Skip to main content
Journal of Clinical Medicine logoLink to Journal of Clinical Medicine
. 2021 Dec 30;11(1):188. doi: 10.3390/jcm11010188

From Emotional (Dys)Regulation to Internet Addiction: A Mediation Model of Problematic Social Media Use among Italian Young Adults

Alessandro Quaglieri 1, Silvia Biondi 2, Paolo Roma 2, Manuel Varchetta 1,3, Angelo Fraschetti 1, Jessica Burrai 1, Giulia Lausi 1, Manuel Martí-Vilar 3, Francisco González-Sala 4, Alberto Di Domenico 5, Anna Maria Giannini 1, Emanuela Mari 1,*
Editor: Elena Tomba
PMCID: PMC8745641  PMID: 35011929

Abstract

Internet addiction (IA) has mostly been investigated with the fear of missing out and difficulties in emotional regulation. The present study examined the link between IA and variables related to problematic social media use (i.e., fear of missing out, social media addiction), together with emotional (dys)regulation and personality traits, providing new insights and an integrated assessment of IA. In total, 397 participants, aged 18–35 years (M = 22.00; SD = 3.83), were administered a set of questionnaires pertaining to IA, problematic social media use, emotional (dys)regulation, and personality traits. Pearson’s correlations showed significant associations between IA and the investigated variables, and the tested mediation model highlighted the crucial role played by emotional (dys)regulation in the fear of missing out and problematic use of social networks. Overall, the findings provide support for a new integrated model for understanding the features, predictors, and risk factors of IA.

Keywords: internet addiction, fear of missing out, social media addiction, mediation, behavioral addiction, personality traits, risk factors, emotion, disorder

1. Introduction

Internet addiction (IA), also known as problematic internet use, has been defined as problematic and compulsive use of the internet, resulting in significant impairments to daily life functioning. IA is closely linked to computer addiction, which may otherwise be referred to as online addiction and technological addiction. More in detail, IA is an addictive disorder characterized by intensive and obsessive use of the internet, including social networking, video, and online gaming sites [1,2].

In particular, social network overuse can be considered as one form of IA, where individuals exhibit a compulsion to use social media [3,4] leading to the possible onset of pathological behaviors. Individuals with social media addiction exhibit an uncontrollable urge to “log in” and to use social media [5]. Furthermore, studies on adolescent populations found out correlations between frequency in the use of social networks (SNs) and diagnostic criteria for IA [6,7,8].

As such, it encapsulates a wide range of psychopathological behaviors (e.g., online gambling addiction, online gaming addiction, communication addiction disorder), whose common matrix seems to be impulse dysregulation [9,10,11] and difficulties in managing emotional states [12,13,14,15].

Dysregulated emotional responses may foster addictive behavior as a means of coping with high levels of stress. In this vein, individuals with a propensity towards emotional distress have been found to be more likely to engage in substance use. Specifically, studies of opioid [16,17], cocaine [18,19], methamphetamine [20,21], alcohol [22,23], and tobacco [24,25] misuse have shown a close link between addiction and emotional dysregulation. Furthermore, the role of emotional (dys)regulation and alexithymia, often conceptualized as emotional regulation disorder [26], has also been investigated in non-substance–related addictions, including gambling disorder [27,28], IA [29,30], gaming addiction [28,31], and problematic internet use [32]. Again, emotional (dys)regulation has been found to strongly correlate with the severity of these behavioral addictions [28]. Studies have also shown that individuals with both emotional (dys)regulation and alexithymia experience difficulties developing healthy relationships, due to their problematic management of emotional states, and consequent mood disorders. Such individuals may consider the internet an arena in which they can achieve greater self-control and engage in better communication with others [30].

Despite this empirical evidence of the role of emotional dysregulation in addictive behavior, many questions still remain. In particular, it is not yet known whether emotional dysregulation is a cause, correlation, or consequence of addictive behavior.

1.1. Internet Addiction and Emotional (Dys) Regulation

IA is not currently listed as a formal disorder by the main diagnostic classifications, such as the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—fifth edition (DSM-5) [33] and the International Classification of Diseases—11th Revision (ICD-11) [34]. However, the inclusion of one of its phenomenological manifestations—internet gaming addiction disorder—has been proposed in the DSM-5, while it is described as a manifestation of the Gaming Disorder (6C51) in the ICD-11. Young [35] was the first to outline specific diagnostic criteria for IA, based on the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria [36] for pathological gambling, with which IA shares symptomatic overlap; drawing on this criteria, she constructed a diagnostic questionnaire (i.e., Young’s Internet Addiction Test; IAT) [35]. Research using the IAT has shown that IA has characteristics and symptoms comparable to those of gambling disorder (e.g., preoccupation, tolerance, withdrawal, failure to control, impaired decision making) and comorbidity with depression and anxiety [37,38,39], obsessive-compulsive symptoms [40,41,42,43], attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder [44], and hostility–aggression behaviors [45,46]. Nevertheless, IA is not classified as a specific psychiatric disorder, but a psychological symptom that may arise in various psychopathological frameworks. Therefore, it may be more useful to identify the individual and environmental factors that predict high internet use than to diagnose IA as a specific addiction or primary disorder.

The internet enables users to connect with others, discover new worlds, acquire new knowledge, and create positive emotions (e.g., happiness, fun, satisfaction). Therefore, IA might reflect an individual’s attempt to cope with and escape from negative emotions in everyday life and promote more positive affect. However, the positive emotional reinforcement that may arise from use of the internet can easily produce an imbalance, leading to abuse or overuse [14]. Indeed, a study by Longstreet et al. [14] showed that IA increased negative and decreased positive emotional states; this suggests that the positive emotional reinforcement that can stem from use of the internet may encourage continued internet use.

Furthermore, deficiencies in the ability to effectively identify and describe emotions (i.e., alexithymia) has been shown to significantly predict IA. For instance, in a sample of adolescents, IA was strongly correlated with a reduced ability to understand emotional reactions, control impulsive behavior in response to negative emotional experiences, and use effective emotional regulation strategies [47]. The use of IA as an escape behavior and manner of coping with emotional difficulties and stressful events has also been confirmed in a sample of young adults: Pettorruso et al. [48] showed that the presence of emotional problems, especially in young adults with low novelty-seeking, predicted the development of problematic social interaction (i.e., problematic use of the Internet), leading to detachment from reality.

1.2. Social Media Addiction and Fear of Missing Out

Fear of missing out (i.e., FoMO) has been defined as an individual’s fear that other people are having fun without them [49]. Wegmann et al. [50] suggested that FoMO is not a unitary phenomenon, but a complex construct reflecting both a personal predisposition and a specific cognition regarding the fear of missing out on a particular event. Although the theoretical construct of FoMO does not explicitly refer to a specific context (i.e., online, offline), most studies have applied the construct to the online context [49,51,52]. Furthermore, FoMO has been found to be associated with a variety of psychopathological symptoms, including social isolation, depression, and anxiety [53,54,55,56,57].

Several studies have linked FoMO to the overuse of SNs [58,59,60,61], which may be considered ideal for satisfying the “desire to stay continuously connected to what others are doing” [49,62]. FoMO-related overuse of SNs [49,63] may manifest in the problematic use of smartphones [64], resulting in a vicious circle [65] whereby frequent and compulsive checking of social media on one’s smartphone may lead individuals to actively seek out events they have missed [57,66].

Excessive use of SNs may also lead to social media addiction—an uncontrollable and compulsive behavior [40,67,68] that has been shown to be associated with emotional, relational, well-being, and performance problems [69,70,71,72].

Although FoMO has been identified as potentially responsible for problematic use of social networks, it should come as no surprise that compulsive users of social networks may experience FoMO as a result. Individuals engaging in higher levels of social network use seem to be more likely to experience online vulnerability and exhibit higher levels of FoMO [73]. One possible reason could be the excessive social surveillance that SNs provide; indeed, prior to both the advent and spread of SNs, individuals were less likely to be aware of the activities of “others”. The continuous exposure to this information could lead individuals to believe that “others” are having a better life and that one is missing out on something.

The continuous need to stay in touch offered by social media may, therefore, exacerbate the FoMO in accordance with the reinforcement spiral model [74], which emphasizes that the use of particular media content may reinforce those needs that led to its initial use. This could place the individual into a spiral of behavior in which it will be difficult to satisfy the sense of control and social belonging.

1.3. Personality Traits

Research has demonstrated a strong relationship between personality and IA, as well as behavioral addictions, more generally [75,76,77,78]. In this context, several studies have investigated the influence of the “Big Five” personality traits (i.e., extroversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness to new experiences) [79] on internet behavior. Such studies have found that conscientiousness—defined as the propensity to “follow socially prescribed norms for impulse control, to be goal-oriented, to plan and to be able to delay gratification and to follow norms and rules” [80]—is negatively associated with excessive social media use [81] and IA [82]. In a similar vein, Andreassen et al. [75] found that conscientiousness was a protective factor for unproductive behavioral addictions (e.g., addiction to Facebook, video games, or the internet) and a risk factor for positive or productive behavioral addictions (e.g., an addiction to studying). On the other hand, neuroticism has been found to predict both social media use and IA, especially in studies using age as a covariate [53]. Neuroticism, or the “tendency to experience negative emotional states, characterized by a tendency to worry and be anxious” [83,84], has been found to be associated with frequent and unbalanced negative affect in response to stressful circumstances [85]. In addition, Ehrenberg et al. [86] found that individuals with high neuroticism showed stronger mobile phone addictive tendencies and a preference for online communication.

1.4. Study Aim and Hypotheses

The present study aimed at investigating the relationships between IA and variables identified in the literature as related to this construct (i.e., social media addiction, FoMO, difficulties in emotional regulation, personality traits). Specifically, the hypotheses were as follows:

Hypothesis 1 (H1).

The following correlations identified in the literature would be verified:

  • a. 

    Positive correlation between IA and variables related to problematic social media use;

  • b. 

    Positive correlation between IA and emotional (dys)regulation; and

  • c. 

    Correlation between IA and personality traits; in particular, a positive correlation with neuroticism and a negative correlation with conscientiousness.

Hypothesis 2 (H2).

There would be a direct positive effect of emotional (dys)regulation (the independent variable) on IA (the dependent variable).

Hypothesis 3 (H3).

There would be an effect of the independent variable on the proposed mediator (FoMO and social media addiction), and an effect of the proposed mediator on the dependent variable; and an effective mediation model could be established including neuroticism and conscientiousness as covariates (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Proposed mediation model (Model 6). Note. X = independent variable; Y = dependent variable; M1 = mediator 1; M2 = mediator 2; C1 = Covariate 1; C2 = Covariate 2.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample was comprised of 397 Italian young adults (122 male, 274 female, 1 other). Ages ranged from 18 to 35 years, with an average age of 22.00 years (SD = 3.83). With respect to education, 1.8% had a primary or middle school diploma, 73.6% had a high school diploma, 16.6% had a bachelor’s degree, 7.1% had a master’s degree, and 1.0% had a postgraduate degree. Regarding occupation, a vast majority of participants were students (80.9%), 0.3% worked in law enforcement, 2.3% were conceptual employees, 1.3% were executive employees, 1.0% were managers, 0.8% were teachers, 1.5% were workers, 2.5% were practitioners, 1.8% were healthcare professionals, and 7.8% were “other”.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Internet Addiction Test

The Internet Addiction Test (IAT) [35] is a widely used instrument for assessing IA. It consists of 20 items (e.g., “How often do you find that you stay online longer than you intended?”, “How often do you form new relationships with fellow Online users?”) that are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very rarely) to 5 (very frequently). Higher scores indicate higher levels of IA (0–30 = normal range; 31–49 = mildly addicted; 50–79 = moderately addicted; and 80–100 = severely addicted) [87]. The present study applied the Italian version of the IAT, developed by Fioravanti and Casale [88]. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.85.

2.2.2. Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale

The Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS) [40] is a self-report questionnaire pertaining to social media addiction. It is based on the six dimensions proposed by Griffiths [89] (i.e., salience, mood, modification, tolerance, withdrawal conflict, relapse) and consists of six items (e.g., “How often during the last year have you tried to cut down on the use of social media without success?”) that are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very rarely) to 5 (very often). Higher scores indicate a greater risk of developing social media addiction, with the score of 24 recently indicated as the best cut-off score for a possible clinical diagnosis [90]. The present study applied the Italian version of the test, developed by Monacis et al. [91]. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.78.

2.2.3. Fear of Missing Out Scale

The Fear of Missing Out Scale (FoMOs) [49] is a 10-item (e.g., “I get anxious when I do not know what my friends are up to”) self-report questionnaire that measures respondents’ experiences of a pervasive apprehension that “others” are engaged in positive activities and relationships without them. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all true of me) to 5 (extremely true of me). The present study applied the Italian version of the scale, developed by Casale and Fioravanti [92]. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.83.

2.2.4. Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) [93] is a 36-item self-report questionnaire (e.g., “I pay attention to how I feel”, “When I’m upset, I become irritated at myself for feeling that way”) that measures six facets of difficulty regulating emotions (i.e., non-acceptance, goal-directed behavior, impulse control, limited access to effective emotional regulation strategies, lack of emotional awareness, lack of emotional clarity). The present study applied the Italian short version of the scale, proposed by Lausi et al. [94], composed of 20 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). Higher scores indicate greater difficulties in emotional regulation. Cronbach’s alpha was 0.88 for the total scale, 0.89 for non-acceptance, 0.91 for goal-directed behavior, 0.91 for impulse control, 0.86 for lack of emotional clarity, and 0.82 for lack of emotional awareness.

2.2.5. Big Five Inventory-10

The Big Five Inventory-10 (BFI-10) [95] is a self-report questionnaire that evaluates five personality dimensions (i.e., Extroversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, Openness to Experience). Items (e.g., “I see myself as a person that is reserved”, “I see myself as a person that tends to find fault with others”) are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The present study applied the Italian version of the scale, developed by Guido et al. [96]. The reliability of each personality dimension was tested using Spearman–Brown coefficients, indicating: 0.69 for Extroversion, 0.39 for Agreeableness, 0.69 for Conscientiousness, 0.42 for Neuroticism, and 0.71 for Openness to Experience.

2.3. Procedure

Participants were recruited online, and they voluntarily and anonymously responded to the survey, which they accessed through a designated link (Qualtrics Online Platform). Participants indicated their informed consent prior to beginning the survey, and they were free to interrupt or quit the survey at any point, without providing an explanation for doing so. Expedited ethics approval was obtained from the Institutional Board of the Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome (IRB 2193/2020), in accordance with the principles embodied in the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.4. Data Analysis

Pearson’s correlation coefficients (r) were calculated to study the associations between scores on the IAT, BSMAS, FoMOs, DERS, and BFI-10 Neuroticism and Conscientiousness (sub)scales. The mediation model was run using PROCESS version 3.5, as developed by Preacher and Hayes for SPSS, version 26 (IBM, Armonk, NY). PROCESS estimates indirect effects (i.e., mediation) and conditional indirect effects (i.e., moderated mediation) using bootstrap confidence intervals. In the present study, the bias-corrected 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated using 5000 bootstrapping resamples. Effects were considered significant when the resulting confidence interval did not contain 0. A serial mediation model (Model 6) was tested using PROCESS Model Templates, in order to explore whether the association between difficulties in emotional regulation (i.e., DERS) and IA (i.e., IAT) were mediated by social media addiction (i.e., BSMAS) and FoMO (i.e., FoMOs), using BFI-10 Neuroticism and Conscientiousness subscale scores as covariates.

3. Results

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics for the main study measures.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for the IAT, BSMAS, FoMOs, DERS, and BFI-10 Neuroticism and Conscientiousness measures.

M (SD)
n = 397
Median
n = 397
IAT Total 43.47 (9.40) 43.00
BSMAS 12.06 (4.04) 12.00
FoMOs 25.08 (6.48) 25.00
DERS 51.74 (12.13) 52.00
DERS_NA 12.47 (4.98) 12.00
DERS_AW 7.40 (3.09) 7.00
DERS_CL 8.47 (2.80) 8.00
DERS_IM 9.49 (3.88) 9.00
DERS_GO 13.91 (3.95) 14.00
BFI-10
Neuroticism subscale 10.23 (2.65) 10.00
Conscientiousness subscale 12.03 (2.45) 13.00

Note. IAT: Internet Addiction Test; BSMAS: Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale; FoMOs: Fear of Missing Out Scale; DERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; DERS_NA = Nonacceptance dimension of Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; DERS_AW = Awareness dimension of Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; DERS_CL = Clarity dimension of Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; DERS_IM = Impulse dimension of Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; DERS_GO = Goals dimension of Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; BFI-10: Big Five Inventory-10.

Pearson’s correlation analysis (see Table 2) showed that the IAT score was significantly and positively correlated with all variables (i.e., BSMAS, FoMOs, DERS, BFI-10 Neuroticism scores), and negatively correlated with the BFI-10 Conscientiousness score. In more detail, the IAT score showed a strong positive correlation with the BSMAS score; a moderate positive correlation with the FoMOs, DERS, and BFI-10 Neuroticism scores; and a weak negative correlation with the BFI-10 Conscientiousness score.

Table 2.

Correlation coefficients (Pearson’s r) between the IAT, BSMAS, FoMOs, DERS, and BFI-10 Neuroticism and Conscientiousness (n = 397) scores.

IAT BSMAS FoMOs DERS BFI-10 Neuroticism BFI-10 Conscientiousness
IAT - 0.786 ** 0.526 ** 0.444 ** 0.396 ** −0.250 **
BSMAS 0.786 ** - 0.492 ** 0.380 ** 0.371 ** −0.146 **
FoMOs 0.526 ** 0.492 ** - 0.447 ** 0.407 ** −0.152 **
DERS 0.444 ** 0.380 ** 0.447 ** - 0.495 ** −0.273 **
BFI-10 Neuroticism 0.396 ** 0.371 ** 0.407 ** 0.495 ** - −0.174 **
BFI-10 Conscientiousness −0.250 ** −0.146 ** −0.152 ** −0.273 ** −0.174 ** -

Note. ** p < 0.01; IAT: Internet Addiction Test; BSMAS: Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale; FoMOs: Fear of Missing Out Scale; DERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; BFI-10: Big Five Inventory-10.

Furthermore, the correlation analysis highlighted a moderate positive correlation between all variables (i.e., BSMAS, FoMOs, DERS, and BFI-10 Neuroticism scores), except for the BFI-10 Conscientiousness score, which showed a weak negative correlation with all the aforementioned variables.

Subsequently, we tested the prediction concerning the link between difficulties in emotional regulation (i.e., DERS), IA (i.e., IAT), media addiction (i.e., BSMAS), and FoMO (i.e., FoMOs). The total effect of the DERS score on the IAT score (B = 0.230 [SE(HC0) = 0.04] p < 0.001 [CI = 0.1522, 0.3071]) was significant. Furthermore, as shown in Table 3, the indirect effect of the DERS score on the IAT score via the BSMAS and FoMOs scores was positive and significant (B = 0.161), and the bootstrapped 95% CI did not include 0 [0.0990, 0.2250].

Table 3.

Model coefficients for the serial mediation analysis (n = 397).

Predictor BSMAS FoMOs IAT
β (SE HC0) p β (SE HC0) p β (SE HC0) p
Independent variable
DERS 0.014 (0.00) <0.001 0.125 (0.02) <0.001 0.068 (0.03) 0.014
BSMAS - - - - 9.197 (0.44) <0.001
FoMOs - - - - 0.192 (0.05) <0.001
Covariate
BFI-10 Neuroticism 0.061 (0.01) <0.001 0.397 (0.12) <0.001 0.131 (0.12) 0.272
BFI-10 Conscientiousness −0.010 (0.01) 0.455 −0.025 (0.12) 0.828 −0.395 (0.12) 0.002
R 2 0.19 0.34 0.67
F HC0 (df) 34.309 (3393) *** 51.207 (4392) *** 176.752 (5391) ***
Total effect on IAT IAT 95% CI
β (SE) p LL UL
DERS 0.227 (0.039) <0.001 0.1522 0.3071
BFI-10 Neuroticism 0.807 (0.169) <0.001 0.4752 1.1379
BFI-10 Conscientiousness −0.496 (0.172) 0.004 −0.8352 −0.1568
R 2 0.25
F HC0 (df) 52.043 (3393) ***
Bootstrap indirect effects on IAT IAT 95% CI
β (SE) LL UL
Total 0.161 (0.03) 0.0990 0.2250
BMSAS 0.128 (0.03) 0.0721 0.1858
FoMOs 0.024 (0.01) 0.0095 0.0431
BMSAS, FoMOs 0.009 (0.00) 0.0033 0.0166

Note. DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation; BSMAS = Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale; FoMOs = Fear of Missing Out Scale; IAT = Internet Addiction Test. Bootstrap sample size = 5000 (two-tailed); p < 0.001 ***.

In the final model, the BFI-10 Neuroticism and Conscientiousness scores (as covariates) were significantly positively (B = 0.807) and negatively (B = −0.496) associated with the IAT score, respectively (Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Mediation model in which the mediators were considered sequentially. Numbers represent standardized coefficients. Numbers within parentheses represent standardized errors. Note. X = independent variable; Y = dependent variable; M1 = mediator 1; M2 = mediator 2; C1 = Covariate 1; C2 = Covariate 2; ** = p < 0.01; *** = p < 0.001.

The final mediation model explained 25% of the variance in IAT.

4. Discussion

The present study examined the connections between IA and variables related to problematic social media use (e.g., social media addiction, FoMO), as well as the connections between IA and emotional (dys)regulation. Despite the considerable literature on this topic, the research aimed at contributing a deeper analysis of the role of emotional (dys)regulation and the interaction between variables related to internet use in the development of IA.

The results were largely consistent with the predictions. Specifically, the first hypothesis (H1) was confirmed: IA was found to correlate with all the investigated variables, demonstrating a strong and positive relation to social media addiction, FoMO, emotional (dys)regulation, and neuroticism; and a weak and negative relation to conscientiousness. Moreover, all variables were found to significantly relate to each other, supporting the frame of a vicious circle whereby underlying emotional (dys)regulation exacerbates problematic social media use and the pervasive apprehension that others are engaged in exclusive positive activities, resulting in severe IA. These findings seem to align with the pathways hypothesis pertaining to a specific group of behavioral dependents (i.e., problematic and pathological gamblers). This hypothesis was first proposed by Blaszczynski and Nower [97], and subsequently elaborated by Ledgerwood and Petry [98] and Valleur et al. [99]. In their studies, these researchers found that individuals showed poor emotional coping skills and experienced more depression, anxiety, and emotional dysregulation prior to the onset of their addiction, which likely emerged as an attempt to ameliorate these negative symptoms. These findings seem to be in line with the “compensatory internet use” model developed by Kardefelt-Winther [100] in which IA was proposed as a coping strategy and presented from a perspective of compensation instead of compulsive behavior. In particular, the negative life situations experienced by the individual may find, in online activities, a way of escaping from reality to alleviate or ameliorate their dysphoric mood problems. The effectiveness of this model appears to be stronger with specific motivations (e.g., life difficulties, negative affect states, higher levels of stress) that precede problematic outcomes.

Furthermore, impulsivity, together with emotional and personality variables, were found to be associated with addictive behaviors in a sample of adolescents, resulting as vulnerable factors for the development of psychopathological diseases in adults [15]. The presence of impulsiveness alongside a personality characterized by high neuroticism seems to predispose individuals to compulsive behavior. As Griffiths [3] showed, excessive use of the internet is often linked to online social contact (e.g., use of chat rooms). More compulsive behavior may consolidate this relationship, potentially resulting in social media addiction. Indeed, social media addiction is one of the most common forms of IA, involving the compulsive use of social media [4,67]. Additionally, Oberst et al. [57] revealed that immersion in SNs can increase one’s sense of belonging in a very positive way, particularly among socially vulnerable populations. At the same time, increased use of SNs can also exacerbate FoMO. In line with the first hypothesis, the present study found a positive correlation between use of SNs and FoMO, suggesting that participants may have used SNs to fulfil a need for connection or belonging (even through online networks), potentially developing addictive behavior in this respect.

Lastly, conscientiousness emerged as a protective factor against behavioral addictions defined as “unproductive” (e.g., addiction to Facebook, video games, or the internet), while neuroticism emerged as a general risk factor for the development of psychopathology, as underlined in the literature [75,82,101,102].

The second hypothesis (H2) was also confirmed: the mediation model showed a direct effect of emotional (dys)regulation on IA, suggesting that emotional regulation may play an important role in the development of IA. Indeed, studies have shown that individuals with IA—especially adolescents—have greater difficulties with emotional regulation, resulting from excessive suppression and poor cognitive reappraisal [47,103,104]. The excessive suppression of negative emotional experiences involved in emotional dysregulation may strengthen its correlation with IA. In accordance with this, the present study found that deficits in emotional regulation resulted in a stronger correlation between negative emotions and IA. Overall, the findings suggest that difficulties managing negative emotions could predict problematic use of the internet and its features, in line with the study of Amendola et al. [105], which found that dimensions of “non-acceptance” and “goals” predicted problematic Internet use.

Finally, the third hypothesis (H3) was also confirmed: the final mediation model showed the influence of emotional (dys)regulation on FoMO and problematic use of social networks, with a consequent effect on IA. While several studies have investigated the relationship between FoMO and problematic smartphone use [64,106] and social media addiction [59,61], little is known about the relationship between FoMO and emotional (dys)regulation. The present results underline a significant effect of emotional (dys)regulation on FoMO, bringing to light a direct relationship between the constructs, whereby an increase in one may predict an increase in the other. The same direct relationship was observed between emotional (dys)regulation and social media addiction, further highlighting the crucial role played by the ability to manage emotions in the development of behavioral addictions. These findings are aligned with the results of previous studies, which have found a strong association between difficulties regulating emotions and problematic use of SNs [107]. In addition, the significant relationship between these three variables had an overall effect on IA, confirming the direct effect of emotional (dys)regulation, FoMO, and social media addiction on the possible development of pathological behavior related to use of the internet.

5. Conclusions

The results of the present study contribute new insights to the literature on the role of emotional (dys)regulation in the development of pathological Internet behaviors. Plausible mediation pathways were tested, based on theoretical models and research suggesting that emotional (dys)regulation could mediate the development of addictive behaviors. Overall, the results suggest that emotional (dys)regulation could be conceptualized as an independent factor and direct predictor of both IA and variables related to problematic social media use. Further investigations should aim at identifying the specific personality traits and interventions that are effective at decreasing the maladaptive aspects of IA while increasing or enhancing its adaptive aspects.

In summary, mediation was revealed between emotional (dys)regulation, IA, and variables related to problematic internet use. Further research should test models that will further our understanding of the relationship between emotional regulation and its adaptive and maladaptive effects. The results of such investigations may facilitate the development of more suitable approaches to assess and treat IA and evaluate outcomes associated with emotional regulation. Research should also seek to replicate the current findings and further investigate which aspects may influence or be influenced by emotional (dys)regulation (e.g., gender differences), in a variety of populations (e.g., college students vs. non-college-educated adults, young vs. older adults, clinical vs. non-clinical samples). Finally, the causal conjectures of the present study design should be examined and verified via longitudinal or experimental research.

Nevertheless, some limitations of the present study must be considered. First, we were unable to draw causal conclusions or establish the direction of effects, due to the cross-sectional research design. For this reason, it cannot be ruled out that, in some cases, emotional (dys)regulation and the presence of IA may have exerted a mutual influence, or emotional dysregulation may have evolved after the onset of IA. Furthermore, the present study used only self-report measures to assess IA and variables related to problematic social media use; these measures were unable to provide diagnoses, thereby limiting the clinical implications of the results. Furthermore, the study sample may have been affected by selection bias, since the questionnaires were more accessible to certain groups of individuals. Finally, participants’ psychiatric history was not collected, though this data might have influenced the interpretation of the results.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.Q., M.V., A.F., M.M.-V., F.G.-S., A.M.G. and E.M.; methodology, A.Q., S.B., P.R., A.F. and E.M.; software, M.V.; formal analysis, A.Q., S.B., P.R. and E.M.; writing—original draft preparation, A.Q., S.B. and E.M.; writing—review and editing, P.R., M.V., A.F., J.B., G.L., M.M.-V., F.G.-S., A.D.D., A.M.G. and E.M.; supervision, P.R., A.M.G. and E.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Department of Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome (protocol number 2193, 12/18/2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Starcevic V., Aboujaoude E. Internet addiction: Reappraisal of an increasingly inadequate concept. CNS Spectr. 2017;22:7–13. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Starcevic V., Billieux J. Does the construct of Internet addiction reflect a single entity or a spectrum of disorders? Clin. Neuropsychiatry. 2017;14:5–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Griffiths M. Internet Addiction—Time to be Taken Seriously? Addict. Res. 2000;8:413–418. doi: 10.3109/16066350009005587. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Starcevic V. Is Internet addiction a useful concept? Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry. 2013;47:16–19. doi: 10.1177/0004867412461693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Andreassen C., Pallesen S. Social Network Site Addiction—An Overview. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2014;20:4053–4061. doi: 10.2174/13816128113199990616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Müller K.W., Dreier M., Beutel M.E., Duven E., Giralt S., Wölfling K. A hidden type of internet addiction? Intense and addictive use of social networking sites in adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016;55:172–177. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kuss D., Griffiths M. Social Networking Sites and Addiction: Ten Lessons Learned. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14:311. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Peris M., de la Barrera U., Schoeps K., Montoya-Castilla I. Psychological Risk Factors that Predict Social Networking and Internet Addiction in Adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2020;17:4598. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Beard K.W., Wolf E.M. Modification in the Proposed Diagnostic Criteria for Internet Addiction. CyberPsychol. Behav. 2001;4:377–383. doi: 10.1089/109493101300210286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dong G., Lu Q., Zhou H., Zhao X. Impulse inhibition in people with Internet addiction disorder: Electrophysiological evidence from a Go/NoGo study. Neurosci. Lett. 2010;485:138–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Gonzalez N.A. Internet Addiction Disorder and Its Relation to Impulse Control. Texas A&M University-Kingsville; Kingsville, TX, USA: 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Akin A., Iskender M. Internet addiction and depression, anxiety and stress. Int. Online J. Educ. Sci. 2011;3:138–148. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hsieh D.-L., Hsiao T.-C. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity of internet addiction abusers in negative and positive emotional states using film clips stimulation. Biomed. Eng. Online. 2016;15:69. doi: 10.1186/s12938-016-0201-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Longstreet P., Brooks S., Gonzalez E.S. Internet addiction: When the positive emotions are not so positive. Technol. Soc. 2019;57:76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.techsoc.2018.12.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Di Nicola M., Ferri V.R., Moccia L., Panaccione I., Strangio A.M., Tedeschi D., Grandinetti P., Callea A., De-Giorgio F., Martinotti G., et al. Gender Differences and Psychopathological Features Associated With Addictive Behaviors in Adolescents. Front. Psychiatry. 2017;8 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Aguilar de Arcos F., Verdejo-García A., Ceverino A., Montañez-Pareja M., López-Juárez E., Sánchez-Barrera M., López-Jiménez Á., Pérez-García M. Dysregulation of emotional response in current and abstinent heroin users: Negative heightening and positive blunting. Psychopharmacology. 2008;198:159–166. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1110-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Garland E.L., Bryan C.J., Nakamura Y., Froeliger B., Howard M.O. Deficits in autonomic indices of emotion regulation and reward processing associated with prescription opioid use and misuse. Psychopharmacology. 2017;234:621–629. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4494-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Decker S.E., Morie K., Hunkele K., Babuscio T., Carroll K.M. Emotion regulation strategies in individuals with cocaine use disorder maintained on methadone. Am. J. Addict. 2016;25:529–532. doi: 10.1111/ajad.12439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Potenza M.N., Hong K.A., Lacadie C.M., Fulbright R.K., Tuit K.L., Sinha R. Neural Correlates of Stress-Induced and Cue-Induced Drug Craving: Influences of Sex and Cocaine Dependence. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2012;169:406–414. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kim Y., Song H., Seo J., Lee J.-J., Lee J., Kwon D., Yoo D., Lee H.J., Suh K., Chang Y. The differences in neural network activity between methamphetamine abusers and healthy subjects performing an emotion-matching task: Functional MRI study. NMR Biomed. 2011;24:1392–1400. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Uhlmann A., Fouche J.-P., Koen N., Meintjes E.M., Wilson D., Stein D.J. Fronto-temporal alterations and affect regulation in methamphetamine dependence with and without a history of psychosis. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging. 2016;248:30–38. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Le Berre A.-P. Emotional processing and social cognition in alcohol use disorder. Neuropsychology. 2019;33:808–821. doi: 10.1037/neu0000572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Schulte T., Müller-Oehring E.M., Pfefferbaum A., Sullivan E. V Neurocircuitry of emotion and cognition in alcoholism: Contributions from white matter fiber tractography. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2010;12:554–560. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2010.12.4/tschulte. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Engelmann J.M., Gewirtz J.C., Cuthbert B.N. Emotional reactivity to emotional and smoking cues during smoking abstinence: Potentiated startle and P300 suppression. Psychophysiology. 2011;48:1656–1668. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01235.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Versace F., Engelmann J.M., Robinson J.D., Jackson E.F., Green C.E., Lam C.Y., Minnix J.A., Karam-Hage M.A., Brown V.L., Wetter D.W., et al. Prequit fMRI Responses to Pleasant Cues and Cigarette-Related Cues Predict Smoking Cessation Outcome. Nicotine Tob. Res. 2014;16:697–708. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Taylor G.J. The alexithymia construct: Conceptualization, validation, and relationship with basic dimensions of personality. New Trends Exp. Clin. Psychiatry. 1994;10:61–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Marchetti D., Verrocchio M.C., Porcelli P. Gambling Problems and Alexithymia: A Systematic Review. Brain Sci. 2019;9:191. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9080191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rogier G., Velotti P. Conceptualizing gambling disorder with the process model of emotion regulation. J. Behav. Addict. 2018;7:239–251. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schimmenti A., Passanisi A., Caretti V., La Marca L., Granieri A., Iacolino C., Gervasi A.M., Maganuco N.R., Billieux J. Traumatic experiences, alexithymia, and Internet addiction symptoms among late adolescents: A moderated mediation analysis. Addict. Behav. 2017;64:314–320. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Scimeca G., Bruno A., Cava L., Pandolfo G., Muscatello M.R.A., Zoccali R. The Relationship between Alexithymia, Anxiety, Depression, and Internet Addiction Severity in a Sample of Italian High School Students. Sci. World J. 2014;2014:1–8. doi: 10.1155/2014/504376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bonnaire C., Baptista D. Internet gaming disorder in male and female young adults: The role of alexithymia, depression, anxiety and gaming type. Psychiatry Res. 2019;272:521–530. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Mei S., Xu G., Gao T., Ren H., Li J. The relationship between college students’ alexithymia and mobile phone addiction: Testing mediation and moderation effects. BMC Psychiatry. 2018;18:329. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1891-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association; Arlington, VA, USA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.World Health Organization . International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics (11th Revision) World Health Organization; Geneva, Switzerland: 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.YOUNG K.S. Internet Addiction: The Emergence of a New Clinical Disorder. CyberPsychol. Behav. 1998;1:237–244. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, (DSM-IV) Volume 152 American Psychiatric Association; Arlington, VA, USA: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kaess M., Durkee T., Brunner R., Carli V., Parzer P., Wasserman C., Sarchiapone M., Hoven C., Apter A., Balazs J., et al. Pathological Internet use among European adolescents: Psychopathology and self-destructive behaviours. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2014;23:1093–1102. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0562-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Quaglieri A., Mari E., Cordellieri P., Paoli E., Dimarco F., Postiglione M., Nicolasi G., Fontanella T., Guidoni U., Vedovi S., et al. An Exploratory Study in Gambling Recovery Communities: A Comparison Between ‘“Pure”’ and Substance-Abusing Gamblers. J. Gambl. Issues. 2021:18–45. doi: 10.4309/jgi.2021.48.2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wölfling K., Beutel M.E., Koch A., Dickenhorst U., Müller K.W. Comorbid Internet Addiction in Male Clients of Inpatient Addiction Rehabilitation Centers. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2013;201:934–940. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Andreassen C.S., Billieux J., Griffiths M.D., Kuss D.J., Demetrovics Z., Mazzoni E., Pallesen S. The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2016;30:252–262. doi: 10.1037/adb0000160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Chamberlain S.R., Ioannidis K., Grant J.E. The impact of comorbid impulsive/compulsive disorders in problematic Internet use. J. Behav. Addict. 2018;7:269–275. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Jorgenson A.G., Hsiao R.C.-J., Yen C.-F. Internet addiction and other behavioral addictions. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. 2016;25:509–520. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2016.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Sussman C.J., Harper J.M., Stahl J.L., Weigle P. Internet and Video Game Addictions: Diagnosis, Epidemiology, and Neurobiology. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2018;27:307–326. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2017.11.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ioannidis K., Chamberlain S.R., Treder M.S., Kiraly F., Leppink E.W., Redden S.A., Stein D.J., Lochner C., Grant J.E. Problematic internet use (PIU): Associations with the impulsive-compulsive spectrum. An application of machine learning in psychiatry. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2016;83:94–102. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Yen J.-Y., Ko C.-H., Yen C.-F., Wu H.-Y., Yang M.-J. The Comorbid Psychiatric Symptoms of Internet Addiction: Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Depression, Social Phobia, and Hostility. J. Adolesc. Health. 2007;41:93–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bersani F.S., Barchielli B., Ferracuti S., Panno A., Carbone G., Massullo C., Farina B., Corazza O., Prevete E., Tarsitani L., et al. The association of problematic use of social media and online videogames with aggression is mediated by insomnia severity: A cross-sectional study in a sample of 18–24 year-old individuals. Aggress. Behav. 2021 doi: 10.1002/ab.22008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Karaer Y., Akdemir D. Parenting styles, perceived social support and emotion regulation in adolescents with internet addiction. Compr. Psychiatry. 2019;92:22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2019.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Pettorruso M., Valle S., Cavic E., Martinotti G., di Giannantonio M., Grant J.E. Problematic Internet use (PIU), personality profiles and emotion dysregulation in a cohort of young adults: Trajectories from risky behaviors to addiction. Psychiatry Res. 2020;289:113036. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Przybylski A.K., Murayama K., DeHaan C.R., Gladwell V. Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2013;29:1841–1848. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wegmann E., Oberst U., Stodt B., Brand M. Online-specific fear of missing out and Internet-use expectancies contribute to symptoms of Internet-communication disorder. Addict. Behav. Rep. 2017;5:33–42. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2017.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Gil F., del Valle G., Oberst U. Nuevas tecnologías¿ Nuevas patologías? El smartphone y el fear of missing out. Aloma. 2015;33:77–83. doi: 10.51698/aloma.2015.33.2.77-83. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Metin-Orta I. Fear of missing out, internet addiction and their relationship to psychological symptoms. Addicta Turk. J. Addict. 2020;7:67–73. doi: 10.15805/addicta.2020.7.1.0070. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Blackwell D., Leaman C., Tramposch R., Osborne C., Liss M. Extraversion, neuroticism, attachment style and fear of missing out as predictors of social media use and addiction. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2017;116:69–72. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.039. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Dhir A., Yossatorn Y., Kaur P., Chen S. Online social media fatigue and psychological wellbeing—A study of compulsive use, fear of missing out, fatigue, anxiety and depression. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 2018;40:141–152. doi: 10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.01.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Elhai J.D., Gallinari E.F., Rozgonjuk D., Yang H. Depression, anxiety and fear of missing out as correlates of social, non-social and problematic smartphone use. Addict. Behav. 2020;105:106335. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Scalzo A.C., Martinez J.A. Not All Anxiety is the Same: How Different “Types” of Anxiety Uniquely Associate With College Students’ Drinking Intentions. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 2017;58:943–947. doi: 10.1353/csd.2017.0073. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Oberst U., Wegmann E., Stodt B., Brand M., Chamarro A. Negative consequences from heavy social networking in adolescents: The mediating role of fear of missing out. J. Adolesc. 2017;55:51–60. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Błachnio A., Przepiórka A. Facebook intrusion, fear of missing out, narcissism, and life satisfaction: A cross-sectional study. Psychiatry Res. 2018;259:514–519. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Dempsey A.E., O’Brien K.D., Tiamiyu M.F., Elhai J.D. Fear of missing out (FoMO) and rumination mediate relations between social anxiety and problematic Facebook use. Addict. Behav. Rep. 2019;9:100150. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.James T.L., Lowry P.B., Wallace L., Warkentin M. The Effect of Belongingness on Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder in the Use of Online Social Networks. J. Manag. Inf. Syst. 2017;34:560–596. doi: 10.1080/07421222.2017.1334496. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Varchetta M., Fraschetti A., Mari E., Giannini A.M. Adicción a redes sociales, Miedo a perderse experiencias (FOMO) y Vulnerabilidad en línea en estudiantes universitarios. Rev. Digit. Investig. en Docencia Univ. 2020;14:e1187. doi: 10.19083/ridu.2020.1187. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Elhai J.D., Levine J.C., Dvorak R.D., Hall B.J. Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016;63:509–516. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Alt D. College students’ academic motivation, media engagement and fear of missing out. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015;49:111–119. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Clayton R.B., Leshner G., Almond A. The Extended iSelf: The Impact of iPhone Separation on Cognition, Emotion, and Physiology. J. Comput. Commun. 2015;20:119–135. doi: 10.1111/jcc4.12109. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Casale S., Rugai L., Fioravanti G. Exploring the role of positive metacognitions in explaining the association between the fear of missing out and social media addiction. Addict. Behav. 2018;85:83–87. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Franchina V., Vanden Abeele M., van Rooij A., Lo Coco G., De Marez L. Fear of Missing Out as a Predictor of Problematic Social Media Use and Phubbing Behavior among Flemish Adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2018;15:2319. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15102319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Hou Y., Xiong D., Jiang T., Song L., Wang Q. Social media addiction: Its impact, mediation, and intervention. Cyberpsychol. J. Psychosoc. Res. Cybersp. 2019;13 doi: 10.5817/CP2019-1-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Sheldon P., Antony M.G., Sykes B. Predictors of Problematic Social Media Use: Personality and Life-Position Indicators. Psychol. Rep. 2021;124:1110–1133. doi: 10.1177/0033294120934706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kuss D.J., Griffiths M.D. Online Social Networking and Addiction—A Review of the Psychological Literature. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2011;8:3528–3552. doi: 10.3390/ijerph8093528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Marino C., Finos L., Vieno A., Lenzi M., Spada M.M. Objective Facebook behaviour: Differences between problematic and non-problematic users. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017;73:541–546. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.04.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Marino C., Gini G., Vieno A., Spada M.M. A comprehensive meta-analysis on Problematic Facebook Use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018;83:262–277. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.02.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Marino C., Gini G., Angelini F., Vieno A., Spada M.M. Social norms and e-motions in problematic social media use among adolescents. Addict. Behav. Rep. 2020;11:100250. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Buglass S.L., Binder J.F., Betts L.R., Underwood J.D.M. Motivators of online vulnerability: The impact of social network site use and FOMO. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017;66:248–255. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.055. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Slater M.D. Reinforcing Spirals: The Mutual Influence of Media Selectivity and Media Effects and Their Impact on Individual Behavior and Social Identity. Commun. Theory. 2007;17:281–303. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.2007.00296.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Andreassen C.S., Griffiths M.D., Gjertsen S.R., Krossbakken E., Kvam S., Pallesen S. The relationships between behavioral addictions and the five-factor model of personality. J. Behav. Addict. 2013;2:90–99. doi: 10.1556/JBA.2.2013.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Grant J.E., Potenza M.N., Weinstein A., Gorelick D.A. Introduction to Behavioral Addictions. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2010;36:233–241. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2010.491884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Kayiş A.R., Satici S.A., Yilmaz M.F., Şimşek D., Ceyhan E., Bakioğlu F. Big five-personality trait and internet addiction: A meta-analytic review. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016;63:35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Weibel D., Wissmath B., Groner R. Motives for creating a private website and personality of personal homepage owners in terms of extraversion and heuristic orientation. Cyberpsychol. J. Psychosoc. Res. Cybersp. 2010;4:5. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Costa P.T., Jr., McCrae R.R. Four ways five factors are basic. Pers. Individ. Dif. 1992;13:653–665. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90236-I. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Roberts B.W., Jackson J.J., Fayard J.V., Edmonds G., Meints J., Conscientiousness M.L., Hoyle R. Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. Guilford Press; New York, NY, USA: 2009. pp. 369–381. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Wilson K., Fornasier S., White K.M. Psychological predictors of young adults’ use of social networking sites. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2010;13:173–177. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Gnisci A., Perugini M., Pedone R., Di Conza A. Construct validation of the Use, Abuse and Dependence on the Internet inventory. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2011;27:240–247. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Robinson M.D., Ode S., Moeller S.K., Goetz P.W. Neuroticism and affective priming: Evidence for a neuroticism-linked negative schema. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2007;42:1221–1231. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Yang J., Mao Y., Niu Y., Wei D., Wang X., Qiu J. Individual differences in neuroticism personality trait in emotion regulation. J. Affect. Disord. 2020;265:468–474. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Yoon K.L., Maltby J., Joormann J. A pathway from neuroticism to depression: Examining the role of emotion regulation. Anxiety Stress Coping. 2013;26:558–572. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2012.734810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Ehrenberg A., Juckes S., White K.M., Walsh S.P. Personality and Self-Esteem as Predictors of Young People’s Technology Use. CyberPsychology Behav. 2008;11:739–741. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Young K.S. Clinical assessment of Internet-addicted clients. Internet Addict. Handb. Guid. Eval. Treat. 2011;33:19–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Fioravanti G., Casale S. Evaluation of the Psychometric Properties of the Italian Internet Addiction Test. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2015;18:120–128. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Griffiths M. A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. J. Subst. Use. 2005;10:191–197. doi: 10.1080/14659890500114359. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Luo T., Qin L., Cheng L., Wang S., Zhu Z., Xu J., Chen H., Liu Q., Hu M., Tong J., et al. Determination the cut-off point for the Bergen social media addiction (BSMAS): Diagnostic contribution of the six criteria of the components model of addiction for social media disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 2021;10:281–290. doi: 10.1556/2006.2021.00025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Monacis L., De Palo V., Griffiths M.D., Sinatra M. Social networking addiction, attachment style, and validation of the Italian version of the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale. J. Behav. Addict. 2017;6:178–186. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Casale S., Fioravanti G. Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Italian version of the fear of missing out scale in emerging adults and adolescents. Addict. Behav. 2020;102:106179. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Gratz K.L., Roemer L. Multidimensional Assessment of Emotion Regulation and Dysregulation: Development, Factor Structure, and Initial Validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2004;26:41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Lausi G., Quaglieri A., Burrai J., Mari E., Giannini A.M. Development of the DERS-20 among the Italian population: A study for a short form of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Mediterr. J. Clin. Psychol. 2020;8:1–11. doi: 10.6092/2282-1619/mjcp-2511. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Rammstedt B., John O.P. Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. J. Res. Pers. 2007;41:203–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Guido G., Peluso A.M., Capestro M., Miglietta M. An Italian version of the 10-item Big Five Inventory: An application to hedonic and utilitarian shopping values. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2015;76:135–140. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.053. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Blaszczynski A., Nower L. A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction. 2002;97:487–499. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00015.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Ledgerwood D.M., Petry N.M. Subtyping pathological gamblers based on impulsivity, depression, and anxiety. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2010;24:680–688. doi: 10.1037/a0019906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Valleur M., Codina I., Vénisse J.-L., Romo L., Magalon D., Fatséas M., Chéreau-Boudet I., Gorsane M.-A., Guilleux A., Grall-Bronnec M., et al. Towards a Validation of the Three Pathways Model of Pathological Gambling. J. Gambl. Stud. 2016;32:757–771. doi: 10.1007/s10899-015-9545-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Kardefelt-Winther D. A conceptual and methodological critique of internet addiction research: Towards a model of compensatory internet use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014;31:351–354. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Hwang J.Y., Choi J.-S., Gwak A.R., Jung D., Choi S.-W., Lee J., Lee J.-Y., Jung H.Y., Kim D.J. Shared psychological characteristics that are linked to aggression between patients with Internet addiction and those with alcohol dependence. Ann. Gen. Psychiatry. 2014;13:6. doi: 10.1186/1744-859X-13-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Winter K. Individual differences in the experience of emotions. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 1997;17:791–821. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(97)00057-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Hormes J.M., Kearns B., Timko C.A. Craving Facebook? Behavioral addiction to online social networking and its association with emotion regulation deficits. Addiction. 2014;109:2079–2088. doi: 10.1111/add.12713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Liang L., Zhu M., Dai J., Li M., Zheng Y. The Mediating Roles of Emotional Regulation on Negative Emotion and Internet Addiction Among Chinese Adolescents From a Development Perspective. Front. Psychiatry. 2021;12:422. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.608317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Amendola S., Spensieri V., Guidetti V., Cerutti R. The relationship between difficulties in emotion regulation and dysfunctional technology use among adolescents. J. Psychopathol. 2019;25:10–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Lo Coco G., Salerno L., Franchina V., La Tona A., Di Blasi M., Giordano C. Examining bi-directionality between Fear of Missing Out and problematic smartphone use. A two-wave panel study among adolescents. Addict. Behav. 2020;106:106360. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Gioia F., Rega V., Boursier V. Problematic internet use and emotional dysregulation among young people: A literature review. Clin. Neuropsychiatry. 2021;18:41–54. doi: 10.36131/cnfioritieditore20210104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Clinical Medicine are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES