Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2019 Nov 6;39(6):459–468. doi: 10.1111/opo.12648

Visual and non‐visual properties of filters manipulating short‐wavelength light

Manuel Spitschan 1,2,3,, Rafael Lazar 2,3, Christian Cajochen 2,3
PMCID: PMC6887545  EMSID: EMS85079  PMID: 31696535

Abstract

Purpose

Optical filters and tints manipulating short‐wavelength light (sometimes called ‘blue‐blocking’ or ‘blue‐attenuating’ filters) are used in the management of a range of ocular, retinal, neurological and psychiatric disorders. In many cases, the only available quantification of the optical effects of a given optical filter is the spectral transmittance, which specifies the amount of light transmitted as a function of wavelength.

Methods

We propose a novel physiologically relevant and retinally referenced framework for quantifying the visual and non‐visual effects of these filters, incorporating the attenuation of luminance (luminous transmittance), the attenuation of melanopsin activation (melanopsin transmittance), the colour shift, and the reduction of the colour gamut (gamut reduction). Using these criteria, we examined a novel database of spectral transmittance functions of optical filters (n = 121) which were digitally extracted from a variety of sources.

Results

We find a large diversity in the alteration of visual and non‐visual properties. The spectral transmittance properties of the examined filters vary widely, in terms of shapes and cut‐off wavelengths. All filters show relatively more melanopsin attenuation than luminance attenuation (lower melanopsin transmittance than luminous transmittance). Across the data set, we find that melanopsin transmittance and luminous transmittance are correlated.

Conclusions

We suggest that future studies and examinations of the physiological effects of optical filters quantify the visual and non‐visual effects of the filters beyond the spectral transmittance, which will eventually aid in developing a mechanistic understanding of how different filters affect physiology. We strongly discourage comparing the downstream effects of different filters on, e.g. sleep or circadian responses, without considering their effects on the retinal stimulus.

Keywords: blue‐blocking filters, circadian, ipRGCs, optics, short-wavelength light, sleep

Introduction

Background

Optical filters can be used to modify the visual input by blocking or attenuating light at specific parts of the visible spectrum.1, 2 So‐called ‘blue‐blocking’ or ‘blue‐attenuating’ filters reduce the amount of short‐wavelength light at the eye’s surface, the cornea. Optically, the filtering is typically realised using one of two ways: (1) using a cut‐off filter, which blocks or attenuates light below a specific wavelength, (2) using a notch filter, which blocks or attenuates light within a specific and limited short‐wavelength range, or a combination of both.

By altering the spectral distribution of the light incident on the retinal surface relative to no filtering,3 filters directly affect the activation of the cones and rods in the retina, which allow us to see during the day and night, respectively. In addition, the activity of the melanopsin‐containing intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) is also modulated by the use of optical filters. These cells, though only making up <1% of retinal ganglion cells, are of great importance to non‐visual functions, such as entrainment of circadian rhythms in physiology and behaviour to the environmental light‐dark cycle, and the suppression of melatonin in response to light.4

Previous investigations have examined the effects of filters manipulating short‐wavelength light on visual performance,5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13 colour vision,11, 13, 14, 15 and steady‐state and dynamic parameters of pupil size.16, 17 Furthermore, they have received attention in the domain of sleep medicine and chronobiology, where their effects on melatonin suppression,18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25 circadian rhythms,26, 27, 28, 29, 30 sleep,6, 17, 21, 25, 29, 30, 31, 32, 33 modulation of alertness by light,34, 35 and for use by shift or night workers36, 37 have been investigated.

In addition, filters manipulating short‐wavelength light have been employed in the management of psychiatric and neurological (bipolar disorder,38, 39, 40 depression,32 ADHD,41 blepharospasm,42, 43 migraine,44, 45, 46 photosensitive epilepsy,47 insomnia32, 41, 48) and retinal conditions (rod monochromacy/achromatopsia,49, 50, 51, 52 retinitis pigmentosa,53 others 54), as well as for reducing non‐specific photophobia46, 55 and eye fatigue.56, 57 Coloured filters have also been used previously to improve reading difficulties and relieve visual stress58, 59 and symptoms of migraine,60 though these are not specifically attenuating short‐wavelength light61 (but may do so, depending on the specific filter chosen).

Across these studies and reports, filters by different manufacturers were used. In addition, the terms ‘blue‐blocking’ or ‘blue‐attenuating’, which are sometimes used to promote filters manipulating short‐wavelength light, are not well‐defined nor standardised. Therefore, there is great ambiguity about the exact properties of filters carrying those names.

The unique characteristic of a filter is its spectral transmittance, which is a quantified specification of how much light is passed through the filter at a given wavelength. Relating the transmittance properties of a filter to a specific retinal mechanism is a special case, e.g. in rod monochromacy, which specifically reduces the scotopic luminance. It is impossible to determine the precise effects on various visual and non‐visual functions just by examining the spectral transmittance function of a filter. While the transmittance is necessary to determine these effects quantitatively, this cannot be done ‘by eye’. To remedy this, we propose a retinally referenced framework for quantifying the visual effects.

Retinal consequences of optical filters

Changing the spectral distribution of the transmitted light can have a range of different effects. Here, we consider four main effects in the trichromatic human retina (Figure 1), all of which are important to assess the properties of a given filter:

  1. Luminous transmittance [%]: By changing the spectral properties of the incident light, the luminance of the stimulus is altered. Luminance is calculated using the V(λ) curve, which is a combination of L and M cones. Luminous transmittance is the fraction of the luminance of the filtered spectral distribution relative to the luminance of the unfiltered spectral distribution. A luminous transmittance of 100% corresponds to no change in luminance by the filter. Values >100% are not possible.

  2. Melanopsin transmittance [%]: The activation of melanopsin may be reduced by the optical filter under investigation. Using the same calculation procedure as for luminous transmittance, melanopsin transmittance is calculated as the proportion of melanopsin activation with filter relative to without filter.

  3. Colour shift: Optical filters with non‐uniform transmittance also lead to a shift in the colour appearance of a colour which would appear as white without filter. With filters manipulating short‐wavelength light, these shifts typically occur in the yellow, orange or amber directions.

  4. Gamut reduction [%]: Objects of different colours will appear different when seen with a filter. Without the filter, the distribution of differently coloured objects is called the gamut, which is simply the area which the object colours ‘inhabit’ in a colour space. We can compare the area of the gamut when objects are seen through a filter with the area of the gamut without the filter.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Visual and non‐visual effects of filters. Luminous transmittance is given as the fraction of the luminance of daylight seen with filter (shown as the beige area) to without filter (shown as the white area). In this case, the luminance with the filter is only about a quarter (23%). Melanopsin transmittance follows the same calculation, except for the melanopsin photopigment. Because the cut‐off wavelength of the filter is outside of the spectral sensitivity of melanopsin, the attenuation is far larger compared to luminance (3%). Colour shift is the Euclidian distance between a specified white point seen with and without the filter in the uniform colour space used here (CIE 1976 u’10v’10 colour space based on the CIE 1964 10° observer.). The smaller the number, the smaller the colour shift and therefore, the closer the reproduction of the white point with the filter relative to without the filter. Gamut reduction is the reduction of the colour space which common surface reflectances inhabit, with and without the filter. In this case, the colour gamut with the filter is only around one fifth (20%) of the gamut without the filter. Here and elsewhere, the spectral power distribution was assumed to be of noon daylight (D65, daylight with a correlated colour temperature [CCT] of 6500K).

We note that these four properties of filters are by no means exhaustive, but they allow for a strongly quantifiable and yet intuitive approach of the effects of different optical filters on visual and non‐visual function. We also note that our analysis does not include consideration of transmission of light in the UV band and makes no claims about damaging effects of UV or other radiation. Similarly, we did not consider the polarisation properties of the filters.

Parametric simulation of optical filters

We started examining the visual and non‐visual filters using simulated filters. We used an analytic description of the typical transmittance profile using a sigmoid function (Figure 2). The model parameters were the (1) cut‐off wavelength, (2) the upper asymptote, and (3) the slope of the function. By changing each of these parameters in isolation, we can examine how properties of the spectral transmittance functions affect the derived parameters.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Exploring effects of cut‐off filters. Spectral transmittance of cut‐off filters is analytically modelled using the sigmoid function of the form T(λ)=L1+e-k(λ-λ0), where L is the upper asymptote, k is the slope, and λ0 is the cut‐off wavelength. Left column: Varying cut‐off wavelengths (λ0), with L = 0.9, k = 0.5. Middle column: Varying top asymptote L, with λ0 = 550 nm, k = 0.5. Right column: Varying slope k, with λ0 = 560 nm, L = 0.9. Small arrows point to the spectral transmittances at one extreme end of specific parameter choice, pointing out the corresponding retinal effect in the other panels as well.

Varying the cut‐off wavelength systematically affects all four parameters (Figure 2, left column). Moving the cut‐off wavelength to longer wavelengths, leads to both more luminance attenuation and more melanopsin attenuation. However, because melanopsin spectral sensitivity has its peak at shorter wavelengths than the luminosity function, the increase in melanopsin attenuation happens at a faster rate than the increase in luminance attenuation. In the colour domain, cut‐off filters move the chromaticity towards the spectral locus, with larger deviations with longer cut‐off wavelengths.

At a fixed wavelength and slope, varying the asymptote, i.e. the ‘plateau’ and maximum transition systematically effects only luminance and melanopsin, though to the same amount (Figure 2, middle column), producing a line that is parallel to the neutral density locus as one would expect. The chromaticity of filters with varying plateaus is the same. The slope of the transmittance function (Figure 2, right column) at a fixed cut‐off wavelength affects largely the attenuation of melanopsin and the change in colour properties, but not so much luminance, though we expect that this will depend on the cut‐off wavelength itself.

Novel database of optical filters

In this work, we examined the spectral transmittance functions of 121 filters manipulating short‐wavelength light (either for spectacles or for contact lenses) with respect to their luminous transmittance, melanopsin transmittance, colour shift, and gamut reduction. These filters were obtained using digital data extraction techniques from a variety of sources, including published graphs from scientific articles, informative patient brochures and manufacturer brochures.

We classify these filters in three loose yet intuitive ad‐hoc categories: medical filters (n = 76), safety filters (n = 11) and task‐specific filters (n = 34). This latter category is subdivided into filters for sports (n = 10), driving (n = 4), visual display unit (VDU) use (n = 12) and a catch‐all ‘Other’ category (n = 8). In this analysis, we are agnostic to the materials that these filters are applied on or produced from, though this is an important consideration for practical use. All extracted filter transmittances are available on the open‐access repository.

Methods

Data sourcing and extraction

Published graphs of the spectral transmittance of filters manipulating short‐wavelength light were sourced using informal searches on PubMed, Google Scholar, and Google, yielding a total of 121 filters to be considered. Where data were obtained from manufacturers’ or other websites, those websites were submitted to the Internet Archive Wayback Machine for permanent archiving (http://www.web.archive.org/).

Transmittance spectra were extracted by one operator (author of this study, RL) from published graphs using WebPlotDigitzer 4.1 (http://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer),62 a free visual tool for extraction of data points from published graphs where no tabulated data are available. WebPlotDigitizer has been found to provide rather high levels of reliability.63 Axis calibration and data points in the given curves were set as precisely as visual inspection allowed. The maximum range of values clearly identifiable in the graphs were used for scaling. All transmission data extraction files underwent a secondary inspection. In some cases where multiple transmission curves were given in one plot, curves overlapped. If in these cases some of the transmission curves were overdrawn and thus not clearly visible, we assumed that the ‘top’ curve also corresponded to the other overdrawn curves. After extraction, data were then interpolated to 1 nm resolution using piecewise cubic hermite interpolating polynomial (PCHIP) interpolation. At wavelengths at the short‐wavelength and long‐wavelength extreme of the visible range at which in some cases no data were available, the transmittance was set to 0, which has the effect that light at those wavelengths is not factored into the calculation. If negative transmission values occurred, the data extraction was again checked and validated. The remaining negative values due to inaccuracy in the given curves were then set to 0.

Calculation procedures

For calculation of melanopsin activation, we used the spectral sensitivity recently standardised by the Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage as CIE S 026/E:2018,64 which assumes a Govardovskii nomogram (λmax = 480 nm) as well as a custom lens function synthesizing different lens transmittance functions.4 While the standard also recommends functions for the cones,65 we opted for the CIE 1964 10° observer in this work to maximise compatibility of the colorimetric calculations. This observer prescribes XYZ functions which are converted to the well‐known u’10v’10 space.

Luminous transmittance was calculated as the fraction of the luminance of the light seen with or without the filter incorporated using numeric integration: TransmittanceLuminance=380780FλED65λV10λΔλ380780ED65λV10λΔλ, where F(λ) corresponds to the spectral transmittance of the filter, E D65(λ) to the spectrum of daylight at 6500K (D65), and V 10° (λ) to the CIE 1964 10° luminosity function.

Melanopsin transmittance was calculated using the same procedure, except with the melanopsin spectral sensitivity: TransmittanceMelanopsin=380780FλED65λSMelanopsinλΔλ380780ED65λSMelanopsinλΔλ, where F(λ) corresponds to the spectral transmittance of the filter, ED65(λ) to the spectrum of daylight at 6500 K (D65), and S Melanopsin (λ) to the CIE melanopsin spectral sensitivity function.

The colour shift was calculated as the Euclidean distance between the chromaticities of the D65 illuminant seen with and without the filter in the uniform CIE 1976 u’10v’10 colour space based on the CIE 1964 10° observer. The colour gamut with and without the filter was calculated using the IES Color Evaluation Samples (CES), a set of 99 representative reflectances66 selected from a large set of 105 000 spectral reflectance functions of paints, textiles, natural objects, skin, tones, inks, and other functions. Gamut reduction was calculated using the ratio of the area of the convex hulls of these 99 reflectance functions seen with and without the filter. The different effects are visualised in Figure 1.

Data and software availability

All tabulated transmittance spectra and code to produce the unedited versions in the figures in this report are available on GitHub (https://github.com/spitschan/Spitschan2019_OPO). Table S1 contains the list of all filters, along with the four tabulated effects. Data S1 contains the data shown in Figure 3 and given in Table S1. Data S2 contains the transmittance spectra.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Visual and non‐visual properties of spectral filters. Columns correspond to the three main filter categories we identified (Figure 2). Row 1: Spectral transmittances of filters. Row 2: Luminous transmittance vs melanopsin transmittance. Dashed line indicates equal reduction of luminance and melanopsin, as would be the case with a spectrally uniform neutral density (ND) filter. Row 3: Chromaticity diagram. The red cross indicates chromaticity of 6500K daylight (D65) and white squares indicate chromaticities of D65 seen through the respective filters. Row 4: Colour shift vs gamut factor. See Introduction and Figure 1 for explanation.

Results

Large diversity of filters

Across all categories, the spectral transmittances of our spectral filters (n = 121, see Table S1 for description of sources) are rather diverse (Figure 3, row 1). Firstly, this diversity is reflected in the shape of the filter transmittance function, i.e. whether it is a cut‐off, notch or some other form of filter. Furthermore, the cut‐off filters differ largely in the wavelength at which they transmit 50% of light. Another differing factor is the ‘plateau’ of transmission at wavelengths longer than the cut‐off wavelength.

All filters show more relative melanopsin attenuation than luminance attenuation (Figure 3 , row 2). This is evidenced by the fact that all data points lie under the identity line, which corresponds to a spectrally flat filter which decreases the activation of all photopigments by the same amount. It appears as though there is filters consistently inhabit a triangular segment in the luminance‐melanopsin attenuation space, bounded by the neutral density along the identity line at the upper end.

All filters show a move of the chromaticity of the D65 white point towards the spectral locus, which is given by the edge of the chromaticity diagram (Figure 3 , row 3). This locus corresponds to the chromaticity of monochromatic and therefore highly saturated lights. In the colour rendering properties of the filters quantified using the gamut attenuation, we find no clear pattern (Figure 3 , row 4). We find that some filters severely reduce the colour gamut of the worlds seen with the filter, reducing it to only <1% of the gamut seen with the filter. This is obviously correlated with the shift of the filter towards the spectral locus as all surfaces seen under monochromatic illumination appear to only change in brightness, and not colour (because the surfaces can only reflect the light that is there). As would be expected, larger colour shifts (i.e. shift of the colour towards the spectral locus) translate to a reduced colour gamut.

Overall, we find that these response variables are correlated, though to different extents (Table 1). The largest correlation is between the luminous transmittance and the melanopsin transmittance. This result can be understood intuitively: Due to the overlap in the spectral sensitivities of luminance and melanopsin, it is practically impossible to modulate one without the other (see also Figure 2). Similarly, the larger a colour shift by a filter, the smaller the gamut: As a filter pushes the visual scene to the spectral locus (i.e. the edges of the chromaticity diagram), the less light reflected from surfaces in the visual scene will be transmitted.

Table 1.

Correlation matrix between variables

Melanopsin transmittance Colour difference Colour gamut
Luminous transmittance 0.86 −0.65 0.43
Melanopsin transmittance −0.74 0.74
Colour difference −0.78

Discussion

General discussion

In our analysis we find that there is large variability in the visual and non‐visual properties filters manipulating short‐wavelength light. While the spectral transmittance of a filter necessarily needs to be known for quantifying the four outcomes we investigated here, it in itself is not of any use as to determine a filter’s effect on the retina. In addition to providing transmittance spectra in tabulated form, future studies should consider quantifying the effect of a filter using the metrics described here.

For many of the applications of filters manipulating short‐wavelength light reaching the retina, we still lack a mechanistic understanding of how the different photoreceptors contribute to the effects. The precise mechanism which a given filter modifies is known only in special cases, e.g. in rod monochromacy.51 For example, at present (2019), we do not know how cones and rods contribute to the basic physiological regulation of melatonin secretion by light. A retinally referenced, or ‘physiologically relevant’ framework to quantify effects of a filter is the first step in using optical filters for developing such mechanistic understanding, which needs to be the basis for the development of recommendations for filter use for specific conditions.

Pupil size effects

The reduction of illumination at the cornea by optical filters reduces the retinal illuminance, i.e. the incident light on the retinal surface. However, retinal illuminance itself is also controlled by the area of the pupil. With a reduction in light intensity, the pupil area becomes bigger. The dynamic range, however, is rather limited, with a maximum possible modulation of retinal illuminance just by pupil size by a factor of ~16 (between a maximally constricted 2 mm pupil and a maximally dilated 8 mm pupil). We investigated the effect of reducing the corneal illuminance with optical filters on the effective retinal illuminance using the unified Watson & Yellott model67 (Figure 4). We assumed a field size of 150°, a 32‐year old observer, as well as binocular stimulation as an approximation to real world viewing conditions, and investigated how the resulting predicted retinal illuminance depends on the luminance of the viewed stimulus with and without neutral density filters of varying densities (ND1.0, ND2.0 and ND3.0). With the near‐parallel lines of log luminance vs log retinal illuminance, it can be seen that the primary determinant of retinal illuminance is luminance ‘seen’ through the filter.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Pupil size effects of neutral‐density filters. Top panel: Predicted pupil size for a 32‐year old observer viewing a 150° field at varying luminances with both eyes. Bottom panel: Predicted retinal illuminance (luminance × pupil area) when viewed either through the natural pupil (dashed lines indicate maximum retinal illuminance given maximum difference in pupil size), or through spectrally uniform filters of varying transmittance (ND1.0, ND2.0, ND3.0).

Chung and Pease16 note that at equivalent luminance, ‘yellow’ filters lead to larger pupils than neutral‐density filters. This is consistent with the view that melanopsin activation, which significantly drives steady‐state pupil size in humans,68, 69 is severely reduced under short‐wavelength filters. Under most real‐world conditions, the correlation between luminance and melanopsin activation is probably well‐constrained, except with chromatic filters and experimental conditions in which the decoupling can be lead to up to a three‐fold difference in melanopsin activation with no or little nominal difference in luminance.70, 71 Importantly, optical quality of the retinal image depends on pupil size,72 which needs to be factored into filter assessments.

Digital data extraction

This study relied on digital data extraction from published graphs. This was necessary because spectral transmittance data are rarely available in digital or tabulated form, even though data storage as supplementary material is often available at scientific journals at the time of writing this article (2019). As shown in this article, this need not be a limitation since data can be extracted from graphs and be subjected to rigorous novel or reanalyses. A limitation of our data‐driven approach is that tabulated spectra given by manufacturers are here treated as bona fide measurements of transmittance spectra. The process of data extraction using this method may also lead to inaccuracies in the tabulated transmittance spectra. These inaccuracies arise from the conversion of graphics (often low‐resolution) to numerical values. Inaccuracies in the assumed transmittances may lead to inaccuracies in the estimation of the effects of the filters.

Other considerations

In some real‐world scenarios, the short‐wavelength properties of lighting or light‐emitted devices may be modified directly (e.g. using applications changing the colour balance on VDUs,71 or spectrally tuneable lighting73), but this is not the general case. Optical filters therefore represent a practical alternative for real‐world scenarios.

In addition to filters affecting the retinal stimulus, light of different colours may also have psychological or higher‐order effects.74 Another effect worth considering is stigma towards wearers of coloured spectacles.75 Here, we only considered the visual and non‐visual properties of different optical filters. Other optical effects such as scattering or polarisation and other higher‐order effects are not captured by the simple filter model that we have applied here.

The proposed retinally referenced framework does not take into consideration the role of adaptation to the modified spectral environments.76, 77, 78 In addition to adaptive mechanisms at play, there may be other long‐term changes in visual processing in habitual wearers of coloured filters.79 The degree of short‐term and long‐term adaptation in visual and non‐visual function in response to specific filter types is an interesting empirical question.

Conclusion

We find large diversity in the visual and non‐visual properties of different spectral filters manipulating short‐wavelength light. We propose that to evaluate the effect of a given optical filter, the spectral transmittance is only the first step in characterising the effect of a filter on the illumination at the eye and suggest a retinally referenced framework to quantify these effects, incorporating the attenuation of luminance, the attenuation of melanopsin activation, shifts in colour, and reduction of colour gamut.

Conflict of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest and have no proprietary interest in any of the materials mentioned in this article.

Supporting information

Table S1. List of filters.

Data S1. Visual and non‐visual properties of the examined filters.

Data S2. Spectral transmittances of the examined filters.

Acknowledgements

MS is supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Fellowship (Wellcome Trust 204686/Z/16/Z) and a Junior Research Fellowship from Linacre College, University of Oxford.

Spitschan M, Lazar R & Cajochen C. Visual and non‐visual properties of filters manipulating short‐wavelength light. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2019; 39: 459–468. 10.1111/opo.12648

Author contributions: MS: involved in all aspects of study conception and design; data acquisition, analysis and interpretation; and drafting and critically revising the manuscript. RL: involved in study conception; data acquisition, analysis and interpretation; and critically revising the manuscript. CC: involved in data interpretation and critically revising the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Bach M & Rohrschneider K. Kantenfilter: physikalische und physiologische Grundlagen [Cut‐off filter: physical and physiological basics]. Ophthalmologe 2018; 115: 922–927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Rohrschneider K & Bach M. Kantenfilter: Medizinische indikation und klinischer einsatz [Edge filters: Medical indications and clinical application]. Ophthalmologe 2018; 115: 916–921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Wyszecki G & Stiles WS. Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulæ, 2nd edn Wiley: New York, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Lucas RJ, Peirson SN, Berson DM et al. Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci 2014; 37: 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Kelly SA. Effect of yellow‐tinted lenses on brightness. J Opt Soc Am A 1990; 7: 1905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Lawrenson JG, Hull CC & Downie LE. The effect of blue‐light blocking spectacle lenses on visual performance, macular health and the sleep‐wake cycle: a systematic review of the literature. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2017; 37: 644–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Leat SJ, North RV & Bryson H. Do long wavelength pass filters improve low vision performance? Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1990; 10: 219–224. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Lee JE, Stein JJ, Prevor MB et al. Effect of variable tinted spectacle lenses on visual performance in control subjects. Eye Contact Lens 2002; 28: 80–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Leung TW, Li RW‐H & Kee C‐S. Blue‐light filtering spectacle lenses: optical and clinical performances. PLoS ONE 2017; 12: e0169114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Wilkins A & Neary C. Some visual, optometric and perceptual effects of coloured glasses. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1991; 11: 163–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Wolffsohn JS, Cochrane AL, Khoo H, Yoshimitsu Y & Wu S. Contrast is enhanced by yellow lenses because of selective reduction of short‐wavelength light. Optometry Vision Sci 2000; 77: 73–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Eperjesi F, Fowler CW & Evans BJ. Do tinted lenses or filters improve visual performance in low vision? A review of the literature. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2002; 22: 68–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Hammond BR Jr. The visual effects of intraocular colored filters. Scientifica (Cairo) 2012; 2012: 424965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Akkaya ZY, Burcu A, Acar MA & Unef GO. Discrimination of Ishihara test plates through a red filter. J Clin Anal Med 2014; 546–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Schürer M, Walter A, Brünner H & Langenbucher A. Einfluss transparenter gelb‐ und orangefarbiger Kontaktlinsen auf das Farbunterscheidungsvermögen im gelben Farbbereich [Effect of transparent yellow and orange colored contact lenses on color discrimination in the yellow color range]. Ophthalmologe 2015; 112: 670–678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Chung ST & Pease PL. Effect of yellow filters on pupil size. Optom Vis Sci 1999; 76: 59–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Ostrin LA, Abbott KS & Queener HM. Attenuation of short wavelengths alters sleep and the ipRGC pupil response. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2017; 37: 440–450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Figueiro M & Overington D. Self‐luminous devices and melatonin suppression in adolescents. Lightning Res Technol 2016; 48: 966–975. [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Figueiro MG & Rea MS. Lack of short‐wavelength light during the school day delays dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) in middle school students. Neuroendocrinol Lett 2010; 31: 92–96. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Figueiro MG, Wood B, Plitnick B & Rea MS. The impact of light from computer monitors on melatonin levels in college students. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 2011; 32: 158–163. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Giménez MC, Beersma DGM, Bollen P, van der Linden ML & Gordijn MCM. Effects of a chronic reduction of short‐wavelength light input on melatonin and sleep patterns in humans: evidence for adaptation. Chronobiol Int 2014; 31: 690–697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Kayumov L, Casper RF, Hawa RJ et al. Blocking low‐wavelength light prevents nocturnal melatonin suppression with no adverse effect on performance during simulated shift work. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2005; 90: 2755–2761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Sasseville A, Paquet N, Sévigny J & Hébert M. Blue blocker glasses impede the capacity of bright light to suppress melatonin production. J Pineal Res 2006; 41: 73–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Wood B, Rea MS, Plitnick B & Figueiro MG. Light level and duration of exposure determine the impact of self‐luminous tablets on melatonin suppression. Appl Ergon 2013; 44: 237–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Zerbini G, Kantermann T & Merrow M. Strategies to decrease social jetlag: reducing evening blue light advances sleep and melatonin. Eur J Neurosci 2018: 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Ayaki M, Hattori A, Maruyama Y et al. Protective effect of blue‐light shield eyewear for adults against light pollution from self‐luminous devices used at night. Chronobiol Int 2016; 33: 134–139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Eastman CI & Martin SK. How to use light and dark to produce circadian adaptation to night shift work. Ann Med 2009; 31: 87–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Esaki Y, Kitajima T, Ito Y et al. Wearing blue light‐blocking glasses in the evening advances circadian rhythms in the patients with delayed sleep phase disorder: An open‐label trial. Chronobiol Int 2016; 33: 1037–1044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Rahman SA, Marcu S, Shapiro CM, Brown TJ & Casper RF. Spectral modulation attenuates molecular, endocrine, and neurobehavioral disruption induced by nocturnal light exposure. Am J Physiol Endoc Metab 2011; 300: E518–E527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Rahman SA, Shapiro CM, Wang F et al. Effects of filtering visual short wavelengths during nocturnal shiftwork on sleep and performance. Chronobiol Int 2013; 30: 951–962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Burkhart K & Phelps JR. Amber lenses to block blue light and improve sleep: a randomized trial. Chronobiol Int 2009; 26: 1602–1612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Esaki Y, Kitajima T, Takeuchi I et al. Effect of blue‐blocking glasses in major depressive disorder with sleep onset insomnia: a randomized, double‐blind, placebo‐controlled study. Chronobiol Int 2017; 34: 753–761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Knufinke M, Fittkau‐Koch L, Møst EIS, Kompier MAJ & Nieuwenhuys A. Restricting short‐wavelength light in the evening to improve sleep in recreational athletes ‐ A pilot study. Eur J Sport Sci 2019; 19: 728–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Sasseville A, Martin JS, Houle J & Hébert M. Investigating the contribution of short wavelengths in the alerting effect of bright light. Physiol Behav 2015; 151: 81–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. van der Lely S, Frey S, Garbazza C et al. Blue blocker glasses as a countermeasure for alerting effects of evening light‐emitting diode screen exposure in male teenagers. J Adolesc Health 2015; 56: 113–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Sasseville A, Benhaberou‐Brun D, Fontaine C, Charon M‐C & Hebert M. Wearing blue‐blockers in the morning could improve sleep of workers on a permanent night schedule: A pilot study. Chronobiol Int 2009; 26: 913–925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Sasseville A & Hébert M. Using blue‐green light at night and blue‐blockers during the day to improves adaptation to night work: A pilot study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2010; 34: 1236–1242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Henriksen TE, Skrede S, Fasmer OB et al. Blue‐blocking glasses as additive treatment for mania: a randomized placebo‐controlled trial. Bipolar Disord 2016; 18: 221–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Henriksen TEG, Skrede S, Fasmer OB, Hamre B, Grønli J & Lund A. Blocking blue light during mania ‐ markedly increased regularity of sleep and rapid improvement of symptoms: a case report. Bipolar Disord 2014; 16: 894–898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Phelps J. Dark therapy for bipolar disorder using amber lenses for blue light blockade. Med Hypotheses 2008; 70: 224–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Fargason RE, Preston T, Hammond E, May R & Gamble KL. Treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder insomnia with blue wavelength light‐blocking glasses. Chronophysiol Ther 2013; 3: 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Adams WH, Digre KB, Patel BCK, Anderson RL, Warner JEA & Katz BJ. The evaluation of light sensitivity in benign essential blepharospasm. Am J Ophthalmol 2006; 142: 82–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Herz NL & Yen MT. Modulation of sensory photophobia in essential blepharospasm with chromatic lenses. Ophthalmology 2005; 112: 2208–2211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Afra J, Ambrosini A, Genicot R, Albert A & Schoenen J. Influence of colors on habituation of visual evoked potentials in patients with migraine with aura and in healthy volunteers. Headache 2000; 40: 36–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Good PA, Taylor RH & Mortimer MJ. The use of tinted glasses in childhood migraine. Headache 1991; 31: 533–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Hoggan RN, Subhash A, Blair S et al. Thin‐film optical notch filter spectacle coatings for the treatment of migraine and photophobia. J Clin Neurosci 2016; 28: 71–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Wilkins AJ, Baker A, Amin D et al. Treatment of photosensitive epilepsy using coloured glasses. Seizure 1999; 8: 444–449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Shechter A, Kim EW, St‐Onge M‐P & Westwood AJ. Blocking nocturnal blue light for insomnia: a randomized controlled trial. J Psychiatric Res 2018; 96: 196–202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Bridgman GF. The use of restricted spectral range red lenses for the diagnosis and relief of dazzlement of a rod monochromat. Clin Exp Optom 1989; 72: 91–93. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Schornack MM, Brown WL & Siemsen DW. The use of tinted contact lenses in the management of achromatopsia. Optometry 2007; 78: 17–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Zeltzer HI. Use of modified X‐Chrom for relief of light dazzlement and color blindness of a rod monochromat. Optometry 1979; 50: 813–818. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Schwerdtfeger G & Gräf M. Kantenfilterkontaktlinse und kantenfiltergläser bei achromatopsie. Zeitschrift für Praktische Augenheilkunde 1994; 15: 322–328. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Carracedo G, Carballo J, Loma E, Felipe G & Cacho I. Contrast sensitivity evaluation with filter contact lenses in patients with retinitis pigmentosa: A pilot study. J Optom 2011; 4: 134–139. [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Rosenblum YZ, Zak PP, Ostrovsky MA et al. Spectral filters in low‐vision correction. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2000; 20: 335–341. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Katz BJ & Digre KB. Diagnosis, pathophysiology, and treatment of photophobia. Surv Ophthalmol 2016; 61: 466–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Lin JB, Gerratt BW, Bassi CJ & Apte RS. Short‐wavelength light‐blocking eyeglasses attenuate symptoms of eye fatigue. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2017; 58: 442–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Palavets T & Rosenfield M. Blue‐blocking filters and digital eyestrain. Optom Vis Sci 2019; 96: 48–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Evans BJ & Allen PM. A systematic review of controlled trials on visual stress using intuitive overlays or the intuitive colorimeter. J Optom 2016; 9: 205–218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Wilkins AJ, Evans BJ, Brown JA et al. Double‐masked placebo‐controlled trial of precision spectral filters in children who use coloured overlays. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1994; 14: 365–370. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Wilkins AJ, Patel R, Adjamian P & Evans BJ. Tinted spectacles and visually sensitive migraine. Cephalalgia 2002; 22: 711–719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Lightstone A, Lightstone T & Wilkins A. Both coloured overlays and coloured lenses can improve reading fluency, but their optimal chromaticities differ. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1999; 19: 279–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Rohatgi A.WebPlotDigitizer 4.1; 2018. https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer (Accessed 11/07/19).
  • 63. Drevon D, Fursa SR & Malcolm AL. Intercoder reliability and validity of WebPlotDigitizer in extracting graphed data. Behav Modif 2017; 41: 323–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. CIE . CIE System for Metrology of Optical Radiation for ipRGC‐Influenced Responses to Light (S 026). Central Bureau of the Commission Internationale de l'Éclairage: Vienna, Austria, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 65. CIE . Fundamental Chromaticity Diagram with Physiological Axes – Part 1 (Technical Report 170–1). Central Bureau of the Commission Internationale de l'Éclairage: Vienna, Austria, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 66. David A, Fini PT, Houser KW et al. Development of the IES method for evaluating the color rendition of light sources. Opt Express 2015; 23: 15888–15906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Watson AB & Yellott JI. A unified formula for light‐adapted pupil size. J Vis 2012; 12: 12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Gamlin PD, McDougal DH, Pokorny J, Smith VC, Yau KW & Dacey DM. Human and macaque pupil responses driven by melanopsin‐containing retinal ganglion cells. Vision Res 2007; 47(7): 946–954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. McDougal DH & Gamlin PD. The influence of intrinsically‐photosensitive retinal ganglion cells on the spectral sensitivity and response dynamics of the human pupillary light reflex. Vision Res 2010; 50: 72–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Spitschan M, Bock AS, Ryan J, Frazzetta G, Brainard DH & Aguirre GK. The human visual cortex response to melanopsin‐directed stimulation is accompanied by a distinct perceptual experience. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2017; 114: 12291–12296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Allen AE, Hazelhoff EM, Martial FP, Cajochen C & Lucas RJ. Exploiting metamerism to regulate the impact of a visual display on alertness and melatonin suppression independent of visual appearance. Sleep 2018; 41: zsy100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Ravikumar S, Thibos LN & Bradley A. Calculation of retinal image quality for polychromatic light. J Opt Soc Am A 2008; 25: 2395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Rahman SA, St Hilaire MA & Lockley SW. The effects of spectral tuning of evening ambient light on melatonin suppression, alertness and sleep. Physiol Behav 2017; 177: 221–229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Elliot AJ & Maier MA. Color psychology: effects of perceiving color on psychological functioning in humans. Ann Rev Psychol 2014; 65: 95–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Eperjesi F. Do tinted spectacle lens wearers have a different personality? Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 2007; 27: 154–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Webster MA & Leonard D. Adaptation and perceptual norms in color vision. J Opt Soc Am A 2008; 25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Delahunt PB, Webster MA, Ma L & Werner JS. Long‐term renormalization of chromatic mechanisms following cataract surgery. Vis Neurosci 2004; 21: 301–307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Neitz J, Carroll J, Yamauchi Y, Neitz M & Williams DR. Color perception is mediated by a plastic neural mechanism that is adjustable in adults. Neuron 2002; 35: 783–792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Engel SA, Wilkins AJ, Mand S, Helwig NE & Allen PM. Habitual wearers of colored lenses adapt more rapidly to the color changes the lenses produce. Vision Res 2016; 125: 41–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Table S1. List of filters.

Data S1. Visual and non‐visual properties of the examined filters.

Data S2. Spectral transmittances of the examined filters.

Data Availability Statement

All tabulated transmittance spectra and code to produce the unedited versions in the figures in this report are available on GitHub (https://github.com/spitschan/Spitschan2019_OPO). Table S1 contains the list of all filters, along with the four tabulated effects. Data S1 contains the data shown in Figure 3 and given in Table S1. Data S2 contains the transmittance spectra.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Visual and non‐visual properties of spectral filters. Columns correspond to the three main filter categories we identified (Figure 2). Row 1: Spectral transmittances of filters. Row 2: Luminous transmittance vs melanopsin transmittance. Dashed line indicates equal reduction of luminance and melanopsin, as would be the case with a spectrally uniform neutral density (ND) filter. Row 3: Chromaticity diagram. The red cross indicates chromaticity of 6500K daylight (D65) and white squares indicate chromaticities of D65 seen through the respective filters. Row 4: Colour shift vs gamut factor. See Introduction and Figure 1 for explanation.


Articles from Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES