Abstract
‘Pant-hoot displays’ are a species-typical, multi-modal communicative behaviour in chimpanzees in which pant-hoot vocalisations are combined with varied behavioural displays. In both captivity and the wild, individuals commonly incorporate striking or throwing elements of their environment into these displays. In this case study, we present five videos of an unenculturated, captive, adult male chimpanzee combining a large rubber feeding tub with excelsior (wood wool) in a multi-step process, which was then integrated into the subject’s pant-hoot displays as a percussive tool or ‘instrument’. During the construction process, the subject demonstrated an understanding of the relevant properties of these materials, ‘repairing’ the tub to be a more functional drum when necessary. We supplement these videos with a survey of care staff from the study site for additional detail and context. Although care must be taken in generalising data from a single individual, the behaviour reported here hints at three intriguing features of chimpanzee communicative cognition: (1) it suggests a degree of voluntary control over vocal production, (2) it is a so-far unique example of compound tool innovation and use in communicative behaviour and (3) it may represent an example of forward planning in communicative behaviour. Each of these would represent hitherto undocumented dimensions of flexibility in chimpanzee communication, mapping fertile ground for future research.
Subject terms: Psychology, Animal behaviour, Biological anthropology
Introduction
Understanding the cognition underlying animal vocal behaviour not only provides crucial insights into the ecology of a species, but can also provide powerful comparative data for understanding the evolution of human cognition1. For instance, data on primate vocalisations has shed light on features of cognition relevant to language evolution including intentionality2,3, combinatoriality4–10 and semantics11–13. However, the study of vocal behaviour can also have relevance to other domains such as social cognition14–17, memory18, and cultural transmission19–24. Indeed, even observations of a single individual from a species can generate important insights into the cognitive constraints in which a species exists25–30, even if the specific conditions and behaviours in question are not generalisable31,32. Here we present a set of observations of an interesting behavioural innovation in the ‘pant-hoot displays’ of a captive male chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) which provides fresh insights into the complexity of cognition underlying communicative behaviour in chimpanzees.
Pant-hoots are a long-distance vocalisation produced by chimpanzees, typified by an introduction (a series of ‘hoo’-like vocalisations), a build-up (similar to an increasingly rapid panted-grunt), a climax (one or more scream-like vocalisations) and sometimes a ‘let-down’ phase (similar in structure to a build-up but with decreasing pace)33. This behaviour often coincides with other visual cues such as piloerection and a facial expression known as ‘pout face’34. This call type is deployed in a number of contexts for various functions including group coordination35, group movement35, strengthening social bonds36, feeding5,35–37 and dominance displays38. During the production of these calls, wild individuals regularly combine their pant-hoot with behaviours that interact with the caller’s environment, such as drumming on the buttresses of trees39, dragging branches40 and throwing rocks41. Although the function of interacting with these materials is little understood, a reasonable assumption is that they augment the vocal component of the signal—perhaps providing contextual information to receivers42, or simply increasing the saliency of the display38,43,44. Recent work even indicates that the timing and rhythmic properties of buttress drumming may encode information about the identity of the signaller45. Incorporation of such behaviours into pant-hoot displays is also common in captive chimpanzees, where individuals may have a diverse range of natural and artificial objects and materials in their enclosures to take advantage of46.
Here, we present five recorded observations of one captive, group-living, adult male chimpanzee who incorporated multiple objects into a multi-step preparatory phase of their pant-hoot displays (available to watch at: https://osf.io/w68fr/). Specifically, our study subject (‘EH’) combines a large plastic tub (65 cm wide x 25 cm deep) with ‘excelsior’ (i.e. wood wool) into a stack (hereafter a ‘tub-stack’), which EH then strikes before the climax phase of their display. We supplement these observations with further anecdotal data from experts working at the study site (care staff, trainers, and veterinarians) regarding their observation of this and other, similar behaviours at the study site. The finer details and timing of various aspects of the tub-stack behaviour may reveal several interesting features of chimpanzee cognition, including voluntary control over vocal production, innovative material use and forward planning.
Methods
Study individual
Our focal subject was individual ‘EH’, an adult male chimpanzee from the sub-species Pan troglodytes verus (aged 24 years) housed in a group of 8 other chimpanzees (3 male, aged 24–36. 5 female, aged 33–50) housed at the National Center for Chimpanzee Care located at the Michale E. Keeling Center for Comparative Medicine and Research of The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center (UTMDACC) in Bastrop, Texas. The group had access to indoor spaces (two “dens” of 14 m2 each) and an outdoor (400 m2) enclosure with a range of enrichment devices and activities, and a variety of climbing and swinging structures to promote species-typical behaviours, which they were able to move freely between throughout the study period. Biscuits and fresh produce were provided to the chimpanzees two and three times per day respectively, water was available ad libitum and fresh nesting material (excelsior) was provided daily. According to keeper reports, EH was the dominant male at the time of our recordings. EH was born in captivity at another facility and raised by their biological mother, who was not present in the group at the time of the study. EH had participated in numerous behavioural experiments prior to these observations, including two run by SKW47,48, the researcher who recorded the videos described here. These experiments were unrelated to vocal behaviour or drumming, and did not involve any of the materials used in the tub-stack, nor was this behaviour observed during their course.
Video recording and coding
Video footage was recorded using a Sony HDR-CX240E handheld digital video recorder at 25 fps. Videos were viewed and coded using BORIS software (version 7.13.9)49. These observations were recorded during the habituation phase of the experiment described in Watson et al.48. In brief, the group were habituated to predictable sequences of different acoustic tones (audible in the videos attached as supplemental material here), of which their ability to recognise and process would later be tested in a ‘violation of expectation’ paradigm. This involved setting up a speaker in the group’s indoor enclosure for 30 min, twice per day for 5 days and playing the stimuli. SKW was the only human present during these sessions, but the chimpanzee groups were free to come and go. EH was typically alone during these observations (though other individuals are present in Video 4), but the rest of the group were a short distance away through a doorway, well within hearing distance. The tub-stack behaviour was not observed in any of the other 24 subjects from Watson et al.48, and the researchers did not observe any influence of the habituation sounds on vocal or any other behaviours. All videos described here, with their corresponding BORIS project files, coding schema and outputs can be found at the following repository: https://osf.io/w68fr/.
Ethics
Ethical approval for this study was granted by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the UTMDACC, adhering to all the legal requirements, guidelines and regulations of U.S. law and the American Society of Primatologists’ principles for the ethical treatment of nonhuman primates. Regarding our staff survey, participation was voluntary and informed consent was obtained.
Results
Tub-stack observations
The tub-stacking behaviour was directly recorded on video on five occasions (Videos 1–5, Table 1) and observed on at least two other occasions (audio recorded—see videos 6 and 7, Table 1) over 5 days. SKW observed the behaviour on one other occasion 2 weeks after these recordings, while no experiment was ongoing, but this was not recorded and notes were not taken. While there is variation between videos, the broad behaviour of interest is the combination of a rubber ‘tub’ (typically used as a feeding tub, Fig. 1A,B) and ‘excelsior’ (wood wool provided as a bedding substrate, Fig. 1C) into a compound object we refer to as a ‘tub-stack’ (Fig. 1D). Tub-stack construction was either concurrent with or followed by (either immediately or after a short pause) a pant-hoot display, during which the subject produced a pant-hoot and struck the tub-stack at least once. In every observation, the tub was struck directly before the climax (scream) element of the pant-hoot. After striking the tub-stack, the tub itself was typically left in a dented state which rendered it less viable for use as a drum (see Video 5, Fig. 1E). It was therefore often necessary for EH to ‘repair’ the tub before creating the tub-stack: This involved flipping the tub, pushing out the bottom and then flipping it again (as shown in Video 2, see Table 1). We note that EH also frequently performed pant-hoot displays that did not include the tub or excelsior in any capacity (See Videos 8–10 for examples). Moreover, even within the few recorded observations of the tub-stack, it appears there may be interesting variation in behaviour (Table 1). The tub-stack behaviour, therefore, seems to be flexible in its deployment (i.e. whether EH chooses to use it or not) and structure (i.e. whether it includes a repair step, and the timing of vocal onset), rather than being a highly ritualised part of EH’s pant-hoot display behaviour. However, with a limited dataset it is not possible to determine whether this variation is statistically robust.
Table 1.
Annotated events with timestamps for each recording of the tub-stack pant-hoot behaviour, which can be downloaded from https://osf.io/w68fr/.
Video 1 | Video 2 | Video 3 | Video 4 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Time (s) | Behaviour | Time (s) | Behaviour | Time (s) | Behaviour | Time (s) | Behaviour |
5.7 | Pick up tub | 11.27 | Touch tub | 1.7 | Pick up tub | 4.0 | EH leaves view |
7.0 | Put down tub | 12.2 | Pout-face begins | 1.7 | Touch tub | 8.0 | Touch tub (out of view) |
7.5 | Touch tub | 12.4 | Pick up tub | 8.7 | Put down tub | 9.0 | Repair tub (out of view) |
16.4 | Pout-face begins | 13.2 | Repair tub | 8.7 | Pushes tub towards corner | 11.2 | Touch tub (out of view) |
16.7 | Excelsior placement | 16.2 | Put down tub | 9.0 | EH leaves frame | 12.0 | EH enters view |
17.4 | Stop touching tub | 16.2 | Stop touching tub | 11.0 | Excelsior placement (out of view) | 12.7 | Put down tub |
18.4 | Vocalising begins | 17.4 | Excelsior placement | 14.0 | Non-focal enters room | 12.7 | Stop touching tub |
19.7 | Excelsior placement | 19.9 | Excelsior placement | 15.0 | EH enters view | 14.4 | Excelsior handling |
23.1 | Pout-face ends | 20.7 | Touch tub | 16.6 | Touch tub | 15.5 | Vocalising begins |
22.5 | Strike tub | 21.2 | Stop touching tub | 20.1 | Stop touching tub | 17.0 | Excelsior placement |
23.0 | Jump and strike wall | 25 | Excelsior handling | 31.0 | EH leaves view | 17.5 | Strike tub |
24.4 | Vocal climax | 37.7 | Vocalising begins | 33.0 | Possible EH vocalising | 17.7 | Vocal climax |
25.1 | Vocalising ends | 43.2 | Strike tub | 38.0 | Possible EH vocalising | 19.8 | Strike wall |
25.4 | Strike tub | 44.2 | Vocal climax | 41.9 | Non-focal enters room | 20.8 | Vocalising ends |
26.1 | Strike tub | 46.2 | Strike wall | 71.3 | Vocalising begins | ||
26.9 | Strike tub | 46.2 | Pout-face ends | 76.0 | EH enters view | ||
27.9 | Strike tub | 47.1 | Vocalising ends | 76.1 | Strike tub | ||
28.4 | Vocalising begins | 77.3 | Vocal climax | ||||
28.9 | Vocalising ends | 78.6 | Vocalising ends | ||||
28.9 | Strike tub |
Video 5 | Video 6 | Video 7 | General notes | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Time (s) | Behaviour | Time (s) | Behaviour | Time (s) | Behaviour | ||
3.0 | EH leaves view | 02:00 | Excelsior placement | 07:00 | Excelsior placement | Video 2: Clear footage of "repair" behaviour in this video | |
6.0 | Excelsior handling audible | 08:00 | Vocalising begins | 16:00 | Vocalising begins | ||
12.0 | EH enters view | 12:00 | Strikes tub | 21:00 | Strikes tub | Video 3: Uncertain vocaliser at 33 s and 38 s—may be beginning of pant-hoot | |
22.9 | Vocalising begins | 13:00 | Vocal climax | 22:00 | Vocal climax | ||
28.7 | Strike tub (gently) | ||||||
29.2 | Strike tub (gently) | Video 4: Repair behaviour occurs out of frame at approx 8 s | |||||
29.4 | Strike tub (gently) | ||||||
30.2 | Climax | ||||||
30.2 | Vocalising ends | Video 5: Tub is dented and EH does not "repair". May explain use of more gentle strikes in this video. Excelsior displaced at 6.0 is visible to left of tub | |||||
Video 6 and 7: EH is out of frame but audible |
Two additional videos (Videos 6 and 7) are included in the repository in which the tub-stacking behaviour is taking place out of frame—no detailed notes exist for these incidents, but we include them for interested readers. Videos 8, 9 and 10 (not annotated) in the repository depict a ‘typical’ pant-hoot display which does not include the tub-stacking behaviour.
Figure 1.
(A) Face-up tub. (B) Face-down tub. (C) Excelsior. (D) Excelsior placed on tub (‘tub-stack’). (E) Broken tub.
In Table 1 below, we annotate the events of each video. We strongly recommend that readers watch the videos for themselves (https://osf.io/w68fr/) and simply use our annotations as a guide towards moments of interest.
Survey of expert staff at study site
We consulted three expert members of staff from UTMDACC, each with over a decade of experience at the site who had known and worked closely with EH for a collective 45 years. They, therefore, have a deep familiarity with the subject and their behaviour, as well as the other chimpanzees living at the study site. The full answers (minus any identifying information) can be found in our data repository (https://osf.io/w68fr/), which we summarise below.
Have you (or anyone you have observed) ever knowingly rewarded the tub-stacking behaviour?
None of the staff surveyed had observed the tub-stacking behaviour, before or after the date of recordings. Therefore, all agreed that they had never knowingly rewarded the behaviour, nor observed another human do so. This may indicate that the tub-stack behaviour was a recent innovation at the time of recording, and does not seem to have been copied by any other individuals (perhaps because of EH’s position as dominant male). EH was moved to another site in September 2019, 10 months after our tub-stack observations were recorded.
-
(2)
Can you think of any reason a keeper or trainer would have created something the 'tub-stack', or anything like it, in front of the chimpanzees?
The staff surveyed unanimously agreed that there was no reason EH or any other chimpanzee would observe a human creating a tub-stack or similar object. This was almost impossible, because feeding tubs were always either inside the enclosure with the chimpanzees (which humans could not enter) or were out of sight while staff cleaned and restocked enclosures, during which the chimpanzees were locked in a separate area. All also agreed there was no reason staff would create a tub-stack like object for EH to find independently. It is therefore extremely unlikely that EH learned the behaviour by learning it from a human, or by recreating the product of a human.
-
(3)
Have you ever observed the tub-stacking behaviour in another individual at the study site?
All staff agreed that the tub-stacking behaviour had not been observed in any other chimpanzees at the site. This indicates that it was an idiosyncratic behaviour innovated by EH, rather than learned through observation of others.
-
(4)
Have you observed any similar behaviours, perhaps using other objects or materials, in the population?
Although no other individuals at the site were reported to use the tub-stacking behaviour or combine materials in a similar fashion, there were reports of potentially related behavioural forms. Some individuals (including EH) were frequently observed to incorporate large (~ 200 L) empty barrels into their pant-hoot displays (pushing them around and striking or drumming on them), which were often brought to a particular spot in the enclosure before the pant-hoot was initiated. Unlike the tub-stacking behaviour, this did not involve any combination or ‘repair’ of materials. One other individual at the site was reported to strategically choose the noisier of two objects (balls which were either empty or contained noisy rattling objects) to throw at the wall during their displays. Taken together, these behaviours would indicate that sensitivity to the acoustic properties of objects and their corresponding utility to a pant-hoot display is relatively widespread in captive chimpanzees. This is consistent with data from the wild, in which chimpanzees were found to target their drumming behaviour towards tree buttresses with wide, thin buttresses50, and likewise target their stone-throwing behaviour towards trees that produce less damped impact sounds44.
Discussion
Here we have presented a set of observations of one captive, group-living, male chimpanzee who combined multiple materials (a feeding tub and excelsior, Fig. 1) into a single composite object (a ‘tub-stack’) which was then used percussively in EH’s pant-hoot displays. While this behaviour was restricted to a single individual, we believe it can nevertheless shed light on the cognition underlying pant-hoot display behaviour, adding to the growing literature demonstrating flexible control over communicative behaviour in chimpanzees51. More specifically, we argue that the behaviours reported here may illuminate three dimensions of chimpanzee cognition: voluntary control over vocal production, innovative material use in communicative behaviour, and forward planning.
Before exploring the interesting properties of our data, it is important to highlight the limitations inherent to a case study such as this. Foremost is that the limited number of observations makes it susceptible to various sampling biases, meaning that elements which exist in these videos may not be representative of the majority of out-of-sample incidents and likewise, common elements in unobserved examples may not be present here. We therefore hope that readers will join us in interpreting this data with both generosity and caution, using it as an intriguing starting point for generating hypotheses and further work on related topics, while also taking care not to overgeneralise.
Voluntary control over vocal production
Production of vocal behaviour in primates has been argued by some researchers to be strictly governed by internal arousal states, in large part due to the finding that apes are lacking key neural circuitry which is known to be important for the voluntary control of vocal production in humans52–54. However, there is also a growing body of evidence demonstrating hitherto unsuspected flexibility in the vocal behaviour of apes51,55,56, to which EH’s tub-stacking behaviour may contribute a further data point. Specifically, we suggest that because in three out of four observations of a completed tub-stack (Videos 1–4) EH initiated vocal production after the condition of a completed tub-stack is satisfied, this indicates that they possessed some degree of control over the moment of deployment of a pant-hoot display.
It seems unlikely to us that EH has total control over the onset of pant-hoot displays, as it is visible in Videos 1, 2 and 4 that the motivational state underlying pant-hoot display behaviour has its onset before EH has finished building the tub-stack (indicated by the ‘pout’ facial expression that often precedes pant-hoots). However, in only one case (Video 4) did EH begin to vocalise before the tub-stack was complete (Video 4). This likely indicates that EH is able somehow to suppress the onset of the pant-hoot vocalisation itself. We also highlight Video 3, where the pant-hoot does not occur until almost a full minute after the tub-stack is created. In this example, we find it plausible that the onset of the display is under top-down control. However, it is also plausible that EH’s motivational state was simply disrupted by the entrance of their group mates to the chamber (timestamp—00:41) and was later resumed. In this case, this may reflect recent findings that chimpanzee signallers are sensitive to the presence and absence of specific audience members, altering the production and structure of their pant-hoot displays accordingly38. In Table 2 below, we propose and evaluate three alternative, speculative accounts which may explain the observed pattern of behaviours in terms of voluntary control. While account 3 has the least contradictory evidence, none of these accounts seem able to readily explain the behaviour in all videos, and it may therefore be wrong to consider them mutually exclusive. In other words, some episodes of pant-hoot display behaviour may be deployed voluntarily while others may result from non-voluntary arousal states (as humans may laugh reflexively or voluntarily, depending on the specific context).
Table 2.
Three speculative accounts for the incorporation of tub-stacks into pant-hoot displays. The accounts are non-exhaustive, and not mutually exclusive.
Account | Description |
---|---|
Subject possesses top-down control over vocal production | EH is able to produce pant-hoot displays at-will. He is therefore able to deploy the pant-hoot display behaviour at any moment |
In favour: | Long pause between completion of tub-stack and pant-hoot behaviour in Video 3 |
Against: | Overlap of pant-hoot behaviour (e.g. 'pout-face') and tub-stack creation in Videos 1, 2 and 4 suggests the onset of former is not under strict control |
Subject can anticipate future motivational state: | Pant-hoot display production is tied to a particular corresponding motivational state. EH anticipates this impending motivational state and creates tub-stack in preparation |
In favour: | Beginning of tub-stacking behaviour appears to precede pant-hoot related behaviours (e.g. vocalising, pout-face) in all videos |
Against: | The overlap between pant-hoot display state and tub-stack creation in videos 1, 2 and 4 indicates not entirely anticipating the pant-hoot display state. Any anticipation may therefore be very short-term |
Subject can suppress current motivational state | Pant-hoot display production is tied to a corresponding particular motivational state, but it can be suppressed for long enough to create tub-stack |
In favour: | Broad overlap of 'pout-face' with tub-stacking in Videos 1 and 2, but calling never begins until tub-stack is complete |
Against: | Break between tub-stack creation and pant-hoot display in Video 3 indicates lack of overlap in motivational states. But may be explained by EH being distracted by new individuals entering room |
Innovative material use in communicative behaviour
We argue that the tub-stack behaviour represents a case of innovation involving the combination of materials applied to a communicative behaviour. While innovations are not particularly uncommon in chimpanzees, especially captive individuals57,58, the tub-stacking behaviour represents an interesting data point for two reasons.
Firstly, tub-stacking represents a rare case of spontaneous multi-material tool construction (combination of more than one material to create a useful object59) in a non-human ape. Captive chimpanzees and orangutans have also both been found to use sticks to push down and then ‘fish out’ sponge materials to access juice rewards60,61, but these behaviours were heavily scaffolded under experimental conditions. The authors also frame these behaviours as ‘sequential’ tool-use, where separate materials are used one after another, rather than compound tools, where multiple materials are combined into a single object. Multi-material tools are also used by chimpanzees in the wild—for example, combining leaves and moss for ‘leaf-sponging’62, or a rock and piece of wood to act as hammer and anvil for nut-cracking63. However, to our knowledge, the tub-stack represents the first example of a multi-material compound tool used as part of communicative behaviour. Although the function of excelsior in the tub-stacks is unclear, it is ostensibly a purposeful step of the behavioural sequence given that is present in 4 out of 5 videos. One possible function for the excelsior might be that it enhances the visual saliency of the display, as it can be seen to fly into the air when the tub-stack is struck. This may be analogous to the way branches are dragged or thrown by wild chimpanzees during their displays64,65. Another possibility is that the excelsior may mute or otherwise alter the acoustics of the tub in a way EH found pleasing. Alternatively, it may be that the excelsior cushions the impact of the tub on EH’s hands. As we can see from Videos 8–10, EH also strikes hard objects during their non-tub-stack pant-hoot displays (a steel sheet over a concrete wall), but it is possible that the use of excelsior nevertheless improves the comfort of the drumming and allows EH to strike the tub harder than they otherwise would. Multi-modal communication, where compound behaviours serve communicative functions by incorporating (for example) both vocal and gestural elements, has recently gained considerable interest as one means by which apes can maximise the expressive power of their limited repertoires66. Innovative use of tools or materials in this domain may be an additional means by which this communicative flexibility can be augmented.
Secondly, while chimpanzees are highly innovative, very few published examples related to the domain of communicative behaviour: across the 74 innovative behaviours collated in a recent cutting-edge review of chimpanzee innovation, only seven of these were related to communication58. Of these seven, just one involved a tool or object: a male chimpanzee incorporated petrol cans into impressively loud displays, to which the original authors attribute their rise to dominance in the group43 (Notably, EH was also the dominant male in their group). The tub-stack construction we describe is considerably more complex than this example, being comprised of a multi-step, multi-material process which i) typically begins before onset of the display it will be used to complement (likely requiring some degree of control over call production and/or forward planning, see below) and ii) requires an evaluation of the materials at hand, altering them if necessary (i.e. repairing the tub to make it viable as a drum). It is unclear whether the relatively few examples of communicative innovation in the literature are representative of a genuinely skewed distribution of the domains of behaviour in which chimpanzees possess the behavioural and cognitive flexibility to innovate, or instead reflect that innovations in the communicative domain have been overlooked by researchers (Bandini and Harrison’s review58 does not include, for example, the ‘raspberry blowing’ communicative innovation described by Hopkins et al.67). Leaf-clipping, where males strip leaves from branches with their teeth, represents one behaviour observed in the wild which incorporates an object with vocal behaviour when accompanying the preparatory phase of pant-hoot displays68. There is even some evidence that this behaviour may vary between communities in frequency and function, indicating a potential learned component (and hence potentially innovative in origins, rather than species-typical)69, but further work in this area is needed.
While chimpanzees can modulate the acoustic structure of some calls22,23,70,71, they are not vocal production learners72,73, and their gestural repertoire appears to be largely innate74, our observations indicate there may be a rich variety of other ways in which communicative behaviour can be augmented through behavioural flexibility and other innovations. Indeed, some studies have suggested that ‘buttress drumming’ in combination with pant-hoots varies between chimpanzee communities, indicating a possible cultural influence39. Recent work has also found that individuals also flexibly alter the rhythm of their drumming behaviour to provide cues to individual identity during travel behaviour, but not during displays45. We suspect that innovative behaviours in the communicative domain may have been overlooked, and encourage researchers to be forthcoming with any examples they have observed so that a more holistic understanding of behavioural flexibility in chimpanzees can be developed.
Two alternatives to EH innovating the behaviour entirely by themselves also exist: a) that EH learned it from observing a human, or b) that EH learned it from another chimpanzee. While the study site was not under constant surveillance, the results of our survey indicate it is highly unlikely EH ever observed a human interacting with a feeding tub in this way. The tub-stack behaviour has also never been observed in another chimpanzee at the study site, making conspecific social learning unlikely. However, given that EH’s behaviour was also unknown to care staff until our survey, we cannot rule out the possibility that it was more widespread and had simply gone unnoticed. Nevertheless, this would still mean the behaviour was innovated by some chimpanzee at some point, even if it was not by EH. Another possible explanation is that, while care staff at the facility had never observed the tub-stack behaviour before, and therefore could not have rewarded it directly, it is possible that individual elements of the behaviour (e.g. drumming, playing with excelsior) were directly or indirectly rewarded. EH could then have integrated these behaviours into a single sequence constituting the tub-stack. Alternatively, EH could have observed individual elements of the behaviour in other chimpanzees and integrated them into the tub-stack behaviour. However, both of these explanations nevertheless involve some degree of innovation in the combining of behaviours for novel purposes58, even if elements of the behaviour were reinforced or socially learned.
Future planning
The ‘Bischof-Köhler’ hypothesis states that non-human animals are unable to dissociate from their current motivational state, and are therefore unable to anticipate future motivational states and plan for them75–77. More recent work has contradicted this hypothesis, demonstrating instances of forward planning in species including birds78,79, monkeys80,81 and apes82–85 (see Raby and Clayton76 for review). However, in each case, the behaviours identified were related only to foraging and tool-use behaviours rather than communication. The tub-stacking behaviour reported here is therefore particularly interesting because it may represent evidence of (albeit short-term) forward planning in the domain of communicative behaviour: EH intentionally organises their time to prepare an object (the tub-stack) which will be used to augment a future pant-hoot display. It is reasonable to interpret tub-stack construction as an intentional behaviour for several reasons. First, it involves the combination of multiple materials which could not plausibly repeatedly occur by chance. Second, it sometimes involves the transportation of those materials to another location (see Video 3). Third, in most cases, vocalisations do not begin until after the tub-stack is complete. Fourth, effective use of the tub-stack has prerequisites (the tub must be ‘in-tact’) which EH adapts their behaviour to meet if necessary. For example, in Video 1 EH does not need to repair the tub, so does not. In Video 2 it is necessary to repair the dent, so EH does. In Video 5 the tub is dented, and EH does not strike it as powerfully as when an in-tact tub is available. In this last case, it is unclear why EH does not repair the tub—we suggest that either the motivational state underlying their pant-hoot was too far along to suppress for long enough to create the tub-stack, or perhaps this video is unrelated to tub-stack behaviour and EH was simply drumming on a nearby object (which happened to be the tub).
The extensive capacity for mental time travel in humans confers many obvious benefits for problem-solving and coordination of behaviour, but has also been proposed as being a major driving force behind language evolution86 and indeed plays a crucial role in language processing87,88. EH’s behaviour suggests that animal communication may prove to be fertile ground for further examination of this topic, yielding potentially important insights into the evolutionary progression of the interaction of forward planning and communicative behaviour.
Summary
In this report, we present an intriguing set of observations of a non-enculturated, captive male chimpanzee carrying out a multi-material, multi-step ‘preparatory phase’ to their pant-hoot displays. We offer several explanations for this behaviour, each of which would suggest a greater degree of voluntary control over the onset of vocal behaviour than has been typically described in chimpanzees52–54, and may even indicate a capacity for short-term forward planning. Significantly, as far as we know this is also the first recorded example of a chimpanzee engaging in communicative behaviour which makes use of a compound object or tool created from two different materials. Being based on a small number of observations from a single individual, we naturally advise caution in overinterpreting these data or generalising to other individuals, but we hope that this report stimulates further consideration of the ways in which vocal behaviour can interact with domains of cognition typically associated with foraging and tool-use in illuminating ways. Indeed, the carefully reported and interpreted anecdotal accounts of great ape behaviour have proven a rich source of insights into the cognition of these species25,67,89. Questions remain over the extent to which such insights can be generalised to wild populations, and this is likely to be a fertile area for more systematic observation and experimentation in the future.
Ethical approval
The footage reported here was recorded during data collection for Watson et al.48, for which ethical approval was granted by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the UTMDACC, adhering to all the legal requirements of U.S. law and the American Society of Primatologists’ principles for the ethical treatment of nonhuman primates.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We are very grateful to JB and RH at UTMDACC for their insights and stories regarding the behaviour reported here and others at the study site. Thanks to JB for providing the photographs for Fig. 1. SKW is additionally grateful to Adrian Soldati, Mael Leroux, Vesta Eleuteri, Isabel Driscoll, Evelin Pandiamakkal, Piera Filippi, Joseph Mine, Chiara Zulberti, Silvan Spiess, Alexandra Bosshard and Simon Townsend for their thoughtful discussion of the footage described here. Thanks to Marta Manser and Mael Leroux for their feedback on this manuscript. SKW was funded by the NCCR Evolving Language, Swiss National Science Foundation Agreement #51NF40_180888. The chimpanzee colony at UTMDACC is supported by NIH contract 75N97020R0004.
Author contributions
S.K.W. coded the videos and wrote the manuscript. S.J.S. and S.P.L. edited the manuscript and provided logistic support. S.P.L. facilitated the carestaff survey.
Data availability
The dataset described in the current study is available in the Open Science Framework repository at the following link: https://osf.io/w68fr/.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-022-24770-w.
References
- 1.Fedurek P, Slocombe KE. Primate vocal communication: A useful tool for understanding human speech and language evolution? Hum. Biol. 2011;83:153–173. doi: 10.3378/027.083.0202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schel AM, Townsend SW, Machanda Z, Zuberbühler K, Slocombe KE. Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e76674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Townsend SW, et al. Exorcising Grice’s ghost: an empirical approach to studying intentional communication in animals: Intentional communication in animals. Biol. Rev. 2017;92:1427–1433. doi: 10.1111/brv.12289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Leroux M, Townsend SW. Call combinations in great apes and the evolution of syntax. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2020;7:131–139. doi: 10.26451/abc.07.02.07.2020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Leroux M, et al. Chimpanzees combine pant hoots with food calls into larger structures. Anim. Behav. 2021;179:41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.06.026. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Leroux M, Chandia B, Bosshard AB, Zuberbühler K, Townsend SW. Call combinations in chimpanzees: A social tool? Behav. Ecol. 2022 doi: 10.1093/beheco/arac074. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Girard-Buttoz C, et al. Chimpanzees produce diverse vocal sequences with ordered and recombinatorial properties. Commun. Biol. 2022;5:1–15. doi: 10.1038/s42003-022-03350-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Arnold K, Zuberbühler K. Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature. 2006;441:303–303. doi: 10.1038/441303a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K. Anti-predator strategies of free-ranging Campbell’s monkeys. Behaviour. 2009;146:1687–1708. doi: 10.1163/000579509X12469533725585. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Berthet M, et al. Titi monkeys combine alarm calls to create probabilistic meaning. Sci. Adv. 2019;5:eaav3991. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav3991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K. Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:1779–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Zuberbühler K, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman primate. J. Comp. Psychol. 1999;113:33–42. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.33. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Manser MB. Semantic communication in vervet monkeys and other animals. Anim. Behav. 2013;86:491–496. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Martinez L, Matsuzawa T. Auditory–visual intermodal matching based on individual recognition in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Anim. Cogn. 2009;12:71–85. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0269-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K. Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. PNAS. 2007;104:17228–17233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706741104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Slocombe KE, Townsend SW, Zuberbühler K. Wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) distinguish between different scream types: Evidence from a playback study. Anim. Cogn. 2009;12:441–449. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0204-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Borgeaud C, van de Waal E, Bshary R. Third-party ranks knowledge in wild vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus aethiops pygerythrus) PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e58562. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Keenan S, et al. Enduring voice recognition in bonobos. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:22046. doi: 10.1038/srep22046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Crockford C, Herbinger I, Vigilant L, Boesch C. Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology. 2004;110:221–243. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.00968.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Mitani JC, Hasegawa T, Gros-Louis J, Marler P, Byrne R. Dialects in wild chimpanzees? Am. J. Primatol. 1992;27:233–243. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350270402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.de la Torre S, Snowdon CT. Dialects in pygmy marmosets? Population variation in call structure. Am. J. Primatol. 2009;71:333–342. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Watson SK, et al. Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of Chimpanzees. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:495–499. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Watson SK, et al. Reply to Fischer et al. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:R1030–R1031. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wich SA, et al. Call cultures in orang-utans? PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e36180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lameira AR, Shumaker RW. Orangutans show active voicing through a membranophone. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:12289. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-48760-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lameira AR, Hardus ME, Mielke A, Wich SA, Shumaker RW. Vocal fold control beyond the species-specific repertoire in an orang-utan. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:30315. doi: 10.1038/srep30315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stoeger AS, et al. An Asian elephant imitates human speech. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:2144–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Whiten A, Byrne RW. Tactical deception in primates. Behav. Brain Sci. 1988;11:233–244. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00049682. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ralls K, Fiorelli P, Gish S. Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals, Phoca vitulina. Can. J. Zool. 1985;63:1050–1056. doi: 10.1139/z85-157. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Soldati A, et al. The ontogeny of vocal sequences: Insights from a newborn wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) Int. J. Primatol. 2022 doi: 10.1007/s10764-022-00321-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sándor K, Miklósi Á. How to report anecdotal observations? A new approach based on a lesson from “puffin tool use”. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:555487. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.555487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ramsay MS, Teichroeb JA. Anecdotes in primatology: Temporal trends, anthropocentrism, and hierarchies of knowledge. Am. Anthropol. 2019;121:680–693. doi: 10.1111/aman.13295. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Goodall J. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Harvard University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Parr LA, Waller BM, Vick SJ, Bard KA. Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscle action. Emotion. 2007;7:172–181. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Notman H, Rendall D. Contextual variation in chimpanzee pant hoots and its implications for referential communication. Anim. Behav. 2005;70:177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.08.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fedurek P, Donnellan E, Slocombe KE. Social and ecological correlates of long-distance pant hoot calls in male chimpanzees. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2014;68:1345–1355. doi: 10.1007/s00265-014-1745-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Clark AP, Wrangham RW. Chimpanzee arrival pant-hoots: Do they signify food or status? Int. J. Primatol. 1994;15:185–205. doi: 10.1007/BF02735273. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Soldati A, Fedurek P, Dezecache G, Call J, Zuberbühler K. Audience sensitivity in chimpanzee display pant hoots. Anim. Behav. 2022;190:23–40. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2022.05.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Arcadi AC, Robert D, Mugurusi F. A comparison of buttress drumming by male chimpanzees from two populations. Primates. 2004;45:135–139. doi: 10.1007/s10329-003-0070-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bessa J, Biro D, Hockings K. Inter-community behavioural variation confirmed through indirect methods in four neighbouring chimpanzee communities in Cantanhez NP, Guinea-Bissau. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2022;9:211518. doi: 10.1098/rsos.211518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kühl HS, et al. Chimpanzee accumulative stone throwing. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:22219. doi: 10.1038/srep22219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Babiszewska M, Schel AM, Wilke C, Slocombe KE. Social, contextual, and individual factors affecting the occurrence and acoustic structure of drumming bouts in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2015;156:125–134. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kummer H, Goodall J, Weiskrantz L. Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 1985;308:203–214. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1985.0020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kalan AK, et al. Chimpanzees use tree species with a resonant timbre for accumulative stone throwing. Biol. Lett. 2019;15:20190747. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2019.0747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Eleuteri V, et al. The form and function of chimpanzee buttress drumming. Anim. Behav. 2022 doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2022.07.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Videan EN, Fritz J, Schwandt M, Howell S. Neighbor effect: evidence of affiliative and agonistic social contagion in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Am. J. Primatol. 2005;66:131–144. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Watson SK, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Whiten A. Chimpanzees prioritise social information over pre-existing behaviours in a group context but not in dyads. Anim. Cogn. 2018;21:407–418. doi: 10.1007/s10071-018-1178-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Watson SK, et al. Nonadjacent dependency processing in monkeys, apes, and humans. Sci. Adv. 2020;6:eabb0725. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abb0725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Friard O, Gamba M. BORIS: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2016;7:1325–1330. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.12584. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fitzgerald M, Willems EP, Gaspard Soumah A, Matsuzawa T, Koops K. To drum or not to drum: Selectivity in tree buttress drumming by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in the Nimba Mountains, Guinea. Am. J. Primatol. 2022;84:e23382. doi: 10.1002/ajp.23382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Townsend, S. W., Watson, S. K. & Slocombe, K. E. Flexibility in great ape vocal production. in Chimpanzees in Context: A comparative perspective on chimpanzee behavior, cognition, conservation, and welfare (University of Chicago Press, 2020).
- 52.Wheeler BC, Fischer J. Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evol. Anthropol. 2012;21:195–205. doi: 10.1002/evan.21319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Deacon TW. The neural circuitry underlying primate calls and human language. Hum. Evol. 1989;4:367–401. doi: 10.1007/BF02436435. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Jürgens U. The Neural control of vocalization in mammals: A review. J. Voice. 2009;23:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Pisanski K, Cartei V, McGettigan C, Raine J, Reby D. Voice modulation: A window into the origins of human vocal control? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:304–318. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Watson SK, et al. Optionality in animal communication: A novel framework for examining the evolution of arbitrariness. Biol. Rev. 2022 doi: 10.1111/brv.12882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Reader SM, Laland KN. Primate innovation: Sex, age and social rank differences. Int. J. Primatol. 2001;22:787–805. doi: 10.1023/A:1012069500899. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Bandini E, Harrison RA. Innovation in chimpanzees. Biol. Rev. 2020;95:1167–1197. doi: 10.1111/brv.12604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.McGrew WC. Chimpanzee technology. Science. 2010;328:579–580. doi: 10.1126/science.1187921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Harrison RA, van Leeuwen EJC, Whiten A. Chimpanzees’ behavioral flexibility, social tolerance, and use of tool-composites in a progressively challenging foraging problem. iScience. 2021;24:102033. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.102033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Lehner SR, Burkart JM, van Schaik CP. Can captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) be coaxed into cumulative build-up of techniques? J. Comp. Psychol. 2011;125:446–455. doi: 10.1037/a0024413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Sousa C, Biro D, Matsuzawa T. Leaf-tool use for drinking water by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Acquisition patterns and handedness. Anim. Cogn. 2009;12:115–125. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0278-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Carvalho S, Biro D, McGrew WC, Matsuzawa T. Tool-composite reuse in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Archaeologically invisible steps in the technological evolution of early hominins? Anim. Cogn. 2009;12:103–114. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0271-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Nishida T, Zamma K, Matsusaka T, Inaba A, McGrew WC. Chimpanzee Behavior in the Wild: An Audio-Visual Encyclopedia. Springer Science & Business Media; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Sanz CM, Morgan DB. Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. J. Hum. Evol. 2007;52:420–433. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Fröhlich M, Sievers C, Townsend SW, Gruber T, Schaik CP. Multimodal communication and language origins: Integrating gestures and vocalizations. Biol. Rev. 2019;94:1809–1829. doi: 10.1111/brv.12535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hopkins WD, Taglialatela JP, Leavens DA. Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Anim. Behav. 2007;73:281–286. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Watts DP. Tool use by chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Int. J. Primatol. 2008;29:83–94. doi: 10.1007/s10764-007-9227-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Kalan AK, Boesch C. Re-emergence of the leaf clip gesture during an alpha takeover affects variation in male chimpanzee loud calls. PeerJ. 2018;6:e5079. doi: 10.7717/peerj.5079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Fischer J, Wheeler BC, Higham JP. Is there any evidence for vocal learning in chimpanzee food calls? Curr. Biol. 2015;25:R1028–R1029. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Fedurek P, Schel AM, Slocombe KE. The acoustic structure of chimpanzee pant-hooting facilitates chorusing. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2013;67:1781–1789. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1585-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K. Towards a new taxonomy of primate vocal production learning. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 2020;375:20190045. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Desai NP, Fedurek P, Slocombe KE, Wilson ML. Chimpanzee pant-hoots encode individual information more reliably than group differences. Am. J. Primatol. 2022 doi: 10.1002/ajp.23430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Graham KE, Hobaiter C, Ounsley J, Furuichi T, Byrne RW. Bonobo and chimpanzee gestures overlap extensively in meaning. PLoS Biol. 2018;16:e2004825. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2004825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Suddendorf T, Busby J. Mental time travel in animals? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003;7:391–396. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00187-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Raby CR, Clayton NS. Prospective cognition in animals. Behav. Proc. 2009;80:314–324. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Cheke LG, Clayton NS. Mental time travel in animals. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 2010;1:915–930. doi: 10.1002/wcs.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Raby CR, Alexis DM, Dickinson A, Clayton NS. Planning for the future by western scrub-jays. Nature. 2007;445:919–921. doi: 10.1038/nature05575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Kabadayi C, Osvath M. Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science. 2017;357:202–204. doi: 10.1126/science.aam8138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Paxton R, Hampton RR. Tests of planning and the Bischof-Köhler hypothesis in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Behav. Process. 2009;80:238–246. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.12.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.McKenzie T, Cherman T, Bird LR, Naqshbandi M, Roberts WA. Can squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) plan for the future? Studies of temporal myopia in food choice. Anim. Learn. Behav. 2004;32:377–390. doi: 10.3758/BF03196035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Janmaat KRL, Polansky L, Ban SD, Boesch C. Wild chimpanzees plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2014;111:16343–16348. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1407524111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Osvath M, Osvath H. Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: Self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Anim. Cogn. 2008;11:661–674. doi: 10.1007/s10071-008-0157-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Mulcahy NJ, Call J. Apes save tools for future use. Science. 2006;312:1038–1040. doi: 10.1126/science.1125456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.van Hooff JA, Lukkenaar B. Captive chimpanzee takes down a drone: tool use toward a flying object. Primates. 2015;56:289–292. doi: 10.1007/s10329-015-0482-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Suddendorf T, Addis DR, Corballis MC. Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 2009;364:1317–1324. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Ferreira F, Chantavarin S. Integration and prediction in language processing: A synthesis of old and new. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018;27:443–448. doi: 10.1177/0963721418794491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Kuperberg GR, Jaeger TF. What do we mean by prediction in language comprehension? Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;31:32–59. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Wich SA, et al. A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan. Primates. 2009;50:56–64. doi: 10.1007/s10329-008-0117-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The dataset described in the current study is available in the Open Science Framework repository at the following link: https://osf.io/w68fr/.