Abstract
To survive in a complex environment, individuals form associations between environmental stimuli and rewards to organize and optimize reward seeking behaviors. The basolateral amygdala (BLA) uses these learned associations to inform decision-making processes. In this review, we describe functional projections between BLA and its cortical and striatal targets that promote learning and motivational processes central to decision-making. Specifically, we compare and contrast divergent projections from the BLA to the orbitofrontal (OFC) and to the nucleus accumbens (NAc) and examine the roles of these pathways in associative learning, value-guided decision-making, choice behaviors, as well as cue and context-driven drug seeking. Finally, we consider how these projections are involved in disorders of motivation, with a focus on Substance Use Disorder.
Keywords: Basolateral amygdala, Orbitofrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, incentive motivation, behavioral flexibility, decision-making
Introduction:
Motivation and decision-making processes depend on associations between salient, environmental cues and the natural rewards they predict, including food, water or drugs of abuse. The basolateral amygdala (BLA) is a critical brain region necessary for associative learning and decision-making processes, which depend on the learned incentive properties of cues and past and present reward value (for focused amygdala reviews, see (Cardinal et al., 2002; Fernando et al., 2013; Janak & Tye, 2015; Parkinson et al., 2001; Wassum & Izquierdo, 2015). The incentive motivational properties of reward-associated cues attract, reinforce and invigorate reward seeking, but are also pitted against changes in reward value. As such, individuals may respond based on learned incentive properties of the cues, or adjust their responding based on the current value of the reward. In this review, we discuss these competing psychological processes and how the tasks used to probe specific facets of motivated behaviors are mediated by distinct or overlapping BLA projections to cortical and striatal targets (see Figure 1 and Table 1). We focus on the nucleus accumbens (NAc) and orbitofrontal (OFC; or anterior insular cortex, when specified) to describe the distinct and overlapping functions of these areas in several behavioral paradigms that probe associative learning, value-guided decision-making, choice behaviors and cue- and context-driven drug seeking. As a result, we aim to highlight how these psychological and neurobiological processes are implicated in disorders of motivation, with an emphasis on Substance Use Disorder (SUD).
Figure 1:
Venn Schematic depicting circuitry contributions to learning and motivational processes categorized within our conceptual framework. See Table 1 for references. Areas of overlap indicate behavioral processes in which both the areas alone and the projections between areas are implicated. We operationally define 1) an incentive process as one in which reinforcing properties of rewarding outcomes transfer to reward-predictive cues, such that the motivational properties of the conditioned cues facilitate new learning or support ongoing appetitive behaviors, 2) a flexibility process as one in which changes to, or differences between, outcome value(s) facilitate changes in conditioned responding to associated cues and 3) a hybrid process as one in which both incentive and flexibility processes contribute to behavior at test. Notably, BLA alone plays a role in each of the behavioral paradigms listed (see Table 1), but for simplicity are not listed under BLA unless only BLA has been implicated. Abbreviations: BLA = Basolateral Amygdala; OFC = Orbitofrontal Cortex; NAc = Nucleus Accumbens; PIT = Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer.
Table 1:
Table listing references demonstrating circuitry contributions to learning and motivational processes categorized within our conceptual framework.
The BLA is involved in appetitive associative learning, specifically for guiding higher-order conditioning after the initial acquisition of appetitive associations. However, the BLA is not necessary for the initial first-order conditioning (FOC), but BLA stimulation increases conditioned responding (for reviews, see (See et al., 2003; Wassum & Izquierdo, 2015)); (Balleine et al., 2003; Blundell et al., 2001; Cole et al., 2013; Corbit & Balleine, 2005; Hall et al., 2001; Hatfield et al., 1996; Holland, 2016; Malvaez et al., 2015; Paton et al., 2006; Schoenbaum et al., 1999; Servonnet et al., 2020; Sias et al., 2021; Tye & Janak, 2007). Instead, the BLA is uniquely situated, anatomically and functionally, to represent previously acquired associations to facilitate higher order learning and motivational processes through interactions with downstream target regions. For the purpose of this review, we focus on the contribution of the BLA to what we broadly characterize as incentive and flexibility processes. Here, we operationally define an incentive process as one in which reinforcing properties of rewarding outcomes transfer to reward-predictive cues, such that the motivational properties of the conditioned cues facilitate new learning or support ongoing appetitive behaviors. We operationally define a flexibility process as one in which changes to, or differences between, outcome value(s) facilitate changes in conditioned responding to associated cues. Determining differences between outcome values may require tracking specific sensory features of outcomes, comparing expected and actual outcome values or choosing between high vs. low risk outcomes. We will review evidence that the BLA and its projections are necessary for incentive processes, including second-order conditioning, sign-tracking, general Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (PIT), conditioned reinforcement and cue- and context-induced drug relapse (Burke et al., 2007; Corbit & Balleine, 2005; Hall et al., 2001; Hatfield et al., 1996; Holland, 2016; Holland & Gallagher, 2003; Malvaez et al., 2015; Ostlund & Balleine, 2008; Parkinson et al., 2001). Next, we will review evidence that BLA and its projections are necessary for flexibility and decision-making processes, including outcome devaluation, over-expectation, outcome-specific PIT, risky decision-making and punished reward seeking (Balleine et al., 2003; Corbit & Balleine, 2005; Hatfield et al., 1996; Izquierdo et al., 2017; Izquierdo & Murray, 2007; Johnson et al., 2009; Málková et al., 1997; Ostlund & Balleine, 2008; Parkes & Balleine, 2013; Paton et al., 2006; Rhodes & Murray, 2013; Sias et al., 2021; Wellman et al., 2005). Overall, we aim to synthesize the literature to identify competing and complementary roles for amygdala-striatal and amygdala-cortical interactions to shape normal and dysregulated incentive learning and flexibility processes. The goal of the current review is to relate how these two general behavioral processes and associated circuitries are inherently involved in choice behaviors during decision-making for natural and drug rewards.
Amygdala-striatal and amygdala-cortical contributions to associative learning processes
Before reviewing the role of these circuitries in psychological processes, we first describe the anatomical connectivity patterns of the structures to be discussed and refer to these distinct pathways throughout the discussion where warranted. The BLA, specifically the anterior portion, is densely and reciprocally connected with OFC and neighboring anterior insular cortex (aIC) (Groenewegen et al., 1991; Kita & Kitai, 1990; Krettek & Price, 1978; Reep et al., 1996; Russchen & Price, 1984). Likewise, the anterior BLA sends dense, unidirectional projections to the NAc core, while the posterior BLA projects more heavily to the medial portion of the NAc shell (Britt et al., 2012; Brog et al., 1993; Christie et al., 1987; Groenewegen et al., 1990; Kelley et al., 1982; Kita & Kitai, 1990; Krettek & Price, 1978; Russchen & Price, 1984). A small proportion of BLA neurons have collaterals that project to both the OFC and NAc core. A double retrograde study indicates that anterior BLA➔aIC projections collateralize more with lateral portions of NAc core regions than with medial NAc shell (Shinonaga et al., 1994), suggesting the anterior portion of the BLA is anatomically situated to simultaneously influence both NAc core and these cortical regions during associative learning and value updating.
The BLA is critically involved in both Pavlovian incentive learning and flexibility processes, such as, in second order conditioning (SOC) and outcome devaluation, respectively (Hatfield et al., 1996; Holland & Rescorla, 1975; Rizley & Rescorla, 1972). These distinct behavioral processes are convenient to compare because both SOC and outcome devaluation have identical FOC (see Table 2). In both procedures rats initially learn to associate a neutral cue (e.g. light) with a rewarding food outcome. During this initial learning, the light cue acquires incentive motivational properties that promote conditioned approach to the reward location in anticipation of reward delivery. In the second phase of SOC, the light cue is paired with a separate, novel cue (e.g. auditory tone) in the absence of the reward. In so much as the light cue itself is reinforcing, the tone now elicits conditioned approach when it is presented alone at test. In contrast, in the second phase of outcome devaluation, no cues are presented. Instead, the value of the outcome is independently modified prior to testing, either through sating rats on the food reward or pairing the food reward with illness, and conditioned responding to the light cue is tested. In comparison, SOC relies upon the positive incentive value of the conditioned stimulus to promote learning about a new stimulus (second order CS), while outcome devaluation relies upon the current value of the unconditioned stimulus (US) to suppress responding to the associated cue (Holland & Rescorla, 1975). In the following sections, we review evidence for amygdala-striatal and amygdala-cortical contributions to incentive and flexible behavioral processes.
Table 2:
Table depicting training and testing parameters often used in behavioral paradigms that probe learning and motivational processes. For simplicity, we do not show control cues or groups often included in these paradigms. Under Phase I for ‘Risky decision-making’, the orange asterisk indicates various forms of high-risk outcomes that accompany high reward delivery, such as low reward probability, increased delay to next trial, footshock punishment, high effort requirement, etc. Table uses images from Biorender.com.
Psychological Process | Behavioral Paradigm | Phase I | Phase II | Test |
---|---|---|---|---|
Incentive | Second Order Conditioning | ![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
Sign-tracking | ![]() |
|||
General PIT/Cued Instrumental | ![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
Conditioned Reinforcement | ![]() |
![]() |
||
Cue/Context-induced drug relapse | ![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
Flexibility | Outcome Devaluation | ![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
Over-expectation | ![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
Outcome-specific PIT | ![]() |
![]() |
![]() |
|
Risky Decision-making | ![]() |
|||
Punished reward seeking | ![]() |
Role of BLA➔NAc communication in incentive processes
As previously stated, the BLA is not necessary for the initial FOC phase, as pre-training lesions of BLA do not affect light-food conditioning, which is common to both procedures. However, BLA lesions impair both SOC and outcome devaluation (Corbit & Balleine, 2005; Hatfield et al., 1996; Holland, 2016; Setlow, Gallagher, et al., 2002). Additionally, pre-training lesion studies also commonly implicate NAc in both SOC and outcome devaluation, suggesting some mechanistic overlap with BLA in mediating both forms of higher-order learning (McDannald et al., 2013; Singh et al., 2010). Given the dense, unilateral projections from the BLA to NAc, pre-training contralateral disconnection lesions do not impair FOC but do impair SOC, further indicating communication between BLA and NAc is critical for higher-order learning that depends on motivational properties of previously reward-associated cues (Setlow et al., 2002). Post-training lesion studies indicate the expression of SOC does not require BLA, but is instead mediated by downstream NAc. The unidirectional projections and temporally distinct involvement of the BLA and NAc in these studies indicate a transfer of information from the BLA to NAc to mediate higher-order learning (for reviews, see (Averbeck & Costa, 2017; Cardinal et al., 2002; Everitt et al., 1999; Floresco et al., 2008; Hart et al., 2014).
Consistent with the role for BLA➔NAc in driving incentive processes like SOC, the BLA➔NAc pathway is necessary for the acquisition and expression of autoshaped lever pressing, a behavior termed “sign-tracking” that is conceptualized as an incentive salience process (see Table 2) (Berridge, 1996; Berridge & Robinson, 2016; Chang et al., 2012; Robinson & Berridge, 1993). To be considered an incentive salience process, cues acquire attracting, reinforcing and invigorating properties (Flagel et al., 2009). Accordingly, sign-tracking rats approach and vigorously engage with the lever cue during Pavlovian Lever Autoshaping and show greater conditioned reinforcement, but not greater SOC, effects than rats showing less sign-tracking behavior (intermediates and goal-trackers) (Nasser et al., 2015; Robinson & Flagel, 2009). The fullest expression of sign-tracking depends on intact BLA➔NAc core communication. Contralateral inactivation of BLA➔NAc core with either lesions or chemogenetic inhibition makes sign-trackers less sensitive to the incentive properties of the lever cue and more sensitive to changes in outcome value (Chang et al., 2012; Kochli et al., 2020). Furthermore, pathway specific tools have revealed that Pavlovian appetitive cue-reward presentations activate and result in synaptic potentiation of BLA➔NAc pathway, which facilitates dopamine release in NAc (Beyeler et al., 2016; Floresco et al., 2001; Namburi et al., 2015) and inhibition of this pathway decreases consummatory responses during Pavlovian conditioning (Stuber et al., 2011). Disconnection of BLA and NAc shell reduces motivation to lever press for food when instrumental response requirements are high, suggesting this pathway is also necessary for enhanced motivation when reward is response contingent (Simmons & Neill, 2009). Altogether, there is considerable evidence that BLA➔NAc communication facilitates incentive motivational processes, in particular, those driven by the reinforcing properties of conditioned cues.
Role of BLA-OFC communication in flexibility processes
In contrast, there is substantial evidence across Pavlovian and instrumental studies that the BLA, OFC and their interactions mediate flexibility processes, indicating a role for these regions for outcome value updating and the use of this information for adaptive behavior in the face of changing outcome value (Burke et al., 2007; Corbit & Balleine, 2005; Gallagher et al., 1999; Hatfield et al., 1996; Johnson et al., 2009; Lichtenberg et al., 2017; Málková et al., 1997; Malvaez et al., 2019; Ostlund & Balleine, 2008; Parkes & Balleine, 2013; Pickens et al., 2003, 2005; Rhodes & Murray, 2013; Wellman et al., 2005; West et al., 2012). Post-training BLA lesions do not affect Pavlovian SOC or single reinforcer outcome devaluation; however, they do impair Pavlovian and instrumental outcome devaluation when specific outcome representations are needed to appropriately suppress responding (Izquierdo & Murray, 2007; Johnson et al., 2009; Pickens et al., 2003; Setlow et al., 2002; Wellman et al., 2005; West et al., 2012). The latter effect implicates BLA’s known role for representing specific sensory properties of outcomes used by projection regions like OFC and IC to update actions or responding to cues when outcome values are changed. As such, when the OFC (including neighboring IC) is lesioned or inactivated either before FOC or immediately prior to outcome value updating, rats fail to suppress conditioned responding during the devaluation probe test (Gallagher et al., 1999; Pickens et al., 2003, 2005; West et al., 2011). These results indicate that both the BLA and OFC are involved in updating outcome value to guide appropriate responding to reward associated cues during probe tests.
Pathway specific studies provide additional insight into the interaction between BLA and OFC during flexibility processes including outcome devaluation. Contralateral inactivation of BLA-IC in rats and BLA-OFC in primates disrupts outcome devaluation (Baxter et al., 2000; Fiuzat et al., 2017; Parkes & Balleine, 2013). Unlike the unidirectional pathway from BLA➔NAc, the BLA and OFC have dense reciprocal connections that warrant temporal- and pathway-specific neural manipulations. The aforementioned contralateral disconnection studies are very informative in that they indicate the necessity of BLA and OFC communication for value updating and action selection, but do not elucidate the directionality or temporal specificity for each pathway in behavior. The use of a sequential cross-hemispheric temporal inactivation technique revealed that BLA is necessary for initial acquisition and value updating, while the IC is needed for retrieval of the current outcome value during the devaluation test (Parkes & Balleine, 2013). This is consistent with earlier lesion work indicating the BLA must be intact during the unconditioned stimulus value manipulation (i.e. satiation; conditioned taste aversion learning) but not during the value retrieval process necessary for devaluation expression, particularly when multiple outcomes must be discriminated (Johnson et al., 2009; Wellman et al., 2005; West et al., 2012). In contrast, the IC is necessary to retrieve the updated outcome value during test, similar to the role of anterior portions of OFC in non-human primates (Murray et al., 2015; Parkes & Balleine, 2013; West et al., 2011). Tests for the role of direct, monosynaptic communication in these reciprocally connected pathways comes from a chemogenetic study that inhibits BLA or OFC terminals via designer receptors activated by designer drugs (DREADDs) (Lichtenberg et al., 2017). When the BLA➔OFC pathway is inactivated during value retrieval, rats displayed insensitivity to specific satiety-induced outcome devaluation, consistent involvement of not only BLA but the BLA➔OFC communication when the current value of multiple reinforcers must be represented for appropriate responding. Importantly, this was only true for Pavlovian but not instrumental outcome devaluation, departing from BLA’s common role in these processes (Johnson et al., 2009; Lichtenberg et al., 2017).
Lesion and recording studies point towards information flow from OFC to BLA in facilitating flexible associative encoding during learning, value reversal and value expectation (Lucantonio et al., 2015; Morrison et al., 2011; Saddoris et al., 2005; Stalnaker, Franz, et al., 2007), with lateral and medial OFC inputs to BLA participating in encoding and retrieval of positive reward value, respectively (Malvaez et al., 2019). Pavlovian over-expectation is a procedure that also requires outcome value inference when responding to cues, and OFC influences neural activity in the BLA during this task. First, two separately conditioned cues predict the same outcome (eg. 2 pellets; see Table 2). Next, the two cues are presented in compound, but instead of predicting double the reward, the amount of reward is consistent with what was associated with either cue alone (eg. 2 pellets). Violating rats’ expectations during compound conditioning results in a spontaneous decrease in conditioned responding when the cues are again presented alone during a probe test. The BLA is not necessary for decreasing conditioned responding during the probe test, but the OFC is necessary for this over-expectation effect. The OFC is also necessary for driving BLA neural activity representing outcome estimates during compound conditioning and for spontaneous decrements in cue-evoked BLA neural activity during the expression of over-expectation during the probe test (Haney et al., 2010; Lucantonio et al., 2015; Takahashi et al., 2009, 2013). Together, devaluation and over-expectation studies indicate that reciprocal BLA-OFC communication is necessary for outcome value updating and reward expectations that drive adaptive behaviors.
Role of BLA➔NAc in flexibility processes
The NAc is also necessary for outcome devaluation, but subregion specific. Bilateral lesions of the either NAc core or shell result in insensitivity to illness-induced outcome devaluation (Singh et al., 2010). However other studies suggest NAc core and shell subregions are differentially involved in the devaluation process (West & Carelli, 2016). The greater proportion of cue-selective NAc core neurons during initial instrumental discrimination predicts better behavioral suppression during outcome devaluation. Conversely, NAc shell neuron activity better discriminates valued and devalued cues during test (West & Carelli, 2016). These results indicate the core is involved in behavioral acquisition and the shell in outcome value tracking. Despite these findings relating neural activity and behavior, disconnection of the BLA and NAc shell does not alter devaluation sensitivity, while BLA and NAc core disconnection abolished instrumental specific-satiety devaluation (Shiflett & Balleine, 2010). Chemogenetic contralateral inactivation of the BLA and NAc core also eliminates specific satiety-induced Pavlovian outcome devaluation, but only in goal-tracking rats (Kochli et al., 2020). In contrast, BLA and NAc core contralateral inactivation makes otherwise devaluation insensitive sign-tracking rats sensitive to outcome devaluation (Keefer et al., 2020; Kochli et al., 2020; Morrison et al., 2015; Nasser et al., 2015). These findings suggest individual variability in BLA➔NAc core communication for mediating incentive and reward value representations.
The aforementioned studies indicate that communication between BLA and NAc is necessary for flexibility processes such as outcome devaluation. However, these studies use contralateral disconnection techniques to inactivate one brain region in one hemisphere and the other brain region in the opposite hemisphere to examine BLA and NAc’s involvement in outcome devaluation. To our knowledge, no study has yet investigated the involvement of direct projections from BLA➔NAc with pathway-specific manipulation techniques in outcome devaluation. It remains likely that direct BLA➔NAc pathway communication is involved in these behaviors, but there could be another intermediary brain region that receives input from BLA and projects to NAc that could explain these findings. Indeed, the BLA projects to OFC and OFC projects to NAc, leaving opportunity for the BLA to indirectly influence NAc through OFC (Berendse et al., 1992; Brog et al., 1993). Another contralateral disconnection study showed IC-NAc communication is necessary for outcome devaluation (Parkes et al., 2015); however, due to reciprocal connections with the BLA, the IC could be influencing NAc indirectly through BLA in this study. Nevertheless, future direct pathway manipulation studies are warranted to tease apart the contribution of these specific projections in outcome devaluation.
Taken together, the seminal work on BLA➔NAc and BLA-OFC contributions to SOC, autoshaping, outcome devaluation and over-expectation point towards some overlapping, but also distinct roles for these amygdala pathways for incentive and flexible learning processes that rely on conditioned stimulus (CS) properties and/or current outcome (US) value. Notably, these associative representations between cues and outcomes are at the root of many learning and decision-making processes that are relevant for normal and dysregulated motivation.
Role of BLA projections in general and specific learning processes
Evidence for amygdala-striatal and amygdala-cortical contributions to incentive motivational processes are also further explored using Pavlovian to Instrumental Transfer (PIT) and conditioned reinforcement paradigms (see Table 2). We provide evidence for these circuitry contributions together, because both paradigms can be evaluated based on general and cue-specific representations. Similar to SOC and outcome devaluation, rats learn a first-order Pavlovian cue-outcome association. In PIT, rats next learn an operant (lever) association with the same outcome. During test, the lever cues are continuously available and presentation of the Pavlovian cue invigorates responding on the instrumental lever. PIT relies on retrieval from previously learned Pavlovian associations to invigorate instrumental responding. Studies have evaluated the neural underpinnings that are responsible for invigorated responding in two forms of PIT, general and specific. General PIT refers to the increased responding on multiple levers, independent of the associated outcome. Specific PIT refers to the increased responding on the lever specifically associated with the common Pavlovian and instrumental outcome but not another lever associated with a different outcome. Similarly, conditioned reinforcement studies examine the extent to which a Pavlovian cue acquires motivational significance such that it can serve as a reinforcer itself. After Pavlovian FOC, rats acquire a novel instrumental action that is reinforced with the Pavlovian reward-associated cue itself in the absence of the reward (e.g. lever press➔light) to assess the incentive, motivational properties of the conditioned cue itself. Similar to PIT, conditioned reinforcement studies have also used sophisticated behavioral designs to tease apart general affective and outcome-specific representations that mediate the reinforcing properties of conditioned cues.
Similar to the lack of BLA lesion effects on acquisition of Pavlovian associations, pre-training BLA lesions also do not affect the acquisition of instrumental responding. However, the BLA is necessary for the expression of outcome-specific, but not general, PIT (Blundell et al., 2001; Corbit & Balleine, 2005; Derman et al., 2020; Scarlet et al., 2012; Sias et al., 2021). Disconnection of BLA➔NAc shell impairs outcome-specific PIT, while disconnection of BLA➔NAc core does not, a set of findings that is consistent with reports that bilateral lesions or inactivation of NAc shell, but not core, also abolish outcome-specific PIT (Corbit & Balleine, 2011; Shiflett & Balleine, 2010). Conversely, bilateral lesions or inactivation of NAc core, but not shell, abolish general PIT (Corbit & Balleine, 2011; Hall et al., 2001). Similar to the BLA and NAc shell, the OFC is necessary for outcome-specific PIT, and OFC neurons show Pavlovian CS-specific activity correlated with the strength of PIT behaviors (Homayoun & Moghaddam, 2009; Scarlet et al., 2012). Two recent chemogenetic studies demonstrate the involvement of BLA and OFC communication in stimulus-outcome associative learning that is necessary for the expression of outcome-specific PIT (Lichtenberg et al., 2017; Sias et al., 2021). Using inhibitory opsins and chemogenetic inactivation to inhibit terminals in reciprocally connected BLA and OFC, these studies showed inactivation of the BLA and OFC➔BLA during the initial cue-reward learning abolished outcome-specific PIT. While inactivation of OFC➔BLA during PIT test had no effect on behavior, inactivation of BLA➔OFC disrupted the expression of outcome-specific PIT. Consistent findings come from a recent sequential inactivation approach that inhibits OFC➔BLA in one hemisphere during cue-reward learning and inhibits BLA➔OFC in the other hemisphere during PIT test, which provides evidence for the direction of information flow; OFC➔BLA for encoding of specific cue-reward associations and BLA➔OFC for expression of cue-specific behavioral invigoration (Lichtenberg et al., 2017; Sias et al., 2021). These results further clarify the critical role for BLA and its projections to OFC and NAc shell in representing specific cue-outcome associations for invigorating specific actions, whereas NAc core participates in general invigoration of instrumental responding.
Conditioned reinforcement studies also point towards overlapping roles of BLA and its striatal and cortical targets in incentive learning processes. In parallel with the aforementioned behavioral paradigms, the BLA is necessary for conditioned reinforcement effects in primates and rats (Burke et al., 2007; Parkinson et al., 2001). Consistent with the outcome specific-PIT and devaluation work, the OFC is also necessary for outcome-specific conditioned reinforcement, but not for general affective contributions to conditioned reinforcement (Burke et al., 2008). Lesion studies have not identified a role for NAc in conditioned reinforcement itself, but have identified opposite roles for NAc core and shell in amphetamine-potentiated conditioned reinforcement effects (Parkinson et al., 1999; Singh et al., 2010). The BLA is necessary for mediating the potentiating effect of intra-NAc shell, but not NAc core, amphetamine on conditioned reinforcement, suggesting a synergistic role of BLA input to NAc in mediating dopamine-dependent behaviors. Optogenetic work has shown that both BLA➔NAc pathway activation and VTA➔NAc core dopamine neuron activation alone are reinforcing, supporting self-stimulation and conditioned reinforcement, respectively (Saunders et al., 2018; Stuber et al., 2011). NAc dopamine receptor 1 signaling is necessary for optogenetic BLA➔NAc self-stimulation, which is consistent with earlier work showing that BLA and dopaminergic convergence in NAc facilitates reward seeking in discriminative stimulus instrumental tasks (Ambroggi et al., 2008; Jones et al., 2010). Notably, VTA➔NAc core, but not VTA➔NAc shell, dopamine neuron optical stimulation alone is sufficient for conditioned reinforcement. These findings depart from the lesion work showing a necessary role for BLA to influence NAc shell amphetamine-induced potentiation of conditioned reinforcement (Parkinson et al., 1999; Saunders et al., 2018). These inconsistencies on the potential role of BLA and dopaminergic input to NAc may be due to limitations of dopamine agonist and optogenetic dopamine activation manipulations to approximate the endogenous mechanisms underlying conditioned reinforcement.
While such inconsistencies may be reconciled by causal role manipulations of BLA➔NAc and VTA➔NAc systems during conditioned reinforcement, we are not aware of such studies to date. Some hints may come from BLA➔NAc cross lesion and NAc DA antagonist studies, both of which are sufficient to disrupt the acquisition and/or expression of sign-tracking responses during Pavlovian lever autoshaping (Chang et al., 2012; Saunders & Robinson, 2012). Sign-tracking rats, that are strongly attracted to Pavlovian cues, also show strong conditioned reinforcement effects while intermediate and goal-tracking rats do not (Robinson & Flagel, 2009). Thus sign-tracking, which depends on both BLA➔NAc input and NAc core DA signaling, may provide a useful tool for determining the casual relationship between BLA inputs and DA in the nucleus accumbens for driving conditioned reinforcement effects (Chang et al., 2012; Robinson & Flagel, 2009; Saunders et al., 2018; Saunders & Robinson, 2012).
Incentive and flexibility processes are higher order learning processes that are behaviorally distinct and often rely on discrete, but sometimes overlapping, neurobiological substrates. Usually, the learning processes discussed herein require initial learning of two or more cue-outcome associations. Whether it is discrimination of separate cues signaling distinct outcomes (i.e. discrimination learning), discrimination of a reinforced from a non-reinforced cue-outcome association (CS+ vs CS-) or discrimination of an appetitive and aversive association (i.e. sucrose vs. quinine in go/no-go tasks), BLA and its projections support distinct associative representations for use in higher order learning and motivational processes. These include value discrimination, reversal learning, and extinction which have been previously and comprehensively reviewed (Averbeck & Costa, 2017; Fernando et al., 2013; Goode & Maren, 2019; Izquierdo et al., 2017; Manning et al., 2021; Sharpe & Schoenbaum, 2016; Wassum & Izquierdo, 2015; Yizhar & Klavir, 2018) and while not detailed here, are important constructs for understanding the amygdala-cortico-striatal contributions to complex decision-making processes.
Role of BLA and its projections in choice behaviors
The contributions of amygdala-striatal and amygdala-cortical interactions to specific associative learning processes described thus far, rarely operate in isolation, particularly in the organization of human behavior. Adaptive and maladaptive decision-making in humans is highly complex and relies on the physiological processes described here, acting in synchrony with sensory, motor, and cognitive processes to mediate choices that benefit the individual. While some decisions may be adaptive in the short term, they may not be the most efficient strategy to maximize reward and avoid punishment in the long term. Such choices are based on learned associations and expectations about future events, and perhaps, not surprisingly, rely on similar underlying brain circuitry. While a distributed network of brain regions is involved in cost-benefit decision-making, rodent studies that aim to model more complex facets of human decision-making also implicate the BLA➔NAc and BLA-OFC circuitries.
Various rodent tasks are used to examine the brain circuits driving risky decision-making, including the risky decision-making task (rDT), the rodent Iowa Gambling Task (rIGT), and probabilistic and effort discounting tasks, as well as the rat “Blackjack” task. The various tasks share some common features, namely pitting “low risk/low reward” options against “high risk/high reward” options. “Low risk/low reward” options are typically small immediate rewards delivered at high probabilities. Meanwhile, the high-risk option is associated with some form of negative consequences for choosing that option. The negative consequence of “high risk” option is typically what varies between tasks, and may include low probability of reward, delay to next trial, footshock punishment, and/or high effort requirement (e.g. many lever presses or overcoming physical obstacle). Depending on the task used, post-training BLA lesion and pharmacological inactivation studies indicate that BLA acts to either inhibit or promote responding for large risky rewards (footshock, delay to next trial, probabilistic reinforcement, high effort) (Ghods-Sharifi et al., 2009; Orsini et al., 2015; Ostrander et al., 2011; Zeeb & Winstanley, 2011). When negative consequences of choosing the large reward option involves footshock or increasing delays to next trial, rats with inactivation of the BLA chose risky reward options more often than intact rats (Orsini et al., 2015; Zeeb & Winstanley, 2011). When large rewards are delivered with decreasing probabilities or increased physical effort, BLA inactivation results in choice of safer options (Ghods-Sharifi et al., 2009). Optogenetic work identifies a more temporally-specific role for BLA in the push and pull of cost-benefit decision-making, such that BLA activity during the deliberation period promotes risky decision-making, while BLA activity during delivery of high-risk (large reward, probabilistic footshock punished) rewards promotes choice of the “low-risk/low reward”, or “safe” option (Orsini et al., 2017). The communication of these temporally specific signals to downstream cortical and striatal targets mediates complex interactions between interconnected nuclei to orchestrate evaluative and decisive action in diverse environments.
The similarly contrasting findings for OFC suggest there may be analogous nuance in OFC’s involvement in risky decision-making. Consistent with the role for the BLA in promoting low-risk choices, there is evidence that the OFC is also necessary to inhibit risky decision-making during learning. Lesions of the OFC, and its connections with BLA, result in riskier choices during learning, slowing the acquisition of optimal response strategies when large rewards are delivered with lower probabilities and are associated with increased delays to next trial (Zeeb & Winstanley, 2011, 2013). However, BLA-OFC’s role is time limited unless sensory-specific features or representations are needed for promoting optimal decision-making (Zeeb & Winstanley, 2013). In contrast, footshock punished choices of large reward options are decreased by OFC lesions, suggesting OFC promotes choice of higher value rewards when probabilistic consequences are aversive (Orsini et al., 2015). Similarly, NAc core lesions bias choices towards smaller, more certain rewards, suggesting for probabilistic large rewards, both BLA and NAc promote “riskier” choices when intact (Cardinal & Howes, 2005; Ghods-Sharifi et al., 2009; Stopper & Floresco, 2011; van Holstein et al., 2020). However, one study identified NAc’s role in promoting large uncertain reward choices only when the “risky” choices lead to greater payout over time, suggesting the NAc-driven ‘win-stay’ behavioral strategy biases rats towards optimal behavior over time (Stopper & Floresco, 2011). Similarly, contralateral disconnection and inactivation of BLA terminals in NAc reduced large uncertain reward choice (St. Onge et al., 2012; van Holstein et al., 2020), particularly when odds were good (reinforced 50% of the time); but the opposite was true when odds were poor (reinforced 12.5% of the time), further lending support to the idea that BLA and NAc communication facilitates long term gains. Consistent with pharmacological disconnection, optogenetic work identifies temporally specific roles for BLA➔NAc communication. Inhibition of this pathway during deliberation reduced preferred choice across good and poor odds blocks, whereas BLA➔NAc terminal inhibition during reward omission increased large risky reward seeking on poor odds trials (Bercovici et al., 2018). These results suggest the BLA➔NAc pathway orchestrates optimal decision-making under reward uncertainty.
Navigating the complex choices in these rodent decision-making tasks requires evaluation of learned associations and expectations about future events, whether they have expected or unexpected outcome values. The extent to which there is individual variability in the psychological and neurobiological processes underlying incentive motivation and behavioral flexibility, suggests there may be paralleled individual variability in the engagement of amygdala-cortical and amygdala-striatal circuitry driving complex decision-making. While this remains to be determined, the implication may be that certain behavioral phenotypes are more prone to develop maladaptive behaviors that are characterized by aberrant decision-making, like SUD and gambling (Barrus et al., 2016). This inference is supported by behavioral evidence from separate rodent studies showing that both sign-tracking rats and risk-preferring rats show increased discrete cue-triggered cocaine relapse, increased incubation of cocaine-craving and increased choice of cocaine versus food reward (Ferland & Winstanley, 2017; Saunders et al., 2013; Saunders & Robinson, 2010; Tunstall & Kearns, 2015). In contrast, goal-tracking rats show greater vulnerability to drug associated discriminative and contextual stimuli (Pitchers et al., 2017; Saunders et al., 2014). Next, we review evidence from rodent studies for amygdala-cortical and amygdala-striatal contributions to drug-seeking, drug-relapse and drug choice.
Role of BLA and its projections in drug seeking, relapse and choice
The incentive properties of cues to attract, reinforce and invigorate responding is remarkably evident of drug associated stimuli (Cardinal et al., 2002; Di Chiara, 1999; Everitt et al., 1999; Robinson & Berridge, 1993; Wise, 1996). In rodents, when discrete cues (e.g. light, tone, lever) or environmental contexts are repeatedly paired with delivery of self-administered drug reward, the incentive properties of drug-associated cues and context promote responding for the drug, even in the absence of drug reinforcement (see Table 2). In many cases, incentive properties of cues and contexts drive drug seeking despite drug non-availability, history of punishment or choice of alternative reinforcers. Thus, despite flexibility of drug seeking during extinction, punishment induced- or voluntary- abstinence, discrete and diffuse environmental stimuli remain powerful drivers of drug-seeking actions. In essence, incentive processes overcome flexibility processes to promote maladaptive behavior in addiction. In this section, we discuss how BLA and its projections described in this review serve to promote cue- and context-driven drug-seeking. We conclude with a discussion of drug choice procedures to evaluate the state of understanding for amygdala and its cortical and striatal targets in mediating adaptive and maladaptive decision associated with SUD.
In humans, neuroimaging studies indicate amygdala activation when presented with drug-associated cues (reviewed in Jasinska et al., 2014; Sharp, 2017). In rodents trained to self-administer cocaine, BLA neurons respond to cocaine-associated cues (Carelli et al., 2003). The BLA is necessary for drug-associated stimuli to control drug seeking following abstinence (for reviews, see (See et al., 2003; Wassum & Izquierdo, 2015). BLA lesions, pharmacological and optogenetic inactivation decrease cue- and context-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking after extinction (Fuchs et al., 2005; Kantak et al., 2002; Meil & See, 1997; See et al., 2003; Stefanik & Kalivas, 2013). In support of BLA’s involvement in drug-stimuli associative learning, the BLA is also necessary for memory consolidation of drug-stimulus associations. Repeated pairings of drugs of abuse with drug-associated cues or contexts result in the formation, consolidation, reactivation and reconsolidation of drug-stimuli associative memories (Sorg, 2012). BLA pharmacological inactivation attenuates consolidation of cocaine-associated cue and context memory (Fuchs et al., 2006, 2009; Gabriele & See, 2010).
Both amygdala-cortical and amygdala-striatal interplay is vital in maintaining learned associations between cues and contexts paired with drug taking. In abstinent cocaine users, cocaine and exposure to cocaine-related stimuli increase neural activity in both the BLA and OFC. Enhanced BLA and OFC activation is associated with an increase in self-reported cocaine craving (Childress et al., 1999; Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Grant et al., 1996; Volkow et al., 2003). Further, BLA and OFC activation is attenuated in cocaine users treated with the GABAB agonist, baclofen, with concomitant reduced cocaine cue-induced craving, further implicating their similar roles and interaction in cue-induced relapse (Brebner et al., 2002; Young et al., 2014). In rodent studies, BLA, OFC and their interactions are necessary to reinstate extinguished drug seeking in response to drug-paired cues and context. BLA or lateral OFC inactivation disrupt drug context-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking behavior in rats (Fuchs et al., 2005; Lasseter et al., 2009). Moreover, chemogenetic inactivation of OFC inhibits cue-induced reinstatement of ethanol and sucrose seeking (Hernandez et al., 2020). An earlier study showed that contralateral or ipsilateral disconnection of BLA and lateral OFC attenuates context-induced cocaine-seeking behavior, suggesting functional interdependence between these structures in this behavior (Lasseter et al., 2011). Since BLA and lateral OFC are reciprocally connected, the direction of information transfer during this behavior remained elusive. In a pathway-specific manipulation study, optogenetic inhibition of lateral OFC➔BLA projections, but not BLA➔lateral OFC projections, diminished cue-induced cocaine seeking behavior (Arguello et al., 2017). As discussed in previous sections, BLA➔OFC, but not OFC➔BLA, communication facilitates cue-induced outcome-specific reward expectations and promotes natural reward seeking in outcome-specific PIT (Lichtenberg et al., 2017). These two studies are seemingly at odds (Arguello et al., 2017; Lichtenberg et al., 2017). There are obvious differences in drug and natural rewards as unconditioned stimuli, but one possibility for the divergent involvement of BLA-OFC communication in these studies is the use of single versus multiple reinforcers. There may be limited involvement of BLA➔OFC circuitry when sensory specific features of rewards are not necessary to track for appropriate responding. Consistent with this, when a single instrumental reinforcer is used, lateral OFC to BLA inhibition was not necessary for cue-induced relapse to food seeking (Arguello et al., 2017). Furthermore, the role for BLA➔OFC in representing sensory-specific features is limited to Pavlovian and not instrumental associations (Lichtenberg et al., 2017). However, OFC➔BLA is necessary during stimulus-outcome encoding to later invigorate instrumental responding (Sias et al., 2021). Perhaps, cue-induced relapse tests better reflect an invigorated reward seeking process that relies on OFC➔BLA memories. From a plasticity perspective, it is also possible that drug exposure may strengthen OFC➔BLA pathway to a greater degree than with natural reward experience and thus usurp behavioral control over cue-driven drug-seeking. Studies also indicate a role for OFC and BLA-OFC interactions in behavioral flexibility after drug experience. OFC-dependent outcome-guided behaviors and reversal learning are impaired after drug exposure (Calu et al., 2007; Schoenbaum & Setlow, 2005; Schoenbaum & Shaham, 2008; Stalnaker et al., 2009; Stalnaker et al., 2007a). Rigid associative encoding in the BLA is the culprit behind OFC-dependent reversal learning deficits after cocaine exposure (McCracken & Grace, 2013; Saddoris et al., 2005; Stalnaker et al., 2007b; Stalnaker et al., 2007a).
BLA or NAc core lesions or direct chemogenetic inhibition of BLA➔NAc core projections reduce cocaine seeking under second-order schedules of reinforcement in which drug-associated cues are the primary driver of drug-seeking (Ito et al., 2004; Puaud et al., 2020; Whitelaw et al., 1996). These findings suggest a critical role for BLA➔NAc projection in the conditioned reinforcing properties of cocaine-associated cues. A study using asymmetrical neuropharmacological disconnection of the BLA➔NAc pathway by antagonizing BLA dopamine receptors and NAc core glutamate receptors also reduced cocaine seeking in a second order schedule of cocaine reinforcement (Di Ciano & Everitt, 2004). Studies using ethanol as a Pavlovian reinforcer strongly implicate BLA, NAc and their projections in the attracting and reinforcing properties of alcohol-associated cues (Chaudhri et al., 2013; Millan et al., 2017; Villaruel & Chaudhri, 2016). In addition, BLA and NAc core are active during the reconsolidation of a cocaine-induced condition place preference memory suggesting that these structures are also important for reconsolidation of a cue-drug memory (Théberge et al., 2010). Overall, these studies provide evidence for functional interaction between the BLA and NAc core in cocaine seeking maintained by cocaine-associated cues. Recent advances in circuit-specific techniques have provided direct evidence for the role of BLA➔NAc projections in cue-driven reward seeking. In support of this, BLA➔NAc core neurons are activated during cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking as indicated by Fos (immediate early gene) analysis (McGlinchey et al., 2016). In addition, optogenetic inhibition of BLA➔NAc core also reduced cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking (Stefanik & Kalivas, 2013). Moreover, ablation of BLA➔NAc projections and chemogenetic inhibition of BLA➔NAc core glutamatergic projections reduced cue-induced reinstatement of alcohol seeking and cue-induced cocaine seeking under second order schedule of reinforcement respectively (Keistler et al., 2017; Puaud et al., 2020). BLA➔NAc synapses have calcium permeable AMPA receptors inserted into the silent synapses during incubation of cocaine craving (Lee et al., 2013) suggesting synaptic plasticity in this amygdala-striatal pathway as a critical mechanism underlying incubation of cocaine craving (for review see (Dong et al., 2017). Incubation describes a time-dependent increase in responding for drug-associated cues that occurs in the absence of extinction. Thus, incubation isolates the incentive properties of drug-associated stimuli to drive behavior in the absence of a competing flexibility process as is the case in many other addiction models.
While we are unaware of studies examining the role of BLA and its projections in mediating choice between drugs and alternative reinforcers, such models are increasingly used in an effort to better model human decision-making in SUD (Ahmed et al., 2013; Beckmann et al., 2019; Chow & Beckmann, 2021; Lenoir et al., 2013; Vandaele et al., 2016; Venniro et al., 2019, 2020). Recent evidence indicates that NAc core, but not NAc shell, is critical for methamphetamine relapse in a procedure that gives rats choice between food and drug to promote voluntary abstinence (Rossi et al., 2020). In a cocaine versus food choice procedure, OFC, but not NAc, neuronal activation was associated with cocaine preference (Chow et al., 2021). There is mounting evidence implicating OFC encoding of cocaine and heroin-associated choices (Guillem et al., 2018; Guillem & Ahmed, 2018, 2019, 2020).
Certain rodent models of addiction incorporate facets of the choice tasks described in the previous section, including delay to reward and punished reward seeking. There is evidence that cocaine exposure shifts delay discounting and BLA encoding of delayed rewards (Roesch et al., 2007; Zuo et al., 2012). Evidence consistent with studies suggesting a role for BLA in suppressing risky choices comes from the punishment induced abstinence model, a procedure that gives rats a choice to seek or suppress drug seeking in the presence of probabilistic footshock of increasing intensity (Campbell et al., 2017; Orsini et al., 2015, 2017; Pelloux et al., 2018). BLA neuronal activation is evident after cue-induced reward seeking after punishment, and BLA inactivation increases context-induced relapse to cocaine seeking after punishment-induced abstinence (Campbell et al., 2017; Pelloux et al., 2018). These studies suggest BLA is important for suppressing “risky” drug seeking behaviors. The conceptual and mechanistic bridge linking the BLA’s role in natural and drug reward choice also requires careful consideration of the neurobiological mechanisms of punishment, probabilistic reinforcement and context (Badiani et al., 2011; Bouton et al., 2006; Crombag et al., 2008; Jean-Richard-Dit-Bressel et al., 2018; Khoo et al., 2017; Vandaele et al., 2016).
While there are many unanswered questions regarding the role of amygdala-cortical and amygdala-striatal projections in choice between drugs vs non-drug alternatives, there is considerable evidence for the reviewed circuitry in driving incentive motivation and flexible decision-making processes. This review highlights the importance of probing the basolateral amygdala and its projections for understanding adaptive motivated behavior and dysregulated behaviors observed in addiction and other disorders of motivation.
Acknowledgements:
This work was supported by a National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) grant R01DA043533, a McKnight Memory and Cognitive Disorders Award (McKnight Foundation), and the Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology at the University of Maryland, School of Medicine. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection, and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Ahmed SH, Lenoir M, & Guillem K (2013). Neurobiology of addiction versus drug use driven by lack of choice. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(4), 581–587. 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ambroggi F, Ishikawa A, Fields HL, & Nicola SM (2008). Basolateral amygdala neurons facilitate reward-seeking behavior by exciting nucleus accumbens neurons. Neuron, 59(4), 648–661. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arguello AA, Richardson BD, Hall JL, Wang R, Hodges MA, Mitchell MP, Stuber GD, Rossi DJ, & Fuchs RA (2017). Role of a Lateral Orbital Frontal Cortex-Basolateral Amygdala Circuit in Cue-Induced Cocaine-Seeking Behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(3), 727–735. 10.1038/npp.2016.157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Averbeck BB, & Costa VD (2017). Motivational neural circuits underlying reinforcement learning. Nature Neuroscience, 20(4), 505–512. 10.1038/nn.4506 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badiani A, Belin D, Epstein D, Calu D, & Shaham Y (2011). Opiate versus psychostimulant addiction: The differences do matter. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 12(11), 685–700. 10.1038/nrn3104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, Killcross AS, & Dickinson A (2003). The effect of lesions of the basolateral amygdala on instrumental conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 23(2), 666–675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barrus MM, Cherkasova M, & Winstanley CA (2016). Skewed by Cues? The Motivational Role of Audiovisual Stimuli in Modelling Substance Use and Gambling Disorders. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 27, 507–529. 10.1007/7854_2015_393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baxter MG, Parker A, Lindner CC, Izquierdo AD, & Murray EA (2000). Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction of amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 20(11), 4311–4319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann JS, Chow JJ, & Hutsell BA (2019). Cocaine-associated decision-making: Toward isolating preference. Neuropharmacology, 153, 142–152. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.03.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bercovici DA, Princz-Lebel O, Tse MT, Moorman DE, & Floresco SB (2018). Optogenetic Dissection of Temporal Dynamics of Amygdala-Striatal Interplay during Risk/Reward Decision Making. ENeuro, 5(6). 10.1523/ENEURO.0422-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berendse HW, Galis-de Graaf Y, & Groenewegen HJ (1992). Topographical organization and relationship with ventral striatal compartments of prefrontal corticostriatal projections in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 316(3), 314–347. 10.1002/cne.903160305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC (1996). Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 20(1), 1–25. 10.1016/0149-7634(95)00033-b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge Kent C., & Robinson TE (2016). Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. The American Psychologist, 71(8), 670–679. 10.1037/amp0000059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beyeler A, Namburi P, Glober GF, Simonnet C, Calhoon GG, Conyers GF, Luck R, Wildes CP, & Tye KM (2016). Divergent Routing of Positive and Negative Information from the Amygdala during Memory Retrieval. Neuron, 90(2), 348–361. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blundell P, Hall G, & Killcross S (2001). Lesions of the basolateral amygdala disrupt selective aspects of reinforcer representation in rats. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 21(22), 9018–9026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Westbrook RF, Corcoran KA, & Maren S (2006). Contextual and temporal modulation of extinction: Behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry, 60(4), 352–360. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brebner K, Childress AR, & Roberts DCS (2002). A potential role for GABA(B) agonists in the treatment of psychostimulant addiction. Alcohol and Alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire), 37(5), 478–484. 10.1093/alcalc/37.5.478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Britt JP, Benaliouad F, McDevitt RA, Stuber GD, Wise RA, & Bonci A (2012). Synaptic and Behavioral Profile of Multiple Glutamatergic Inputs to the Nucleus Accumbens. Neuron, 76(4), 790–803. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brog JS, Salyapongse A, Deutch AY, & Zahm DS (1993). The patterns of afferent innervation of the core and shell in the “accumbens” part of the rat ventral striatum: Immunohistochemical detection of retrogradely transported fluoro-gold. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 338(2), 255–278. 10.1002/cne.903380209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke KA, Franz TM, Miller DN, & Schoenbaum G (2007). Conditioned reinforcement can be mediated by either outcome-specific or general affective representations. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 1, 2. 10.3389/neuro.07.002.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke KA, Franz TM, Miller DN, & Schoenbaum G (2008). The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in the pursuit of happiness and more specific rewards. Nature, 454(7202), 340–344. 10.1038/nature06993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calu DJ, Stalnaker TA, Franz TM, Singh T, Shaham Y, & Schoenbaum G (2007). Withdrawal from cocaine self-administration produces long-lasting deficits in orbitofrontal-dependent reversal learning in rats. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 14(5), 325–328. 10.1101/lm.534807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell EJ, Barker DJ, Nasser HM, Kaganovsky K, Dayas CV, & Marchant NJ (2017). Cue-induced food seeking after punishment is associated with increased Fos expression in the lateral hypothalamus and basolateral and medial amygdala. Behavioral Neuroscience, 131(2), 155–167. 10.1037/bne0000185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardinal RN, & Howes NJ (2005). Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core on choice between small certain rewards and large uncertain rewards in rats. BMC Neuroscience, 6, 37. 10.1186/1471-2202-6-37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, Hall J, & Everitt BJ (2002). Emotion and motivation: The role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(3), 321–352. 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00007-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carelli RM, Williams JG, & Hollander JA (2003). Basolateral amygdala neurons encode cocaine self-administration and cocaine-associated cues. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 23(23), 8204–8211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang SE, Wheeler DS, & Holland PC (2012). Roles of nucleus accumbens and basolateral amygdala in autoshaped lever pressing. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 97(4), 441–451. 10.1016/j.nlm.2012.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaudhri N, Woods CA, Sahuque LL, Gill TM, & Janak PH (2013). Unilateral inactivation of the basolateral amygdala attenuates context-induced renewal of Pavlovian-conditioned alcohol-seeking. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 38(5), 2751–2761. 10.1111/ejn.12278 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, & O’Brien CP (1999). Limbic activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(1), 11–18. 10.1176/ajp.156.1.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chow JJ, & Beckmann JS (2021). Remifentanil-food choice follows predictions of relative subjective value. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 218, 108369. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108369 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chow JJ, Hofford RS, & Beckmann JS (2021). Neuronal activity associated with cocaine preference: Effects of differential cocaine intake. Neuropharmacology, 184, 108441. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2020.108441 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christie MJ, Summers RJ, Stephenson JA, Cook CJ, & Beart PM (1987). Excitatory amino acid projections to the nucleus accumbens septi in the rat: A retrograde transport study utilizingd[3H]aspartate and [3H]GABA. Neuroscience, 22(2), 425–439. 10.1016/0306-4522(87)90345-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole S, Powell DJ, & Petrovich GD (2013). Differential recruitment of distinct amygdalar nuclei across appetitive associative learning. Learning & Memory, 20(6), 295–299. 10.1101/lm.031070.113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, & Balleine BW (2005). Double dissociation of basolateral and central amygdala lesions on the general and outcome-specific forms of pavlovian-instrumental transfer. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 25(4), 962–970. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4507-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit LH, & Balleine BW (2011). The general and outcome-specific forms of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer are differentially mediated by the nucleus accumbens core and shell. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(33), 11786–11794. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2711-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crombag HS, Bossert JM, Koya E, & Shaham Y (2008). Review. Context-induced relapse to drug seeking: A review. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363(1507), 3233–3243. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derman RC, Bass CE, & Ferrario CR (2020). Effects of hM4Di activation in CamKII basolateral amygdala neurons and CNO treatment on sensory-specific vs. general PIT: Refining PIT circuits and considerations for using CNO. Psychopharmacology, 237(5), 1249–1266. 10.1007/s00213-020-05453-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Chiara G (1999). Drug addiction as dopamine-dependent associative learning disorder. European Journal of Pharmacology, 375(1–3), 13–30. 10.1016/s0014-2999(99)00372-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Ciano P, & Everitt BJ (2004). Direct Interactions between the Basolateral Amygdala and Nucleus Accumbens Core Underlie Cocaine-Seeking Behavior by Rats. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(32), 7167–7173. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1581-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dong Y, Taylor JR, Wolf ME, & Shaham Y (2017). Circuit and Synaptic Plasticity Mechanisms of Drug Relapse. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 37(45), 10867–10876. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1821-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Parkinson JA, Olmstead MC, Arroyo M, Robledo P, & Robbins TW (1999). Associative processes in addiction and reward. The role of amygdala-ventral striatal subsystems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 877, 412–438. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09280.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferland J-MN, & Winstanley CA (2017). Risk-preferring rats make worse decisions and show increased incubation of craving after cocaine self-administration. Addiction Biology, 22(4), 991–1001. 10.1111/adb.12388 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernando ABP, Murray JE, & Milton AL (2013). The amygdala: Securing pleasure and avoiding pain. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 190. 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00190 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiuzat EC, Rhodes SEV, & Murray EA (2017). The Role of Orbitofrontal-Amygdala Interactions in Updating Action-Outcome Valuations in Macaques. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 37(9), 2463–2470. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1839-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flagel SB, Akil H, & Robinson TE (2009). Individual differences in the attribution of incentive salience to reward-related cues: Implications for addiction. Neuropharmacology, 56 Suppl 1, 139–148. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floresco SB, Blaha CD, Yang CR, & Phillips AG (2001). Dopamine D1 and NMDA Receptors Mediate Potentiation of Basolateral Amygdala-Evoked Firing of Nucleus Accumbens Neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(16), 6370–6376. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-06370.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floresco SB, St Onge JR, Ghods-Sharifi S, & Winstanley CA (2008). Cortico-limbic-striatal circuits subserving different forms of cost-benefit decision making. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(4), 375–389. 10.3758/CABN.8.4.375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs RA, Bell GH, Ramirez DR, Eaddy JL, & Su Z (2009). Basolateral amygdala involvement in memory reconsolidation processes that facilitate drug context-induced cocaine seeking. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(5), 889–900. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06888.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs RA, Evans KA, Ledford CC, Parker MP, Case JM, Mehta RH, & See RE (2005). The role of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdala, and dorsal hippocampus in contextual reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 30(2), 296–309. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs RA, Feltenstein MW, & See RE (2006). The role of the basolateral amygdala in stimulus-reward memory and extinction memory consolidation and in subsequent conditioned cued reinstatement of cocaine seeking. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 23(10), 2809–2813. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04806.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabriele A, & See RE (2010). Reversible inactivation of the basolateral amygdala, but not the dorsolateral caudate-putamen, attenuates consolidation of cocaine-cue associative learning in a reinstatement model of drug-seeking. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 32(6), 1024–1029. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07394.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher M, McMahan RW, & Schoenbaum G (1999). Orbitofrontal cortex and representation of incentive value in associative learning. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 19(15), 6610–6614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghods-Sharifi S, St Onge JR, & Floresco SB (2009). Fundamental contribution by the basolateral amygdala to different forms of decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(16), 5251–5259. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0315-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein RZ, & Volkow ND (2002). Drug Addiction and Its Underlying Neurobiological Basis: Neuroimaging Evidence for the Involvement of the Frontal Cortex. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(10), 1642–1652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goode TD, & Maren S (2019). Common neurocircuitry mediating drug and fear relapse in preclinical models. Psychopharmacology, 236(1), 415–437. 10.1007/s00213-018-5024-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C, Phillips RL, Kimes AS, & Margolin A (1996). Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(21), 12040–12045. 10.1073/pnas.93.21.12040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groenewegen HJ, Berendse HW, Wolters JG, & Lohman AH (1990). The anatomical relationship of the prefrontal cortex with the striatopallidal system, the thalamus and the amygdala: Evidence for a parallel organization. Progress in Brain Research, 85, 95–116; discussion 116–118. 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)62677-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groenewegen Henk J., Berendse HW, Wolters JG, & Lohman AHM (1991). Chapter 5 The anatomical relationship of the prefrontal cortex with the striatopallidal system, the thalamus and the amygdala: Evidence for a parallel organization. In Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 85, pp. 95–118). Elsevier. 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62677-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillem K, & Ahmed SH (2018). Preference for Cocaine is Represented in the Orbitofrontal Cortex by an Increased Proportion of Cocaine Use-Coding Neurons. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 28(3), 819–832. 10.1093/cercor/bhw398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillem K, & Ahmed SH (2019). A neuronal population code for resemblance between drug and nondrug reward outcomes in the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Structure & Function, 224(2), 883–890. 10.1007/s00429-018-1809-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillem K, & Ahmed SH (2020). Reorganization of theta phase-locking in the orbitofrontal cortex drives cocaine choice under the influence. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 8041. 10.1038/s41598-020-64962-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillem K, Brenot V, Durand A, & Ahmed SH (2018). Neuronal representation of individual heroin choices in the orbitofrontal cortex. Addiction Biology, 23(3), 880–888. 10.1111/adb.12536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall J, Parkinson JA, Connor TM, Dickinson A, & Everitt BJ (2001). Involvement of the central nucleus of the amygdala and nucleus accumbens core in mediating Pavlovian influences on instrumental behaviour. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(10), 1984–1992. 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01577.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haney RZ, Calu DJ, Takahashi YK, Hughes BW, & Schoenbaum G (2010). Inactivation of the central but not the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala disrupts learning in response to overexpectation of reward. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 30(8), 2911–2917. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0054-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart G, Leung BK, & Balleine BW (2014). Dorsal and ventral streams: The distinct role of striatal subregions in the acquisition and performance of goal-directed actions. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 108, 104–118. 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatfield T, Han JS, Conley M, Gallagher M, & Holland P (1996). Neurotoxic lesions of basolateral, but not central, amygdala interfere with Pavlovian second-order conditioning and reinforcer devaluation effects. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 16(16), 5256–5265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez JS, Binette AN, Rahman T, Tarantino JD, & Moorman DE (2020). Chemogenetic Inactivation of Orbitofrontal Cortex Decreases Cue-induced Reinstatement of Ethanol and Sucrose Seeking in Male and Female Wistar Rats. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 44(9), 1769–1782. 10.1111/acer.14407 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC (2016). Enhancing second-order conditioning with lesions of the basolateral amygdala. Behavioral Neuroscience, 130(2), 176–181. 10.1037/bne0000129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC, & Gallagher M (2003). Double dissociation of the effects of lesions of basolateral and central amygdala on conditioned stimulus-potentiated feeding and Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(8), 1680–1694. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02585.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC, & Rescorla RA (1975). Second-order conditioning with food unconditioned stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88(1), 459–467. 10.1037/h0076219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homayoun H, & Moghaddam B (2009). Differential representation of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer by prefrontal cortex subregions and striatum. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 29(7), 1461–1476. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06679.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito R, Robbins TW, & Everitt BJ (2004). Differential control over cocaine-seeking behavior by nucleus accumbens core and shell. Nature Neuroscience, 7(4), 389–397. 10.1038/nn1217 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo A, Brigman JL, Radke AK, Rudebeck PH, & Holmes A (2017). The neural basis of reversal learning: An updated perspective. Neuroscience, 345, 12–26. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo A, & Murray EA (2007). Selective bilateral amygdala lesions in rhesus monkeys fail to disrupt object reversal learning. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(5), 1054–1062. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3616-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Janak PH, & Tye KM (2015). From circuits to behaviour in the amygdala. Nature, 517(7534), 284–292. 10.1038/nature14188 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jasinska AJ, Stein EA, Kaiser J, Naumer MJ, & Yalachkov Y (2014). Factors modulating neural reactivity to drug cues in addiction: A survey of human neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 38, 1–16. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jean-Richard-Dit-Bressel P, Killcross S, & McNally GP (2018). Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of punishment: Implications for psychiatric disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 43(8), 1639–1650. 10.1038/s41386-018-0047-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson AW, Gallagher M, & Holland PC (2009). The Basolateral Amygdala Is Critical to the Expression of Pavlovian and Instrumental Outcome-Specific Reinforcer Devaluation Effects. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(3), 696–704. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3758-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones JL, Day JJ, Aragona BJ, Wheeler RA, Wightman RM, & Carelli RM (2010). Basolateral Amygdala Modulates Terminal Dopamine Release in the Nucleus Accumbens and Conditioned Responding. Biological Psychiatry, 67(8), 737–744. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kantak KM, Black Y, Valencia E, Green-Jordan K, & Eichenbaum HB (2002). Dissociable effects of lidocaine inactivation of the rostral and caudal basolateral amygdala on the maintenance and reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 22(3), 1126–1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keefer SE, Bacharach SZ, Kochli DE, Chabot JM, & Calu DJ (2020). Effects of Limited and Extended Pavlovian Training on Devaluation Sensitivity of Sign- and Goal-Tracking Rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, 3. 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keistler CR, Hammarlund E, Barker JM, Bond CW, DiLeone RJ, Pittenger C, & Taylor JR (2017). Regulation of Alcohol Extinction and Cue-Induced Reinstatement by Specific Projections among Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Nucleus Accumbens, and Basolateral Amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 37(17), 4462–4471. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3383-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley AE, Domesick VB, & Nauta WJ (1982). The amygdalostriatal projection in the rat—An anatomical study by anterograde and retrograde tracing methods. Neuroscience, 7(3), 615–630. 10.1016/0306-4522(82)90067-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khoo SY-S, Gibson GD, Prasad AA, & McNally GP (2017). How contexts promote and prevent relapse to drug seeking. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 16(1), 185–204. 10.1111/gbb.12328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kita H, & Kitai ST (1990). Amygdaloid projections to the frontal cortex and the striatum in the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 298(1), 40–49. 10.1002/cne.902980104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kochli DE, Keefer SE, Gyawali U, & Calu DJ (2020). Basolateral Amygdala to Nucleus Accumbens Communication Differentially Mediates Devaluation Sensitivity of Sign- and Goal-Tracking Rats. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14. 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.593645 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krettek JE, & Price JL (1978). Amygdaloid projections to subcortical structures within the basal forebrain and brainstem in the rat and cat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 178(2), 225–254. 10.1002/cne.901780204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lasseter HC, Ramirez DR, Xie X, & Fuchs RA (2009). Involvement of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex in drug context-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(7), 1370–1381. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06906.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lasseter HC, Wells AM, Xie X, & Fuchs RA (2011). Interaction of the basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex is critical for drug context-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(3), 711–720. 10.1038/npp.2010.209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee BR, Ma Y-Y, Huang YH, Wang X, Otaka M, Ishikawa M, Neumann PA, Graziane NM, Brown TE, Suska A, Guo C, Lobo MK, Sesack SR, Wolf ME, Nestler EJ, Shaham Y, Schlüter OM, & Dong Y (2013). Maturation of silent synapses in amygdala-accumbens projection contributes to incubation of cocaine craving. Nature Neuroscience, 16(11), 1644–1651. 10.1038/nn.3533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenoir M, Augier E, Vouillac C, & Ahmed SH (2013). A choice-based screening method for compulsive drug users in rats. Current Protocols in Neuroscience, Chapter 9, Unit 9.44. 10.1002/0471142301.ns0944s64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lichtenberg NT, Pennington ZT, Holley SM, Greenfield VY, Cepeda C, Levine MS, & Wassum KM (2017). Basolateral Amygdala to Orbitofrontal Cortex Projections Enable Cue-Triggered Reward Expectations. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 37(35), 8374–8384. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0486-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucantonio F, Gardner MPH, Mirenzi A, Newman LE, Takahashi YK, & Schoenbaum G (2015). Neural Estimates of Imagined Outcomes in Basolateral Amygdala Depend on Orbitofrontal Cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 35(50), 16521–16530. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3126-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Málková L, Gaffan D, & Murray EA (1997). Excitotoxic Lesions of the Amygdala Fail to Produce Impairment in Visual Learning for Auditory Secondary Reinforcement But Interfere with Reinforcer Devaluation Effects in Rhesus Monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(15), 6011–6020. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-15-06011.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malvaez M, Greenfield VY, Wang AS, Yorita AM, Feng L, Linker KE, Monbouquette HG, & Wassum KM (2015). Basolateral amygdala rapid glutamate release encodes an outcome-specific representation vital for reward-predictive cues to selectively invigorate reward-seeking actions. Scientific Reports, 5, 12511. 10.1038/srep12511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malvaez M, Shieh C, Murphy MD, Greenfield VY, & Wassum KM (2019). Distinct cortical–amygdala projections drive reward value encoding and retrieval. Nature Neuroscience, 22(5), 762–769. 10.1038/s41593-019-0374-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning EE, Bradfield LA, & Iordanova MD (2021). Adaptive behaviour under conflict: Deconstructing extinction, reversal, and active avoidance learning. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 120, 526–536. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCracken CB, & Grace AA (2013). Persistent cocaine-induced reversal learning deficits are associated with altered limbic cortico-striatal local field potential synchronization. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(44), 17469–17482. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1440-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDannald MA, Setlow B, & Holland PC (2013). Effects of ventral striatal lesions on first- and second-order appetitive conditioning. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 38(4), 2589–2599. 10.1111/ejn.12255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlinchey EM, James MH, Mahler SV, Pantazis C, & Aston-Jones G (2016). Prelimbic to Accumbens Core Pathway Is Recruited in a Dopamine-Dependent Manner to Drive Cued Reinstatement of Cocaine Seeking. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 36(33), 8700–8711. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1291-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meil WM, & See RE (1997). Lesions of the basolateral amygdala abolish the ability of drug associated cues to reinstate responding during withdrawal from self-administered cocaine. Behavioural Brain Research, 87(2), 139–148. 10.1016/s0166-4328(96)02270-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Millan EZ, Kim HA, & Janak PH (2017). Optogenetic activation of amygdala projections to nucleus accumbens can arrest conditioned and unconditioned alcohol consummatory behavior. Neuroscience, 360, 106–117. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison SE, Bamkole MA, & Nicola SM (2015). Sign Tracking, but Not Goal Tracking, is Resistant to Outcome Devaluation. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 468. 10.3389/fnins.2015.00468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison SE, Saez A, Lau B, & Salzman CD (2011). Different time courses for learning-related changes in amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron, 71(6), 1127–1140. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray EA, Moylan EJ, Saleem KS, Basile BM, & Turchi J (2015). Specialized areas for value updating and goal selection in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. ELife, 4. 10.7554/eLife.11695 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Namburi P, Beyeler A, Yorozu S, Calhoon GG, Halbert SA, Wichmann R, Holden SS, Mertens KL, Anahtar M, Felix-Ortiz AC, Wickersham IR, Gray JM, & Tye KM (2015). A circuit mechanism for differentiating positive and negative associations. Nature, 520(7549), 675–678. 10.1038/nature14366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nasser HM, Chen Y-W, Fiscella K, & Calu DJ (2015). Individual variability in behavioral flexibility predicts sign-tracking tendency. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 289. 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsini CA, Hernandez CM, Singhal S, Kelly KB, Frazier CJ, Bizon JL, & Setlow B (2017). Optogenetic Inhibition Reveals Distinct Roles for Basolateral Amygdala Activity at Discrete Time Points during Risky Decision Making. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 37(48), 11537–11548. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2344-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsini CA, Trotta RT, Bizon JL, & Setlow B (2015). Dissociable roles for the basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in decision-making under risk of punishment. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 35(4), 1368–1379. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3586-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, & Balleine BW (2008). Differential involvement of the basolateral amygdala and mediodorsal thalamus in instrumental action selection. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 28(17), 4398–4405. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5472-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostrander S, Cazares VA, Kim C, Cheung S, Gonzalez I, & Izquierdo A (2011). Orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala lesions result in suboptimal and dissociable reward choices on cue-guided effort in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 125(3), 350–359. 10.1037/a0023574 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkes SL, & Balleine BW (2013). Incentive memory: Evidence the basolateral amygdala encodes and the insular cortex retrieves outcome values to guide choice between goal-directed actions. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(20), 8753–8763. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5071-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkes SL, Bradfield LA, & Balleine BW (2015). Interaction of insular cortex and ventral striatum mediates the effect of incentive memory on choice between goal-directed actions. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 35(16), 6464–6471. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4153-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkinson JA, Crofts HS, McGuigan M, Tomic DL, Everitt BJ, & Roberts AC (2001). The role of the primate amygdala in conditioned reinforcement. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 21(19), 7770–7780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkinson JA, Olmstead MC, Burns LH, Robbins TW, & Everitt BJ (1999). Dissociation in effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on appetitive pavlovian approach behavior and the potentiation of conditioned reinforcement and locomotor activity by D-amphetamine. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 19(6), 2401–2411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paton JJ, Belova MA, Morrison SE, & Salzman CD (2006). The primate amygdala represents the positive and negative value of visual stimuli during learning. Nature, 439(7078), 865–870. 10.1038/nature04490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelloux Y, Minier-Toribio A, Hoots JK, Bossert JM, & Shaham Y (2018). Opposite Effects of Basolateral Amygdala Inactivation on Context-Induced Relapse to Cocaine Seeking after Extinction versus Punishment. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 38(1), 51–59. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2521-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M, & Holland PC (2005). Orbitofrontal lesions impair use of cue-outcome associations in a devaluation task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119(1), 317–322. 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.317 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Setlow B, Gallagher M, Holland PC, & Schoenbaum G (2003). Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 23(35), 11078–11084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitchers KK, Phillips KB, Jones JL, Robinson TE, & Sarter M (2017). Diverse Roads to Relapse: A Discriminative Cue Signaling Cocaine Availability Is More Effective in Renewing Cocaine Seeking in Goal Trackers Than Sign Trackers and Depends on Basal Forebrain Cholinergic Activity. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 37(30), 7198–7208. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0990-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puaud M, Higuera-Matas A, Brunault P, Everitt BJ, & Belin D (2020). The Basolateral Amygdala to Nucleus Accumbens Core Circuit Mediates the Conditioned Reinforcing Effects of Cocaine-Paired Cues on Cocaine Seeking. Biological Psychiatry. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Reep RL, Corwin JV, & King V (1996). Neuronal connections of orbital cortex in rats: Topography of cortical and thalamic afferents. Experimental Brain Research, 111(2). 10.1007/BF00227299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes SEV, & Murray EA (2013). Differential effects of amygdala, orbital prefrontal cortex, and prelimbic cortex lesions on goal-directed behavior in rhesus macaques. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(8), 3380–3389. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4374-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizley RC, & Rescorla RA (1972). Associations in second-order conditioning and sensory preconditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 81(1), 1–11. 10.1037/h0033333 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE, & Berridge KC (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews, 18(3), 247–291. 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-p [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson Terry E., & Flagel SB (2009). Dissociating the predictive and incentive motivational properties of reward-related cues through the study of individual differences. Biological Psychiatry, 65(10), 869–873. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roesch MR, Takahashi Y, Gugsa N, Bissonette GB, & Schoenbaum G (2007). Previous cocaine exposure makes rats hypersensitive to both delay and reward magnitude. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(1), 245–250. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4080-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi LM, Reverte I, Ragozzino D, Badiani A, Venniro M, & Caprioli D (2020). Role of nucleus accumbens core but not shell in incubation of methamphetamine craving after voluntary abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 45(2), 256–265. 10.1038/s41386019-0479-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russchen FT, & Price JL (1984). Amygdalostriatal projections in the rat. Topographical organization and fiber morphology shown using the lectin PHA-L as an anterograde tracer. Neuroscience Letters, 47(1), 15–22. 10.1016/0304-3940(84)90379-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saddoris MP, Gallagher M, & Schoenbaum G (2005). Rapid associative encoding in basolateral amygdala depends on connections with orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron, 46(2), 321–331. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders BT, O’Donnell EG, Aurbach EL, & Robinson TE (2014). A cocaine context renews drug seeking preferentially in a subset of individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(12), 2816–2823. 10.1038/npp.2014.131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders BT, Richard JM, Margolis EB, & Janak PH (2018). Dopamine neurons create Pavlovian conditioned stimuli with circuit-defined motivational properties. Nature Neuroscience, 21(8), 1072–1083. 10.1038/s41593-018-0191-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders BT, & Robinson TE (2010). A cocaine cue acts as an incentive stimulus in some but not others: Implications for addiction. Biological Psychiatry, 67(8), 730–736. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders BT, & Robinson TE (2012). The role of dopamine in the accumbens core in the expression of Pavlovian-conditioned responses. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 36(4), 2521–2532. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08217.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders BT, Yager LM, & Robinson TE (2013). Cue-evoked cocaine “craving”: Role of dopamine in the accumbens core. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(35), 13989–14000. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0450-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scarlet J, Delamater AR, Campese V, Fein M, & Wheeler DS (2012). Differential Involvement of the Basolateral Amygdala and the Orbitofrontal Cortex in the Formation of Sensory-Specific Associations in Conditioned Flavor Preference and Magazine Approach Paradigms. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 35(11), 1799–1809. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08113.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, & Gallagher M (1999). Neural encoding in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during olfactory discrimination learning. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 19(5), 1876–1884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum Geoffrey, & Setlow B (2005). Cocaine makes actions insensitive to outcomes but not extinction: Implications for altered orbitofrontal-amygdalar function. Cerebral Cortex (New York, N.Y.: 1991), 15(8), 1162–1169. 10.1093/cercor/bhh216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum Geoffrey, & Shaham Y (2008). The role of orbitofrontal cortex in drug addiction: A review of preclinical studies. Biological Psychiatry, 63(3), 256–262. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- See RE, Fuchs RA, Ledford CC, & McLaughlin J (2003). Drug addiction, relapse, and the amygdala. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985, 294–307. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07089.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Servonnet A, Hernandez G, El Hage C, Rompré P-P, & Samaha A-N (2020). Optogenetic Activation of the Basolateral Amygdala Promotes Both Appetitive Conditioning and the Instrumental Pursuit of Reward Cues. The Journal of Neuroscience, 40(8), 1732–1743. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2196-19.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Setlow B, Gallagher M, & Holland PC (2002). The basolateral complex of the amygdala is necessary for acquisition but not expression of CS motivational value in appetitive Pavlovian second-order conditioning: Amygdala and motivational value. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15(11), 1841–1853. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02010.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Setlow B, Holland PC, & Gallagher M (2002). Disconnection of the basolateral amygdala complex and nucleus accumbens impairs appetitive pavlovian second-order conditioned responses. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116(2), 267–275. 10.1037//0735-7044.116.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharp BM (2017). Basolateral amygdala and stress-induced hyperexcitability affect motivated behaviors and addiction. Translational Psychiatry, 7(8), e1194. 10.1038/tp.2017.161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe MJ, & Schoenbaum G (2016). Back to basics: Making predictions in the orbitofrontal- amygdala circuit. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 131, 201–206. 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shiflett MW, & Balleine BW (2010). At the limbic-motor interface: Disconnection of basolateral amygdala from nucleus accumbens core and shell reveals dissociable components of incentive motivation. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 32(10), 1735–1743. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07439.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinonaga Y, Takada M, & Mizuno N (1994). Topographic organization of collateral projections from the basolateral amygdaloid nucleus to both the prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens in the rat. Neuroscience, 58(2), 389–397. 10.1016/0306-4522(94)90045-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sias AC, Morse AK, Wang S, Greenfield VY, Goodpaster CM, Wrenn TM, Wikenheiser AM, Holley SM, Cepeda C, Levine MS, & Wassum KM (2021). A bidirectional corticoamygdala circuit for the encoding and retrieval of detailed reward memories. BioRxiv, 2021.03.20.436233. 10.1101/2021.03.20.436233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Simmons DA, & Neill DB (2009). Functional interaction between the basolateral amygdala and the nucleus accumbens underlies incentive motivation for food reward on a fixed ratio schedule. Neuroscience, 159(4), 1264–1273. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh T, McDannald MA, Haney RZ, Cerri DH, & Schoenbaum G (2010). Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell are Necessary for Reinforcer Devaluation Effects on Pavlovian Conditioned Responding. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 4, 126. 10.3389/fnint.2010.00126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sorg BA (2012). Reconsolidation of drug memories. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(5), 1400–1417. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onge JR St., Stopper CM, Zahm DS, & Floresco SB (2012). Separate PrefrontalSubcortical Circuits Mediate Different Components of Risk-Based Decision Making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(8), 2886–2899. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5625-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Franz TM, Singh T, & Schoenbaum G (2007). Basolateral amygdala lesions abolish orbitofrontal-dependent reversal impairments. Neuron, 54(1), 51–58. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Roesch MR, Franz TM, Calu DJ, Singh T, & Schoenbaum G (2007). Cocaine-induced decision-making deficits are mediated by miscoding in basolateral amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 10(8), 949–951. 10.1038/nn1931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Takahashi Y, Roesch MR, & Schoenbaum G (2009). Neural substrates of cognitive inflexibility after chronic cocaine exposure. Neuropharmacology, 56 Suppl 1, 63–72. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.07.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefanik MT, & Kalivas PW (2013). Optogenetic dissection of basolateral amygdala projections during cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 213. 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stopper CM, & Floresco SB (2011). Contributions of the nucleus accumbens and its subregions to different aspects of risk-based decision making. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11(1), 97–112. 10.3758/s13415-010-0015-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuber GD, Sparta DR, Stamatakis AM, van Leeuwen WA, Hardjoprajitno JE, Cho S, Tye KM, Kempadoo KA, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, & Bonci A (2011). Excitatory transmission from the amygdala to nucleus accumbens facilitates reward seeking. Nature, 475(7356), 377–380. 10.1038/nature10194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi YK, Chang CY, Lucantonio F, Haney RZ, Berg BA, Yau H-J, Bonci A, & Schoenbaum G (2013). Neural estimates of imagined outcomes in the orbitofrontal cortex drive behavior and learning. Neuron, 80(2), 507–518. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi YK, Roesch MR, Stalnaker TA, Haney RZ, Calu DJ, Taylor AR, Burke KA, & Schoenbaum G (2009). The orbitofrontal cortex and ventral tegmental area are necessary for learning from unexpected outcomes. Neuron, 62(2), 269–280. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Théberge FRM, Milton AL, Belin D, Lee JLC, & Everitt BJ (2010). The basolateral amygdala and nucleus accumbens core mediate dissociable aspects of drug memory reconsolidation. Learning & Memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.), 17(9), 444–453. 10.1101/lm.1757410 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tunstall BJ, & Kearns DN (2015). Sign-tracking predicts increased choice of cocaine over food in rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 281, 222–228. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tye KM, & Janak PH (2007). Amygdala Neurons Differentially Encode Motivation and Reinforcement. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(15), 3937–3945. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5281-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Holstein M, MacLeod PE, & Floresco SB (2020). Basolateral amygdala—Nucleus accumbens circuitry regulates optimal cue-guided risk/reward decision making. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 98, 109830. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandaele Y, Cantin L, Serre F, Vouillac-Mendoza C, & Ahmed SH (2016). Choosing Under the Influence: A Drug-Specific Mechanism by Which the Setting Controls Drug Choices in Rats. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(2), 646–657. 10.1038/npp.2015.195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venniro M, Banks ML, Heilig M, Epstein DH, & Shaham Y (2020). Improving translation of animal models of addiction and relapse by reverse translation. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 21(11), 625–643. 10.1038/s41583-020-0378-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Venniro M, Caprioli D, & Shaham Y (2019). Novel models of drug relapse and craving after voluntary abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(1), 234–235. 10.1038/s41386-018-0196-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villaruel FR, & Chaudhri N (2016). Individual Differences in the Attribution of Incentive Salience to a Pavlovian Alcohol Cue. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 238. 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Fowler JS, & Wang G-J (2003). The addicted human brain: Insights from imaging studies. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 111(10), 1444–1451. 10.1172/JCI200318533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wassum KM, & Izquierdo A (2015). The basolateral amygdala in reward learning and addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 271–283. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wellman LL, Gale K, & Malkova L (2005). GABAA-mediated inhibition of basolateral amygdala blocks reward devaluation in macaques. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 25(18), 4577–4586. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2257-04.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West EA, & Carelli RM (2016). Nucleus Accumbens Core and Shell Differentially Encode Reward-Associated Cues after Reinforcer Devaluation. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 36(4), 1128–1139. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2976-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West EA, DesJardin JT, Gale K, & Malkova L (2011). Transient inactivation of orbitofrontal cortex blocks reinforcer devaluation in macaques. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(42), 15128–15135. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3295-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West EA, Forcelli PA, Murnen AT, McCue DL, Gale K, & Malkova L (2012). Transient inactivation of basolateral amygdala during selective satiation disrupts reinforcer devaluation in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 126(4), 563–574. 10.1037/a0029080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitelaw RB, Markou A, Robbins TW, & Everitt BJ (1996). Excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala impair the acquisition of cocaine-seeking behaviour under a second-order schedule of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology, 127(3), 213–224. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA (1996). Neurobiology of addiction. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6(2), 243–251. 10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80079-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yizhar O, & Klavir O (2018). Reciprocal amygdala-prefrontal interactions in learning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 52, 149–155. 10.1016/j.conb.2018.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young KA, Franklin TR, Roberts DCS, Jagannathan K, Suh JJ, Wetherill RR, Wang Z, Kampman KM, O’Brien CP, & Childress AR (2014). Nipping Cue Reactivity in the Bud: Baclofen Prevents Limbic Activation Elicited by Subliminal Drug Cues. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34(14), 5038–5043. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4977-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeeb FD, & Winstanley CA (2011). Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex Differentially Affect Acquisition and Performance of a Rodent Gambling Task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(6), 2197–2204. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5597-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeeb FD, & Winstanley CA (2013). Functional disconnection of the orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala impairs acquisition of a rat gambling task and disrupts animals’ ability to alter decision-making behavior after reinforcer devaluation. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(15), 6434–6443. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3971-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuo Y, Wang X, Cui C, Luo F, Yu P, & Wang X (2012). Cocaine-induced impulsive choices are accompanied by impaired delay-dependent anticipatory activity in basolateral amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(1), 196–211. 10.1162/jocn_a_00131 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]