Abstract
Background
ADHD is associated with multiple adverse outcomes and early identification is important. The present study sets out to identify early markers and developmental characteristics during the first 30 months of life that are associated with ADHD 6 years later.
Methods
9201 participants from the prospective Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort were included. Outcome measures were parent‐rated ADHD symptom scores (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, SDQ) and ADHD diagnosis (Development and Wellbeing Assessment, DAWBA) at age 7. Seventeen putative markers were identified from previous literature and included: pre‐ and peri‐natal risk factors, genetic liability (ADHD polygenic risk scores, PRS), early development, temperament scores and regulatory problems. Associations were examined using regression analysis.
Results
Univariable regression analysis showed that multiple early life factors were associated with future ADHD outcomes, even after controlling for sex and socio‐economic status. In a multivariable linear regression model; temperament activity scores (B = 0.107, CI = 0.083–0.132), vocabulary delay (B = 0.605, CI = 0.211–0.988), fine motor delay (B = 0.693, CI = 0.360–1.025) and ADHD PRS (B = 0.184, CI = 0.074–0.294) were associated with future symptoms (R 2 = 10.7%). In a multivariable logistic regression model, ADHD PRS (OR = 1.39, CI = 1.10–1.77) and temperament activity scores (OR = 1.09, CI = 1.04–1.16) showed association with ADHD diagnosis.
Conclusion
As well as male sex and lower socio‐economic status, high temperament activity levels and motor and speech delays in the first 30 months of life, are associated with childhood ADHD. Intriguingly, given that genetic risk scores are known to explain little of the variance of ADHD outcomes, we found that ADHD PRS added useful predictive information. Future research needs to test whether predictive models incorporating aspects of early development and genetic risk scores are useful for predicting ADHD in clinical practice.
Keywords: ADHD, ALSPAC, developmental delay, early markers, polygenic risk scores, precursors, temperament
Key points.
Identifying early antecedents and markers of ADHD is important to support early detection and intervention.
In a large comprehensive study of children from a population cohort, we identified that ADHD PRS, fine motor delays at 18 months, speech delays and higher temperament activity both at 24 months, showed independent association with ADHD traits at age 7. ADHD PRS and temperament activity were also associated with age 7 ADHD diagnosis.
This study is the first to suggest that ADHD PRS, when combined with clinical and developmental variables, is associated with later ADHD symptoms and diagnosis.
This study potentially helps inform practitioners which children may need more intensive surveillance or prevention strategies to support their later ADHD.
Future research could investigate the specificity and potential causality of the identified markers and antecedents.
INTRODUCTION
Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a highly heritable neurodevelopmental disorder that typically onsets early in life (Thapar & Cooper, 2016). Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder is associated with later educational failure, social and family difficulties, substance misuse, criminality, mental health disorders (Pelham et al., 2007), maltreatment and victimisation (Dinkler et al., 2017).
Although ADHD is often first identified from school‐age, epidemiological surveys suggest that rates in pre‐schoolers are similar to those in older children and adolescents (Egger & Angold, 2006). Also, a growing body of research suggests that the manifestation of core ADHD symptoms and diagnoses may be preceded by earlier risk markers, including infant development, temperament and neural markers (Johnson et al., 2015; Shephard et al., 2022). Identifying early risk markers is important as early detection and intervention could alter adverse developmental trajectories, especially during critical periods of child development, when the brain is rapidly developing and neuroplasticity is highest (Sonuga‐Barke et al., 2011).
A large body of research has focused on early temperament and infant behaviour as antecedents for ADHD (Nigg et al., 2004; Willoughby et al., 2017). Meta‐analyses and prospective cohorts have shown that aspects of temperament such as multiple and persistent regulatory problems during the first months of life (e.g., excessive crying, feeding, and sleeping difficulties), are also associated with later behavioural and ADHD outcomes (Baumann et al., 2019; Hemmi et al., 2011; Schmid & Wolke, 2014).
Studies investigating acquisition of early developmental milestones in children with ADHD indicate poorer motor skills and language development scores in children with, compared to without, ADHD (Arnett et al., 2013; Havmoeller et al., 2019; Lemcke et al., 2016, Sephard et al., 2022), although a recent review (Athanasiadou et al., 2020) suggests that differences in early motor development are non‐specific to ADHD.
Other early infant studies have focused on neonatal and postnatal measures of head circumference and there is some evidence to suggest that microcephaly at birth is associated with an increased risk of ADHD (Aagaard et al., 2018; Ferrer et al., 2019).
ADHD is multifactorial in origin, and early markers include known risk factors as well as infant antecedents. The most well‐established risk factors for ADHD include preterm birth, low birth weight and genetic liability (Sucksdorff et al., 2015; Thapar et al., 2013), others include restricted foetal growth (Murray et al., 2016), younger maternal age (Galéra et al., 2011) and advanced paternal age (D’Onofrio et al., 2014).
Genetic factors contribute to ADHD (Thapar & Cooper, 2016) with genome‐wide association studies (GWAS) suggesting that numerous common genetic variants contribute risk (Demontis et al., 2019). Polygenic risk scores (PRS) represent an individual's estimated total burden of common risk alleles for a particular disorder and ADHD PRS have been shown to be associated with ADHD across clinical and population samples (Stergiakouli et al., 2015).
To our knowledge, no longitudinal studies have undertaken a comprehensive examination of the simultaneous contribution of multiple infant early markers of ADHD and little research has successfully identified clinically informative early predictors. We used a UK population‐based birth cohort and sought to identify early markers during the first 30 months of life associated with ADHD symptom scores or DSM‐IV ADHD diagnosis. Both ADHD symptoms and diagnoses were explored to provide clinically relevant information, whilst also recognising that ADHD behaviours are dimensional, with similar risks associated across the continuum (Thapar & Cooper, 2016). Markers included pre‐perinatal factors (intra‐uterine growth restriction, maternal age at birth, preterm birth, APGAR score and head circumference at birth), early developmental difficulties (including speech and motor delays), infant temperament dimensions of activity and distractibility, regulatory problems (early sleeping, crying, and feeding problems), and ADHD PRS.
METHODS
Sample
We analysed data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), a prospective longitudinal population‐based birth cohort of a representative sample of parents and children (Boyd et al., 2013; Fraser et al., 2013) Our sample included a total of 9201 children with information on ADHD measures at age 7 years. Where families included multiple births (twins), we included only the oldest sibling. Specific details of the sample are provided in the Supporting Information.
MEASURES
Ratings of ADHD at age 7 years
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)
The SDQ is a brief, extensively evaluated behavioural screening questionnaire for children aged 3–16 years, with demonstrated reliability and validity (Goodman, 2001) and was used to provide a continuous, symptom score at age 7 years. The five questions in the hyperactivity/inattention subscale rated on a 3‐point scale are summed to give a total score. Data were available for 8302 children.
Development and Wellbeing Assessment (DAWBA)
The DAWBA structured assessment was sent as a paper questionnaire to carers and teachers when the children were 7 years old and provided information for ADHD diagnoses. The ADHD section assesses inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity, age of onset and impairment. In accordance with the DAWBA protocol, initially the DAWBA computer algorithm ascertained the probability of having ADHD for each participant (Goodman et al., 2011; www.dawba.info). Child psychiatrists then reviewed the data for each participant, to determine a final definitive DSM‐IV diagnosis of ADHD.
DAWBA data were available for 8121 children. Good validity of the diagnoses assigned with the DAWBA has been confirmed (Goodman et al., 2000).
Potential early markers
Genetic data
We used previously calculated PRSs for ADHD, defined using alleles associated at p < .05 in an independent GWAS (Demontis et al., 2019). Details of how the ADHD PRS scores were derived are provided in the Supporting Information. Scores were standardised using Z‐score transformation.
Parental age
These variables were dichotomised to make them clinically relevant. Younger maternal age was defined as less than 20 years and advanced paternal age as above 45 years (D’Onofrio et al., 2014; Galéra et al., 2011).
Perinatal factors
Intra‐uterine Growth Restriction (IUGR), preterm birth and APGAR score at 5 min were obtained from obstetric clinical records. Head circumference (HC) at birth was assessed by trained ALSPAC staff.
Preterm birth was defined as those born at <37 weeks of gestation based on best clinical estimate of expected date of delivery. APGAR score is a measure of vitality of a new‐born infant, routinely calculated by the attending clinician after every birth (Cnattingius et al., 2017), and IUGR is a clinical definition implying a pathological restriction of the growth potential in utero, recorded as present or absent. APGAR score at 5 min after birth was dichotomised to define healthy new‐borns (score of 10) and those with some problems after birth (score of 0–9). Individuals with a head circumference ≤2 SD below the mean for their sex were considered to have microcephaly.
Concerns over vision and hearing
At age 15 months, parents were asked as part of a paper questionnaire if their child had been referred to a Hearing Assessment Centre or Eye Specialist. Where the answer was “yes” for either question children were considered to have early concerns with vision or hearing.
Motor development
Fine and gross motor development were assessed at 18 months old using an adapted version of the Denver Developmental Screening Test (DDST; Frankenburg & Dodds, 1967). Parents completed a questionnaire about their child demonstrating 16 fine motor and 12 gross motor skills rated on a three‐point scale (0 = not yet started, 1 = once or twice, 2 = yes, can do well). Analysed motor scores were age adjusted z‐scores. To identify a clinically relevant category of motor developmental delay, a dichotomous variable was created for each score with motor skills >1 SD z‐score below the mean, indicating the presence or absence of motor delays.
Speech development
Vocabulary and grammar skills were assessed at 24 months old with the ALSPAC adaptation of the MacArthur‐Bates Communicative Development Inventory (MB‐CDI; Feldman et al., 2000). The Vocabulary section of the MB‐CDI contains a checklist of 123 words. Parents rated their child's use of each word on a three‐point scale (2 = child says, 1 = child understands, 0 = neither). Answers were summed to give a Vocabulary score. The Grammar section contains 29 items with questions about basic use of grammar (0 = not yet, 1 = sometimes, 2 = often), use of plurals and past tense (2 = says, 1 = understands, 0 = neither), summed to give a Grammar Score. Vocabulary and grammar scores were dichotomised as well with scores >1SD below the mean, to create the clinically relevant categories of vocabulary and grammar delays.
Temperament
Mothers completed an 89‐item adapted version of the Carey Temperament Scale (CTS; Fullard et al., 1984) with questions about frequency of child behaviour at age 2 years rated on a five‐point scale (1 = almost never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = almost always). 10 Distractibility items (effectiveness of extraneous stimuli in altering the direction of ongoing behaviour) and nine Activity items (level of the motor component in the child's functioning) were summed to give the relevant sub‐scores.
Regulatory problems
Parents completed questionnaires about their child's feeding (at age 24 months), sleeping and crying (at 30 months) habits and these questions were utilised to assess regulatory problems, following a previously used scoring system. The presence of seven sleeping problems and six feeding problems were assessed on a 4‐point scale used to identify the presence of a problem (1–3 vs. 0) and summed to create total scores. A total crying problem score was derived by summing the answers to five items and binary clinically relevant crying, sleeping and feeding variables were defined as >1 SD above the sample mean, in‐line with previous work (Winsper & Wolke, 2014).
Details of how scores were derived prorated measures were used for SDQ Score, Denver motor score, Carey temperament score and regulatory problems score where <50% items were missing.
Covariates
Sex and socioeconomic status (SES) were included as covariates because ADHD is more common in males and in more socially disadvantaged groups (Thapar & Cooper, 2016). SES was based on mother's occupation during pregnancy coded using the Standard Occupational Classification SOC (OPCS, 1991) and dichotomised in‐line with previous work (Eyre et al., 2019; manual and non‐manual skilled occupations, partly skilled occupations and unskilled occupations vs. professional occupations and managerial and technical occupations).
Statistical analyses
Analyses were undertaken using SPSS version 21. Linear and logistic regression analyses were conducted to investigate associations between 17 early markers and ADHD symptoms (assessed using the SDQ) and diagnosis (assessed using the DAWBA), respectively. First, a univariable model analysed each predictor separately (Model 1), then adjusted for sex (Model 2) and SES (Model 3). Variables showing an association in the univariable adjusted model (Model 3), were subsequently included in a fully adjusted multivariable model. We used Bonferroni correction (α = 0.05/number of significant tests) in Model 3, prior to conducting multivariate regression analysis, to correct for multiple testing. Sensitivity analyses, were conducted separately for females and males to evaluate if predictors varied by sex (Tables S2 and S3).
RESULTS
We analysed data from 9201 children (51.3% males). Descriptive statistics for all study variables can be found in Table 1.
TABLE 1.
Descriptive statistics for all study variables
N | Mean (95% CI)/%(N) | |
---|---|---|
Outcome measures | ||
SDQ score | 8302 | 3.39 (SD = 2.36) |
Boys | 4261 | 3.78 (SD = 2.45) |
Girls | 4041 | 2.97 (SD = 2.20), t(8275) = 15.9, p < .001 |
DAWBA ADHD diagnosis | 8121 | Yes = 2.1% (N = 174) |
Boys | 3.5% (N = 147) | |
Girls | 0.7% (N = 27), χ2(1) = 78,8, p = <.001 | |
Demographic factors | ||
Sex | 9201 | Males = 51.3% (4724) |
Maternal age at birth | 8709 | 28.89 (28.79–28.99) |
<20 years | 3.7% | |
Paternal age at birth | 7571 | 30.02 (29.88–30.17) |
>45 years | 2.1% (161) | |
Perinatal factors | ||
Preterm birth | 5578 | 7.3% (406) |
APGAR < 10 | 5388 | 41.6% (2244) |
IUGR | 5617 | 2.8% (160) |
Head circumference | 5934 | 34.86 (34.83–34.90) |
>2 SD below mean | 2.3% (134) | |
Early developmental concerns | ||
Eye/hearing concerns | 8567 | 4.8% (415) |
Gross motor | 8382 | −0.0062 |
>1SD below mean (gross motor delay) | 11.4% (959) | |
Fine motor | 8347 | 0.0189 (−0.002–0.039) |
>1SD below mean (fine motor delay) | 14.6% (1219) | |
Vocabulary | 8461 | 152.5 (151.3–153.7) |
>1SD below mean (vocabulary delay) | 18% (1527) | |
Grammar | 7878 | 19.69 (19.36–20.02) |
>1SD below mean (grammar delay) | 15.2% (1195) | |
Temperament | ||
Activity | 8413 | 23.14 (23.04–23.24) |
Distractibility | 8411 | 24.61 (24.51–24.71) |
Regulatory problems | ||
Feeding difficulties | 8460 | 2.84 (2.79–2.87) |
>1SD above mean | 19.8% (1674) | |
Sleeping problems | 8305 | 3.09 (3.04–3.00) |
>1SD above mean | 14.1% (1172) | |
Excessive crying | 8382 | 0.11 (0.10–0.12) |
>1SD above mean | 6.7% (561) |
There was no evidence of multicollinearity between early markers (see Table S1), therefore all were included in regression models (Ernst & Albers, 2017).
Associations between early markers and ADHD symptom scores
Seven early markers showed association with ADHD scores in univariable analysis when controlling for sex and SES (Table 2, Model 3): ADHD PRS, fine motor delays, vocabulary delays, grammar delays, higher temperament activity, higher temperament distractibility and feeding difficulties (Bonferroni correction threshold p < .004). There was also weaker evidence of associations with younger mothers at birth, IUGR and microcephaly at birth.
TABLE 2.
Associations between ADHD symptoms and early markers and precursors
Predictors | Model 1 (unadjusted) | Model 2 (adjusted by sex) | Model 3 (adjusted by sex and SES) | Model 4, Multivariable (adjusted), N = 3777, R 2 = .107 | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | Unstandard.B (95% CI) | Stand.B | P | N | Unstandard.B (95% CI) | Stand.B | P | N | Unstandard.B (95% CI) | Stand.B | P | Unstandard. B (95% CI) | Stand. B | P | |
ADHD PRS | 5509 | 0.229 (0.167, 0.291) | 0.097 | <.001 | 5509 | 0.230 (0.169, 0.290) | 0.098 | <.001 | 4719 | 0.198 (0.132, 0.264) | 0.084 | <.001 | 0.167 (0.097, 0.238) | 0.072 | <.001 |
Mother <20 years at birth | 7976 | 0.627 (0.342, 0.913) | 0.048 | <.001 | 7976 | 0.633 (0.351, 0.914) | 0.049 | <.001 | 6703 | 0.392 (0.049, 0.735) | 0.027 | .025 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
Father >45 years at birth | 6954 | 0.178 (−0.212, 0.568) | 0.011 | .371 | 6954 | 0.141 (−0.243, 0.525) | 0.009 | .471 | 6028 | 0.087 (−0.32, 0.49) | 0.005 | .674 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
Prematurity | 5047 | 0.302 (0.043, 0.56) | 0.032 | .022 | 5047 | 0.243 (−0.012, 0.497) | 0.026 | .062 | 4230 | 0.252 (−0.03, 0.53) | 0.026 | .081 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
IUGR | 5082 | 0.475 (0.07, 0.881) | 0.032 | .022 | 5082 | 0.497 (0.098, 0.895) | 0.034 | .015 | 4260 | 0.573 (0.140, 1.006) | 0.039 | .009 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
Microcephaly | 5403 | 0.496 (0.076, 0.916) | 0.031 | .021 | 5403 | 0.493 (0.078, 0.908) | 0.031 | .020 | 4498 | 0.408 (−0.062, 0.878) | 0.025 | .089 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
APGAR | 4874 | 0.121 (−0.015, 0.257) | 0.025 | .081 | 4874 | 0.118 (−0.016, 0.252) | 0.024 | .085 | 4077 | 0.098 (−0.047, 0.244) | 0.020 | .186 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
Hear/Vision referral | 7854 | 0.179 (−0.06, 0.422) | 0.016 | .149 | 7854 | 0.156 (−0.084, 0.396) | 0.014 | .203 | 6609 | 0.154 (−0.11, 0.419) | 0.014 | .253 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
Fine motor delay | 7720 | 0.820 (0.675, 0.965) | 0.126 | <.001 | 7720 | 0.769 (0.626, 0.911) | 0.118 | <.001 | 6500 | 0.724 (0.564, 0.884) | 0.108 | <.001 | 0.521 (0.309, 0.734) | 0.075 | <.001 |
Gross motor delay | 7755 | 0.047 (−0.116, 0.211) | 0.006 | .57 | 7755 | 0.078 (−0.083, 0.239) | 0.011 | .343 | 6527 | 0.117 (−0.059, 0.292) | 0.016 | .193 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
Vocabulary delay | 7832 | 0.832 (0.697, 0.968) | 0.135 | <.001 | 7832 | 0.679 (0.543, 0.815) | 0.110 | <.001 | 6576 | 0.593 (0.443, 0.744) | 0.094 | <.001 | 0.349 (0.098, 0.600) | 0.084 | .007 |
Grammar delay | 7306 | 0.473 (0.324, 0.621) | 0.073 | <.001 | 7306 | 0.355 (0.207, 0.502) | 0.054 | <.001 | 6148 | 0.338 (0.178, 0.498) | 0.052 | <.001 | 0.081 (−0.145, 0.306) | 0.012 | .484 |
Activity | 7791 | 0.138 (0.127, 0.149) | 0.266 | <.001 | 7791 | 0.131 (0.120, 0.142) | 0.252 | <.001 | 6549 | 0.134 (0.122, 0.146) | 0.259 | <.001 | 0.123 (0.107, 0.139) | 0.240 | <.001 |
Distractibility | 7790 | 0.020 (0.009, 0.031) | 0.039 | .001 | 7790 | 0.022 (0.011, 0.033) | 0.044 | <.001 | 6549 | 0.028 (0.016, 0.039) | 0.055 | <.001 | 0.005 (−0.010, 0.020) | 0.010 | .535 |
Sleeping >1SD | 7526 | 0.074 (−0.079, 0.227) | 0.011 | .346 | 7526 | 0.080 (−0.071, 0.230) | 0.012 | .31 | 6276 | 0.072 (−0.09, 0.235) | 0.011 | .388 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
Crying >1SD | 7591 | 0.210 (−0.003, 0.423) | 0.022 | .053 | 7591 | 0.228 (0.018, 0.439) | 0.024 | .033 | 6327 | 0.124 (−0.108, 0.356) | 0.013 | .293 | ‐ | ‐ | ‐ |
Feeding >1SD | 7668 | 0.212 (0.079, 0.345) | 0.036 | .002 | 7668 | 0.198 (0.067, 0.329) | 0.033 | .003 | 6390 | 0.207 (0.064, 0.350) | 0.035 | .004 | 0.110 (−0.067, 0.287) | 0.019 | .222 |
Abbreviations: ADHD, Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder; IUGR: Intrauterine Growth Restriction; PRS, Polygenic Risk Scores.
In multivariable adjusted analysis (Model 4), four markers remained associated with ADHD scores: ADHD PRS, fine motor delay, vocabulary delay and higher temperament activity (see Table 2).
Stratified multivariable analysis showed that associations were generally consistent across males and females, except that associations with vocabulary delays were only seen in males (See Table S2).
Associations between early markers and ADHD diagnosis
When controlling for sex and SES, ADHD diagnosis was associated with five early markers (Table 3, Model 3): ADHD PRS, fine motor delay, high temperament activity, vocabulary delay and grammar delay (Bonferroni correction threshold p < .006). There was also weaker evidence of association with gross motor delay. In the multivariable model (Model 4), ADHD PRS and temperamental activity showed strong evidence of association (see Table 3). Due to the limited number of girls with ADHD diagnoses (N = 27), stratified analysis was carried out only for males which were generally consistent with those in the whole sample (Table S3).
TABLE 3.
Associations between ADHD diagnosis and early markers and precursors
Predictors | Model 1 | Model 2 (adjusted by sex) | Model 3 (adjusted by sex and SES) | Model 4, multivariable (adjusted sex & SES) N = 3978 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | OR (95% CI) | P | N | OR (95% CI) | P | N | OR (95% CI) | P | OR (95% CI) | P | |
ADHD PRS | 5491 | 1.35 (1.12, 1.62) | .001 | 5491 | 1.36 (1.13, 1.64) | .001 | 4673 | 1.38 (1.12, 1.70) | .002 | 1.39 (1.09, 1.77) | .006 |
Mother <20 years at birth | 7731 | 2.36 (1.29, 4.32) | .005 | 7731 | 2.41 (1.31, 4.43) | .005 | 6515 | 2.05 (0.97, 4.32) | .057 | ‐ | ‐ |
Father >45 years at birth | 6771 | 0.69 (0.25, 1.9) | .477 | 6771 | 0.66 (0.24, 1.82) | .424 | 5890 | 0.55 (0.19, 1.54) | .257 | ‐ | ‐ |
Prematurity | 4910 | 1.82 (1.04, 3.15) | .033 | 4910 | 1.66 (0.95, 2.9) | .071 | 4116 | 1.77 (0.97, 3.22) | .062 | ‐ | ‐ |
IUGR | 4949 | 1.72 (0.74, 3.98) | .203 | 4949 | 1.81 (0.78, 4.23) | .166 | 4149 | 1.74 (0.69, 4.41) | .240 | ‐ | ‐ |
Microcephaly | 5241 | 0.41 (0.06, 2.99) | .383 | 5241 | 0.392 (0.05, 2.84) | .354 | 4364 | 0.51 (0.07, 3.73) | .507 | ‐ | ‐ |
APGAR | 4749 | 1.08 (0.759, 1.56) | .642 | 4749 | 1.09 (0.76, 1.57) | .615 | 3972 | 1.11 (0.74, 1.65) | .606 | ‐ | ‐ |
Hear/Vision referral | 7621 | 2.16 (1.25, 3.72) | .005 | 7621 | 2.15 (1.24, 3.71) | .006 | 6427 | 1.79 (0.92, 3.48) | .084 | ‐ | ‐ |
Fine motor delay | 7450 | 1.89 (1.29, 2.77) | .001 | 7450 | 1.77 (1.20, 2.59) | .003 | 6289 | 1.84 (1.21, 2.79) | .004 | 1.36 (0.75, 2.48) | .305 |
Gross motor delay | 7476 | 1.54 (0.99, 2.38) | .052 | 7476 | 1.63 (1.05, 2.53) | .029 | 6312 | 1.78 (1.11, 2.87) | .016 | ‐ | ‐ |
Vocabulary delay | 7551 | 2.66 (1.9, 3.7) | <.001 | 7551 | 2.66 (1.90, 3.71) | <.001 | 6354 | 2.19 (1.50, 3.18) | <.001 | 1.72 (0.91, 3.24) | .094 |
Grammar delay | 7041 | 2.23 (1.5, 3.2) | <.001 | 7041 | 1.84 (1.26, 2.69) | .002 | 5940 | 1.88 (1.24, 2.85) | .003 | 1.49 (0.81, 2.74) | .200 |
Activity | 7511 | 1.12 (1.08, 1.16) | <.001 | 7511 | 1.11 (1.07, 1.15) | <.001 | 6331 | 1.12 (1.07, 1.17) | <.001 | 1.09 (1.04, 1.16) | .001 |
Distractibility | 7508 | 1.01 (0.98, 1.05) | .405 | 7508 | 1.01 (0.97, 1.04) | .541 | 6330 | 0.98 (0.94, 1.02) | .394 | ‐ | ‐ |
Sleeping >1SD | 7346 | 1.22 (0.79, 1.86) | .366 | 7346 | 1.21 (0.78, 1.85) | .387 | 6114 | 1.23 (0.76, 1.98) | .392 | ‐ | ‐ |
Crying >1SD | 7412 | 1.35 (0.77, 2.36) | .289 | 7412 | 1.42 (0.81, 2.48) | .219 | 6167 | 1.37 (0.71, 2.65) | .350 | ‐ | ‐ |
Feeding >1SD | 7487 | 1.03 (0.69, 1.54) | .866 | 7487 | 1.01 (0.68, 1.51) | .934 | 6224 | 0.81 (0.50, 1.30) | .387 | ‐ | ‐ |
Abbreviations: ADHD, Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder; IUGR: Intrauterine Growth Restriction; PRS: Polygenic Risk Scores.
DISCUSSION
In this prospective cohort, we observed that ADHD PRS, fine motor delays at 18 months, speech delays and higher temperament activity at 24 months showed independent association with ADHD traits at age seven. ADHD PRS and temperament activity were also associated with subsequent ADHD diagnosis.
The aspects of temperament and regulation analysed here are related to the core features of ADHD and could represent the starting point of a developmental trajectory of dysregulation (Baumann et al., 2019; Schmid & Wolke, 2014). Prospective studies and meta‐analysis (Kostyrka‐Allchorne et al., 2020; Willoughby et al., 2017) found modest associations with later psychopathology, and activity domains were particularly associated with ADHD and externalising disorders. Representing aspects of temperament, infant regulatory problems result in frequent help‐seeking and are easy to identify. Although not observed in this study, previous meta‐analysis showed that regulatory problems had low‐to‐medium effect sizes in predicting ADHD problems, although higher associations were found for excessive crying and highest effect sizes for sleeping problems (Hemmi et al., 2011).
The developmental antecedents found here are in line with those observed in other cohorts such as the national birth cohort in Denmark (Lemcke et al., 2016), in which observations at 18 months of age associated with ADHD were the child not being able to fetch things on request, having speech delays or being significantly more active than average.
The results of our study also suggest that genetic liability indexed as ADHD PRS were associated with subsequent ADHD diagnosis. Many studies have shown that participants with higher ADHD PRS have an increased chance of meeting ADHD diagnostic criteria, higher ADHD symptom levels and ADHD persistence (Riglin et al., 2016; Thapar, 2020). Although PRS are not strong predictors, when combined with clinical predictors they may have clinical utility in risk assessments both in Psychiatry and Child Health as well as for physical health conditions such as cardiovascular disease or cancer (Lewis & Vassos, 2020; Murray et al., 2021).
This study is the first to our knowledge to suggest that ADHD PRS, when combined with clinical and developmental variables, is associated with later ADHD symptoms and diagnosis. Our results may therefore have important practical implications because identifying precursors and antecedents of ADHD, even when not causal, could be important in risk assessments of ADHD, for example, determining follow‐up options for individuals who are at clinical risk of future illness. However, the clinical utility of these results needs evaluation especially as the variance explained by this group of early markers was limited.
As we learn more about the early manifestations of clinical psychopathology, we will also be able to suggest and evaluate preventive intervention approaches targeting some of the early predictors. Moreover, this area of research is particularly relevant for ADHD because, in contrast to Autism spectrum disorder, clinicians lack effective screening tools that help them to distinguish ADHD precursors (Carter et al., 2004).
Methodological strengths and limitations
This study has several strengths, such as a large representative sample and prospectively collected information using well‐validated instruments. Although some variables are based on objective measures or obstetric data, most of the variables were collected through parental report, which may have introduced a measurement error. However, our variables represent data available in the real world and in general, evidence suggests that parent report and directly assessed measures of language and motor skills are significantly correlated (Miller et al., 2017; Torrens & Ruiz, 2021). We looked at dichotomised variables to indicate delay in development which may have decreased the statistical power to detect associations. However, this approach is more in line with clinical practise, whilst results were the same when looking at continuous scores. Furthermore, an adapted version of the Denver developmental scale was used, necessitating dichotomisation of the sample using sample‐specific rather than standardised scores.
The study also has some limitations. The ALSPAC cohort shows attrition (Taylor et al., 2018) necessitating replication. However, we found no differences in rates of ADHD symptoms and diagnosis in those with complete and some missing data (see Table S4 and Figure S1) and although selective attrition can lead to a loss of power and underestimation of the prevalence of psychiatric disorders, it has been shown in the ALSPAC sample that this is less likely to affect the patterns of associations between disorders and risk factors (Wolke et al., 2009). The prevalence of a diagnosis of ADHD was not high in this sample (2.1%) which may have limited our power to find associations with potential precursors. However, this prevalence rate is in line with other diagnostic prevalence rates in UK epidemiological studies using the DAWBA (e.g. NHS Digital, n.d.).
Also, ADHD shows a high level of co‐occurrence with other neurodevelopmental disorders (Thapar & Cooper, 2016) and it is not clear whether the developmental markers we identify are specifically associated with ADHD.
Future research may focus on interaction between predictors, non‐linear interactions and the specificity of early markers. It is also of interest to analyse the persistence of predictors in ADHD diagnosis at an older age or studying if early features represent markers or predictors at the individual level. Future research may also look at the use of the identified early markers and precursors to develop a prediction model for later ADHD. However, the use of prediction models within child and adolescent psychiatry has been shown to have limited utility, in part due to methodological issues such as sample size and replicability, which would have been seen in this study had we developed such a model (Larsson, 2021; Senior et al., 2021).
CONCLUSION
The results of this study suggest that genetic liability indexed by ADHD PRS, aspects of temperament, and early developmental delays act as antecedents and represent early precursors of later ADHD.
This area of research has important clinical implications because it may inform practitioners about which children need more intense surveillance or preventive strategies. The combination of PRS with clinical and developmental variables has potential to contribute to risk assessment and aid clinical decision‐making.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Esther Tobarra‐Sanchez: Conceptualization, Data Curation, Formal Analysis, Funding Acquisition, Methodology, Investigation, Visualization, Writing – Original Draft. Lucy Riglin: Resources, Writing Review and Editing. Sharifah S. Agha: Writing Review and Editing, Supervision. Evie Stergiakouli: Conceptualization, Funding Acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing Review and Editing. Kate Langley: Conceptualization, Funding Acquisition, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project Administration, Supervision, Writing Review and Editing. Anita Thapar: Conceptualization, Funding Acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project Administration, Supervision, Writing Review and Editing.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declares that there is no conflict of interest that could be perceived as prejudicing the impartiality of the research reported.
ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS
Approval obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee.
Supporting information
Supporting information S1
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 217065/Z/19/Z) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. This publication is the work of the authors and Esther Tobarra, Kate Langley and Anita Thapar will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant‐acknowledgements.pdf). This research was funded by Wellcome Trust Institutional Strategic Support Fund awarded by Cardiff University, Grant number AC1130IF03. For the purpose of Open Access, the author has applied a CC BY public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. GWAS data was generated by Sample Logistics and Genotyping Facilities at Wellcome Sanger Institute and LabCorp (Laboratory Corporation of America) using support from 23 and Me.
Tobarra‐Sanchez, E. , Riglin, L. , Agha, S. S. , Stergiakouli, E. , Thapar, A. , & Langley, K. (2022). Preschool development, temperament and genetic liability as early markers of childhood ADHD: A cohort study. JCPP Advances, 2(3), e12099. 10.1002/jcv2.12099
Anita Thapar and Kate Langley contributed equally to this study.
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analysed in this study.
REFERENCES
- Aagaard, K. , Bach, C. C. , Henriksen, T. B. , Larsen, R. T. , & Matthiesen, N. B. (2018). Head circumference at birth and childhood developmental disorders in a nationwide cohort in Denmark. Paediatric & Perinatal Epidemiology, 32(5), 458–466. 10.1111/ppe.12479 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnett, A. B. , Macdonald, B. , & Pennington, B. F. (2013). Cognitive and behavioral indicators of ADHD symptoms prior to school age. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54(12), 1284–1294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Athanasiadou, A. , Buitelaar, J. K. , Brovedani, P. , Chorna, O. , Fulceri, F. , Guzzetta, A. , & Scattoni, M. L. (2020). Early motor signs of attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(7), 903–916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumann, N. , Jaekel, J. , Breeman, L. , Bartmann, P. , Bäuml, J. G. , Avram, M. , Sorg, C. , & Wolke, D. (2019). The association of infant crying, feeding, and sleeping problems and inhibitory control with attention regulation at school age. Infancy, 24(5), 768–786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyd, A. , Golding, J. , Macleod, J. , Lawlor, D. A. , Fraser, A. , Henderson, J. , Molloy, L. , Ness, A. , Ring, S. , & Davey‐Smith, G. (2013). Cohort profile: The ‘children of the 90s’; the index offspring of The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 111–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter, A. S. , Briggs‐Gowan, M. J. , & Davis, N. O. (2004). Assessment of young children’s social‐emotional development and psychopathology: Recent advances and recommendations for practice. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 45(1), 109–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cnattingius, S. , Norman, M. , Granath, F. , Petersson, G. , Stephansson, O. , & Frisell, T. (2017). Apgar score components at 5 minutes: Risks and prediction of neonatal mortality. Paediatric & Perinatal Epidemiology, 31(4), 328–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demontis, D. , Walters, R. K. , Martin, J. , Mattheisen, M. , Als, T. D. , Agerbo, E. , Baldursson, G. , Belliveau, R. , Bybjerg‐Grauholm, J. , Bækvad‐Hansen, M. , Cerrato, F. , Chambert, K. , Churchhouse, C. , Dumont, A. , Eriksson, N. , Gandal, M. , Goldstein, J. I. , Grasby, K. L. , Grove, J. , … Neale, B. M. (2019). Discovery of the first genome‐wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(1), 63–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dinkler, L. , Lundström, S. , Gajwani, R. , Lichtenstein, P. , Gillberg, C. , & Minnis, H. (2017). Maltreatment‐associated neurodevelopmental disorders: A co‐twin control analysis. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 58(6), 691–701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Onofrio, B. M. , Rickert, M. E. , Frans, E. , Kuja‐Halkola, R. , Almqvist, C. , Sjölander, A. , Larsson, H. , & Lichtenstein, P. (2014). Paternal age at childbearing and offspring psychiatric and academic morbidity. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(4), 432–438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egger, H. L. , & Angold, A. (2006). Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 47(3–4), 313–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst, A. F. , & Albers, C. J. (2017). Regression assumptions in clinical psychology research practice‐a systematic review of common misconceptions. PeerJ, 2017(5). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eyre, O. , Hughes, R. A. , Thapar, A. K. , Leibenluft, E. , Stringaris, A. , Davey Smith, G. , Stergiakouli, E. , Collishaw, S. , & Thapar, A. (2019). Childhood neurodevelopmental difficulties and risk of adolescent depression: The role of irritability. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 60(8), 866–874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman, H. M. , Dollaghan, C. A. , Campbell, T. F. , Kurs‐Lasky, M. , Janosky, J. E. , & Paradise, J. L. (2000). Measurement properties of the MacArthur communicative development inventories at ages one and two years. Child Development, 71(2), 310–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrer, M. , García‐Esteban, R. , Iñiguez, C. , Costa, O. , Fernández‐Somoano, A. , Rodríguez‐Delhi, C. , Ibarluzea, J. , Lertxundi, A. , Tonne, C. , Sunyer, J. , & Julvez, J. (2019). Head circumference and child ADHD symptoms and cognitive functioning: Results from a large population‐based cohort study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(3), 377–388. 10.1007/s00787-018-1202-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankenburg, W. K. , & Dodds, J. B. (1967). The Denver developmental screening test. The Journal of Pediatrics, 71(2), 181–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraser, A. , Macdonald‐wallis, C. , Tilling, K. , Boyd, A. , Golding, J. , Davey smith, G. , Henderson, J. , Macleod, J. , Molloy, L. , Ness, A. , Ring, S. , Nelson, S. M. , & Lawlor, D. A. (2013). Cohort profile: The Avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 97–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fullard, W. , Mcdevitt, S. C. , & Carey, W. B. (1984). Assessing temperament in one‐to three‐year‐old children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 9(2), 205–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galéra, C. , Côté, S. M. , Bouvard, M. P. , Pingault, J. B. , Melchior, M. , Michel, G. , Boivin, M. , & Tremblay, R. E. (2011). Early risk factors for hyperactivity‐impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68(12), 1267–1275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337–1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, R. , Ford, T. , Richards, H. , Gatward, R. , & Meltzer, H. (2000). The development and well‐being assessment: Description and initial validation of an Integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(5), 645–655. Cambridge University Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, A. , Heiervang, E. , Collishaw, S. , & Goodman, R. (2011). The “DAWBA bands” as an ordered‐categorical measure of child mental health: Description and validation in British and Norwegian samples. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 46(6), 521–532. 10.1007/s00127-010-0219-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Havmoeller, S. R. , Thomsen, P. H. , & Lemcke, S. (2019). The early motor development in children diagnosed with ADHD: A systematic review. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 11(3), 233–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hemmi, M. H. , Wolke, D. , & Schneider, S. (2011). Associations between problems with crying, sleeping and/or feeding in infancy and long‐term behavioural outcomes in childhood: A meta‐analysis. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 96(7), 622–629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M. H. , Gliga, T. , Jones, E. , & Charman, T. (2015). Annual research review: Infant development, autism, and ADHD – Early pathways to emerging disorders. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 56(3), 228–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kostyrka‐Allchorne, K. , Wass, S. V. , & Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. S. (2020). Research review: Do parent ratings of infant negative emotionality and self‐regulation predict psychopathology in childhood and adolescence? A systematic review and meta‐analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 61(4), 401–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemcke, S. , Parner, E. T. , Bjerrum, M. , Thomsen, P. H. , & Lauritsen, M. B. (2016). Early development in children that are later diagnosed with disorders of attention and activity: A longitudinal study in the Danish national birth cohort. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 25(10), 1055–1066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis, C. M. , & Vassos, E. (2020). Polygenic risk scores: From researc tools to clinical instruments. Genome Medicine, 12(1), 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, L. E. , Perkins, K. A. , Dai, Y. G. , & Fein, D. A. (2017). Comparison of parent report and direct assessment of child skills in toddlers. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 41–42, 57–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray, E. , Pearson, R. , Fernandes, M. , Santos, I. S. , Barros, F. C. , Victora, C. G. , Stein, A. , & Matijasevich, A. (2016). Are fetal growth impairment and preterm birth causally related to child attention problems and ADHD? Evidence from a comparison between high‐income and middle‐income cohorts. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 70(7), 704–709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray, G. K. , Lin, T. , Austin, J. , McGrath, J. J. , Hickie, I. B. , & Wray, N. R. (2021). Could polygenic risk scores be useful in psychiatry?: A review. JAMA Psychiatry, 78(2), 210–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NHS Digital . (n.d.). Mental health of the children and young people in England, 2017 [PAS]. Retrieved April 1, 2022, from https://digital.nhs.uk/data‐and‐information/publications/statistical/mental‐health‐of‐children‐and‐young‐people‐in‐england/2017/2017
- Nigg, J. T. , Goldsmith, H. H. , & Sachek, J. (2004). Temperament and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The development of a multiple pathway model. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 42–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Office of Population Censuses & Surveys . (1991). Standard occupational classification. Her Majesty’s Stationery Office. [Google Scholar]
- Pelham, W. E. , Foster, E. M. , & Robb, J. A. (2007). The economic impact of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(6), 711–727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riglin, L. , Collishaw, S. , Thapar, A. K. , Dalsgaard, S. , Langley, K. , Smith, G. D. , Stergiakouli, E. , Maughan, B. , O’Donovan, M. C. , & Thapar, A. (2016). Association of genetic risk variants with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories in the general population. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(12), 1285–1292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmid, G. , & Wolke, D. (2014). Preschool regulatory problems and attention‐deficit/hyperactivity and cognitive deficits at school age in children born at risk: Different phenotypes of dysregulation? Early Human Development, 90(8), 399–405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senior, M. , Fanshawe, T. , Fazel, M. , & Fazel, S. (2021). Prediction models for child and adolescent mental health: A systematic review of methodology and reporting in recent research. JCPP Advances, 1(3), e12034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shephard, E. , Zuccolo, P. F. , Idrees, I. , Godoy, P. B. G. , Salomone, E. , Ferrante, C. , Sorgato, P. , Catão, L. F. C. C. , Goodwin, A. , Bolton, P. F. , Tye, C. , Groom, M. J. , & Polanczyk, G. v. (2022). Systematic review and meta‐analysis: The science of early‐life precursors and interventions for attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 61(2), 187–226. 10.1016/J.JAAC.2021.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. , Koerting, J. , Smith, E. , Mccann, D. C. , & Thompson, M. (2011). Early detection and intervention for attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 11(4), 557–563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stergiakouli, E. , Martin, J. , Hamshere, M. L. , Langley, K. , Evans, D. M. , St Pourcain, B. , Timpson, N. J. , Owen, M. J. , O’Donovan, M. , Thapar, A. , & Davey Smith, G. (2015). Shared genetic influences between attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) traits in children and clinical ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(4), 322–327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sucksdorff, M. , Lehtonen, L. , Chudal, R. , Suominen, A. , Joelsson, P. , Gissler, M. , & Sourander, A. (2015). Preterm birth and poor fetal growth as risk factors of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 136(3), e599–e608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, A. E. , Jones, H. J. , Sallis, H. , Euesden, J. , Stergiakouli, E. , Davies, N. M. , Zammit, S. , Lawlor, D. A. , Munafò, M. R. , Smith, G. D. , & Tilling, K. (2018). Exploring the association of genetic factors with participation in the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 47(4), 1207–1216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar, A. (2020). Infant neuromotor development: An early indicator of genetic liability for neurodevelopmental disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 87(2), 93–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar, A. , & Cooper, M. (2016). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Lancet, 387(10024), 1240–1250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar, A. , Cooper, M. , Eyre, O. , & Langley, K. (2013). Practitioner review: What have we learnt about the causes of ADHD? The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54(1), 3–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torrens, V. , & Ruiz, C. (2021). Language and communication in preschool children with autism and other developmental disorders. Children, 8(3), 192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willoughby, M. T. , Gottfredson, N. C. , & Stifter, C. A. (2017). Observed temperament from ages 6 to 36 months predicts parent‐ and teacher‐reported attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in first grade. Development and Psychopathology, 29(1), 107–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winsper, C. , & Wolke, D. (2014). Infant and toddler crying, sleeping and feeding problems and trajectories of dysregulated behavior across childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(5), 831–843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolke, D. , Waylen, A. , Samara, M. , Steer, C. , Goodman, R. , Ford, T. , & Lamberts, K. (2009). Selective drop‐out in longitudinal studies and non‐biased prediction of behaviour disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(3), 249–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supporting information S1
Data Availability Statement
Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no new data were created or analysed in this study.