studies are recommended for children in private schools and/or other regions of Ghana.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

The authors declare that they do not have any conflict of interest regarding publication of this study.

ETHICAL STATEMENT

Permission to carry out the study in the randomly selected schools was obtained from the Ghana Education Service (Kumasi Metropolis), following ethical approval from the Committee on Human Research Publication and Ethics (CHRPE) of the School of medical Sciences, KNUST. In each school, the study was explained to the Heads, who also gave approval and dates for data to be collected. All the children also gave informed assent to be included in the study, and parents/guardians were followed up for a household survey. No blood samples were taken from participants. We declare that this manuscript is an authentic product of our research and is not published or communicated for publication elsewhere either in part or full. The manuscript is an accurate account of the study being reported.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We acknowledge the staff of the selected basic schools in Kumasi Metropolis for the support during the course of this study.

Annan RA, Apprey C, Asamoah‐Boakye O, Okonogi S, Yamauchi T, Sakurai T. The relationship between dietary micronutrients intake and cognition test performance among school‐aged children in government‐owned primary schools in Kumasi metropolis, Ghana. Food Sci Nutr. 2019;7:3042–3051. 10.1002/fsn3.1162

REFERENCES

  1. Ahmadi, A. , Sohrabi, Z. , & Eftekhari, M. H. (2009). Evaluating the relationship between breakfast pattern and short‐term memory in junior high school girls. Pakistan Journal of Biological Sciences, 12, 742–745. 10.3923/pjbs.2009.742.745 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ali, H. , Hamadani, J. , Mehra, S. , Tofail, F. , Hasan, M. I. , Shaikh, S. , … Christian, P. (2017). Effect of maternal antenatal and newborn supplementation with vitamin A on cognitive development of school‐aged children in rural Bangladesh: A follow‐up of a placebo‐controlled, randomized trial. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 106, 77–87. 10.3945/ajcn.116.134478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alicke, M. , Boakye‐Appiah, J. K. , Abdul‐Jalil, I. , Henze, A. , van der Giet, M. , Schulze, M. B. , … Danquah, I. (2017). Adolescent health in rural Ghana: A cross‐sectional study on the co‐occurrence of infectious diseases, malnutrition and cardio‐metabolic risk factors. PLoS ONE, 12, e0180436 10.1371/journal.pone.0180436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beard, J. L. (2001). Iron biology in immune function, muscle metabolism, and neuronal functioning. Journal of Nutrition, 131, 568S–579S. 10.1093/jn/131.2.568S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bellisle, F. (2004). Effects of diet on behaviour and cognition in children. British Journal of Nutrition, 92, S227–S232. 10.1079/BJN20041171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Black, M. M. (2003). The evidence linking zinc deficiency with children’s cognitive and motor functioning. Journal of Nutrition, 133, 1473S–1476S. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Black, M. M. , Walker, S. P. , Fernald, L. C. H. , Andersen, C. T. , DiGirolamo, A. M. , Lu, C. , … Grantham‐McGregor, S. (2017). Early childhood development coming of age: Science through the life course. Lancet, 389, 77–90. 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31389-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boeke, C. E. , Gillman, M. W. , Hughes, M. D. , Rifas‐Shiman, S. L. , Villamor, E. , & Oken, E. (2013). Choline intake during pregnancy and child cognition at age 7 years. American Journal of Epidemiology, 177, 1338–1347. 10.1093/aje/kws395 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bryan, J. , Osendarp, S. , Hughes, D. , Baghurst, K. , et al. (2004). Nutrients for cognitive development in school children. Nutrition Reviews, 62, 295–306. 10.1301/nr.2004.aug.295-306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buckley, G. J. , Murray‐Kolb, L. E. , Khatry, S. K. , LeClerq, S. C. , Wu, L. , West, K. P. , & Christian, P. (2013). Cognitive and motor skills in school‐aged children following maternal vitamin A supplementation during pregnancy in rural Nepal: A follow‐up of a placebo‐controlled, randomised cohort. British Medical Journal Open, 3, e002000 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-002000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Egbi, G. , Alatiah, G. A. , Ayi, I. , & Steiner‐Asiedu, M. (2017). Red palm oil bean‐stew improved serum vitamin A and haemoglobin concentrations and anthropometric indicators of school children with low vitamin A concentrations in a malaria‐endemic setting. African Journal of Food, Agriculture, Nutrition and Development, 17, 12817–12836. 10.18697/ajfand.80.16750 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Finkelstein, J. L. , Layden, A. J. , & Stover, P. J. (2015). Vitamin B‐12 and perinatal health. Advances in Nutrition, 6, 552–563. 10.3945/an.115.008201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fiorentino, M. , Perignon, M. , Kuong, K. , de Groot, R. , Parker, M. , Burja, K. , … Wieringa, F. T. (2017). Effect of multi‐micronutrient‐fortified rice on cognitive performance depends on premix composition and cognitive function tested: Results of an effectiveness study in Cambodian schoolchildren. Public Health Nutrition, 21, 816–827. 10.1017/S1368980017002774 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Food Science and Nutrition Department, University of Ghana (2010). The nutrient analysis template software excel spreadsheet for Ghanaian foods. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gewa, C. A. , Weiss, R. E. , Bwibo, N. O. , Whaley, S. , Sigman, M. , Murphy, S. P. , … Neumann, C. G. (2009). Dietary micronutrients are associated with higher cognitive function gains among primary school children in rural Kenya. British Journal of Nutrition, 101, 1378–1387. 10.1017/S0007114508066804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gibson, R. S. , Vanderkooy, P. D. S. , MacDonald, A. C. , Goldman, A. , Ryan, B. A. , & Berry, M. (1989). A growth‐limiting, mild zinc‐deficiency syndrome in some Southern Ontario boys with low height percentiles. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 49, 1266–1273. 10.1093/ajcn/49.6.1266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gomez‐Pinilla, F. (2008). Brain foods: The effects of nutrients on brain function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 568–578. 10.1038/nrn2421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Grantham‐McGregor, S. , & Ani, C. (2001). A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. Journal of Nutrition, 131, 649S–666S. 10.1093/jn/131.2.649S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Humphrey, J. H. , Agoestina, T. , Juliana, A. , Septiana, S. , Widjaja, H. , Cerreto, M. C. , … West, K. P. (1998). Neonatal vitamin A supplementation: Effect on development and growth at 3 y of age. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 68, 109–117. 10.1093/ajcn/68.1.109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Katona, P. , & Katona-Apte, J. (2008). The Interaction between Nutrition and Infection. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 46(10), 1582–1588. 10.1086/587658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kvestad, I. , Hysing, M. , Shrestha, M. , Ulak, M. , Thorne‐Lyman, A. L. , Henjum, S. , … Strand, T. A. (2017). Vitamin B‐12 status in infancy is positively associated with development and cognitive functioning 5 y later in Nepalese children. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 105, 1122–1131. 10.3945/ajcn.116.144931 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lemaire, J. B. , Wallace, J. E. , Dinsmore, K. , Lewinm, A. M. , Ghali, W. A. , & Roberts, D. (2010). Physician nutrition and cognition during work hours: Effect of a nutrition‐based intervention. BMC Health Services Research, 10, 241 10.1186/1472-6963-10-241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Luo, R. , Yue, A. I. , Zhou, H. , Shi, Y. , Zhang, L. , Martorell, R. , … Sylvia, S. (2017). The effect of a micronutrient powder home fortification program on anemia and cognitive outcomes among young children in rural China: A cluster randomized trial. BMC Public Health, 17, 38 10.1186/s12889-017-4755-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Monti, J. M. , Moulton, C. J. , & Cohen, N. J. (2015). The role of nutrition on cognition and brain health in ageing: A targeted approach. Nutrition Research Reviews, 28, 167–180. 10.1017/S0954422415000141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Moore, E. , Mander, A. , Ames, D. , Carne, R. , Sanders, K. , & Watters, D. (2012). Cognitive impairment and vitamin B12: A review. International Psychogeriatrics, 24, 541–556. 10.1017/S1041610211002511 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. National Academy of Sciences, Food and Nutrition Board, Institute of Medicine (2005). Dietary reference intakes series. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. in text ‘Mahan, L.K., Escott‐Stump, S. and Raymond, J.L. (2012). Krause’s Food and the Nutrition Care Process, 13th Editions, Elsevier Saunders Publication, Missouri, USA, page 1. [Google Scholar]
  27. Peet, E. D. , McCoy, D. C. , Danaei, G. , Ezzati, M. , Fawzi, W. , Jarvelin, M. R. , … Fink, G. (2015). Early childhood development and schooling attainment: Longitudinal evidence from British, Finnish and Philippine birth cohorts. PLoS ONE, 10, e0137219 10.1371/journal.pone.0137219 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Penland, J. , Sanstead, H. , Egger, N. , Dayal, H. , Alcock, N. , Plotkin, R. , … Zavaleta, A. (1999). Zinc, iron and micronutrient supplementation effects on cognitive and psychomotor function of Mexican‐American school children. The FASEB Journal, 13, 921. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ramakrishan, U. , Aburto, N. , McCabe, G. , & Martorell, R. (2004). Multimicronutrient interventions but not vitamin A or iron interventions alone improve child growth: Results from three meta‐analyses. Journal of Nutrition, 134, 2592–2602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Rathod, R. , Kale, A. , & Joshi, S. (2016). Novel insights into the effect of vitamin B12 and omega‐3 fatty acids on brain function. Journal of Biomedical Science, 23, 17 10.1186/s12929-016-0241-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Raven, J. C. (1936). Mental tests used in genetic studies: The performance of related individuals on tests mainly educative and mainly reproductive. MSc thesis, University of London. [Google Scholar]
  32. Raven, J. (2000). The Raven’s progressive matrices: Change and stability over culture and time. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 1–48. 10.1006/cogp.1999.0735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sanstead, H. H. , Penland, J. G. , Alcock, N. W. , Dayal, H. H. , Chen, X. C. , Li, J. S. , … Yang, J. J. (1998). Effects of repletion with zinc and other micronutrients on neuropsychologic performance and growth of Chinese children. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 68, 470S–475S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sorensen, L. B. , Damsgaard, C. T. , Dalskov, S. M. , Petersen, R. A. , Egelund, N. , Dyssegaard, C. B. , … Lauritzen, L. (2015). Diet‐induced changes in iron and n‐3 fatty acid status and associations with cognitive performance in 8–11‐year‐old Danish children: Secondary analyses of the Optimal Well‐Being, Development and Health for Danish Children through a Healthy New Nordic Diet School Meal Study. British Journal of Nutrition, 114, 1623–1637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Strand, T. A. , Taneja, S. , Ueland, P. M. , Refsum, H. , Bahl, R. , Schneedem, J. , … Bhandari, N. (2013). Cobalamin and folate status predict mental development scores in North Indian children 12–18 months of age. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 97, 310–317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Swaminathan, S. , Thomas, T. , & Kurpad, A. V. (2015). B‐vitamin interventions for women and children in low‐income populations. Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition and Metabolic Care, 18, 295–306. 10.1097/MCO.0000000000000166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Thankachan, P. , Selvam, S. , Surendran, D. , Chellan, S. , Pauline, M. , Abrams, S. A. , & Kurpad, A. V. (2013). Efficacy of a multi micronutrient‐fortified drink in improving iron and micronutrient status among schoolchildren with low iron stores in India: A randomised, double‐masked placebo‐controlled trial. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 67, 36–41. 10.1038/ejcn.2012.188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Van de Rest, O. , van Hooijdonk, L. W. , Doets, E. , Schiepers, O. J. , Eilander, A. , & de Groot, L. C. (2012). B vitamins and n‐3 fatty acids for brain development and function: Review of human studies. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism, 60, 272–292. 10.1159/000337945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Venkatramanan, S. , Armata, I. E. , Struppm, B. , & Finkelstein, J. L. (2016). Vitamin B12 and cognition in children. Advances in Nutrition, 7, 879–888. 10.3945/an.115.01202 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Yehuda, S. , Rabinovitzm, S. , & Mostofskym, D. I. (2006). Nutritional deficiencies in learning and cognition. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 43, S22–S25. 10.1097/01.mpg.0000255847.77034.a4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Food Science & Nutrition are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES