Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2021 Jun 25;63(3):305–314. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13472

Clinical precursors of tics: an EMTICS study

Thaïra JC Openneer 1, Chaim Huyser 2,3, Davide Martino 4, Anette Schrag 5; EMTICS Collaborative Group*, Pieter J Hoekstra 1, Andrea Dietrich 1,
PMCID: PMC9292724  PMID: 34170010

Abstract

Background

Children with Tourette syndrome (TS) often have comorbid disorders, particularly attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and obsessive‐compulsive disorder (OCD). While subtle premorbid symptoms have been described in various psychiatric disorders, the presence of clinical precursors that may exist before the onset of tics is unknown. This longitudinal study aimed to find clinical precursors of tics by assessing a range of clinical characteristics prior to tic onset in comparison with children without onset of tics.

Methods

A sample of 187 3‐ to 10‐year‐old first‐degree unaffected relatives of children with TS were followed up to 7 years in the European Multicentre Tics in Children Study (EMTICS). We investigated whether clinical characteristics assessed at baseline predicted tic onset, comparing 126 children without tic onset to 61 children who developed tics. We used the least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) method, a penalised logistic regression approach. We also explored sex differences and repeated our analyses in an age‐ and sex‐matched subsample.

Results

Children with tic onset were more frequently male (β = −0.36), had higher baseline severity of conduct problems (β = 0.23), autism spectrum disorder symptoms (ASD; β = 0.08), compulsions (β = 0.02) and emotional problems (β = 0.03) compared to children without tic onset. Conduct and ASD problems were male‐specific predictors, whereas severity of compulsions and oppositional (β = 0.39) and emotional problems were female‐specific predictors.

Conclusion

This study supports the presence of clinical precursors prior to tic onset and highlights the need of sex‐specific monitoring of children at risk of developing tics. This may aid in the earlier detection of tics, particularly in females. We moreover found that tics most often persisted one year after tic onset, in contrast to the common belief that tics are mostly transient.

Keywords: Compulsions, conduct problems, clinical precursors, sex differences, tic onset

Introduction

Chronic tic disorders, that is Tourette syndrome (TS) and persistent (chronic) motor or vocal tic disorder, are childhood‐onset disorders characterised by the presence of multiple motor and/or vocal tics, lasting at least one year (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Tic disorders are more common in males than in females (with a 3:1 ratio; Hirschtritt et al., 2015) with an average onset of 6 years of age (range 3–8.6; Leckman et al., 1998). An estimated 87.5% of individuals with TS have psychiatric comorbidity (Hirschtritt et al., 2015), most frequently attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; 54%) and obsessive‐compulsive disorder (OCD; 50%), but also disruptive behaviour disorders (oppositional defiant disorder [ODD] or conduct disorder [CD]; 30%), internalising disorders (anxiety and depression; 30%; Hirschtritt et al., 2015) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD; 4,5%‐15%; Rizzo, Gulisano, Domini, Ferro, & Curatolo, 2017). Similar to tic disorders, ADHD, ODD, CD and ASD show a male preponderance and an onset in (early) childhood (Hirschtritt et al., 2015; Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019). In contrast, OCD and internalising disorders occur more frequently in females, especially in adolescents (Leckman et al., 1998; Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019). The potentially disabling tics and the high rates of psychiatric comorbidity can have a significant impact on the quality of life (Eapen, Snedden, Črnčec, Pick, & Sachdev, 2016). Tic disorders are often under‐recognised, with retrospectively estimated delays of 10 years between onset and diagnosis (Bäumer, Sajin, & Münchau, 2016), and reasons for first referral to mental healthcare are frequently due to other behavioural or emotional problems rather than tics (Khalifa & Von Knorring, 2005). As tic disorders and comorbid conditions share a common genetic background (Brainstorm Consortium et al., 2018), an early general liability to the presence of (milder levels of) behavioural and emotional problems seems plausible. A body of research has focussed on identifying (subtle) early‐life clinical characteristics that precede the onset of psychiatric disorders (e.g. inattention as a precursor of ASD or neurological soft signs as a precursor of ADHD; Athanasiadou et al., 2020; Hafeman et al., 2017; Möricke et al., 2019). However, it is not yet known whether early symptom characteristics precede the onset of tics. Insight into these factors may possibly aid in the timely detection of tics and early intervention. We therefore followed high‐risk children aged 3–10 years for up to 7 years, who at study entry were unaffected first‐degree relatives (siblings) of children with chronic tic disorders. We compared a variety of dimensional clinical symptom scales and a quality‐of‐life score assessed at baseline between children with and without subsequent onset of tics and explored possible sex differences. We expected the presence of premorbid symptoms prior to tic onset that differed between males and females in line with the sex‐specific distribution of comorbid conditions.

Methods

Participants and procedures

As part of the ONSET cohort of the European Multicentre Tics in Children Study (EMTICS; Schrag et al., 2019), including 16 European clinical sites, we followed 259 3‐ to 10‐year‐old first‐degree relatives (siblings) of children with a chronic tic disorder, who had no tics at baseline. Children were assessed every two months for possible tic onset for three years using the symptom list of the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale assessed by a semi‐structured clinician interview (YGTSS; Leckman et al., 1989). After the study ended, we re‐evaluated the unaffected children for possible tic onset by a brief telephone interview in a similar way. The overall average participation in the study was 3.5 [range 0–6.9] years between February 2013 and May 2020. A total of 68 confirmed tic onsets occurred, confirmed by the study clinician (that is, in 26.3% of the siblings, of which 57.4% [n = 39] in males and 42.6% [n = 29] in females). To define the unaffected comparison group, we selected unaffected children who were at least 10 years old at the last regular study visit, to limit the possibility of tic onset in this group. Our final sample consisted of 187 children: n = 61 children with a tic onset and n = 126 children without a tic onset. Finally, during a brief 1‐year follow‐up telephone interview by the study clinician, we assessed the chronicity of tics in the children with a tic onset confirming a tic disorder diagnosis according to DSM‐IV‐TR. See Appendix S2 for a detailed description of the study participants and procedures, and Figures S1 and S2 for study flow charts. Study methods have been extensively described in Schrag et al., (2019).

Exclusion criteria for the ONSET cohort at study entry were presence of tics, OCD and trichotillomania, presence of a serious medical or neurological illness, treatment with antibiotics during the last month (given another EMTICS sub‐study) and being unable to understand and comply with study procedures. Subjects were allowed to receive treatment for mental health problems. The children’s parents or legal guardians provided written informed consent in line with the local medical‐ethical regulations. The study was approved by the local research ethics committees of the participating centres.

Clinical measures

At the baseline visit, parents were asked to complete questionnaires regarding their child’s current behavioural and emotional symptoms and quality of life. We used the Children’s Yale‐Brown Obsessive‐Compulsive Scale (CY‐BOCS; Scahill et al., 1997; Storch et al., 2006) to assess past week severity of obsessions and compulsions using a semi‐structured clinician interview at baseline. Further, we used the parent‐rated Swanson Nolan and Pelham‐IV rating scale (SNAP‐IV; Bussing et al., 2008; Swanson et al., 2001) to assess past week inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity and ODD severity; the parent‐rated Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire (ASSQ; Ehlers, Gillberg, & Wing, 1999) to rate symptoms of ASD; and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, Ford, Simmons, Gatward, & Meltzer, 2000) to assess conduct and emotional problems over the past two weeks. Health‐related quality of life was measured by the parent‐rated KINDL‐R questionnaire over the past week (Ravens‐Sieberer & Bullinger, 1998). See Appendix S3 for more information on the clinical measures.

Data analytic strategy

The percentage of missing data was <1% and imputed by means of the expectation‐maximisation algorithm (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Appendix S4 describes the statistical analyses of sample characteristics. We applied a logistic least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO; Tibshirani, 1996) using the glmnet‐package (Friedman, Hastie, & Tibshirani, 2010) in R to determine predictors of tic onset. LASSO has good prediction accuracy, reduces overfitting and mitigates the issue of multicollinearity, especially when facing a small number of observations and a larger number of predictors. While LASSO is well‐suited to select relevant predictors, the strengths of associations can be less well inferred. This method automatically assigns a penalised term to the standardised predictors. As a result, all nonrelevant predictors are set to zero, and only the relevant (best fitting) predictors are selected. The selection of the penalty term is done by cross‐validation, where the average mean cross‐validated error is calculated through 10,000 iterations to get the best fitting penalty term. Of importance, no statistical significance is calculated with this analysis, only the relevance of a predictor to the outcome (tic onset). Statistical relevance of a predictor was defined as the presence of a nonzero beta coefficient and nonzero explained deviance (comparable to explained variance, but based on the −2 log‐likelihood instead of the residuals of the model; explained deviance indicates model fit with a range of 0–1, with a higher score indicating better fit; Finch & Hernandez Finch, 2016). Of note, due to the shrinkage process of LASSO (i.e. only selecting the best fitting predictors and setting all other predictors to zero), the nonzero beta coefficients are estimates; hence, they should be interpreted with caution since LASSO creates an upward bias for nonzero coefficients. The explained deviance of the predictors was calculated through the leave‐one‐out method; the predictor of interest is left out of the total model causing a change in the explained deviance of the total model. The difference between the explained deviance of the total model and the model without the predictor of interest is the contribution of this predictor to the model (Finch & Hernandez Finch, 2016), with a larger difference in explained deviance indicating a higher relevance as a predictor for tic onset.

As a first analysis, we performed a logistic LASSO regression with tic onset as an outcome measure in the whole sample (n = 187; of whom 61 had a tic onset) and the symptoms scales and health‐related quality of life as predictors of interest. We furthermore added age and sex (given significant univariate results) and site (using dummy variables) as covariates in all analyses. As a second analysis, we repeated the logistic LASSO regression separately for males (n = 86; of whom 36 had a tic onset) and females (n = 101; of whom 25 had a tic onset), respectively.

As a sensitivity analysis, we re‐analysed the logistic LASSO regression in a matched group on age and sex (n = 122; n = 61 children with and without a tic onset), using propensity score matching in R (Randolph, Falbe, Kureethara Manuel, & Balloun, 2014). A second sensitivity analysis was performed using the ASSQ severity score without the three items that may resemble tic‐like movements (a) ‘Expresses sounds involuntarily; clears throat, grunts, smacks, cries or screams’; (b) ‘Has involuntary face or body movements’; (c) ‘Has clumsy, ill‐coordinated, ungainly, awkward movements or gestures’, to remove the possible influence of these tic‐like items on the ASSQ as a possible predictor for tic onset.

Results

Group characteristics

Children (n = 61) were on average 7.9 years of age (range 3.5–13 years) at tic onset, and it took on average 1.1 years (range 0.1–5.4 years) from the start of the study until tics occurred. Table S1 shows the distribution of age of tic onset, with most tic onsets occurring between 4 and 8 years of age. See Table 1 for the group characteristics and univariate group differences in baseline measures in children with and without onset of tics (see Table S2 for group differences divided by sex). There were more boys, and children were younger in the tic onset group compared to the non‐tic onset group. Ethnicity, education of the child, and parental education level did not differ between groups. Children in the tic onset group had higher baseline hyperactivity‐impulsivity, ODD, CD, and ASD symptom scores compared to those in the non‐tic onset group. Of note, all clinical measures significantly increased in severity between the baseline visit and tic onset, while the health‐related quality‐of‐life score decreased.

Table 1.

Group characteristics in, and univariate group differences between children with and without a tic onset in baseline measures

Total sample

(n = 187)

Tic onset baseline

(n = 61)

No tic onset baseline (n = 126) Test statistic (tic onset vs no tic onset) Δ tic onset – baseline2 Δ last visit (no tic onset) – baseline3
Male sex, n (%) 86 (46.0) 36 (59.0) 50 (39.7) χ 2 = 6.19a*

Age years at baseline,

M (SD), range

7.43 (1.89)

3.21–10.93

6.71 (1.85)

3.21–10.39

7.74 (1.83)

4.12–10.93

U = 2529.50b** 0.92 (0.70) 2.27 (1.44)

Age years at tic onset,

M (SD), range

7.93 (2.00)

3.52–13.00

Caucasian ethnicity, n (%) 149 (79.7) 49 (80.3) 100 (79.4) χ 2 = 0.02a
Regular education, n (%) 167 (89.3) 49 (80.3) 118 (93.7) χ 2 = 0.41a

Parental education level,

M (SD), range

4.17 (1.07)

(1.50–6.00)

4.21 (1.00)

(1.50–6.00)

4.15 (1.11)

(1.50–6.00)

T(182) = −0.35c
OCD symptom severity
Obsessions, M (SD) 0.63 (2.34) 0.52 (2.29) 0.67 (2.34) T(185) = 0.41c 0.78 (3.12) 0.12 (0.80)
Compulsions, M (SD) 0.59 (2.34) 0.92 (2.29) 0.44 (1.94) T(198) = −1.34c 0.12 (4.10) 0.66 (2.51)
ADHD symptom severity
Inattention, M (SD) 5.28 (6.30) 5.73 (6.79) 5.07 (6.07) T(198) = −0.66c 1.85 (4.91) −0.39 (4.06)
Hyperactivity ‐impulsivity, M (SD) 5.03 (6.11) 6.48 (6.87) 4.33 (5.61) T(198) = −2.28c* 1.49 (4.00) −0.36 (3.49)
ODD symptom severity 4.07 (5.00) 5.19 (5.72) 3.52 (4.53) T(198) = −2.18c* 1.49 (3.12) 0.62 (3.21)

ASD symptom severity1,

M (SD)

3.60 (6.25) 5.24 (8.97) 2.81 (4.19) T(198) = −2.53c*
SDQ symptom severity

Emotional problems,

M (SD)

1.82 (2.09) 2.18 (2.29) 1.64 (1.97) T(198) = −1.67c 0.52 (2.09) 0.02 (1.96)

Conduct problems,

M (SD)

1.37 (1.56) 1.89 (1.63) 1.13 (1.46) T(198) = −3.20c* 0.24 (1.39) −0.17 (1.45)
HR‐QoL, M (SD) 79.07 (9.35) 77.54 (9.42) 79.69 (9.25) T(198) = 1.48c −0.86 (9.68) −0.77 (9.70)
OCD1, n (%) 5 (2.7) 2 (3.3) 3 (2.4) χ 2 = 4.10a
ADHD1, n (%) 21 (11.2) 7 (11.5) 14 (11.1) χ 2 = 0.01a

Ethnicity measured as Caucasian or other; Regular education measured as regular education primary school or special education primary school; Parental education level measured as the average highest education level of both parents, with 1 = under 7 years of schooling to 6 = post‐graduate/graduate/professional degree; OCD (obsessive‐compulsive disorder) severity assessed by the Children’s Yale‐Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Scahill et al., 1997); ADHD (attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and ODD (oppositional defiant disorder) symptom severity by the parent‐rated Swanson Nolan and Pelham‐IV rating scale (SNAP‐IV [Swanson et al., 2001]); ASD (autism spectrum disorder) symptom severity by the Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire (Ehlers et al., 1999); Severity of emotional problems and conduct problems by the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman et al., 2000); HR‐QoL (Health‐Related Quality of Life) by the KINDL‐R (Ravens‐Sieberer & Bullinger, 1998). A diagnosis of comorbid OCD and ADHD was assigned by a trained study clinician according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Between‐group differences were tested by aa Pearson's chi‐squared test, ba Mann‐Whitney U test, and can independent t‐test; *p < .05, **p < .001; Δ = delta, indicating change; 1 Assessed only at baseline, 2 n = 49 participants with a complete tic onset visit during the study; 3 n = 57 participants with a complete final non‐tic onset visit during the study.

Correlations between symptom scales

See Table S3, for the Pearson’s product‐moment correlation coefficients (r) between symptom scales for the total sample and separately for the tic onset group and the non‐tic onset group, and for males and females. Overall, correlations between the symptom scales were largely similar for the tic onset/non‐tic onset and for males/females groups. However, a large correlation (r = .73, p < .01) between obsessions and compulsions was observed in the tic onset group, whereas a small correlation (r = .23, p < .01) was found in the non‐tic onset group. Additionally, there were small‐to‐medium‐sized significant correlations between compulsions and most clinical characteristics in males (r = .22 – r = .40, p < .05), but not in females (r = −.10 – r = .22, p > .05).

Chronicity of tics after 1‐year follow‐up

A total of 44 (72%) from 61 children with a tic onset (detected during the regular study term) took part in a 1‐year follow‐up assessment. We confirmed the continuation of tics after tic onset during at least several weeks in all but one case (n = 43, 97.7%). In 66% (n = 29), the tics were chronic lasting at least 12 months (TS: n = 17, 38.6%; chronic motor tic disorder: n = 12, 27.3%), and 32% (n = 14) fulfilled criteria for tic disorders lasting less than 12 months in accordance with DSM‐IV‐TR, namely transient tic disorder (i.e. lasting at least 4 weeks but less than 12 months; n = 9, 20.5%) and tic disorder‐not otherwise specified (i.e. lasting less than 4 weeks, n = 5, 11.4%).

Predictors of tic onset in the total sample

See Table 2 for the beta coefficients and explained deviances of the first analysis in the total sample, using a logistic LASSO regression to investigate predictors of tic onset. Several factors were relevant (i.e. the beta coefficient and explained deviance were nonzero) as precursors for tic onset: being male, a younger age and a higher severity of compulsions, ASD symptoms and emotional and conduct problems. Note that, compared to the univariate analyses, hyperactivity‐impulsivity was no longer relevant, possibly due to conduct problems in the LASSO model (as the correlation between hyperactivity‐impulsivity symptoms and conduct problems was r = .76, p < .01).

Table 2.

Estimated coefficients and explained deviance of predictors of tic onset using a logistic LASSO regression (n = 187; of which 61 had tic onset)

β Explained deviance (%)
Sex −0.36 0.74
Age −0.14 3.21
OCD symptom severity
Obsessions 0 0
Compulsions 0.02 0.11
ADHD symptom severity
Inattentive 0 0
Hyperactivity‐impulsivity 0 0
ODD symptom severity 0 0
ASD symptom severity 0.08 0.12
SDQ symptom severity
Emotional problems 0.03 0.07
Conduct problems 0.23 0.93
HR‐QoL 0 0
Total model 13.31

See for more information Table 1. LASSO; logistic least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (Tibshirani, 1996)

Statistically relevant predictors were determined by the presence of nonzero betas and nonzero explained deviance. Due to the shrinkage process of LASSO (i.e. only selecting the best fitting predictors and setting all other predictors to zero), the beta coefficients are estimations and must be interpreted with caution. The explained deviance shows the contribution in percentage (%) of the predictor to the model, with a higher explained deviance indicating a higher relevance of the predictor to tic onset. Of note, the explained deviances of the individual variables do not add up to the explained deviance in the total model due to the interrelationship of variables (i.e. leaving out one variable can lead to other variables taking over explained deviance).

Predictors of tic onset in males and females

See Table 3 for the beta coefficients and explained deviances in a logistic LASSO regression divided by sex. Precursors associated with tic onset in males were a younger age and a higher severity of ASD symptoms and conduct problems. Precursors associated with tic onset in females were a younger age, and a higher severity of compulsions, and ODD and emotional problems. Note that ODD emerged as a new symptom domain in females compared to the LASSO analysis not stratified by sex, perhaps due to the absence of conduct problems as a predictor in females. Obsessions, ADHD symptom severity and health‐related quality of life were not relevant predictors of tic onset in any of the LASSO analyses.

Table 3.

Estimated coefficients and explained deviance of predictors of tic onset using a logistic LASSO regression divided by sex

Males (n = 86, of which 36 with tic onset) Females (n = 101, of which 25 with tic onset)
β Explained deviance (%) β Explained deviance (%)
Age −0.31 2.62 −0.35 3.40
OCD symptom severity
Obsessions 0 0 0 0
Compulsions 0 0 0.39 2.65
ADHD symptom severity
Inattentive 0 0 0 0
Hyperactivity‐impulsivity 0 0 0 0
ODD symptom severity 0.39 1.13
ASD symptom severity 0.01 0.03 0 0
SDQ symptom severity
Emotional problems 0 0 0.11 0.39
Conduct problems 0.20 1.49 0 0
HR‐QoL 0 0 0 0
Total model 17.10 22.56

See for information Tables 1 and 2.

Sensitivity analyses

See Table S4 for the logistic LASSO regression in a matched group on age and sex. Both conduct problems and compulsions remained relevant precursors of tics. Note that due to matching, the sample size was reduced leaving less power to detect relevant results.

In the second sensitivity analysis in the total sample, the beta for ASD symptoms after removing the three questions from the ASSQ resembling tic‐like movements remained relevant (β = 0.08, explained deviance 0.17%). In the sex‐specific analysis, the beta for ASD symptoms increased from β = 0.01 to β = 0.07, and the explained deviance from 0.03% to 0.31%. The other predictors in the model remained the same. This indicates the relevance of ASD symptoms reflecting social difficulties and autistic traits for tic onset, especially in males.

Discussion

This is the first study to prospectively investigate clinical precursors of tics in high‐risk children aged 3‐10 years. Tic onset occurred at an average age of 7.9 years, with a range of 3.5 to 13 years, yet mostly between ages 4 and 8 years. The frequency of tic onsets is in line with the recurrence risk estimate for chronic tic disorders in first‐degree relatives (i.e. 29.9% [95% CI = 23.2%–38.5%]; Heiman et al., 2020). The average time between the assessment of the clinical characteristics and tic onset was 1.1 years. We observed higher levels of conduct and oppositional problems, ASD symptoms, compulsions and emotional problems in children prior to tic onset compared to children who did not develop tics. Notably, findings appeared to be sex‐specific, largely in line with the sex distribution of psychiatric disorders (Hirschtritt et al., 2015; Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019). However, ADHD symptoms, obsessions and quality of life were no relevant predictors of tic onset. Furthermore, during follow‐up one year after tic onset, we confirmed the presence of a chronic tic disorder in the majority (66%) of children, while 32% were diagnosed with transient tics lasting less than 12 months. This supports new insights that the persistence of tics is more often the rule than the exception (Kim et al., 2019), in contrast to common belief that tics in children are mostly transient.

Conduct problems including aggression, lying, deceitfulness or other rule‐breaking behaviours (Goodman et al., 2000) were the strongest predictor of tic onset in males. CD has been found to be more prevalent in individuals with TS in two large studies (lifetime prevalence 7%–14.5%; Pérez‐Vigil et al., 2018; Robertson, Cavanna, & Eapen, 2015) than in the general population (0.3%–9%; INSERM Collective Expertise Centre, 2015; Pérez‐Vigil et al., 2018), but this was not suggested in all studies (e.g. 3.2% in a clinical TS sample; Hirschtritt et al., 2015). Conduct problems may already start in early childhood, with a male preponderance (INSERM Collective Expertise Centre, 2015; Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019). Although the presence of CD in TS has often been linked to the co‐occurrence of ADHD rather than to TS per se (Hirschtritt et al., 2015; Robertson et al., 2015), one study found that conduct problems correlated with tic severity directly (Sukhodolsky et al., 2003). Still, the association of conduct problems, and not ADHD symptoms, with tic onset was somewhat surprising, given the strong association of ADHD symptoms with TS and the usually early onset of ADHD (e.g. ages 3–6; Hirschtritt et al., 2015), compared to the rarer occurrence of CD, certainly in younger children (e.g. 2% below age 12 years; INSERM Collective Expertise Centre, 2015). Indeed, our analyses indicate that hyperactivity–impulsivity as a univariate predictor of tic onset did not hold in the multivariable LASSO model, suggesting that conduct problems are a more relevant predictor of tic onset within the externalising behaviour domain. However, as many as 11% of the children who did not develop tics had a diagnosis of ADHD, possibly due to a shared genetic liability for ADHD in the first‐degree relatives. It should be noted that the SDQ conduct problems scale partly includes ODD symptoms; yet, we did not find an association of the SNAP‐IV ODD scale with tic onset in males. More large‐scale studies are needed to further elucidate the relationship between externalising behaviours and tic disorders.

In males, tics were also preceded by ASD symptoms reflecting social difficulties and autistic traits such as repetitiveness or restricted interests (Posserud et al., 2008). Symptoms of autism are often already noticeable in early childhood (2–3 years of age; Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019), generally before the occurrence of tics (range 3–8.6 years of age; Leckman et al., 1998) with a male predominance (Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019). The association of ASD symptoms with tic onset may thus reflect the known co‐occurrence of ASD in children with TS (e.g. 21%; Rizzo et al., 2017). Still, in contrast to ADHD and OCD, a recent genome‐wide association study meta‐analysis has shown that ASD has a low magnitude of genetic correlation with TS, suggesting that ASD and TS are more distinct than the other psychiatric disorders (Brainstorm Consortium et al., 2018). Yet other studies support the involvement of abnormalities in cortico‐striatal circuits in both ASD and TS (McBride & Parker, 2015; Rizzo et al., 2017). Importantly, although certain repetitive stereotypic behaviours (e.g. hand/body movements) may be observed in both ASD and TS (Huisman‐van Dijk, Schoot, Rijkeboer, Mathews, & Cath, 2016; Rizzo et al., 2017), ASD symptoms became an even stronger predictor of tic onset in our study after removing items that may resemble tic‐like behaviours (e.g. 'has involuntary face or body movements’ and ‘expresses sounds involuntarily’), confirming that indeed other ASD symptoms (such as social communication impairments and preoccupation with routines) precede tic onset, especially in males.

Precursors of tic onset in females were compulsions, emotional problems and ODD behaviours. Both OCD and emotional problems (anxiety/depression) show a higher occurrence in females than males, both in samples with TS and in the general population, increasing with age (Hirschtritt et al., 2015; Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019). In children with TS, being female was a strong predictor of emotional disorders (including anxiety, phobia and depression) in early adulthood (Groth, Skov, Lange, & Debes, 2019). Yet the onset of OCD and mood disorders is typically in the early adolescent years (from age 11–14 years; Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019) in females and thus after the usual age of onset of tics (Leckman et al., 1998). However, subthreshold OCD symptoms and anxiety disorders have also been reported in children as early as age 5–6 years (Fineberg et al., 2013; Hirschtritt et al., 2015), and OCD symptoms may be experienced up to 5 years before meeting criteria for an OCD diagnosis (Coles, Johnson, & Schubert, 2011). Consistent with our prospective finding, early‐onset OCD (≤10 years of age) has been associated with a higher risk of tic disorders (Chabane et al., 2005; Janowitz et al., 2009). Although early‐onset OCD has often been linked to male sex (Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019), Hirschtritt et al., (2015) reported an age of onset of comorbid OCD as early as seven years in both boys and girls with TS. In line with our study, females with TS were more likely than males to display compulsive tics at tic onset (Santangelo et al., 1994). The close interrelationship between OCD and TS is also reflected in recent studies suggesting a shared genetic architecture (Brainstorm Consortium et al., 2018; Yu et al., 2015), and in shared or similar clinical characteristics such as repetitive behaviours (e.g. counting, touching), often not without ambiguity in distinguishing between (compulsive) tics, compulsions or ASD‐related movements (Huisman‐van Dijk et al., 2016; Rizzo et al., 2017). Of note, despite OCD being underrepresented in our study (as an OCD diagnosis was an exclusion criterion; Schrag et al., 2019), compulsions still emerged as a consistent predictor for tic onset in our study. Yet, the lack of association of compulsions with tic onset in males in our study may perhaps be due to the considerable correlation of compulsions with the other symptom domains found only in males. Finally, we may not have observed obsessions as a significant predictor of tics due to the age of our sample, in line with previous observations that compulsions are generally reported earlier than symptoms of obsessions (Coles et al., 2011; Fineberg et al., 2013).

ODD problems as a female predictor of tic onset were unexpected given that externalising disorders (ADHD, ODD and CD) are typically male‐predominant, and frequently co‐occurring (Hirschtritt et al., 2015; INSERM Collective Expertise Centre, 2015; Steinhausen & Jakobsen, 2019). Perhaps ODD problems emerged as a predictor in female participants, given the absence of conduct problems as a predictive factor of tic onset in females. Consistent with this, females with ODD generally present with higher comorbidity of symptoms in the internalising domain (anxiety and depression), whereas in males ODD has been more associated with symptoms in the externalising domain (CD and ADHD; Trepat & Ezpeleta, 2011). More recently, an affective dimension of ODD (irritability/negative affect) has been distinguished from a behavioural dimension (defiant behaviour/headstrong) suggestive of distinct aetiologies (Leadbeater & Homel, 2015). Indeed, a recent study in TS indicated that these ODD dimensions in childhood are developmentally linked to respectively internalising (as well as to OCD; Thériault et al., 2014) and externalising mental health symptoms in adulthood (Thériault et al., 2018). Overall, our sex‐specific findings in females seem to reflect symptoms in the affective‐anxiety domain (note that anxiety and harm avoidance also largely underlie compulsions), consistent with their higher prevalence in females. These symptoms may be closely watched in females with a familial risk to develop tics.

Important strengths of this study are the large sample and prospective design enabling us to investigate clinical precursors of tic onset in children at heightened risk of developing tics. Also, the use of LASSO regression overcomes issues with multicollinearity and minimises overfitting, yet the method is most suited to indicate the relevance of associations rather than their strengths. Weaknesses of this study are an underestimation of the presence of OCD in our sample; nevertheless, compulsions still emerged as a predictor for tic onset in our study. Moreover, the assessment of past week symptom severity at baseline rather than longer time windows may also have caused an underestimation of effects. In addition, we made comparisons with unaffected first‐degree relatives, who may have higher rates of psychiatric problems than the general population, thus potentially masking differences in clinical characteristics preceding tics, in comparison to the general population. Also, we cannot rule out that some of the unaffected children may still develop tics after the age of 10, even though the typical age of onset of tics is well below that age; still, the recurrence risk rate of tic onset of our sample is in line with the literature (Heiman et al., 2020), thus not suggesting many missing tic onsets. Further, we may have failed to include young children in our high‐risk sample, perhaps having resulted in a higher average age of tic onset than in other studies (Leckman et al., 1998). Lastly, the time difference between baseline and tic onset varied between children; we chose characteristics at baseline to capture the earliest possible precursors. Future studies may focus on longitudinal analyses assessing multiple time points that lead up to tic onset. Moreover, future research may include younger children than in our cohort and explore genetic loading (polygenic risk) across symptom domains between cases with tics and unaffected siblings as well as nonfamiliar controls. Other factors may be considered, including family history, contextual variables that may be shared between family members (e.g. family and parental functioning) and other environmental risk factors (e.g. prenatal adversities, early life stress) potentially affecting premorbid symptomatology.

Conclusion

Our study points to a distinct symptom presentation in children prior to the onset of tics. Our results clearly suggest a sex‐specific profile, with conduct problems and ASD symptoms preceding tic onset in males, and compulsions, and emotional and ODD problems as precursors in females. A variety of premorbid subtle symptoms may exist as precursors to tics, highlighting the need of regular monitoring of high‐risk children from preschool age onwards, taking sex differences into account. Increased awareness of health professionals and parents may benefit the early detection of tics setting off treatment strategies preventing negative outcomes often associated with this highly stigmatising condition compromising quality of life. Girls may particularly benefit as they may be particularly prone to late diagnosis of tics, due to their different symptom presentation than in boys who typically show problematic behaviour at an earlier age than girls.

Supporting information

Appendix S1. EMTICS group authorship.

Appendix S2. Full description of study sample, procedures and quality assessment.

Appendix S3. Further description of clinical measures.

Appendix S4. Description of the statistical analyses of the sample characteristics.

Appendix S5. Further funding support and acknowledgements.

Table S1. Distribution of age of tic onset.

Table S2. Group characteristics for males and females.

Table S3. Correlations between clinical symptom scales.

Table S4. Logistic LASSO regression in a matched sample on age and sex.

Figure S1. EMTICS ONSET cohort study procedures flow chart.

Figure S2. Study flow chart: final study n=187 participants.

Acknowledgements

The authors are deeply grateful to all children and their parents who willingly participated to make this research possible. This project has received funding from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration under Grant agreement no. 278367. Appendix S5 provides further funding support.

D.M. has received honoraria for lecturing from the Movement Disorders Society, Tourette Syndrome Association of America and Dystonia Medical Research Foundation Canada; honoraria for advisory board attendance from Sunovion Pharmaceuticals Canada Inc.; research and education funding support from Dystonia Medical Research Foundation Canada, the University of Calgary, the Michael P Smith Family, the Owerko Foundation, Ipsen Corporate, the Parkinson Association of Alberta and the Canadian Institutes for Health Research; royalties from Springer‐Verlag. K.M‐V. has received financial or material research support from Abide Therapeutics, Almirall Hermal GmbH, GW pharmaceuticals, Lundbeck, Syneos Health, and Therapix Biosciences Ltd. She has received consultant's honoraria from Abide Therapeutics, Allmiral, Boehringer Ingelheim International GmbH, Bionorica Ethics GmbH, CannaMedical Pharma GmbH, Canopy Grouth, Columbia Care, CTC Communications Corp., Eurox Deutschland GmbH, Global Praxis Group Limited, IMC Germany, Lundbeck, Resalo Vertrieb GmbH, Sanity Group, STADAPHARM GmbH, Synendos Therapeutics AG, and Tilray. She is/was a consultant or advisory board member for Abide Therapeutics, The Academy of Medical Cannabis Limited, Alirio, Aphria Deutschland GmbH, CannaMedical Pharma GmbH, Bionorica Ethics GmbH, CannaXan GmbH, Canopy Growth, Columbia Care, CTC Communications Corp., IMC Germany, Leafly Deutschland GmbH, Lundbeck, Nuvelution TS Pharma Inc., Resalo Vertrieb GmbH, Sanity Group, Syqe Medical Ltd., Therapix Biosciences Ltd., Tilray, Wayland Group, and CTC Communications Corporation. She has received speaker’s fees from Aphria Deutschland GmbH, Cogitando GmbH, Emalex, Eurox Deutschland GmbH, Ever pharma GmbH, PR Berater, Spectrum Therapeutics GmbH, Tilray, and Wayland Group. She has received royalties from Medizinisch Wissenschaftliche Verlagsgesellschaft Berlin, Elsevier, and Kohlhammer. A.M. received Royalties for the book Neurogenetics (Oxford University Press). A.S. has received consultancy or advisory board honoraria from Biogen, Abbvie, Bial and Neurotechnology, research support from the National Institute of Health Research, Parkinson’s UK and the Economic and Social Research Council and the European Commission, and Royalties from Oxford University Press. S.W. has received in the last 5 years royalties from Thieme Hogrefe, Kohlhammer, Springer, Beltz. Her work was supported in the last 5 years by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNF), diff. EU FP7s, HSM Hochspezialisierte Medizin of the Kanton Zurich, Switzerland, Bfarm Germany, ZInEP, Hartmann Müller Stiftung, Olga Mayenfisch, Gertrud Thalmann, Vontobel, Unicentia, Erika Schwarz Fonds. Outside professional activities and interests are declared under the link of the University of Zurich www.uzh.ch/prof/ssl‐dir/interessenbindungen/client/web/. The remaining authors have declared that they have no competing or potential conflicts of interest.

Key points.

  • Whether subtle clinical precursors may exist before the onset of tics is currently unknown.

  • This prospective study aimed to find subtle clinical precursors of tics by assessing a range of symptoms prior to tic onset in comparison with children without tic onset.

  • Our results indicate a distinct symptom presentation prior to the development of tics.

  • Findings are sex‐specific, with conduct problems and autism spectrum symptoms as precursors of tics in males and with compulsions and emotional and oppositional‐defiant problems in females.

  • Our results highlight the need of regular monitoring of high‐risk children from preschool age onwards, taking sex differences into account.

Conflict of interest statement: See Acknowledgements for full disclosures.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th edn). Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
  2. Athanasiadou, A. , Buitelaar, J.K. , Brovedani, P. , Chorna, O. , Fulceri, F. , Guzzetta, A. , & Scattoni, M.L. (2020). Early motor signs of attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 903–916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bäumer, T. , Sajin, V. , & Münchau, A. (2016). Childhood‐onset movement disorders: A clinical series of 606 cases. Movement Disorders Clinical Practice, 4, 437–440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Brainstorm Consortium , Anttila, V. , Bulik‐Sullivan, B. , Finucane, H.K. , Walters, R.K. , … & Murray, A. (2018). Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science, 360, eaap8757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bussing, R. , Fernandez, M. , Harwood, M. , Wei, H. , Garvan, C.W. , Eyberg, S.M. , & Swanson, J.M. (2008). Parent and teacher SNAP‐IV ratings of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: Psychometric properties and normative rating from a school district sample. Assessment, 15, 317–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chabane, N. , Delorme, R. , Millet, B. , Mouren, M.C. , Leboyer, M. , & Pauls, D. (2005). Early‐onset obsessive‐compulsive disorder: A subgroup with a specific clinical and familial pattern? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 881–887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Coles, M.E. , Johnson, E.M. , & Schubert, J.R. (2011). Retrospective reports of the development of obsessive‐compulsive disorder: Extending knowledge of the protracted symptom phase. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 39, 579–589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Eapen, V. , Snedden, C. , Črnčec, R. , Pick, A. , & Sachdev, P. (2016). Tourette syndrome, co‐morbidities and quality of life. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 50, 82–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Ehlers, S. , Gillberg, C. , & Wing, L. (1999). A screening questionnaire for Asperger syndrome and other high‐functioning autism spectrum disorders in school age children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 129–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Finch, W.H. , & Hernandez Finch, M.E. (2016). Regularization methods for fitting linear models with small sample sizes: Fitting the lasso estimator using R. Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation, 21, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
  11. Fineberg, N.A. , Hengartner, M.P. , Bergbaum, C.E. , Gale, T.M. , Gamma, A. , Ajdacic‐Gross, V. , … & Angst, J. (2013). A prospective population‐based cohort study of the prevalence, incidence and impact of obsessive‐compulsive symptomatology. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 17, 170–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Friedman, J. , Hastie, T. , & Tibshirani, R. (2010). Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. Journal of Statistical Software, 33, 1–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Goodman, R. , Ford, T. , Simmons, H. , Gatward, R. , & Meltzer, H. (2000). Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 534–539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Groth, C. , Skov, L. , Lange, T. , & Debes, N.M. (2019). Predictors of the clinical course of Tourette syndrome: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Neurology, 34, 913–921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hafeman, D.M. , Merranko, J. , Goldstein, T.R. , Axelson, D. , Goldstein, B.I. , Monk, K. , … & Birmaher, B. (2017). Assessment of a person‐level risk calculator to predict new‐onset bipolar spectrum disorder in youth at familial risk. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 841–847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Heiman, G.A. , Rispoli, J. , Seymour, C. , Leckman, J.F. , King, R.A. , & Fernandez, T.V. (2020). Empiric recurrence risk estimates for chronic tic disorders: Implications for genetic counseling. Frontiers in Neurology, 11, 770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hirschtritt, M.E. , Lee, P.C. , Pauls, D.L. , Dion, Y. , Grados, M.A. , … & Mathews, C.A. (2015). Lifetime prevalence, age of risk, and etiology of comorbid psychiatric disorders in Tourette syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(4), 325–333. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2650 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Huisman‐van Dijk, H.M. , Schoot, R.V. , Rijkeboer, M.M. , Mathews, C.A. , & Cath, D.C. (2016). The relationship between tics, OC, ADHD and autism symptoms: A cross‐ disorder symptom analysis in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome patients and family‐members. Psychiatry Research, 237, 138–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. INSERM Collective Expertise Centre . (2005). INSERM Collective Expert Reports [Internet]. Paris: Institut national de la santé et de la recherche médicale; 2000‐. Conduct: Disorder in children and adolescents. Available from: https://www‐ncbi‐nlm‐nih‐gov.proxy‐ub.rug.nl/books/NBK7133/ [last accessed 20 January 2021]. [PubMed]
  20. Janowitz, D. , Grabe, H.J. , Ruhrmann, S. , Ettelt, S. , Buhtz, F. , Hochrein, A. , … & Wagner, M. (2009). Early onset of obsessive‐compulsive disorder and associated comorbidity. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 1012–1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Khalifa, N. , & Von Knorring, A.L. (2005). Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders in a total population of children: Clinical assessment and background. Acta Paediatrica, 94, 1608–1614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kim, S. , Greene, D.J. , Bihun, E.C. , Koller, J.M. , Hampton, J.M. , Acevedo, H. , … & Black, K.J. (2019). Provisional Tic Disorder is not so transient. Science Report, 9, 3951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Leadbeater, B.J. , & Homel, J. (2015). Irritable and defiant sub‐dimensions of ODD: Their stability and prediction of internalizing symptoms and conduct problems from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 407–421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Leckman, J.F. , Riddle, M.A. , Hardin, M.T. , Ort, S.I. , Swartz, K.L. , Stevenson, J. , & Cohen, D.J. (1989). The Yale Global Tic Severity Scale: Initial testing of a clinician‐rated scale of tic severity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 566–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Leckman, J.F. , Zhang, H. , Vitale, A. , Lahnin, F. , Lynch, K. , Bondi, C. , … & Peterson, B.S. (1998). Course of tic severity in Tourette syndrome: the first two decades. Pediatrics, 102(1 Pt 1), 14–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. McBride, S.D. , & Parker, M.O. (2015). The disrupted basal ganglia and behavioural control: An integrative cross‐domain perspective of spontaneous stereotypy. Behavioural Brain Research, 276, 45–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Möricke, E. , Greven, C.U. , Visser, J.C. , Oosterling, I.J. , Buitelaar, J.K. , & Rommelse, N.N.J. (2019). Social‐communicative and attention problems in infancy and toddlerhood as precursors of preschool autistic traits. Attention Deficitand Hyperactivity Disorders, 11, 113–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Pérez‐Vigil, A. , Fernández de la Cruz, L. , Brander, G. , Isomura, K. , Jangmo, A. , Kuja‐Halkola, R. , … & Mataix‐Cols, D. (2018). Association of Tourette syndrome and chronic tic disorders with objective indicators of educational attainment: A population‐based sibling comparison study. JAMA Neurology, 75, 1098–1105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Posserud, B. , Lundervold, A.J. , Steijnen, M.C. , Verhoeven, S. , Stormark, K.M. , & Gillberg, C. (2008). Factor analysis of the Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire. Autism, 12, 99–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Randolph, J.J. , Falbe, K. , Kureethara Manuel, A. , & Balloun, J.L. (2014). A step‐by‐step guide to Propensity Score Matching in R. Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation, 19, 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ravens‐Sieberer, U. , & Bullinger, M. (1998). Assesing health related quality of life in chronically ill children with the German KINDL: First psychometric and content analytical results. Quality of Life Research, 7, 399–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rizzo, R. , Gulisano, M. , Domini, C.N. , Ferro, M.C. , & Curatolo, P. (2017). The relationship between autism spectrum disorder and Tourette syndrome in childhood: An overview of shared characteristics. Journal of Pediatric Neurology, 15, 115–122. [Google Scholar]
  33. Robertson, M.M. , Cavanna, A.E. , & Eapen, V. (2015). Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and disruptive behavior disorders: prevalence, associations, and explanation of the relationships. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 27, 33–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Santangelo, S.L. , Pauls, D.L. , Goldstein, J.M. , Faraone, S.V. , Tsuang, M.T. , & Leckman, J.F. (1994). Tourette's syndrome: what are the influences of gender and comorbid obsessive‐compulsive disorder? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 795–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Scahill, L. , Riddle, M.A. , McSwiggin‐Hardin, M. , Ort, S.I. , King, R.A. , … & Leckman, J.F. (1997). Children’s Yale‐Brown Obsessive‐Compulsive Scale: Reliability and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 844–852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Schrag, A. , Martino, D. , Apter, A. , Ball, J. , Bartolini, E. , Benaroya‐Milshtein, N. , … & Hoekstra, P.J. (2019). European Multicentre Tics in Children Studies (EMTICS): Protocol for two cohort studies to assess risk factors for tic onset and exacerbation in children and adolescents. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 91–109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Steinhausen, H.‐C. , & Jakobsen, H. (2019). Incidence rates of treated mental disorders in childhood and adolescence in a complete nationwide birth cohort. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 80, 3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Storch, E.A. , Murphy, T.K. , Adkins, J.W. , Lewin, A.B. , Geffken, G.R. , Johns, N.B. , … & Goodman, W.K. (2006). The children’s Yale‐brown obsessive‐compulsive scale: Psychometric properties of child‐ and parent‐report formats. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20, 1055–1070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sukhodolsky, D.G. , Scahill, L. , Zhang, H. , Peterson, B.S. , King, R.A. , Lombroso, P.J. , … & Leckman, J.F. (2003). Disruptive behavior in children with Tourette's syndrome: Association with ADHD comorbidity, tic severity, and functional impairment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 98–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Swanson, J.M. , Kraemer, H.C. , Hinshaw, S.P. , Arnold, L.E. , Connors, C.K. , … & Wu, M. (2001). Clinical relevance of the primary findings of the MTA: Success rates based on the severity of ADHD and ODD symptoms at the end of treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 168–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Tabachnick, B.G. , & Fidell, L.S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. [Google Scholar]
  42. Thériault, M.G. , Bécue, J.C. , Lespérance, P. , Chouinard, S. , Rouleau, G.A. , & Richer, F. (2018). Oppositional behavior and longitudinal predictions of early adulthood mental health problems in chronic tic disorders. Psychiatry Research, 266, 301–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Thériault, M.‐C. , Lespérance, P. , Achim, A. , Tellier, G. , Diab, S. , Rouleau, G.A. , … & Richer, F. (2014). ODD irritability is associated with obsessive‐compulsive behavior and not ADHD in chronic tic disorders. Psychiatry Research, 220, 447–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Tibshirani, R. (1996). Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 1, 267–288. [Google Scholar]
  45. Trepat, E. , & Ezpeleta, L. (2011). Sex differences in oppositional defiant disorder. Psicothema, 23, 666–671. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Yu, D. , Mathews, C.A. , Scharf, J.M. , Neale, B.M. , Davis, L.K. , Gamazon, E.R. , … & Pauls, D.L. (2015). Cross‐disorder genome‐wide analyses suggest a complex genetic relationship between Tourette's syndrome and OCD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 82–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix S1. EMTICS group authorship.

Appendix S2. Full description of study sample, procedures and quality assessment.

Appendix S3. Further description of clinical measures.

Appendix S4. Description of the statistical analyses of the sample characteristics.

Appendix S5. Further funding support and acknowledgements.

Table S1. Distribution of age of tic onset.

Table S2. Group characteristics for males and females.

Table S3. Correlations between clinical symptom scales.

Table S4. Logistic LASSO regression in a matched sample on age and sex.

Figure S1. EMTICS ONSET cohort study procedures flow chart.

Figure S2. Study flow chart: final study n=187 participants.


Articles from Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES