Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2021 Jun 26;27(1):215–264. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12542

Interventions to increase personal protective behaviours to limit the spread of respiratory viruses: A rapid evidence review and meta‐analysis

Olga Perski 1,, Dorothy Szinay 2, Elizabeth Corker 3, Lion Shahab 1, Robert West 1, Susan Michie 3
PMCID: PMC12086758  PMID: 34173697

Abstract

Purpose

Increasing personal protective behaviours is critical for stopping the spread of respiratory viruses, including SARS‐CoV‐2: We need evidence to inform how to achieve this. We aimed to synthesize evidence on interventions to increase six personal protective behaviours (e.g., hand hygiene, face mask use, maintaining physical distancing) to limit the spread of respiratory viruses.

Methods

We used best practice for rapid evidence reviews. We searched Ovid MEDLINE and Scopus. Studies conducted in adults or children with active or passive comparators were included. We extracted data on study design, intervention content, mode of delivery, population, setting, mechanism(s) of action, acceptability, practicability, effectiveness, affordability, spill‐over effects, and equity impact. Study quality was assessed with Cochrane’s risk‐of‐bias tool. A narrative synthesis and random‐effects meta‐analyses were conducted.

Results

We identified 39 studies conducted across 15 countries. Interventions targeted hand hygiene (n = 30) and/or face mask use (n = 12) and used two‐ or three‐arm study designs with passive comparators. Interventions were typically delivered face‐to‐face and included a median of three behaviour change techniques. The quality of included studies was low. Interventions to increase hand hygiene (k = 6) had a medium, positive effect (d = .62, 95% CI = 0.43–0.80, p < .001, I 2 = 81.2%). Interventions targeting face mask use (k = 4) had mixed results, with an imprecise pooled estimate (OR = 4.14, 95% CI = 1.24–13.79, p < .001, I 2 = 89.67%). Between‐study heterogeneity was high.

Conclusions

We found low‐quality evidence for positive effects of interventions targeting hand hygiene, with unclear results for interventions targeting face mask use. There was a lack of evidence for most behaviours of interest within this review.

Keywords: behaviour change, COVID‐19, intervention, personal protective behaviours, rapid review, SARS‐CoV‐2


Statement of Contribution.

What is already known on this subject?

  • Widespread adoption of personal protective behaviours (PPBs) is needed to block respiratory viral transmission.

  • Effective interventions that increase PPBs primarily target health care professionals.

  • The generalizability of such interventions to the general population remains unclear.

What does this study add?

  • This rapid review extends findings from health care to community settings.

  • We found evidence for a medium, positive effect of hand hygiene interventions.

  • Interventions targeting face mask use had unclear results.

  • Few relevant studies were identified, and study quality was low.

Background

Respiratory viruses such as influenza, respiratory syncytial virus, parainfluenza, rhinovirus, coronavirus (including SARS‐CoV‐2), and adenovirus enter the body through the eyes, nose, and mouth (the ‘T‐Zone’) (Killingley & Nguyen‐Van‐Tam, 2013; West, Michie, Rubin, & Amlôt, 2020). Changing human behaviour is critical for stopping the spread of respiratory viruses in general and the SARS‐CoV‐2 virus in particular, and for supporting the easing of financially and psychologically costly physical distancing measures during viral epidemics (Ferguson et al., 2006; Michie, Rubin, & Amlôt, 2020; Michie, West, & Amlôt, 2020; West et al., 2020). Personal protective behaviours, including hand washing, disinfecting fomites such as clothes or furniture, and face mask wearing, are advocated for limiting the spread of SARS‐CoV‐2 (Lunn et al., 2020; World Health Organization, 2019). Simply advising people to adopt these behaviours has been found to be insufficient, just as has explaining what to do and why these behaviours are necessary (Bish & Michie, 2010). Directly relevant evidence on interventions to promote adherence to personal protective behaviours in community‐dwelling children and adults is sparse but there is an urgent need to identify and synthesize what evidence does exist. Policymakers need evidence to inform the development of public health guidance and decide which interventions to prioritize. We adopted best practice for rapid evidence reviews to evaluate the acceptability, practicability, effectiveness, affordability, spill‐over effects (i.e., unintended consequences), and equity impact (the ‘APEASE’ criteria (Michie, Atkins, & West, 2014)) of interventions to increase personal protective behaviours to limit the spread of respiratory viruses.

During pandemics of respiratory viruses, multipronged approaches involving both pharmacological (e.g., vaccination) and behavioural measures (e.g., hand washing, physical distancing) are required to bring the reproductive number below 1 (Ferguson et al., 2006; Michie, Rubin, & Amlôt, 2020; Michie et al., 2020; Michie, West, Amlôt, & Rubin, 2020). Vaccination of populations will take months, even years, to roll out, especially in low‐ and middle‐income countries. Hence, physical distancing and other behavioural measures will be required, possibly permanently. Population‐wide restrictions are costly from financial, social, and psychological perspectives: The world economy has been projected to shrink by approximately 4.9% in 2020 (International Monetary Fund, 2020), with an additional 88 million people globally being pushed into extreme poverty (i.e., living on less than $1.90/day) (Blake & Wadhwa, 2020), and prolonged periods of social isolation are associated with increases in domestic violence (SafeLives, 2020) and negative mental health effects, such as post‐traumatic stress disorder, confusion, and anger (Brooks et al., 2020). Less costly, yet highly effective (Warren‐Gash, Fragaszy, & Hayward, 2012) personal protective behaviours are thus important for supporting the easing of lockdown measures to ensure long‐term suppression of viral transmission and preparedness for new viral waves and future pandemics (West et al., 2020). To successfully block the spread of respiratory viruses including (but not limited to) SARS‐CoV‐2 – which are transmitted via droplets, aerosols, and direct physical contact (Killingley & Nguyen‐Van‐Tam, 2013; West et al., 2020) – several personal protective behaviours must be adopted across the population (see Figure 1). Although systematic reviews of interventions to change hand hygiene in health care professionals are available (Edwards et al., 2012; Huis et al., 2012; Luangasanatip et al., 2015; Mbakaya, Lee, & Lee, 2017; Olena Doronina, Jones, Martello, Biron, & Lavoie‐Tremblay, 2017), generalizability to community settings is limited. There also appears to be little evidence about interventions to change behaviours such as not touching the T‐Zone (eyes, nose, and mouth), which would have a significant effect if adopted (Kwok, Gralton, & McLaws, 2015) and carry little or no costs to people or society. If adopted at scale across the population including disadvantaged communities, such interventions have the potential to reduce health inequalities. Here, we aimed to conduct a rapid evidence review to evaluate the acceptability, practicability, effectiveness, affordability, spill‐over effects, and equity of interventions to increase personal protective behaviours that limit the spread of respiratory viruses.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Map of personal protective behaviours relevant for blocking transmission of respiratory viruses, including SARS‐CoV‐2: hand washing and use of hand sanitizers; avoiding touching the ‘T‐Zone’; catching droplets in tissues and discarding these; face mask use; disinfecting surfaces; and maintaining physical distancing. Reproduced with permission from the authors (West & Michie, 2020).

Methods

Study design

The study protocol was pre‐registered on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/7cphy/). During ongoing pandemics, the World Health Organization recommends the use of rapid evidence reviews for swift knowledge generation (World Health Organization, 2017). We adopted acknowledged best practice for rapid evidence reviews, which involved completing the review in a timely fashion, limiting the search to main databases and the published literature, and having one reviewer extract data and another verify (Haby et al., 2016; Tricco et al., 2015).

Criteria for considering studies for this review

Population

We included studies that recruited as participants community‐dwelling children or adults (as opposed to qualified or trainee health care professionals in hospital or care home settings) across any type of study setting (e.g., schools, primary care).

Intervention

We included reports of evaluations of any type of intervention (e.g., mass media, face‐to‐face, technology‐mediated) designed to change at least one of six personal protective behaviours to block transmission of respiratory viruses, such as influenza, respiratory syncytial virus, parainfluenza, rhinovirus, coronavirus, or adenovirus, which have a shared route of transmission (i.e., droplets, aerosols, direct physical contact) (Killingley & Nguyen‐Van‐Tam, 2013). Although the relative importance of different personal protective behaviours depends on properties of the specific respiratory virus in addition to the clinical and/or environmental context – for example, fomite transmission may be more pronounced for respiratory syncytial virus compared with coronaviruses (Boone & Gerba, 2007) – at the time of planning this rapid review, little was known about the properties of SARS‐CoV‐2, and we therefore opted for an inclusive scope.

Comparison

We included studies with an active or passive (e.g., wait‐list control, baseline) comparator.

Outcomes

We included studies that reported as outcome either the acceptability, practicability, effectiveness, affordability, spill‐over effects, and/or equity of interventions, provided that these were measured at the individual level via self‐report or direct observation.

Study designs

We included primary research studies that used experimental (e.g., randomized controlled trial) or quasi‐experimental (e.g., pre‐ and post‐test) study designs, with individuals or clusters as the unit of randomization, providing that they were conducted under free‐living (as opposed to laboratory) conditions. In line with rapid review guidelines, we only included studies that were published in peer‐reviewed journals and written in English (Tricco et al., 2015; World Health Organization, 2017).

Search methods for identification of studies

Electronic searches

We searched Ovid MEDLINE and Scopus. Search terms for each behaviour (e.g., ‘hand hygiene’, ‘hand washing’, ‘face mask’) were piloted and refined to achieve balance between sensitivity and specificity (see Appendix S1).

Searching for other sources

Expertise within the review team and consultation with topic experts was used to identify additional articles of interest. We had specified the use of reference chaining in the review protocol; however, given the rapid focus of the review and large number of identified studies, reference chaining was not performed.

Data collection and analysis

Selection of studies

Two reviewers (OP and DS) independently screened (1) titles and abstracts and (2) full texts against the inclusion criteria. Discrepancies were discussed and resolved through consulting with a third reviewer (EC) if necessary.

Data extraction and management

A data extraction form was developed on the basis of the Behaviour Change Intervention Ontology (www.humanbehaviourchange.org) and Cochrane’s PICO ontology (https://linkeddata.cochrane.org/pico‐ontology). Ontologies are classification systems which enable researchers to specify entities (e.g., behaviours, interventions) and their inter‐relationships. The use of ontologies in systematic reviews can help ensure that a comprehensive set of entities are considered and defined in standardized ways, thus facilitating systematic knowledge synthesis (Norris, Finnerty, Hastings, Stokes, & Michie, 2019). We extracted data from relevant sections of published articles and available Appendix S1 on study design, intervention content (i.e., behaviour change techniques (BCTs), coded against the BCT Taxonomy v1 (Michie et al., 2013)), mode of delivery, population, setting, and mechanism(s) of action (Carey et al., 2019; Moore & Evans, 2017). As a validated taxonomy of mechanisms of action is, to our knowledge, not yet available, authors’ own definitions of mechanisms of action were extracted if they explicitly discussed how the selected intervention components/BCTs were expected to influence the target behaviour. Due to limited resources, we did not contact study authors for more detail on intervention descriptions. As an intervention may be effective but have negative spill‐over effects to other behaviours, or be impracticable and/or unacceptable to key stakeholders, we also deemed it important to extract data from relevant sections of published articles and available supplementary materials pertaining to the APEASE criteria (see Table 1) (Michie et al., 2014). Although criteria such as affordability or practicability are arguably closely tied to the context in which the intervention was/will be implemented, APEASE intends to capture higher‐order criteria against which to rate interventions, broadly applicable irrespective of the specific context. Therefore, authors’ own descriptions of, for example, acceptability or spill‐over effects (as opposed to reviewers’ ratings) were extracted. Spill‐over effects were broadly defined as any unintended consequences (positive or negative) reported by the authors, including but not limited to other behaviours that were changed by the intervention that it was not designed to target (e.g., teachers’ improved hand hygiene positively or negatively influencing children’s hand hygiene). Data were extracted by one reviewer (OP or DS). In the review protocol, we had specified that extracted data would be verified by a second reviewer to assess accuracy and completeness. However, given the large number of identified studies, a second reviewer (EC) verified 10% of studies.

Table 1.

APEASE criteria for evaluating intervention approaches or components (Michie et al., 2014)

Criterion To what extent …
Acceptability …is the intervention judged to be acceptable by all key stakeholders
Practicability …can the intervention be delivered as intended at the scale intended and in the context intended
Effectiveness …will the intervention deliver the desired outcome in the target population
Affordability …can the intervention be afforded within an acceptable budget
Spill‐over effects …is the intervention likely to have additional negative or positive consequences
Equity …is the intervention likely to increase or decrease inequalities in society

Quality appraisal

The methodological rigour of included evaluation reports was assessed by one reviewer (OP or DS) using Cochrane’s risk‐of‐bias tool (The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011). A second reviewer (EC) verified 10% of studies.

Stakeholder involvement

We solicited input from key stakeholders, including patient and public representatives recruited via panels convened by Public Health England (n = 282) and the University of East Anglia (n = 3), and UK policymakers and academic researchers contacted via a mailing list on the research objectives, target behaviours, and outcomes assessed. Feedback from patient and public representatives (n = 20) was incorporated into the review protocol; we did not receive any suggestions for improvement or clarification from the policymakers and academic researchers. The rapid review results will be disseminated to stakeholders via an infographic.

Data synthesis

A narrative (descriptive) synthesis was conducted for each of the personal protective behaviours. We had specified in the review protocol that meta‐analyses would be conducted if practicable and appropriate (i.e., >5 studies with homogeneous study designs and outcome variables). After inspection of study designs and outcome variables, however, we deemed it useful to conduct a meta‐analysis with k = 4 studies. Random‐effects meta‐analyses to estimate a pooled odds ratio (OR) or standardized mean difference (d) were conducted in RStudio v.1.2.5033 with the metafor package (Viechtbauer, 2010). Cohen’s conventions for small (d = .2), medium (d = .5), and large (d = .8) effects were used in the interpretation of the results (Cohen, 1988). In studies with more than two arms (e.g., three‐arm RCTs), we compared the ‘most active’ (i.e., the arm with the greatest number of intervention components) and the ‘most passive’ arms (i.e., the arm with the lowest number of intervention components, typically labelled the ‘control’ arm by study authors). Where studies reported more than one hand hygiene outcome (e.g., hand washing and hand sanitizer use), only the first reported outcome was included in the meta‐analysis, so as not to violate the assumption of independence (Cheung, 2019). Where studies did not report sufficient detail to calculate effect sizes, authors’ own description/interpretation of results were grouped into ‘positive’ effects (i.e., a significant difference between intervention and control groups, favouring the intervention group, was detected), ‘no difference’ (i.e., a significant difference between groups was not detected), ‘negative’ effects (i.e., a significant difference between groups, favouring the control group, was detected), or ‘indeterminate’ (i.e., differences between groups were not reported or could not be computed given the study design). To aid interpretation, for behaviours where a majority of positive or negative results were observed, overall results were categorized as either ‘positive’ or ‘negative’, respectively. If consistent results were not observed or could not be determined, overall results were categorized as ‘mixed’.

Results

Study selection

After removing duplicates, 5,595 records were identified, with 159 studies carried forward to the full text screening. Of the 39 studies included in the narrative evidence synthesis, 10 were included in meta‐analyses (see Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

PRISMA flow chart of included studies.

Study characteristics

Studies were conducted in the United States (13/39; 33%) (Aiello et al., 2010, 2012; Arbogast et al., 2016; Azman et al., 2013; Koep et al., 2016; Larson, Ferng, McLoughlin, Wang, & Morse, 2009; Larson et al., 2010; Mott et al., 2007; Sandora et al., 2005; Stebbins, Stark, & Vukotich, 2010; Stedman‐Smith et al., 2015; Updegraff, Emanuel, Gallagher, & Steinman, 2011; White, Kolble, Carlson, Lipson, & Dolan, 2003), China (5/39; 13%) (Chan, So, Wong, Lee, & Tiwari, 2007; Cowling et al., 2008, 2009; Liu et al., 2019; Or, Ching, & Chung, 2020), Germany (3/39; 13%) (Hübner, Hübner, Wodny, Kampf, & Kramer, 2010; Suess et al., 2011, 2012), Thailand (3/39; 13%) (Apisarnthanarak, Apisarnthanarak, Cheevakumjorn, & Mundy, 2009; Kaewchana et al., 2012; Simmerman et al., 2011), Australia (2/39; 5%) (MacIntyre et al., 2009; Roberts et al., 2000), Denmark (2/39; 5%) (Bundgaard et al., 2020; Nandrup‐Bus, 2009), Spain (2/39; 5%) (Azor‐Martinez et al., 2016, 2018), and the United Kingdom (2/39; 5%) (Little et al., 2015; Yardley, Miller, Schlotz, & Little, 2011), with one study each in Australia/Saudi Arabia (Barasheed et al., 2014), Bangladesh (Ram et al., 2015), Costa Rica (Reyes Fernández, Lippke, Knoll, Moya, & Schwarzer, 2015), Finland (Savolainen‐Kopra et al., 2012), France (Canini et al., 2010), the Netherlands (Zomer et al., 2016), and Turkey (Öncü et al., 2019) (see Table 2). Studies had a median of 419 participants (range: 96–20,066).

Table 2.

Characteristics of included studies

Lead author (year) Country Respiratory virus targeted Target behaviour(s) Study design Population Sample size Mean age (SD) % Female % Post‐16 educational qualifications Setting Recruitment strategy
(1) Aiello (2010) United States Pandemic influenza A (H1N1) Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT University students in residence halls 1,297 (face mask + hand hygiene = 367; face mask = 378; control = 552) 18.7 (0.8) 66% 100% University residence halls Not reported
(2) Aiello (2012) United States Influenza A or B Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT University students in residence halls 1178 (face mask + hand hygiene = 362; face mask = 420; control = 396) 19.0 (0.9) 55% 100% University residence halls Not reported
(3) Apisarnthanarak (2009) Thailand Influenza Hand hygiene Single arm, pre‐ and post‐intervention study Preschool children 240 5.0 (1.7) 49% 0% Private kindergarten Not reported
(4) Arbogast (2016) United States Respiratory syncytial virus, adenovirus, influenza Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT Office workers 1,386 (intervention = 604; control = 782) 47.0 (0.4) 78% Not reported Office buildings E‐mail sent to employees
(5) Azman (2013) United States Influenza A or B Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT Households 3,360 Not reported Not reported 0% Schools Not reported
(6) Azor‐Martinez (2016) Spain Influenza Hand hygiene Two‐arm RCT School children 1,341 8.0 (2.3) 68% 0% Schools Not reported
(7) Azor‐Martinez (2018) Spain Respiratory viral infections Hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT Households with a child at a daycare centre 911 children Unclear Unclear Unclear Daycare centres Not reported
(8) Barasheed (2014) Australia/Saudi Arabia Rhinovirus, influenza, parainfluenza Face mask use Two‐arm, pilot RCT Hajj pilgrims 164 Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Study brochures distributed in mosques, Islamic centres and pre‐travel seminars, and in hotels in Mecca
(9) Bundgaard (2020) Denmark SARS‐CoV‐2 Face mask use Two‐arm RCT Community‐dwelling adults 6,304 47.2 (13.5) 64% Not reported Participants’ own homes Media advertisements and through contacting private companies and public organizations
(10) Canini (2010) France Influenza Face mask use Two‐arm, cluster RCT Households in three regions of France with an index patient 105 26.5 (16.0) Not reported Not reported GP offices GPs
(11) Chan (2007) China SARS Catching droplets in tissues and hand hygiene Single arm, pre‐ and post‐intervention study Registered members of a government subsidized social service centre 122 Not reported 63% 20% Participants’ own homes Telephone calls
(12) Cowling (2008) China Influenza A or B Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT Households with an index patient 198 Not reported 56% Not reported Participants’ own homes Outpatient clinics
(13) Cowling (2009) China Influenza A or B Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT Households with an index patient 407 index patients Not reported 51% Not reported Participants’ own homes Outpatient clinics
(14) Hübner (2010) Germany Common cold, influenza Hand hygiene Two‐arm RCT Office workers 134 44.6 (–) 86% Not reported Office buildings E‐mails
(15) Kaewchana (2012) Thailand Influenza Hand hygiene Two‐arm RCT Households with an index paediatric patient 275 (frequency assessment) + 330 (quality assessment) households 34.7 (13.8) 58% Not reported Participants’ own homes Not reported
(16) Koep (2016) United States Influenza Hand hygiene Two‐arm, non‐randomized cohort study School children 260 Not reported 46% 0% Schools Not reported
(17) Larson (2009) United States Rhinovirus, coronavirus, parainfluenza virus, respiratory syncytial virus, adenovirus, influenza, enterovirus, etc. Hand hygiene Single arm, pre‐ and post‐intervention study Households 422 households Not reported Not reported 60% Participants’ own homes Neighbourhood snowballing techniques (e.g., churches, schools, clinics)
(18) Larson (2010) United States Influenza, respiratory syncytial virus, parainfluenza, enterovirus, rhinovirus, adenovirus, metapneumovirus Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm RCT Households 509 Not reported 52% 54% Participants’ own homes Neighbourhood snowballing techniques (e.g., churches, schools, clinics)
(19) Little (2015) United Kingdom Influenza Hand hygiene Two‐arm RCT Community‐dwelling adults 20,066 56.6 (13.6) 56% Not reported Online Mailed invitations through GP surgeries
(20) Liu (2019) China Respiratory viral infections Hand hygiene Single arm, pre‐ and post‐intervention study Kindergarten teachers 361 29.0 (8.7) 95% Not reported Schools Not reported
(21) MacIntyre (2009) Australia Influenza, respiratory syncytial virus, adenovirus, parainfluenza virus, coronavirus, metapneumovirus Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT Households with an index paediatric patient 145 families, 290 caregivers Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Paediatric clinic waiting rooms
(22) Mott (2007) United States Respiratory illnesses (e.g., flu, colds) Hand hygiene Three‐arm, non‐randomized cohort study Military trainees 2,728 20.2 (–) 0% Unclear US army training centre Not reported
(23) Nandrup‐Bus (2020) Denmark Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT School children 652 Not reported 51% 0% Schools Parents were sent written study information
(24) Or (2020) China Influenza Hand hygiene Single arm, pre‐ and post‐intervention study Households with a child in kindergarten 58 parents; 60 children Not reported Parents = 93%; children = 40% Not reported Not reported Letters sent to principals of 15 kindergartens to invite parents and children to participate
(25) Ram (2015) Bangladesh Influenza Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT Households 377 index cases; 384 household compounds 121.2 months (181.7) 40% Not reported Participants’ own homes Outpatient clinics
(26) Reyes Fernández (2015) Costa Rica Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT University students 242 21.0 (3.9) 61% 100% University classrooms Not reported
(27) Roberts (2000) Australia Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT Preschool children and daycare staff 232 childcare centres; 558 children Not reported Not reported 0% Childcare centres Directors of daycare centres were invited
(28) Sandora (2005) United States Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT Households with a child at a daycare centre 292 families 36.7 (9.5) Not reported 91% Participants’ own homes Directors of daycare centres were invited. A recruitment letter was sent to parents.
(29) Savolainen‐Kopra (2012) Finland Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT Office workers 683 Not reported Not reported Not reported Office buildings E‐mails
(30) Simmerman (2011) Thailand Influenza Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm RCT Households 442 index cases; 1,147 household contacts Not reported 59% Not reported Participants’ own homes Outpatient clinics
(31) Stebbins (2010) United States Influenza and related respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT School children and teachers 151 teachers Not reported Not reported Not reported Schools Not reported
(32) Stedman‐Smith (2015) United States Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT Office workers 324 Not reported 84% 100% Office buildings E‐mails
(33) Suess (2011) Germany Pandemic influenza A (H1N1) Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT Households with an index patient during the H1N1 pandemic 147 Index cases = 7.9 (3.3); household contacts = 30.0 (14.2) 52% Not reported Participants’ own homes Outpatient clinics
(34) Suess (2012) Germany Pandemic influenza A (H1N1) Face mask use and hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT Households with an index patient during the H1N1 pandemic 302 Not reported Not reported Not reported Participants’ own homes Outpatient clinics
(35) Updegraff (2011) United States Pandemic influenza A (H1N1) Hand hygiene Randomized cross‐over study University students and staff 65 units Not reported Not reported Not reported Public areas of a university No active recruitment
(36) White (2003) United States Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cohort studya University students 430 18.3 (0.7) 62% 0% University residence halls Not reported
(37) Yardley (2011) United Kingdom Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, pilot RCT Community‐dwelling adults 517 49.8 (11.4) 64% Not reported Online Mailed invitations from GP surgeries
(38) Zomer (2016) The Netherlands Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Two‐arm, cluster RCT Households with a child at a daycare centre 71 centres Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported
(39) Öncü (2019) Turkey Respiratory infections Hand hygiene Three‐arm, cluster RCT School children 96 9.2 (1.0) 54% 0% School laboratory Not reported

aUnclear if randomized.

The majority of studies (20/39; 51%) targeted multiple respiratory infections, including (but not limited to) influenza, respiratory syncytial virus, and adenovirus (Arbogast et al., 2016; Azor‐Martinez et al., 2018; Barasheed et al., 2014; Hübner, Hübner, Wodny, Kampf, & Kramer, 2010; Larson et al., 2009; Larson et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2019; MacIntyre et al., 2009; Mott et al., 2007, p. 20; Nandrup‐Bus, 2020; Öncü et al., 2019; Reyes Fernández, Lippke, Knoll, Moya, & Schwarzer, 2015; Roberts et al., 2000; Sandora et al., 2005; Savolainen‐Kopra et al., 2012; Stebbins et al., 2010; Stedman‐Smith et al., 2015, p.; White et al., 2003; Yardley et al., 2011; Zomer et al., 2016), with the remaining studies targeting influenza (13/39; 33%) (Aiello et al., 2012; Apisarnthanarak et al., 2009; Azman et al., 2013; Azor‐Martinez et al., 2016; Canini et al., 2010; Cowling et al., 2008, 2009; Kaewchana et al., 2012; Koep et al., 2016; Little et al., 2015; Or et al., 2020; Ram et al., 2015; Simmerman et al., 2011), pandemic influenza A (H1N1) (4/39; 10%) (Aiello et al., 2010; Suess et al., 2011, 2012; Updegraff et al., 2011), SARS (1/39; 3%) (Chan et al., 2007), and SARS‐CoV‐2 (1/39; 3%) (Bundgaard et al., 2020) (see Table 2).

The majority of studies (28/39; 72%) targeted hand hygiene, with the remaining studies targeting a combination of hand hygiene and/or face mask use (9/39; 23%) (Aiello et al., 2010, 2012; Cowling et al., 2008, 2009; Larson et al., 2010; MacIntyre et al., 2009; Simmerman et al., 2011; Suess et al., 2011, 2012), face mask use only (3/39; 8%) (Barasheed et al., 2014; Bundgaard et al., 2020; Canini et al., 2010), or a combination of catching of droplets in tissues and hand hygiene (1/39; 3%) (Chan et al., 2007) (see Table 2). Interventions were delivered in participants’ own homes (12/39; 31%), nurseries/schools (10/39; 26%), university residence halls/public areas (5/39; 13%), offices (4/39; 10%), online (2/39; 5%), outpatient clinics (1/39; 3%), or an army training centre (1/39; 3%). Four studies did not state the setting for intervention delivery. Studies targeted children and adult household members (16/39; 41%), pre‐ or school children and/or teachers (8/39; 21%), university staff and/or students (5/39; 13%), office workers (4/39; 10%), community‐dwelling adults (3/39; 8%), Hajj pilgrims (1/39; 3%), military trainees (1/39; 3%), or adult members of a social service centre (1/39; 3%).

Study designs used were two‐arm, cluster RCTs (11/39; 28%), three‐arm, cluster RCTs (10/39; 26%), two‐arm RCTs (5/39; 13%), single‐arm, pre‐ and post‐intervention studies (5/39; 13%), two‐arm, non‐randomized cohort studies (2/39; 5%), three‐arm RCTs (2/39; 5%), two‐arm, pilot RCTs (1/39; 3%), or randomized, cross‐over studies (1/30; 3%).

Intervention characteristics

Intervention durations ranged from one day (i.e., one‐off interventions) to 3 years (see Table 3). In studies using a two‐ or three‐arm design (33/39; 85%), comparators included no intervention/usual care (19/33; 58%), educational materials (9/33; 27%), the provision of soap/hand sanitizer (3/33; 9%), or a combination of educational materials and soap/hand sanitizer (2/33; 6%). Interventions were delivered via face‐to‐face sessions (23/39; 59%), written materials (including books and newsletters) (14/39; 36%), posters/bulletin boards/cue cards (8/39; 21%), cartoons/games (4/39; 10%), videos (4/39; 10%), telephone (3/39; 8%), and/or websites (2/39; 5%). Five studies did not clearly report on the mode of intervention delivery. Where reported, participants received a flat payment for study completion (Aiello et al., 2010; Cowling et al., 2008, 2009; Simmerman et al., 2011; Suess et al., 2011, 2012; White et al., 2003), payment per survey completed (Stebbins et al., 2010), points for study completion (Arbogast et al., 2016), or points per survey completed (Stedman‐Smith et al., 2015).

Table 3.

Characteristics of interventions to change personal protective behaviours

Lead author (year) Comparator Intervention Intervention duration BCTs (comparator arm) BCTs (intervention arm) Intervention mode of delivery Theoretical mechanism(s) of action of the intervention Incentive structure for study participation
(1) Aiello (2010) Educational materials The face mask group received face masks; written instructions on how to use, store, and safely discard masks. The face mask + hand hygiene group also received hand sanitizer 6 weeks 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 6.1. Demonstration of the behaviour; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Video link, written materials Not reported Those with influenza‐like illness were offered $25 for providing a throat specimen
(2) Aiello (2012) Educational materials The face mask group received face masks; written instructions on how to use, store, and safely discard masks. The face mask + hand hygiene group also received hand sanitizer 6 weeks 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Written materials Not reported Not reported
(3) Apisarnthanarak (2009) NA Children, teachers and parents received hand hygiene (including sanitizer) education via cartoons and workshops. A single dispenser of alcohol‐based hand rub was placed in each nursery room 3 years NA 2.2 Feedback on behaviour; 2.7 Feedback on outcomes of behaviour; 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 6.1. Demonstration of the behaviour; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face workshop, cartoons, written materials Not reported Not reported
(4) Arbogast (2016) An educational video about hand hygiene; soap and hand sanitizer provided in toilets Same as the control group in addition to hand sanitizers being provided in different areas of the office building 13.5 months 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 6.1. Demonstration of the behaviour; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Videos Not reported 25 ‘wellness points’ were offered to employees who completed both baseline and post‐study survey
(5) Azman (2013) Hand sanitizer Same as control group in addition to a live demonstration of hand washing behaviour. Information about hand hygiene was sent home Not reported 12.5. Adding objects to the environment 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 6.1. Demonstration of the behaviour; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face workshop, written materials Not reported Not reported
(6) Azor‐Martinez (2016) No intervention Handwashing workshop; hand hygiene practices were periodically reinforced in the classroom; younger children were supervised during hand hygiene procedures; provision of hand sanitizer 8 months NA 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 6.1. Demonstration of the behaviour; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face workshop Not reported Not reported
(7) Azor‐Martinez (2018) No intervention Hand hygiene workshop with instruction on how to correctly perform the behaviour; use of stories, songs, posters; provision of hand sanitizer in one group and liquid soap in the other group; written materials on hand hygiene 8 months NA 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 6.1. Demonstration of the behaviour; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face workshop, written materials Not reported Not reported
(8) Barasheed (2014) Hygiene information In addition to the general hygiene information, face masks and written and verbal instructions on how to use these were provided 5 days Not reported 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face instructions, written materials Not reported Not reported
(9) Bundgaard (2020) Weekly e‐mails encouraging participants to follow current COVID‐19 recommendations Instructions to wear a mask when outside the home during the next month; provision of 50 three‐layer, disposable, surgical face masks with ear loops 1 month NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Written materials Not reported Not reported
(10) Canini (2010) No intervention Provision of face masks and demonstration of how to use them 3 weeks NA 6.1. Demonstration of the behaviour; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face instructions Not reported Not reported
(11) Chan (2007) NA Health education 7 days NA Not reported Telephone calls with trained nursing students Not reported Not reported
(12) Cowling (2008) No intervention (education about healthy diet and lifestyle) The face mask group received face masks, information about the efficacy of masks and instruction on how to use and safely dispose of masks; the hand hygiene group received hand sanitizer, liquid soap, information about the efficacy of hand hygiene and demonstration of hand hygiene behaviour 9 days NA 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1. Information about health consequences; 6.1. Demonstration of the behaviour; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment; Not reported Not reported At the final home visit, households were reimbursed for their participation with a supermarket voucher worth approximately US $20
(13) Cowling (2009) No intervention (education about healthy diet and lifestyle) Hand hygiene; surgical face masks plus hand hygiene 6 days NA 5.1. Information about health consequences; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Not reported Not reported At the final home visit, households were reimbursed for their participation with a supermarket voucher worth approximately US $25
(14) Hübner (2010) No intervention Provision of hand sanitizer and instruction on how and when to use it at work 12 months NA 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Not reported Not reported Not reported
(15) Kaewchana (2012) 30‐minute routine health education on influenza infection, nutrition, physical activity, and smoking cessation 30‐minute intensive hand washing education; individual training on hand washing; provision of liquid soap; self‐monitoring diary; written materials on hand washing techniques 3 months 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1. Information about health consequences 2.3. Self‐monitoring of behaviour; 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1. Information about health consequences; 5.3. Information about social and environmental consequences; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face workshop Not reported Not reported
(16) Koep (2016) No intervention Educational intervention with information about microorganisms; provision of liquid soap and hand sanitizer in toilets and classrooms Unclear NA 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1. Information about health consequences; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face with trained teachers Not reported Not reported
(17) Larson (2009) NA Educational intervention focused on infection control; question and answer fact sheets; two groups of households were randomized to receive hand sanitizer, face masks, or both 2 to 20 months NA 4.1. Instruction of how to perform the behaviour; 5.1. Information about health consequences; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face with trained researchers Not reported Not reported
(18) Larson (2010) Educational materials on infection control The hand sanitizer group received education plus hand sanitizer; the hand sanitizer and face mask group received the same interventions plus face masks 19 months Not reported 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1. Information about health consequences; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal; 12.5. Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face with trained researchers Not reported Not reported
(19) Little (2015) No intervention Four weekly web‐based sessions with new content focused on the role of hand washing, setting up a plan to wash hands, reinforcement of helpful attitudes and norms, addressing negative beliefs, tailored feedback and prompts to login to the website 4 months NA 1.4. Action planning; 2.2. Feedback on behaviour; 4.1. Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 7.1 Prompts/cues; 8.1. Behaviour practice/rehearsal Website Not reported Not reported
(20) Liu (2019) NA Hand hygiene training and information booklet; provision of soap, towels, posters, stickers, books, memory games, and diplomas 6 months NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 10.4 Social reward; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face sessions, posters, stickers, books Knowledge, perceived susceptibility, perceived severity, perceived behavioural control Not reported
(21) MacIntyre (2009) No intervention Information about infection control; provision of either P2 or surgical face masks Unclear NA 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Not reported Not reported Not reported
(22) Mott (2007) Hand sanitizer and instructions to wash or sanitize hands after key events (e.g., coughing, sneezing) In the PI group, hand sanitizer dispensers were installed throughout the training environment; provision of personal hand sanitizer bottles; posters were placed in training facilities to encourage hand hygiene and foster a sense of pride in staying healthy; instruction on hand hygiene; and weekly reminders by drill sergeants to carry, use, and refill hand sanitizer bottles. The SI group received personal hand sanitizer bottles and instruction on hand hygiene only Not reported 12.5 Adding objects to the environment 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 9.1 Credible source; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face sessions, posters Not reported Not reported
(23) Nandrup‐Bus (2020) NA Children were required to wash their hands three times per day; training on hand washing and infection control was provided; posters with step‐by‐step hand washing instructions placed by wash basins; parents were asked to remind children to wash their hands before the first lesson each day 3 months NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1 Information about health consequences Face‐to‐face sessions, posters Not reported Not reported
(24) Or (2020) No intervention Children attended 4 weekly sessions with information about infection control and hand hygiene techniques; parents attended a separate session with similar content 4 weeks NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1 Information about health consequences Face‐to‐face sessions with an infection control nurse Knowledge, skills Not reported
(25) Ram (2015) No intervention Household compounds were provided with a hand washing station (e.g., water container with a tap, soap); information on infection control and skills training; cue cards placed in a common area in compound courtyards Tailored; daily intervention visits until 10 days following the resolution of the index case patient's symptoms NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1 Information about health consequences; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face sessions, cue cards Not reported Not reported
(26) Reyes Fernández (2015) No intervention Instructions on how and when to clean hands and a planning task to help students set action and coping plans One‐off NA 1.2 Problem solving; 1.4 Action planning; 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour A face‐to‐face session with research assistants, pamphlets Action control, coping planning Not reported
(27) Roberts (2000) No intervention Staff received training in hand washing and were asked to teach the techniques to the children in their care via songs about hand washing to the melodies of nursery rhymes; training was reinforced with fortnightly visits and newsletters Not reported NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 6.1 Demonstration of the behaviour Face‐to‐face training sessions, newsletters Not reported Not reported
(28) Sandora (2005) No intervention (educational materials about healthy eating; participants were asked not to use hand sanitizer during the study period) Provision of alcohol‐based hand sanitizer; educational materials (e.g., fact sheets, games, toys) about hand hygiene 5 months NA 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Fact sheets, games, toys Not reported Not reported
(29) Savolainen‐Kopra (2012) No intervention Both groups received information on infection control. In the soap and water group, toilets were equipped with liquid hand soap. In the hand sanitizer arm, toilets were equipped with both liquid hand soap and alcohol‐based hand rub 18 months NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1 Information about health consequences; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Not reported Not reported Not reported
(30) Simmerman (2011) No intervention (nutritional, physical activity, and smoking cessation education) The hand washing group received education, instruction on hand washing techniques and a hand washing kit with liquid hand soap. The hand washing + face mask group received the same interventions as the hand washing group in addition to paper surgical face masks, training on how to use them appropriately and information about benefits of use 7 days NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1 Information about health consequences; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face sessions with trained study nurses Not reported Households were compensated with approximately US $60 in Thai baht
(31) Stebbins (2010) No intervention Students and staff received training in hand hygiene behaviours; schools placed and maintained supplies of alcohol‐based hand sanitizer in all classrooms and common areas; parents and guardians received educational materials on hand hygiene and home isolation practices Not reported NA 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1 Information about health consequences; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face sessions, videos Not reported Teachers were offered a $5 gift card for completion of each survey
(32) Stedman‐Smith (2015) A brief training video to promote more effective communication with healthcare providers, branded key chains, brochures and posters with information about the programme A brief training video on infection control and demonstration of effective hand washing/gel techniques; hand sanitizer and motivational/educational hand hygiene posters were placed in break rooms, kitchens, and conference rooms Not reported Not reported 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Video, posters Not reported

Points towards health promotion items within an employee wellness programme were provided for each survey completed

(33) Suess (2011) Educational materials about infection control and recommendation to sleep in a different room than the index patient Participants in the mask + hand hygiene and mask groups were provided with surgical face masks with ear loops and written information on their correct use; participants in the mask + hand hygiene group were provided with alcohol‐based hand rub and instructions on correct use 8 days Not reported 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Written materials, telephone, face‐to‐face visits by trained study personnel Not reported €150 for study participation
(34) Suess (2012) Educational materials about infection control and recommendation to sleep in a different room than the index patient Participants in the mask + hand hygiene and mask groups were provided with surgical face masks with ear loops and written information on their correct use; participants in the mask + hand hygiene group were provided with alcohol‐based hand rub and instructions on correct use 8 days Not reported 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 6.1 Demonstration of the behaviour; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Written materials, telephone, face‐to‐face visits by trained study personnel Not reported €150 for study participation
(35) Updegraff (2011) Hand sanitizer Four signs were placed above hand sanitizer units. The perceived susceptibility headline read ‘Germs are out to get you. Get them first!’; the social norms headline read ‘Everybody is doing it. Are you?’; the gain‐framed headline read ‘Stay healthy this season. Sanitize your hands’; and the loss‐framed headline read ‘H1N1. Getting it is as easy as passing me by’. Each sign contained a ‘fact box’ with more detailed information reinforcing the theme 12 weeks 12.5 Adding objects to the environment 5.1 Information about health consequences; 5.2 Information about social and environmental consequences; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Foam boards Perceived susceptibility, social norms, and attitudes towards the behaviour Not reported
(36) White (2003) No intervention Alcohol‐gel dispensers were installed in every room, washroom, and dining hall in residence halls; a hand washing message campaign was implemented with bulletin boards and weekly messages to encourage hand washing 10 weeks NA 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Bulletin boards Not reported Cash incentives totalling a maximum of $65
(37) Yardley (2011) No intervention Four weekly web‐based sessions with information about the medical team behind the advice (to enhance credibility), infection control, expert recommendations for hand washing frequency and technique, and instructions for picking up a free supply of hand gel from one’s local GP practice; a hand washing plan to promote intention formation with situational cueing; tailored feedback to help participants improve their plan; reinforcement of positive attitudes and norms; addressing common negative beliefs; e‐mail prompts to login to the website 4 weeks NA 1.4 Action planning; 2.2 Feedback on behaviour; 5.1 Information about health consequences; 7.1 Prompts/cues; 9.1 Credible source; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Website Intention to wash hands, attitude, subjective norms, perceived behavioural control, perceived risk of infection Not reported
(38) Zomer (2016) Usual care Four components: (1) free hand hygiene products with refills for 6 months (e.g., soap, hand sanitizer); (2) a handwashing exercise with 'UV Glow Cream' and an information booklet with the training content; (3) two team training sessions focused on goal setting and identifying hand hygiene improvement activities; and (4) posters and stickers placed in daycare centres acting as reminders to practise hand hygiene 6 months Not reported 1.1 Goal setting (behaviour); 7.1 Prompts/cues; 8.1 Behavioural practice/rehearsal; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face sessions, posters, stickers, booklets Not reported Not reported
(39) Öncü (2019) Hand gel and 'photo shoots' before and after handwashing The first group received the same intervention as the control group plus information about getting rid of microbes on hands if washing hands 'properly'; the second group received the same interventions as the first plus a 30‐minute hand hygiene educational and training session on types of microbes and diseases caused by microbes, in addition to WHO's 9‐stage handwashing programme 4 weeks 12.5 Adding objects to the environment 4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour; 5.1 Information about health consequences; 8.1 Behavioural practice/rehearsal; 12.5 Adding objects to the environment Face‐to‐face session Not reported Not reported

NA = not applicable.

We coded 15 different BCTs across intervention descriptions (see Figure 3); however, details on intervention content were typically lacking. Interventions included a median of three BCTs (range: 0 to 6). The most frequently coded BCTs were ‘12.5 Adding objects to the environment’ (17/39; 44%), ‘4.1 Instruction on how to perform the behaviour’ (16/39; 41%), and ‘5.1 Information about health consequences’ (10/39; 26%). Few studies reported targeting a specific mechanism of action. Where reported, interventions were designed to target intentions (Yardley et al., 2011), attitudes (Updegraff et al., 2011; Yardley et al., 2011), subjective norms (Updegraff et al., 2011; Yardley et al., 2011), perceived behavioural control (Liu et al., 2019; Yardley et al., 2011), perceived risk of infection/disease severity (Liu et al., 2019; Updegraff et al., 2011; Yardley et al., 2011), action control (Reyes Fernández et al., 2015), coping planning (Reyes Fernández et al., 2015), knowledge (Liu et al., 2019; Or et al., 2020), or skills (Or et al., 2020).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Frequencies of behaviour change techniques (BCTs) coded in published intervention descriptions.

Acceptability

Fourteen studies reported on the acceptability of interventions. Indicators assessed included mask comfort (Aiello et al., 2012; Barasheed et al., 2014; Canini et al., 2010; MacIntyre et al., 2009; Suess et al., 2011, 2012), skin problems/irritation (Azor‐Martinez et al., 2016, 2018; Little et al., 2015; Nandrup‐Bus, 2009; Sandora et al., 2005), adverse events (Cowling et al., 2008), liking/positive impressions (Arbogast et al., 2016), and ease of understanding (Updegraff et al., 2011), with a small number of participants experiencing discomfort or irritation in the majority of studies that reported these outcomes (see Table 4).

Table 4.

APEASE criteria, reach, and engagement

Lead author (year) Acceptability Practicability Affordability Spill‐over effects Equity Reach Engagement How primary outcome was assessed Effectiveness
(1) Aiello (2010) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 7/15 residence halls were included in the study Not reported Self‐report On average, the mask only group wore masks 3.9 hrs per day (SD = 3.3) vs. 3.0 hr per day (SD = 2.4) in the mask and hand hygiene group (‐). Log mask hours were significantly higher in the mask only group compared with the mask and hand hygiene group at each time point except for week 4 (p < .05). The mask only group washed their hands 8.2 times per day (SD = 9.0) vs. 6.1 times per day (SD = 4.8) in the mask and hand hygiene group and 8.8 times per day (SD = 9.3) in the control group (‐). On the log scale, the mask and hand hygiene group washed their hands significantly fewer times per day than the control group from weeks 2 through 4. The mask only group used hand sanitizer 2.3 times per day (SD = 3.5) vs. 5.2 times per day (SD = 5.1) in the mask and hand hygiene group and 2.0 times per day (SD = 3.9) in the control group (+). On the log scale, participants in the mask and hand hygiene group reported a significantly greater use of hand sanitizer compared to the mask only and control groups at each week (p < .0001). There were no significant differences between the mask only group and control group in the frequency of hand sanitizer use
(2) Aiello (2012) On average, participants in the two intervention groups rated mask comfort as 4.7/10 (SD = 0.2) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 5/15 residence halls were included in the study Not reported Self‐report and observation of mask wearing by trained staff Participants in the face mask and hand hygiene group and the face mask only group wore masks on average 5.1 hrs per day (SD = 2.2) and 5.0 hrs per day (SD = 2.2), respectively (p > .05) (/). The face mask and hand hygiene group and the face mask only group reported an average use of hand sanitizer 4.5 times per day (SD = 4.1) and 1.3 times per day (SD = 1.8), respectively (p < .05). This compares with 1.5 times per day (SD = 2.3) in the control group (+). The face mask and hand hygiene group and face mask only group washed their hands 5.2 (SD = 3.3) and 5.5 (SD = 3.3) times per day, respectively. The control group washed their hands an average of 5.8 times (SD = 5.0) per day (p > .05) (/)
(3) Apisarnthanarak (2009) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Observation of hand hygiene and cough etiquette with a Web camera 1 hr, twice weekly The observed rate of hand hygiene compliance increased from 21% (50/240) in period 1 to 71% (170/240) in period 2 (p < .001) and to 69% (166/240) in period 3 (p < .001) (+)
(4) Arbogast (2016) Employees in the intervention group were significantly more likely than those in the control group to have a positive impression of the programme because of the presence of alcohol‐based hand sanitizers in the workplace (80 vs. 69%, p < .001). 88% of employees in the intervention group reported liking the products provided Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 1,386/1,609 employees agreed to participate Not reported Self‐report Self‐reported hand washing frequency improved significantly over time in the intervention group (p < .05), as did self‐reported hand sanitizer use for every activity assessed, including before eating, after sneezing, coughing, handling money, using the restroom, returning to their desk, and interacting with others who may be sick (p < .05) (+)
(5) Azman (2013) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Age and ethnicity were significant risk factors for self‐reported influenza‐like illness (p < .05) Not reported Not reported Self‐report Not reported
(6) Azor‐Martinez (2016) One child showed worsening of existing atopic dermatitis due to hand sanitizer gel use and was excluded from the study Not reported Not reported Not reported Predictors of a lower rate of absenteeism due to respiratory illness included older age, higher parental income, and correct handwashing technique (p < .05) 1,616/1,640 children were randomized Not reported Self‐report Not reported
(7) Azor‐Martinez (2018) One child showed worsening of existing localized atopic dermatitis due to hand sanitizer gel use and was excluded from the study Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 25/52 daycare centres contacted were randomized Not reported Self‐report Not reported
(8) Barasheed (2014) The most commonly reported reason for not wearing face masks was discomfort (15% of participants) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 164/4200 pilgrims took part Not reported Self‐report Compliance with face mask use was 76% (56/75) in the intervention group and 12% (11/89) in the control group (p < .001) (+)
(9) Bundgaard (2020) Not reported Not reported The authors note that costs and availability may reduce the efficacy of face masks to prevent SARS‐CoV‐2 infection Not reported Not reported 17,258/6304 of those responding to recruitment adverts were randomized Not reported Self‐report 46% of participants in the intervention arm wore the mask as recommended, 47% predominantly as recommended, and 7% not as recommended. Participants used an average of 1.7 masks per weekday and 1.3 per weekend day (@)
(10) Canini (2010) 75% of participants in the intervention arm reported discomfort with mask use. The three main causes of discomfort were warmth (45%), respiratory difficulties (33%), and humidity (33%). Children wearing child size face masks reported feeling pain more frequently than those wearing adult face masks (p = .036) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 95/105 randomized households completed the study Self‐report Index patients in intervention households reported wearing a total of 11.0 (SD = 7.2) masks during 4.0 (SD = 1.6) days with an average use of 2.5 (SD = 1.3) masks per day and a duration of use of 3.7 (SD = 2.7) hours per day (@)
(11) Chan (2007) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 182/295 registered members were successfully contacted; 122/182 took part Not reported Self‐report Significant improvements were observed with regards to washing hands after sneezing/coughing (M before = 1.26, SD = 0.44; M after = 1.65, SD = 0.89), washing hands with liquid soap (M before = 1.14, SD = 0.35; M after = 1.53, SD = 0.82), and wearing masks in public (M before = 1.31, SD = 0.46; M after = 2.15, SD = 1.12) (all p < .0001) (+)
(12) Cowling (2008) There were no reported adverse events requiring medical attention Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 198/944 index patients were randomized Not reported Self‐report In the face mask group, 45% (10/22) of index cases reported wearing a mask often or always, compared with 30% (22/74) and 28% (9/32) in the control and hand hygiene groups, respectively (+). In the face mask group, 21% (14/65) of household contacts reported wearing a mask often or always, compared with 1% (2/213) and 4% (4/92) in the control and hand hygiene groups, respectively. Index cases in the face mask group used a median of 12 masks (IQR = 6–18) whereas household contacts used a median of six masks (IQR = 1–20). 63% (41%) of index cases (household contacts) in the hand hygiene group reported washing their hands often or always after sneezing, coughing or cleaning their nose, compared with 63% (47%) and 31% (27%) in the face mask and control groups, respectively (+). In the hand hygiene group, households used a median of 56 g (IQR = 27–93) of alcohol from the automatic sanitizer and a median of 88 g (IQR = 63–149) of liquid hand soap over the course of the study
(13) Cowling (2009) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 407/2,750 index patients and their households were randomized Not reported Self‐report The proportion index cases wearing face masks was 15% in the control group (14/91); 31% in the hand hygiene group (26/85); and 49% (41/83) in the face mask and hand hygiene group (+). The proportion household contacts wearing face masks was 7% (20/279) in the control group; 5% (13/257) in the hand hygiene group; and 26% (67/258) in the face mask and hand hygiene group. The median (IQR) use of liquid soap in the hand hygiene group was 77.6 g (42.4–162.6) and 78.9 (35.2–114.2) in the face mask and hand hygiene groups. The median (IQR) hand rub used in the hand hygiene group was 3.2 g (1.1–9.7) in index cases and 1.5 g (0.3–5.3) in contacts. This compared with 1.6 g (0.7–5.1) in index cases and 1.5 g (0.3–3.8) in contacts in the face mask and hand hygiene group (/)
(14) Hübner (2010) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 134/850 participants were randomized Not reported Self‐report The mean hand disinfection frequency was >5 times daily in 19%, 3–5 times daily in 60%, and 1–2 times daily in 21% (@)
(15) Kaewchana (2012) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Self‐report On day 7, the control and intervention groups reported 3.9 (SD = 2.4) and 5.7 (SD = 3.4) hand washing episodes/day, respectively (p < .001) (+). The percentage of participants who used soap increased from 34% (53/158) to 88% (139/158) (p < .001)
(16) Koep (2016) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Self‐report and objective measure of soap and sanitizer use No significant differences in hand soap compliance were observed across groups (estimates not reported) (/). Hand sanitizer use increased slightly in the intervention school from 0.04 to 0.06 times per student per day in grades 3 and 4 (p > .05)
(17) Larson (2009) Not reported Not reported Not reported Hand sanitizer use increased but hand washing with soap decreased Respondents with college degrees had higher knowledge scores than the other groups, adjusting for baseline scores (p = .04) Not reported Not reported Self‐report (questionnaire administered by trained researchers) and objective measures of hand sanitizer and mask use A significantly greater proportion of participants reported using hand sanitizer at the end of the study (282/422) compared with baseline (6/422) (+), but a lower proportion reported using antibacterial soap at the end of the study (105/422) compared with baseline (191/422) (‐)
(18) Larson (2010) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 617/672 households that expressed interest in participation met eligibility criteria; 509/617 completed the initial home visit Not reported Self‐report (questionnaire administered by trained researchers) and objective measures of hand sanitizer and mask use Participants in the hand sanitizer group used a mean of 12.1 ounces/month and those in the hand sanitizer and face mask group used a mean of 11.6 ounces/month (p = .36) (/). Half of the households with a case of infection reported using masks within 48 hrs of symptom onset. Those who used masks reported a mean of two masks/day/episode (range: 0–9) (@)
(19) Little (2015) Minor self‐reported skin irritation increased among those who did not report problems at baseline. Among individuals who had a (skin complaint at baseline, reported skin complaints did not significantly increase over time) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 20,066/80,4897 who received a mailed invitation were randomized Not reported Self‐report Not reported
(20) Liu (2019) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 12/213 kindergarten clusters received the intervention Not reported Self‐report The average self‐reported compliance with hand hygiene guidelines was significantly greater after the intervention (9.7, IQR = 0.23) compared with baseline (9.4, IQR = 0.47), p < .01 (+). Teachers reported significantly more hand washing behaviour compared with baseline after coughing/sneezing, blowing their nose, changing a diaper, contacting bodily fluids and soiled textiles, going to the toilet, wiping the nose of a child, wiping a child’s bottom, and before helping a child with food (p < .05) (+)
(21) MacIntyre (2009) There were no significant differences in difficulties with mask use between the P2 and surgical mask groups, but >50% reported concerns, the main one being that wearing a face mask was uncomfortable. Other concerns were that the child did not want the parent wearing a mask. Some participants mentioned that the mask did not fit well and that it was not practical to wear at mealtime or while asleep Fit testing for P2 masks was not conducted because this was judged unlikely to be feasible in the general community during a pandemic Not reported Not reported Not reported 145/401 families assessed for eligibility took part Not reported Self‐report On day 1 of mask use, 38% (36/94) of the surgical mask users and 46% (42/92) of the P2 mask users stated that they were wearing the mask ‘most or all of the time’ (p = .37) (/). Adherence dropped to 31% (29/94) and 25% (23/92), respectively, by day 5 of mask use
(22) Mott (2007) Not reported Not reported Not reported Hand sanitizer use increased in leaders in both intervention groups (from 3.0 to 13.4 times/day and from 3.2 to 4.7 times/day, respectively) Not reported Not reported Not reported Self‐report Post‐intervention, there was a decrease in the daily frequency of hand washing in the SI group (from 4.4 to 2.6 times/day) (‐) and no change in the PI group (from 4.9 to 5.0 times/day). Hand sanitizer use increased in both intervention groups (from 3.7 to 10.4 times/day and from 4.0 to 6.0 times/day, respectively) (+)
(23) Nandrup‐Bus (2020) Three children withdrew from the intervention arm due to skin problems Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Objective measure of soap use School records showed a usual monthly consumption of 2–2.5 litres of liquid soap at the intervention school. During the intervention period, consumption increased to 16 L of liquid soap (but accurate monthly measurement was not possible as soap was continuously replenished). No reliable measurement of soap consumption in the control school was available (@)
(24) Or (2020) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Self‐report and observation of hand washing with fluorescent stain gel and photographs After the programme, the percentages of properly washed areas on both hands increased significantly, in particular the wrists (from 0.5 to 82%), p < .001 (+)
(25) Ram (2015) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 377/766 eligible index cases took part Not reported The intervention staff weighed the soap each day and replaced it if the bar weighed <20 g A median per capita soap consumption of 2.3 g (IQR = 1.7–3.2) was observed in the first 12 days of the programme (@)
(26) Reyes Fernández (2015) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 242/440 students completed the study Not reported Self‐report Self‐reported frequency of hand sanitizer use per day, measured on a 5‐point Likert scale, increased in both control (from M = 1.5, SD = 0.9 to M = 1.7, SD = 1.3) and intervention groups (from M = 1.8, SD = 1.3 to M = 2.1, SD = 1.4), p = .09 (/)
(27) Roberts (2000) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 23/26 eligible childcare centres took part Not reported Observation by trained researchers Hand washing compliance in children was divided into three groups, corresponding to intervention centres with a score of low (53–69%; four centres), moderate (70–79%; four centres), and high (>80%; three centres) compliance (@)
(28) Sandora (2005) Forty‐five families reported 112 adverse events related to hand sanitizer use. Seventy‐one (63%) of the adverse events were in relation to ‘dry skin’, and 20 (18%) were related to ‘irritation’. Other reported adverse events included ‘stinging’, ‘smells bad’, ‘dislike it’, ‘allergic reaction’, and ‘too slippery’ Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 292/647 families assessed for eligibility took part Not reported Self‐report Primary caregivers reported using the hand sanitizer with a median frequency of 5.2 times/day (@)
(29) Savolainen‐Kopra (2012) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 683/1,270 employees considered for eligibility took part Not reported Assessment by study nurses The average amount of soap or disinfectant use per participant was 6.1 and 6.9 g in the active groups (@)
(30) Simmerman (2011) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 465/20,537 paediatric outpatients with influenza‐like illness took part Not reported Self‐report Participants in the hand washing group reported 4.7 washing episodes/day, compared with 4.9 times/day in the hand washing plus face mask arm and 3.9 times/day in controls (p = .001) (+). Participants in the face mask arm used an average of 12 masks per person, per week (median = 11, IQR = 7–16) and reported wearing face masks a median of 211 min/day (IQR = 17–317) (@). Parents wore their masks for a median of 153 (IQR = 40–411) minutes per day, which was greater than other relations (median = 59; IQR = 9–266), index cases themselves (median = 35; IQR = 4–197), or their siblings (median = 17; IQR = 6–107)
(31) Stebbins (2010) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 151/167 enrolled teachers took part Not reported Self‐report The proportion of students washing their hands more than three times per day in the post‐flu season was significantly greater in the intervention arm (M = 3.7) compared with the control arm (M = 3.4), p = .04 (+)
(32) Stedman‐Smith (2015) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 324/1,708 enrolled office workers took part Not reported Self‐report In both intervention and control groups, >80% of employees reported hand sanitizer use at least 25% of the time (p > .05) (/)
(33) Suess (2011) The majority (60%) of participants did not report any problems when wearing face masks. Of those who reported having removed their masks in transmission‐prone situations, ‘feeling hot’ was the main reason. Other problems mentioned less frequently were pain when wearing the mask and shortness of breath. The majority of adult household contacts in the control and intervention groups perceived wearing face masks as well as intensified hand hygiene as an effective means of preventing transmission of influenza Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Self‐report 81% of index cases and 71% of household members from the combined active groups wore a mask ‘always’ or ‘most of the time’ when in the same room with either a healthy or an infected person (@). Participants (index cases and contacts combined) in the mask plus hygiene group washed/disinfected their hands significantly more frequently (51/56) compared with those in the mask and control groups combined (59/89) (p = .007) (+)
(34) Suess (2012) The majority of participants (62%) did not report any problems with mask wearing. This proportion was significantly higher in adults (71%) than children (50%) (p = .005). The main problem stated by participants (adults as well as children) was ‘heat/humidity’, followed by ‘pain’ and ‘shortness of breath’ when wearing a face mask Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported The amount of remaining intervention materials was assessed at the end of the study period Participants in the mask group used a median of 12.9 (IQR = 9.5–16) face masks per individual. Participants in the mask plus hygiene group used a median of 12.6 (IQR = 7.8–14) face masks (@). The number of hand disinfections per day was 7.4 and 4.1 in index cases in the 2009/10 and 2010/11 cohorts, respectively. The number of hand disinfections per day was 8.8 and 7.5 in household contacts in the 2009/10 and 2010/11 cohorts, respectively
(35) Updegraff (2011) There were no significant differences between the four signs in how easy they were to understand. The loss‐framed and perceived susceptibility signs elicited significantly more negative affect than the gain‐framed and norms signs (p < .01) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Grams of sanitizer used per day, measured with a digital scale All signs resulted in significantly greater hand sanitizer use compared with the no sign condition (+), but they were not equally effective. Dispensers with the gain‐framed signs had the greatest hand sanitizer use, with 66.4% more use than dispensers with no signs (p < .001). Loss‐framed signs were associated with a 58.4% increase in use compared with no sign (p < .001). The social norms signs (44.3% increase) and the perceived susceptibility signs (40.6% increase) were associated with somewhat lower increases in usage compared with the gain‐framed and loss‐framed signs, but both led to significantly more use than no sign (both p < .01)
(36) White (2003) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Self‐report Over the course of the study, the product group washed their hands 10.4% more frequently than the control group (0.5 times/hr vs. 0.4 times/hr; p < .05) (+). The frequency of hand sanitizer use was also significantly greater in the product group (0.3 times/hr vs. 0.0 times/hr; p < .05)
(37) Yardley (2011) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Moderator analyses indicated that the intervention was similarly effective for those from higher and lower socio‐economic status groups 517/8,150 of those invited participated Of the 324 participants who were randomly assigned to the intervention, 251 (77.5%) progressed to the second session, 219 (67.6%) completed three sessions, and 188 (58.0%) completed all four sessions. The free hand gel was collected by 170/324 (52.5%) eligible participants Self‐report Significant differences between groups in hand washing frequency, measured on a 5‐point scale, were observed at 4 weeks (M = 4.4, SD = 0.9 vs. M = 4.0, SD = 0.9; Cohen’s d = .42) and at 12 weeks (M = 4.1, SD = 1.1 vs. M = 4.5, SD = 0.8; Cohen’s d = .34), p < .001 (+)
(38) Zomer (2016) Not reported Not reported Due to budget restrictions, hand hygiene products were only provided for two groups within each daycare centre, even if the centre had more than two groups No significant effect of the intervention was found on supervising children’s hand hygiene (36 vs. 32%) Not reported 71/122 daycare centres participated Of 274 caregivers, 21% (54/261) attended none of the training sessions, 25% (66/261) attended one training session, 29% (75/261) attended two training sessions and 25% (66/261) attended all three sessions Direct observation by trained researchers Hand hygiene compliance in intervention daycare centres was 66 vs. 43% in control centres (OR = 6.33, 95% CI = 3.71–10.80) (+)
(39) Öncü (2019) Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported Not reported 96/552 children Not reported Photographs examined by researchers not directly involved in the study In the most intensive intervention group, post‐intervention hand washing effectiveness scores increased significantly across all regions of both hands (p < .05) (+)

+ = positive effect; − = negative effect; / = no difference; @ = indeterminate; and NA = not applicable.

Practicability

One study (MacIntyre et al., 2009) considered the practicability of scaling up mask fit testing outside the study setting and decided against including routine fit testing as part of the intervention (see Table 4).

Effectiveness

Studies relied on self‐report (25/39; 64%), direct observation (7/39; 18%), a combination of self‐report and direct observation (5/39; 13%), or photographs/video (2/39; 5%) to examine intervention effectiveness (see Table 4). Outcome variables were heterogeneous across studies (e.g., the frequency or amount of hand sanitizer/soap use per day, the rate of compliance with hand hygiene, or the rate of compliance with face mask use).

Hand hygiene

Overall, the 30 studies pertaining to hand hygiene behaviours (including hand washing and/or hand sanitizer/soap use) had positive results, with 19 studies reporting positive effects (Aiello et al., 2010, 2012; Apisarnthanarak et al., 2009; Arbogast et al., 2016; Chan et al., 2007; Cowling et al., 2008; Kaewchana et al., 2012; Larson et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2019; Mott et al., 2007; Öncü et al., 2019; Or et al., 2020; Simmerman et al., 2011; Stebbins et al., 2010; Suess et al., 2011; Updegraff et al., 2011; White et al., 2003; Yardley et al., 2011; Zomer et al., 2016), three studies reporting negative effects (Aiello et al., 2010; Larson et al., 2009; Mott et al., 2007), six studies reporting no difference (Aiello et al., 2012;; Cowling et al., 2009; Koep et al., 2016; Larson et al., 2010; Reyes Fernández et al., 2015; Stedman‐Smith et al., 2015), and six studies with indeterminate results (Hübner et al., 2010; Nandrup‐Bus, 2009; Ram et al., 2015; Roberts et al., 2000; Sandora et al., 2005; Savolainen‐Kopra et al., 2012) (see Figure 4). It should be noted that some studies reported more than one hand hygiene outcome (e.g., hand washing and hand sanitizer use).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Proportions of reported effects (i.e., positive, negative, no difference, or indeterminate) of interventions targeting hand hygiene behaviours and face mask use.

A random‐effects meta‐analysis (k = 6) found a medium, positive effect of interventions on the average frequency of hand hygiene behaviour, d = .62, 95% CI = 0.43–0.80, p < .001 (see Figure 5). However, between‐study heterogeneity was high (I 2 = 81.2%).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Forest plot for the standardized mean difference (d) in the frequency of hand hygiene behaviour in intervention and control or pre‐ and post‐study comparisons. The comparison in Chan (Chan et al., 2007) pertains to a pre‐ and post‐study comparison; the remaining studies were two‐ or three‐arm RCTs.

Face mask use

Overall, the 12 studies pertaining to face mask use reported mixed results, with three studies reporting positive effects (Barasheed et al., 2014; Cowling et al., 2008, 2009), two studies reporting no difference (Aiello et al., 2012; MacIntyre et al., 2009), one study reporting negative effects (Aiello et al., 2010), and six studies with indeterminate results (Bundgaard et al., 2020; Canini et al., 2010; Larson et al., 2010; Simmerman et al., 2011; T Suess et al., 2011; Suess et al., 2012) (see Figure 4).

A random‐effects meta‐analysis (k = 4) found a large, positive effect of interventions on the odds of compliance with face mask use, OR = 4.14, 95% CI = 1.24–13.79, p < .001 (see Figure 6). However, between‐study heterogeneity was high (I 2 = 89.67%) and the confidence interval for the pooled effect was wide.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Forest plot for the odds of compliance with face mask use in intervention compared with control arms. The comparison in MacIntyre (MacIntyre et al., 2009) pertains to the surgical mask (intervention) vs. the P2 mask arm (control) at the longest point of follow‐up.

Affordability

Two studies considered the affordability of interventions, with one study discussing the cost of face masks (Bundgaard et al., 2020), which may act as a barrier for wider roll‐out, and a second study (Zomer et al., 2016) reporting that although they wanted to provide hand hygiene products to all daycare centre groups, they could only afford to do so for a maximum of two groups per centre due to budget restrictions, thus indicating that the selected intervention was not affordable at scale (see Table 4).

Spill‐over effects

Three studies reported on secondary behaviour change (i.e., positive or negative spill‐over to other behaviours), with one study (Zomer et al., 2016) assessing teachers’ supervision of children’s hand washing (in addition to their own hand washing), a second (Mott et al., 2007) assessing the impact of the intervention on military leaders’ hand sanitizer use (in addition to trainees’), and a third (Larson et al., 2009) examining multiple hand hygiene behaviours in the same group of participants. The first study reported no change, the second a significant increase in hand sanitizer use, and the third a negative impact on hand washing with soap (see Table 4).

Equity

Four studies reported on the equity of interventions, with one study (Yardley et al., 2011) reporting that the intervention was equally effective for participants from high and low socio‐economic status groups and three studies (Azman et al., 2013; Azor‐Martinez et al., 2016; Larson et al., 2009) reporting differential intervention effectiveness by educational attainment, parental income, or ethnicity, with better outcomes reported in those with high educational attainment, high parental income, and from a Black ethnic background (see Table 4).

Quality of included studies

One study received an overall rating of ‘low risk of bias’, with 16 studies rated as ‘some concern’, 18 as ‘high risk of bias’, and for four studies, an overall rating could not be applied (see Table 5).

Table 5.

Quality appraisal

Lead author (year) (1) Bias arising from the randomization process (2) Bias due to deviations from the intended interventions (3) Bias due to missing outcome data (4) Bias in measurement of the outcome (5) Bias in selection of the reported result Overall rating
(1) Aiello (2010) Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns
(2) Aiello (2012) Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns
(3) Apisarnthanarak (2009) NA NA NA NA NA NA
(4) Arbogast (2016) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns
(5) Azman (2013) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns
(6) Azor‐Martinez (2016) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias High risk of bias
(7) Azor‐Martinez (2018) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns
(8) Barasheed (2014) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(9) Bundgaard (2020) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias High risk of bias
(10) Canini (2010) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns
(11) Chan (2007) NA Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(12) Cowling (2008) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns
(13) Cowling (2009) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns
(14) Hübner (2010) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(15) Kaewchana (2012) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(16) Koep (2016) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(17) Larson (2009) NA Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(18) Larson (2010) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(19) Little (2015) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns High risk of bias
(20) Liu (2019) NA NA NA NA NA NA
(21) MacIntyre (2009) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns
(22) Mott (2007) NA NA NA NA NA NA
(23) Nandrup‐Bus (2020) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias High risk of bias
(24) Or (2020) NA NA NA NA NA NA
(25) Ram (2015) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns
(26) Reyes Fernández (2015) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias High risk of bias
(27) Roberts (2000) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns High risk of bias
(28) Sandora (2005) Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(29) Savolainen‐Kopra (2012) Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias High risk of bias
(30) Simmerman (2011) Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns
(31) Stebbins (2010) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(32) Stedman‐Smith (2015) Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns
(33) Suess (2011) Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns
(34) Suess (2012) Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(35) Updegraff (2011) Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Low risk of bias
(36) White (2003) High risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Some concerns High risk of bias
(37) Yardley (2011) Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns
(38) Zomer (2016) Low risk of bias Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns Low risk of bias Some concerns
(39) Öncü (2019) Some concerns Some concerns Low risk of bias Low risk of bias Some concerns Some concerns

NA = not applicable.

Discussion

This rapid review of interventions to increase personal protective behaviours to limit the spread of respiratory viruses identified 39 studies conducted across 15 countries. The majority of interventions targeted hand hygiene and/or face mask use, with one intervention targeting the catching of droplets in tissues in addition to hand hygiene. None of the identified interventions focused on avoiding touching the T‐Zone, disinfecting surfaces or maintaining physical distancing. Interventions were typically delivered in participants’ own homes or in nurseries/schools, targeting children/adult household members or pre‐ or school children/teachers. Two‐ or three‐arm study designs with passive comparators were typically used. The overall quality of included studies was low, with only one study rated as ‘low risk of bias’. The majority of interventions had a face‐to‐face component and delivered a median of three BCTs; the most frequent were ’Adding objects to the environment’, ‘Instruction on how to perform the behaviour’ and ‘Information about health consequences’. Where investigated, interventions were considered acceptable by participants, with a minority reporting issues with mask wear discomfort or skin irritation from hand hygiene products. Few studies reported the practicability, affordability, spill‐over effects or equity of interventions. In a narrative synthesis, interventions targeting hand hygiene behaviour were found to have positive effects and those targeting face mask use had a mixture of positive and negative effects. Random‐effects meta‐analyses of a small number of studies found positive effects of interventions targeting hand hygiene behaviour and face mask use. However, between‐study heterogeneity was high and the confidence interval for the pooled effect of interventions targeting face mask use was wide, partly due to the small number of studies included in the comparison.

Strengths and limitations

This review was conducted rapidly (July–December 2020) with input on the research questions and review scope from public health and behavioural science experts and lay members as part of a written stakeholder consultation. However, the pragmatic nature of this review, conducted during an ongoing pandemic, also means that it has several important limitations. First, given the expected large number of hand hygiene studies related to gastrointestinal infections, we limited the review to studies explicitly studying behaviour change in relation to respiratory viruses. However, data from interventions targeting personal protective behaviours to prevent gastrointestinal illness are likely to add to our understanding of the acceptability, effectiveness, and equity of hand hygiene interventions. Second, evidence indicates that the relative importance of different personal protective behaviours may depend on properties of the specific respiratory virus and context (e.g., fomite transmission may be more pronounced for respiratory syncytial virus compared with coronaviruses) (Boone & Gerba, 2007). However, at the time of planning this rapid evidence review, little was known about SARS‐CoV‐2. We therefore opted for a broad scope and included interventions targeting personal protective behaviours to limit the spread of any respiratory viral infection. It was also not possible to group the results based on the specific viral infections studied as the majority of studies targeted multiple (as opposed to single) respiratory viral infections and there was little variability in the viral infections targeted. In addition, as the majority of studies targeted a host of different viruses within their interventions, this further limits the conclusions that can be drawn: The perceived susceptibility to different viruses likely differs between, for example, age groups (Rosenstock, 1974), and tailored intervention strategies may therefore be needed for younger (vs. older) adults. However, the current review was unable to address such nuanced questions due to the limited design and reporting of extant studies and the need to synthesize evidence quickly during an ongoing pandemic. Third, our electronic search was restricted to two databases, which may have limited the results, and data extraction was performed by a single reviewer, with a proportion verified by a second reviewer. Fourth, although most of identified studies were two‐ or three‐arm RCTs, they were typically designed to study rates of respiratory infection as their primary outcome, with behaviour change outcomes less clearly reported. This hindered quantitative synthesis, with only a small number of included studies contributing to meta‐analyses. Future studies specifically designed to examine the effectiveness of interventions on behavioural outcomes are needed. Fifth, in line with guidelines (Wood et al., 2015), we only coded BCTs when there was clear evidence of their presence; interventions may have included additional BCTs not documented in this review. Sixth, as this was a rapid review with limited resources, we limited our analyses of intervention content to information presented in the published papers and/or available supplementary materials, and no attempts were made to contact study authors for access to detailed intervention descriptions. Seventh, as most interventions targeting multiple behaviours (e.g., hand hygiene and face mask use) did not clearly distinguish BCTs that targeted one (but not the other) behaviour, and none of the outcome evaluations considered potential behavioural dependencies (or statistical interactions), it was not possible to consider the extent of BCT overlap and/or behavioural interactions in the present review. Finally, due to the small number of studies available for meta‐analysis, we were unable to group studies by, for example, population type, study setting, type of virus, type of outcome assessment, etc., which would have further improved our understanding of intervention effectiveness.

Implications for policy and practice

Although we caution against drawing firm conclusions due to the low quality of the evidence, positive effects of interventions targeting hand hygiene behaviour and face mask use were observed, with the majority of interventions providing free hand hygiene products and/or face masks to participants in addition to instructions on how to perform the behaviour and information about health consequences. As far as is practicably feasible, authorities should aim to provide free products to staff, clients, and visitors during respiratory viral epidemics. The limited range of BCTs detected in published intervention descriptions may suggest a missed opportunity for harnessing techniques indicated by relevant behaviour change theory and evidence. We encourage policymakers and health care practitioners to work collaboratively with behavioural scientists to incorporate techniques that theory or evidence predicts are effective for enabling personal protective behaviours (Warren‐Gash et al., 2012), such as techniques targeting motivational or self‐regulatory processes. For example, while hand hygiene is a well‐established, often private and widely accepted protective behaviour that most people have long experience with, face mask wearing is a relatively new (at least in some countries), public behaviour, where there is more debate and uncertainties about the impact of the behaviour among the public and scientists (Cowling, Zhou, Ip, Leung, & Aiello, 2010). It is therefore important to involve behavioural scientists in the development of any new interventions targeting personal protective behaviours in the context of respiratory viral infections to help map out potential influences (e.g., social, self‐regulatory) that may help or hinder the target behaviour, acknowledging that different interventions are likely needed for different behaviours.

Avenues for future research

Findings highlight the need for evaluations of interventions to support people to avoid touching the T‐Zone, disinfect surfaces, maintain physical distancing, and ensure efficient ventilation. Ventilation is increasingly seen as an important personal protective behaviour but was missed from the present review as it was planned during an earlier epidemic phase when the emphasis was on viral transmission via droplets rather than aerosols (Anderson, Turnham, Griffin, & Clarke, 2020; Morawska & Milton, 2020). In addition, we need studies designed to detect effects on behavioural outcomes and data on the affordability and equity of interventions to increase personal protective behaviours, particularly in low‐ and middle‐income countries. Although the provision of hand hygiene products and face masks may offset costs related to primary and secondary care or work absenteeism for those with severe respiratory viral illness, the provision of free products at scale may be prohibitively costly. Future research involving health and social care economists should evaluate the cost‐effectiveness of different types of interventions to enable personal protective behaviours, including those targeting motivational and self‐regulatory processes. We also need further evidence from studies evaluating interventions to improve adherence to face mask use, with unclear results observed at present. Finally, due to the small number of studies with data suitable for meta‐analysis, we did not conduct moderator analyses to examine whether, for example, particular BCTs, broader content categories, or the unit of randomization (e.g., individual vs. cluster) were related to intervention effectiveness; this would be important to examine in future meta‐analyses with larger sample sizes. We did not consider here the use of, for example, the Theoretical Domains Framework (Cane, O’Connor, & Michie, 2012) when coding the mechanisms of action of interventions; this may be useful to consider in future empirical studies and evidence reviews. Finally, although evidence generation during ongoing pandemics is challenging (with a need to balance a pragmatic approach and limited resources with scientific rigour), drawing primarily on studies conducted outside the pandemic setting to inform what behavioural interventions to implement is suboptimal. We therefore recommend that experimental studies of behavioural interventions are prioritized during future respiratory viral pandemics.

Conclusions

This rapid review identified 39 studies across 15 countries with interventions targeting hand hygiene and/or face mask use. Positive effects of interventions targeting hand hygiene were observed, with unclear results for interventions targeting face mask use. There was a lack of evidence for interventions targeting most behaviours of interest within this review.

Funding

OP receives salary support from Cancer Research UK (C1417/A22962).

Conflicts of interest

All authors declare no conflict of interest.

Author contributions

Olga Perski (Conceptualization; Data curation; Formal analysis; Methodology; Visualization; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing) Dorothy Szinay (Data curation; Writing – review & editing) Elizabeth Corker (Conceptualization; Data curation; Methodology; Writing – review & editing) Lion Shahab (Conceptualization; Formal analysis; Methodology; Writing – review & editing) Robert West (Conceptualization; Methodology; Writing – review & editing) Susan Michie (Conceptualization; Methodology; Writing – review & editing).

Supporting information

Appendix S1. Search strategy.

BJHP-27-215-s001.docx (14.3KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the funding listed. The funders had no final role in the study design; in the analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication. All researchers listed as authors are independent from the funders, and all final decisions about the research were taken by the investigators and were unrestricted.

Data Availability Statement

The extracted data that support the findings of this rapid review are available on request from the corresponding author, OP.

References

  1. Aiello, A. E. , Murray, G. F. , Perez, V. , Coulborn, R. M. , Davis, B. M. , Uddin, M. , … Monto, A. S. (2010). Mask use, hand hygiene, and seasonal influenza‐like illness among young adults: A randomized intervention trial. The Journal of Infectious Diseases, 201(4), 491–498. 10.1086/650396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aiello, A. E. , Perez, V. , Coulborn, R. M. , Davis, B. M. , Uddin, M. , & Monto, A. S. (2012). Facemasks, hand hygiene, and influenza among young adults: A randomized intervention trial. PLoS One, 7(1), e29744. 10.1371/journal.pone.0029744 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson, E. L. , Turnham, P. , Griffin, J. R. , & Clarke, C. C. (2020). Consideration of the aerosol transmission for COVID‐19 and public health. Risk Analysis, 40, 902–907. 10.1111/risa.13500 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Apisarnthanarak, A. , Apisarnthanarak, P. , Cheevakumjorn, B. , & Mundy, L. M. (2009). Intervention with an infection control bundle to reduce transmission of influenza‐like illnesses in a thai preschool. Infection Control and Hospital Epidemiology, 30, 1–6. 10.1086/599773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arbogast, J. W. , Moore‐Schiltz, L. , Jarvis, W. R. , Harpster‐Hagen, A. , Hughes, J. , & Parker, A. (2016). Impact of a comprehensive workplace hand hygiene program on employer health care insurance claims and costs, absenteeism, and employee perceptions and practices. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 58, e231–e240. 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000738 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Azman, A. S. , Stark, J. H. , Althouse, B. M. , Vukotich, C. J. , Stebbins, S. , Burke, D. S. , & Cummings, D. A. T. (2013). Household transmission of influenza A and B in a school‐based study of non‐pharmaceutical interventions. Epidemics, 5, 181–186. 10.1016/j.epidem.2013.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Azor‐Martinez, E. , Cobos‐Carrascosa, E. , Seijas‐Vasquez, M. L. , Fernandez‐Sanchez, C. , Strizzi, J. M. , Torres‐Alegrie, P. , … Gimenez‐Sanchez, F. (2016). Hand hygiene program decreases school absenteeism due to upper respiratory infections. Journal of School Health, 86, 873–881. 10.1111/josh.12454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Azor‐Martinez, E. , Yui‐Hifume, R. , Munoz‐Vico, F. J. , Jimenez‐Noguera, E. , Strizzi, J. M. , Martinez‐Martinez, I. , … Gimenez‐Sanchez, F. (2018). Effectiveness of a hand hygiene program at child care centers: A cluster randomized trial. Pediatrics, 142, e20181245. 10.1542/peds.2018-1245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barasheed, O. , Almasri, N. , Badahdah, A.‐M. , Heron, L. , Taylor, J. , McPhee, K. , … Booy, R. (2014). Pilot Randomised controlled trial to test effectiveness of facemasks in preventing influenza‐like illness transmission among australian hajj pilgrims in 2011. Infectious Disorders ‐ Drug Targets, 14, 110–116. 10.2174/1871526514666141021112855 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bish, A. , & Michie, S. (2010). Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a pandemic: A review. British Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 797–824. 10.1348/135910710X485826 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Blake, P. , & Wadhwa, D. (2020). 2020 Year in Review: The impact of COVID‐19 in 12 charts. World Bank Blog. https://blogs.worldbank.org/voices/2020‐year‐review‐impact‐covid‐19‐12‐charts [Google Scholar]
  12. Boone, S. A. , & Gerba, C. P. (2007). Significance of fomites in the spread of respiratory and enteric viral disease. Applied and Environment Microbiology, 73, 10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Brooks, S. K. , Webster, R. K. , Smith, L. E. , Woodland, L. , Wessely, S. , Greenberg, N. , & Rubin, G. J. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. The Lancet, 395, 912–920. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bundgaard, H. , Bundgaard, J. S. , Raaschou‐Pedersen, D. E. T. , von Buchwald, C. , Todsen, T. , … Iversen, K. (2021). Effectiveness of adding a mask recommendation to other public health measures to prevent SARS‐CoV‐2 infection in danish mask wearers. Annals of Internal Medicine, 174(3), 335–343. 10.7326/M20-6817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cane, J. , O’Connor, D. , & Michie, S. (2012). Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implementation Science, 7(1), 37. 10.1186/1748-5908-7-37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Canini, L. , Andréoletti, L. , Ferrari, P. , Angelo, D. R. , Blanchon, T. , Lemaitre, M. , … Carrat, F. (2010). Surgical mask to prevent influenza transmission in households: A cluster randomized trial. PLoS One, 5, 1–6. 10.1371/journal.pone.0013998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carey, R. N. , Connell, L. E. , Johnston, M. , Rothman, A. J. , Bruin, M. D. , Kelly, M. P. , & Michie, S. (2019). Behavior change techniques and their mechanisms of action: A synthesis of links described in published intervention literature. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 53, 693–707. 10.1093/abm/kay078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chan, S. S. C. , So, W. K. W. , Wong, D. C. N. , Lee, A. C. K. , & Tiwari, A. (2007). Improving older adults’ knowledge and practice of preventive measures through a telephone health education during the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong: A pilot study. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 44, 1120–1127. 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2006.04.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cheung, M. W. L. (2019). A guide to conducting a meta‐analysis with non‐independent effect sizes. Neuropsychology Review, 29, 387–396. 10.1007/s11065-019-09415-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. 10.4324/9780203771587 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cowling, B. J. , Chan, K.‐H. , Fang, V. J. , Cheng, C. K. Y. , Fung, R. O. P. , Wai, W. , … Leung, G. M. (2009). Facemasks and hand hygiene to prevent influenza transmission in households: A cluster randomized trial. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151, 437–446. 10.7326/0003-4819-151-7-200910060-00142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cowling, B. J. , Fung, R. O. P. , Cheng, C. K. Y. , Fang, V. J. , Chan, K. H. , Seto, W. H. , … Leung, G. M. (2008). Preliminary findings of a randomized trial of non‐pharmaceutical interventions to prevent influenza transmission in households. PLoS One, 3(5), e2101. 10.1371/journal.pone.0002101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cowling, B. J. , Zhou, Y. , Ip, D. K. M. , Leung, G. M. , & Aiello, A. E. (2010). Face masks to prevent transmission of influenza virus: A systematic review. Epidemiology and Infection, 138(4), 449–456. 10.1017/S0950268809991658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Edwards, R. , Charani, E. , Sevdalis, N. , Alexandrou, B. , Sibley, E. , Mullett, D. , … Holmes, A. (2012). Optimisation of infection prevention and control in acute health care by use of behaviour change: A systematic review. The Lancet Infectious Diseases, 12(4), 318–329. 10.1016/S1473-3099(11)70283-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ferguson, N. M. , Cummings, D. A. T. , Fraser, C. , Cajka, J. C. , Cooley, P. C. , & Burke, D. S. (2006). Strategies for mitigating an influenza pandemic. Nature, 442, 448–452. 10.1038/nature04795 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Haby, M. M. , Chapman, E. , Clark, R. , Barreto, J. , Reveiz, L. , & Lavis, J. N. (2016). What are the best methodologies for rapid reviews of the research evidence for evidence‐informed decision making in health policy and practice: A rapid review. Health Research Policy and Systems, 14(1), 1–12. 10.1186/s12961-016-0155-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hübner, N.‐O. , Hübner, C. , Wodny, M. , Kampf, G. , & Kramer, A. (2010). Effectiveness of alcohol‐based hand disinfectants in a public administration: Impact on health and work performance related to acute respiratory symptoms and diarrhoea. BMC Infectious Diseases, 10, 1–8. 10.1186/1471-2334-10-250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Huis, A. , van Achterberg, T. , de Bruin, M. , Grol, R. , Schoonhoven, L. , & Hulscher, M. (2012). A systematic review of hand hygiene improvement strategies: A behavioural approach. Implementation Science, 7(1), 1–14. 10.1186/1748-5908-7-92 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. International Monetary Fund . (2020). world economic outlook update. Washington, DC: International Monetary Fund. [Google Scholar]
  30. Kaewchana, S. , Simmerman, M. , Somrongthong, R. , Suntarattiwong, P. , Lertmaharit, S. , & Chotipitayasunondh, T. (2012). Effect of intensive hand washing education on hand washing behaviors in thai households with an influenza‐positive child in Urban Thailand. Asia‐Pacific Journal of Public Health, 24(4), 577–585. 10.1177/1010539510393728 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Killingley, B. , & Nguyen‐Van‐Tam, J. (2013). Routes of influenza transmission. Influenza and Other Respiratory Viruses, 7, 42–51. 10.1111/irv.12080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Koep, T. H. , Jenkins, S. , Hammerlund, M. E. M. , Clemens, C. , Fracica, E. , Ekker, S. C. , … Pierret, C. (2016). Promotion of influenza prevention beliefs and behaviors through primary school science education. Journal of Community Medicine & Health Education, 6(3), 1–8. 10.4172/2161-0711.1000444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kwok, Y. L. A. , Gralton, J. , & McLaws, M. L. (2015). Face touching: A frequent habit that has implications for hand hygiene. American Journal of Infection Control, 43(2), 112–114. 10.1016/j.ajic.2014.10.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Larson, E. L. , Ferng, Y.‐H. , McLoughlin, J. W. , Wang, S. , & Morse, S. S. (2009). Effect of intensive education on knowledge, attitudes, and practices regarding upper respiratory infections among urban latinos. Nursing Research, 58, 150–157. 10.1097/NNR.0b013e3181a30951 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Larson, E. L. , Ferng, Y.‐H. , Wong‐McLoughlin, J. , Wang, S. , Haber, M. , & Morse, S. S. (2010). Impact of non‐pharmaceutical interventions on URIs and Influenza in crowded, Urban Households. Public Health Reports, 125, 178–191. 10.1177/003335491012500206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Little, P. , Stuart, B. , Hobbs, F. D. R. , Moore, M. , Barnett, J. , Popoola, D. , … Yardley, L. (2015). An internet‐delivered handwashing intervention to modify influenza‐like illness and respiratory infection transmission (PRIMIT): A primary care randomised trial. The Lancet, 386, 1631–1639. 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60127-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Liu, X. , Zhao, Z. , Hou, W. , Polinder, S. , van Beeck, E. F. , Zhang, Z. , … Erasmus, V. (2019). A multimodal intervention to improve hand hygiene compliance via social cognitive influences among kindergarten teachers in China. PLoS One, 14(5), e0215824. 10.1371/journal.pone.0215824 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Luangasanatip, N. , Hongsuwan, M. , Limmathurotsakul, D. , Lubell, Y. , Lee, A. S. , Harbarth, S. , … Cooper, B. S. (2015). Comparative efficacy of interventions to promote hand hygiene in hospital: Systematic review and network meta‐analysis. BMJ (Online), 351, 10.1136/bmj.h3728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lunn, P. , Belton, C. , Lavin, C. , Mcgowan, F. , Timmons, S. , & Robertson, D. (2020). Using behavioural science to help fight the coronavirus. Economic and Social Research Institute, 1–24. [Google Scholar]
  40. MacIntyre, C. R. , Cauchemez, S. , Dwyer, D. E. , Seale, H. , Cheung, P. , Browne, G. , … Ferguson, N. (2009). Face mask use and control of respiratory virus transmission in households. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 15(2), 233–241. 10.3201/eid1502.081167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mbakaya, B. C. , Lee, P. H. , & Lee, R. L. T. (2017). Hand hygiene intervention strategies to reduce diarrhoea and respiratory infections among schoolchildren in developing countries: A systematic review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14, 1–14. 10.3390/ijerph14040371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Michie, S. , Atkins, L. , & West, R. (2014). The behaviour change wheel: A guide to designing interventions. Surrey, UK: Silverback Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  43. Michie, S. , Richardson, M. , Johnston, M. , Abraham, C. , Francis, J. , Hardeman, W. , … Wood, C. E. (2013). The behavior change technique taxonomy (v1) of 93 hierarchically clustered techniques: Building an international consensus for the reporting of behavior change interventions. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 46(1), 81–95. 10.1007/s12160-013-9486-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Michie, S. , Rubin, J. , & Amlôt, R. (2020, February 28). Behavioural science must be at the heart of the public health response to covid‐19. BMJ Opinion. [Google Scholar]
  45. Michie, S. , West, R. , & Amlôt, R. (2020, March 3). Behavioural strategies for reducing covid‐19 transmission in the general population. BMJ Opinion. [Google Scholar]
  46. Michie, S. , West, R. , Amlôt, R. , & Rubin, J. (2020, March 11). Slowing down the covid‐19 outbreak: Changing behaviour by understanding it. BMJ Opinion. [Google Scholar]
  47. Moore, G. F. , & Evans, R. E. (2017). What theory, for whom and in which context? Reflections on the application of theory in the development and evaluation of complex population health interventions. SSM ‐ Population Health, 3, 132–135. 10.1016/j.ssmph.2016.12.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Morawska, L. , & Milton, D. K. (2020). It is time to address airborne transmission of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID‐19). Clinical Infectious Diseases, 71, 2311–2313. 10.1093/cid/ciaa939 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mott, P. J. , Sisk, B. W. , Arbogast, J. W. , Ferrazzano‐Yaussy, C. , Bondi, C. A. M. , & Sheehan, J. J. (2007). Alcohol‐based instant hand sanitizer use in military settings: A prospective cohort study of army basic trainees. Military Medicine, 172, 1170–1176. 10.7205/MILMED.172.11.1170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nandrup‐Bus, I. (2009). Mandatory handwashing in elementary schools reduces absenteeism due to infectious illness among pupils: A pilot intervention study. American Journal of Infection Control, 37(10), 820–826. 10.1016/j.ajic.2009.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Norris, E. , Finnerty, A. N. , Hastings, J. , Stokes, G. , & Michie, S. (2019). A scoping review of ontologies related to human behaviour change. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(2), 164–172. 10.1038/s41562-018-0511-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Olena Doronina, R. N. , Jones, D. , Martello, M. , Biron, A. , & Lavoie‐Tremblay, M. (2017). A systematic review on the effectiveness of interventions to improve hand hygiene compliance of nurses in the hospital setting. Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 49(2), 143–152. 10.1111/jnu.12274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Öncü, E. , Köksoy Vayısoğlu, S. , Lafci, D. , Yurtsever, D. , Ravlı Bulut, E. , & Peker, E. (2019). Comparison of interactive education versus fluorescent concretization on hand hygiene compliance among primary school students: A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of School Nursing, 35(5), 337–347. 10.1177/1059840518785447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Or, P.‐P.‐L. , Ching, P.‐T.‐Y. , & Chung, J.‐W.‐Y. (2020). Can flu‐like absenteeism in kindergartens be reduced through hand hygiene training for both parents and their kindergarteners? Journal of Primary Care and Community Health, 11, 1–6. 10.1177/2150132719901209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ram, P. K. , Divita, M. A. , Khatun‐e‐Jannat, K. , Islam, M. , Krytus, K. , Cercone, E. , … Luby, S. P. (2015). Impact of intensive handwashing promotion on secondary household influenza‐like illness in rural bangladesh: Findings from a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One, 10(6), e0125200. 10.1371/journal.pone.0125200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Reyes Fernández, B. , Lippke, S. , Knoll, N. , Moya, E. B. , & Schwarzer, R. (2015). Promoting action control and coping planning to improve hand hygiene. BMC Public Health, 15, 1–7. 10.1186/s12889-015-2295-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Roberts, L. , PhD, E. , Smith, W. , Jorm, L. , PhD, B. V. S. M. S. E. , Patel, M. , … McGilchrist, C. (2000). Effect of infection control measures on the frequency of upper respiratory infection in child care: A randomized. Controlled Trial. Pediatrics, 105, 738–742. 10.1542/peds.105.4.738 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Education Monographs, 2(4), 328–335. 10.1177/109019817400200403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. SafeLives . (2020). Domestic abuse and COVID‐19. https://safelives.org.uk/news‐views/domestic‐abuse‐and‐covid‐19 [Google Scholar]
  60. Sandora, T. J. , Taveras, E. M. , Shih, M.‐C. , Resnick, E. A. , Lee, G. M. , Ross‐Degnan, D. , & Goldmann, D. A. (2005). A randomized, controlled trial of a multifaceted intervention including alcohol‐based hand sanitizer and hand‐hygiene education to reduce illness transmission in the home. Pediatrics, 116(3), 587–594. 10.1542/peds.2005-0199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Savolainen‐Kopra, C. , Haapakoski, J. , Peltola, P. A. , Ziegler, T. , Korpela, T. , Anttila, P. , … Hovi, T. (2012). Hand washing with soap and water together with behavioural recommendations prevents infections in common work environment: An open cluster‐randomized trial. Trials, 13, 1–11. 10.1186/1745-6215-13-10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Simmerman, J. M. , Suntarattiwong, P. , Levy, J. , Jarman, R. G. , Kaewchana, S. , Gibbons, R. V. , … Chotipitayasunondh, T. (2011). Findings from a household randomized controlled trial of hand washing and face masks to reduce influenza transmission in Bangkok, Thailand. Influenza and Other Respiratory Viruses, 5, 256–267. 10.1111/j.1750-2659.2011.00205.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Stebbins, S. , Stark, J. H. , & Vukotich, Jr, C. J. (2010). Compliance with a multilayered nonpharmaceutical intervention in an urban elementary school setting. Journal of Public Health Management and Practice, 16(4), 316–324. 10.1097/PHH.0b013e3181cb4368 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Stedman‐Smith, M. , DuBois, C. L. Z. , Grey, S. F. , Kingsbury, D. M. , Shakya, S. , Scofield, J. , & Slenkovich, K. (2015). Outcomes of a pilot hand hygiene randomized cluster trial to reduce communicable infections among US office‐based employees. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 57(4), 374–380. 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Suess, T. , Remschmidt, C. , Schink, S. , Luchtenberg, M. , Haas, W. , Krause, G. , & Buchholz, U. (2011). Facemasks and intensified hand hygiene in a German household trial during the 2009/2010 influenza A (H1N1) pandemic: Adherence and tolerability in children and adults. Epidemiology and Infection, 139, 1895–1901. 10.1017/S0950268810003006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Suess, T. , Remschmidt, C. , Schink, S. B. , Schweiger, B. , Nitsche, A. , Schroeder, K. , … Buchholz, U. (2012). The role of facemasks and hand hygiene in the prevention of influenza transmission in households: results from a cluster randomised trial; Berlin, Germany, 2009‐2011. BMC Infectious Diseases, 12(1), 2009–2011. 10.1186/1471-2334-12-26 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. The Cochrane Collaboration . (2011). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 5.1.0. [Updated March 2011]. Higgins J., & Green S. (Eds.). London, UK: The Cochrane Collaboration. [Google Scholar]
  68. Tricco, A. C. , Antony, J. , Zarin, W. , Strifler, L. , Ghassemi, M. , Ivory, J. , … Straus, S. E. (2015). A scoping review of rapid review methods. BMC Medicine, 13, 224. 10.1186/s12916-015-0465-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Updegraff, J. A. , Emanuel, A. S. , Gallagher, K. M. , & Steinman, C. T. (2011). Framing flu prevention—an experimental field test of signs promoting hand hygiene during the 2009–2010 H1N1 pandemic. Health Psychology, 30(3), 295–299. 10.1037/a0023125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta‐analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1–48. [Google Scholar]
  71. Warren‐Gash, C. , Fragaszy, E. , & Hayward, A. C. (2012). Hand hygiene to reduce community transmission of influenza and acute respiratory tract infection: A systematic review. Influenza and Other Respiratory Viruses, 7, 738–749. 10.1111/irv.12015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. West, R. , & Michie, S. (2020). Routes of transmission of SARS‐CoV‐2 and behaviours to block it: A summary. Qeios, 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  73. West, R. , Michie, S. , Rubin, J. , & Amlôt, R. (2020). Applying principles of behaviour change to reduce SARS‐CoV‐2 transmission. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 451–459. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562‐020‐0887‐9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. West, R. , Michie, S. , Rubin, J. , & Amlôt, R. (2020, April 3). Don’t touch the T‐Zone—How to block a key pathway to infection with SARS‐CoV‐2. BMJ Opinion. [Google Scholar]
  75. White, C. , Kolble, R. , Carlson, R. , Lipson, N. , & Dolan, M. (2003). The effect of hand hygiene on illness rate among students in university residence halls. American Journal of Infection Control, 31, 364–370. 10.1067/mic.2003.76 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wood, C. E. , Richardson, M. , Johnston, M. , Abraham, C. , Francis, J. , Hardeman, W. , & Michie, S. (2015). Applying the behaviour change technique (BCT) taxonomy v1: A study of coder training. Translational Behavioral Medicine, 5, 134–148. 10.1007/s13142-014-0290-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. World Health Organisation . (2017). Rapid reviews to strengthen health policy and systems: A practical guide. In Tricco A. C., Langlois E. V., & Straus S. E. (Eds.). Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organisation. 10.1111/j.1365-2966.2008.12859.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. World Health Organisation . (2019). Non‐pharmaceutical public health measures for mitigating the risk and impact of epidemic and pandemic influenza. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organisation. [Google Scholar]
  79. Yardley, L. , Miller, S. , Schlotz, W. , & Little, P. (2011). Evaluation of a web‐based intervention to promote hand hygiene: Exploratory randomized controlled trial. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 13(4), e107. 10.2196/jmir.1963 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zomer, T. P. , Erasmus, V. , Looman, C. W. , van Beeck, E. F. , Tjon‐a‐Tsien, A. , Richardus, J. H. , & Voeten, H. A. C. M. (2016). Improving hand hygiene compliance in child daycare centres: A randomized controlled trial. Epidemiology & Infection, 144, 2552–2560. 10.1017/S0950268816000911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix S1. Search strategy.

BJHP-27-215-s001.docx (14.3KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

The extracted data that support the findings of this rapid review are available on request from the corresponding author, OP.


Articles from British Journal of Health Psychology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES