Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Heart Lung. 2023 Feb 18;59:146–156. doi: 10.1016/j.hrtlng.2023.02.002

Evaluating Depressive Symptoms, BDNF Val66Met, and APOE-ε4 as Moderators of Response to Computerized Cognitive Training in Heart Failure

Susan J Pressler a, Miyeon Jung b, Bruno Giordani c, Marita G Titler d, Irmina Gradus-Pizlo e, Kittie Reid Lake f, Kelly L Wierenga g, David G Clark h, Susan M Perkins i, Dean G Smith j, Evelina Mocci k, Susan G Dorsey l
PMCID: PMC10065971  NIHMSID: NIHMS1877969  PMID: 36805256

Abstract

Background:

Depressive symptoms, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met, and apolipoprotein (APOE)-ε4 may moderate response to computerized cognitive training (CCT) interventions among patients with heart failure (HF).

Objectives:

The purpose of this study was to examine moderators of intervention response to CCT over 8 months among patients with HF enrolled in a 3-arm randomized controlled trial. Outcomes were memory, serum BDNF, working memory, instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs), and health-related quality of life (HRQL).

Methods:

256 patients with HF were randomized to CCT, computerized crossword puzzles active control, and usual care control groups for 8 weeks. Data were collected at enrollment, baseline, 10 weeks, and 4 and 8 months. Mixed effects models were computed to evaluate moderators.

Results:

As previously reported, there were no statistically significant group by time effects in outcomes among the 3 groups over 8 months. Tests of moderation indicated that depressive symptoms and presence of BDNF Val66Met and APOE-ε4 were not statistically significant moderators of intervention response in outcomes of delayed recall memory, serum BDNF, working memory, IADLs, and HRQL. In post hoc analysis evaluating baseline global cognitive function, gender, age, and HF severity as moderators, no significant effects were found. HF severity was imbalanced among groups (P = .049) which may have influenced results.

Conclusions:

Studies are needed to elucidate biological mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction in HF and test novel interventions to improve memory, serum BDNF, working memory, IADLs and HRQL. Patients may need to be stratified or randomized by HF severity within intervention trials.

Keywords: heart failure, congestive heart failure, computerized cognitive training, memory, brain-derived neurotrophic factor, health-related quality of life

Introduction

Heart failure (HF) is a serious syndrome that affects approximately 6 million people in the United States1 and 26 million people worldwide.2 It is associated with high mortality rates that range from 15% at 1 year to 53% at 5 years after diagnosis.1 Patients with HF have decreased cardiac output that may lead to inadequate cerebral perfusion,3 cerebral microemboli,4 damage to brain structures with neuronal loss,5 and cognitive dysfunction.6 Cognitive dysfunction occurs in 23% to 80% of patients, depending upon the sample (e.g., HF severity, age) and study characteristics (e.g., control group, screening or neuropsychological test battery).6,7 Memory and working memory are 2 cognitive domains most often impaired in HF. Importantly, cognitive dysfunction is an independent predictor of 12-month all-cause mortality among patients with HF.8 It is associated with decreased ability to perform instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs)9 and worse health-related quality of life (HRQL).10 Few interventions have been tested that directly target improving cognitive function in HF.

Computerized cognitive training (CCT) interventions are designed to increase neurogenesis and neuroplasticity and thereby improve cognitive function. CCT has been found to be successful in improving memory, working memory, and IADLs in community-dwelling older adults11 and older adults with mild cognitive impairment.12 Preliminarily, CCT interventions were found to have efficacy among patients with HF.1317 In pilot studies among 4015 and 2716 patients with HF, patients randomized to CCT using BrainHQ© (developed by PositScience) had improved memory,15 working memory,16 and serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels.16 These studies supported testing CCT in a larger study over a longer period of time.

The “Cognitive Intervention to Improve Memory in Heart Failure Patients” study (MEMOIR-HF) (NR016116; NCT#03035565) was a 3-arm randomized controlled trial to evaluate efficacy of CCT using BrainHQ among 256 patients with HF.18,19 The co-primary outcomes were delayed recall memory and serum BDNF level and the secondary outcomes were working memory, IADLs, and HRQL. The 3 arms were the CCT treatment group, the computerized puzzles active control group, and the usual care group with no computerized intervention (See Methods for details).

In the main intention-to-treat analysis of MEMOIR-HF, delayed recall memory, serum BDNF levels, working memory, IADLs, and HRQL were not statistically significantly different by randomization group or group by time.19 Delayed recall memory, working memory, IADLs, and HRQL significantly improved among patients in all 3 groups over the 8 months. Serum BDNF levels significantly decreased over time. Consistent results were found in the per protocol analyses19 in which patients randomized to CCT who had ≥ 90% adherence were not significantly different in the group and group by time analyses but were significantly different by time. It is plausible that the lack of significant group or group by time differences was because other variables moderated patients’ responsiveness to CCT. In MEMOIR-HF, the pre-specified exploratory aim was to examine 3 potential moderators of CCT’s effect on outcomes, including depressive symptoms, BDNF genotype of the Val66Met polymorphism, and apolipoprotein (APOE)-ε4 allele.18 Moderation analysis was conducted to evaluate the potential boundaries of the CCT intervention, particularly the participants for who the intervention did and did not work.2023

The rationale was twofold for examining depressive symptoms as a moderator. First, depressive symptoms are common in HF and are associated with increased mortality.24 In a systematic review of 26 studies and 80,627 total patients with HF, the overall rate of depression was 29% and it was significantly associated with increased mortality risk (HR 1.4, 95% CI, 1.22–1.60).24 Second, there is a body of empirical literature to support CCT as an intervention to reduce depressive symptoms and depression and improve cognition among people with serious chronic conditions.2529 In a systematic review and meta-analysis of 9 randomized controlled trials conducted to evaluate the efficacy of CCT among adults with depressive disorders, Motter and colleagues25 found that CCT had statistically significant small to moderate effects for reducing depressed mood and improving daily functioning and moderate to large effects for improving measures of attention, working memory, and global functioning. In a systematic review and meta-analysis of 36 RCTs among 2551 people with mild cognitive impairment or dementia (mean age > 60), Chan and colleagues26 evaluated cognitive training effects on depression scores. Participants who received cognitive training interventions had statistically significant decreases in depression scores compared with control groups (i.e., social, recreational, and psychosocial interventions and usual care).26 In the 7 randomized controlled trials testing CCT, effect sizes were medium to large in decreasing depression (mean difference 1.16, 95% CI = −2.13 to −0.91).26

In a meta-analysis conducted to evaluate effects of cognitive remediation on post-treatment depressive symptoms, cognitive functioning, and daily functioning, Legemaat and colleagues27 reviewed 21 studies (438 patients with depression who received cognitive remediation with at least a component of a computerized format and 540 patients in control conditions; randomized and non-randomized trials). Results indicated small effects on depressive symptoms (g = 0.28, 95% CI, 0.09–0.46, I2 40%) and daily functioning (g = 0.22, 95% CI, 0.06–0.39, I2 3%), medium effects on cognitive functioning (g = 0.60, 95% CI, 0.37–0.83, I2 44%), and confounding bias that may have led to overestimated effects for depressive symptoms and daily functioning.27 In a systematic literature review and meta-analysis of 8 controlled studies conducted among 268 adults with depression, Therond and colleagues28 estimated efficacy of cognitive remediation on improving cognitive outcomes (N = 145 received cognitive remediation and 123 control group). Results indicated that cognitive remediation improved global cognition (g = 0.44, p < .0001), verbal memory (g = 0.60, p < .0001), attention/processing speed (g = 0.41, p = .04), working memory (g = 0.32, p = .02), and executive functioning (g = 0.30, p = .02). The authors concluded that results supported cognitive remediation as an intervention to improve global cognition and selected cognitive domains among adults with major depressive disorder.28

Woolf and colleagues29 conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive training among adults with major depressive disorders. The review included 9 studies with sample sizes ranging from 27 to 46 participants. The authors found statistically significantly improved outcomes in cognitive and affective scores with pooled effect sizes that were moderate across studies. They concluded there was strong evidence for cognitive training as a primary therapeutic intervention for treating major depressive disorder.29 However, given the small sample sizes larger clinical trials are warranted among subgroups of adults.

There were 2 rationale for examining BDNF Val66Met and APOE-ε4 as moderators of intervention response in MEMOIR-HF. First, these alleles are associated with increased individuals’ risk of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease.30,31 Second, this increased risk may influence intervention responsiveness to cognitive outcomes.30,31 BDNF is a molecule belonging to the neurotrophin family which has an important role in neurological development and degeneration.3032 Pleiotropic effects of BDNF in the brain are promotion of neuronal growth and survival and regulation of synaptic neuroplasticity, all of which support learning and memory.32,33 Circulating blood levels of BDNF have been used to characterize phenotypes of cognitive impairment among persons with different serious chronic conditions (e.g., schizophrenia,34,35 Alzheimer disease,36 HF37,38).

Circulating BDNF levels have been associated with the severity of Alzheimer disease39 and may be influenced by the single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in the BDNF locus located on chromosome 11.30 One BDNF SNP (rs6265) produces a valine (Val)-to-methionine (Met) substitution in the pro-BDNF protein at codon 66 (Val66Met) and inhibits the production and release of BDNF.32 In past studies, presence of this Met allele was associated with learning and memory disorders among adults living in the U.S. and Australia.33,4042 Approximately 20% of people of European American descent have at least 1 BDNF Met allele (Val/Met or Met/Met genotype), almost 50% of people of East Asian descent have at least 1 Met allele (Val/Met or Met/Met), and 5% of people of African American descent have at least 1 Met allele.43 Thus, the prevalence of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism varies by population genetic structure, ranging from 0% to 72%.44 In a small study among 27 patients with HF living in the U.S., 29.6% had BDNF Val66Met present.16

The APOE-ε4 allele is associated with memory.31,45,46 The APOE gene locus is on Chromosome 19 which encodes the protein product of APOE, apolipoprotein E.46 The APOE gene has 3 common isoforms of ε2, ε3, and ε4. This classification of alleles is from variations in 2 SNPs, rs7412 and rs429358.46 APOE-ε4 is a genetic risk factor for late-onset Alzheimer disease.31,46,47 In a meta-analysis of APOE genotype among persons of White race in 11 countries (U.S., Canada, Australia, Belgium, Finland, France, Germany, Italy, Japan, Netherlands, United Kingdom), presence of 1 APOE-ε4 allele was associated with a 4.3 times greater odds of developing late-onset Alzheimer disease and presence of 2 APOE-ε4 alleles was associated with a 15.6 times greater odds of developing it.48 Inheriting 1 or 2 copies of the APOE-ε4 allele is associated with earlier onset of Alzheimer disease.31,46 The increased risk of late-onset Alzheimer disease has been validated among people with APOE-ε4 alleles across countries with some differences by ethnicity and geographical regions.4951 For example, in a meta-analysis conducted to compare prevalence of APOE genotype among 27,109 persons with Alzheimer disease in 6 regions (Asia; Central, North, and South Mediterranean Europe; North America, South America), the APOE-ε4 prevalence estimates were higher in Northern Europe and lowest in Asia and Southern Europe.49,51 In the general U.S. population, prevalence of APOE-ε4 was 23%.45

Prevalence of the APOE-ε4 allele has been investigated in HF. In 62 patients with HF in the Netherlands, APOE-ε4 prevalence was 16 (33%) and presence of at least 1 allele was an independent predictor of memory loss.52 In 29 patients with HF in the U.S., APOE-ε4 prevalence was 7 (24.1%),37 similar in prevalence to the U.S. general population.

APOE-ε4 has been examined as a moderator of intervention response in past studies. In a classic study evaluating efficacy of donepezil on mild cognitive impairment among 769 adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment, APOE-ε4 was a significant moderator.53 In a study among 50 older adults (50% with APOE-ε4 allele present) who completed a multidomain cognitive training intervention with 30 sessions over 12 weeks, the adults without any APOE-ε4 alleles had significant improvement in sentence comprehension.54 In a quasi-experimental study to improve cognition among 202 adults with mild cognitive impairment, adults were assigned to 3 computerized intervention groups (combined physical and CCT; CCT control, and physical control) and divided by APOE-ε4 presence or absence.55 Interventions were completed over 8 to 12 weeks and follow-up data were collected 1 or 2 weeks after intervention completion. No differences were found in intervention effects among the 3 groups, but APOE-ε4 presence moderated the effect of the combined physical and CCT intervention on task-switching processing speed and the effect of the physical control intervention on working memory. No moderation was found for the CCT control group. Although no significant intervention effects were found in this non-randomized study, authors concluded that the combined physical and CCT interventions and the physical control intervention may be more effective for the subgroup of patients with mild cognitive impairment and APOE-ε4.55

The rationale was provided in prior studies for evaluating gender, global cognitive function, age, and HF severity as moderators of cognitive outcomes.6,56 Global cognitive function as measured by screening questionnaires such as Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) test have been significantly associated with performance on domain-based neuropsychological tests.6,56 Compared with women, men had significantly worse scores on tests of total and delayed recall memory, psychomotor speed, and visuospatial ability than women in a study among 249 patients with HF.6 HF severity was significantly associated with measures of memory, visuospatial ability, executive function, and psychomotor speed.6 There was a significant interaction between HF and measures of executive function with older HF patients with more severe HF having poorer executive function.6

The purpose of the analysis for this aim was to examine level of depressive symptoms, BDNF Val66Met, and APOE-ε4 as moderators of response to CCT associated with improvement in 5 outcomes over 8 months among patients randomized to the 3 groups. The outcomes were delayed recall memory, serum BDNF, working memory, IADLs, and HRQL. Hypotheses were that patients with moderate to severe depressive symptoms, BDNF Val66Met polymorphism present, or APOE-ε4 allele present would be less responsive to the CCT intervention. In post hoc analyses, gender, global cognitive function, age, and HF severity were examined as moderating variables

Methods

Design and Procedures

MEMOIR-HF was a 3-arm randomized controlled trial. The protocol was approved by the university institutional review board and patients provided written informed consent before participation. After consent, patients completed baseline data collection and were randomized to the CCT intervention, the computerized puzzles active control group, or the usual care group. The randomization assignments were stratified by gender and baseline global cognitive function. Patients randomized to CCT were requested to complete training for a total of 40 hours over 8 weeks (5 hours per week × 8 weeks). Patients randomized to the active control group were requested to complete computerized crossword puzzles for 40 hours over 8 weeks (5 hours per week × 8 weeks). Patients randomized to the usual care control group did not complete any specific computerized intervention. Patients in all 3 groups received nurse enhancement interventions that consisted of weekly telephone calls for 8 weeks to provide support and education about the computerized interventions, surveil changes in patients’ clinical condition, and monitor treatment fidelity. Outcomes were measured at baseline and at 10 weeks, 4 months, and 8 months after baseline. These follow-up timepoints were selected based on literature about the time for brain plasticity to emerge. The specific time needed for brain plasticity to develop after CCT is not known, but could take at least 1 month.57,58 Structural integrity of white matter increased after 8 weeks of training among 11 healthy adults.59 Additionally, the follow-up times were selected for evaluation of sustained change in outcomes.60,61 Data collection was completed at locations preferred by patients from February 2017 to November 2020. Research assistants masked to randomization assignment collected the follow-up data.

Sample

The sample was recruited from HF and cardiology clinics in a Midwestern city. Inclusion criteria were: (1) age 21 years and older; (2) English speaking; (3) access to working telephone; (4) able to hear normal conversation; (5) able to see and read computer monitor; (6) Stage C HF validated by echocardiography; (7) New York Heart Association (NYHA) class I, II, and III; and (8) receiving guideline-derived medical therapies. Exclusion criteria were: (1) neurodegenerative condition known to cause memory loss (e.g., Alzheimer disease; Parkinson’s disease); (2) terminal illness; and (3) MoCA test score lower than 19.62

Interventions

The interventions are described in detail in separate publications.15,18,19 The CCT intervention using BrainHQ by Posit Science (https://www.brainhq.com/) was developed to improve brain plasticity, memory, and working memory. This program was designed based on scientific principles of brain plasticity. It is a tailored program that gradually increases in difficulty as the individually progresses through it. Six BrainHQ exercises were used in MEMOIR-HF: (1) Sound Sweeps; (2) Fine Tuning; (3) Memory Grid; (4) Syllable Stacks; (5) To-Do List Training, and (6) In the Know. Patients were provided a laptop computer and if necessary, a mobile internet access card, to complete the intervention at home. Patients were asked to complete the program 5 hours a week for 8 weeks, for a total of 40 hours as recommended by the developers of BrainHQ.63,64

The computerized puzzles active control intervention was designed by the research team members to match the CCT intervention mode by laptop computer and time of 5 hours per week for 8 weeks for a total of 40 hours. Patients randomized to puzzles were provided free puzzles available in the public domain from Crossword Fun (Crossword Fun-Chrome Web Store) at google.com (most recently accessed 12/09/2022) and Bestcrosswords (Free Crossword Puzzles/BestCrosswords.com (most recently accessed 12/09/2022). This intervention was not tailored to individual performance but did allow patients to select the puzzles they wanted to work. Patients were provided a laptop computer and mobile internet access if needed to complete the intervention at home.

The usual care control group did not receive any computerized cognitive interventions from the research team. They continued to receive health care from their multidisciplinary teams.

Patients in all 3 groups received weekly telephone calls to deliver nurse-enhancement interventions over the 8 weeks of the intervention phase.18,19 The rationale for the development of these interventions was threefold. First, there is strong empirical evidence that nurse-led education and support interventions improve HRQL and reduce hospital readmissions among patients with HF.6567 Second, patients with HF experience frequent changes in clinical conditions that may interfere with intervention uptake.68 Third, treatment fidelity is necessary to ensure intervention performance and evaluate efficacy.69 Therefore, the nurse-enhancement intervention had 4 core elements: (1) provision of support and education for the CCT intervention and puzzles groups; (2) assessment of patients’ health status; (3) surveillance of changes in clinical condition; and (4) monitoring treatment fidelity. The nurse enhancement intervention was developed by nurse researchers with graduate nursing degrees and tested in the preliminary MEMOIR studies15,16 Intervenors received training from study investigators that included education and practice of intervention delivery prior to beginning in the intervenor role. In MEMOIR-HF, a co-investigator (MJ) supervised delivery of the nurse enhancement interventions for the entire length of the study. Intervenors were not masked to group assignment. The weekly intervenors remained the same for individual patients as much as possible except when scheduling in personnel required a change. Standardized scripts were used to guide the weekly telephone calls to maintain treatment fidelity. During the weekly telephone calls, patients randomized to the CCT and puzzles groups were asked about intervention performance time, health status, changes in clinical condition, and other study issues over the past week. Patients randomized to the usual care group were asked about health status and changes in clinical condition. The intervenors made additional telephone calls and home visits to patients who needed more assistance performing interventions.

Intervention adherence was measured in the CCT group by the BrainHQ program time tracker and patient written self-report on calendar time sheets. Intervention adherence was measured in the puzzles group by patient written self-report on calendar time sheets.

Measures

Delayed recall memory was measured by the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised (HVLT-R).70 This test was selected because it measures one of the cognitive domains most often impaired among people with vascular cognitive disorders (delayed recall memory) and is commonly used in neuropsychological examinations for people with these disorders. The HVLT-R is a test of delayed recall in which the patient is requested to remember 12 words repeated over 3 trials, after 20 minutes. Possible scores range from 0 to 12 and higher scores indicate better delayed recall memory. Validity and reliability have been supported70 and the test was sensitive to CCT training in a preliminary study.15

Serum BDNF levels were measured using commercially available ELISA (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN). Levels were obtained at the enrollment visit and at 10 weeks, 4 months, and 8 months. Batches with duplicates were obtained for each patient. The limit of detection for the serum BDNF was 20 pg/mL and no samples were below this limit.71

Working memory was measured by the CogState One Back Accuracy task.72 This test was selected because it measures a cognitive domain often impaired among people with vascular cognitive disorders (working memory) and is used in neuropsychological examinations for people with this type of disorder. This task is a computerized neuropsychological test on which the patient is asked to use playing cards as the stimulus for response. As recommended by developers of the task, scores used were the arcsine of the patient’s correct responses. Higher scores indicate better working memory performance. Validity and reliability have been supported and the test was sensitive to CCT in a preliminary study.16

IADLs were measured using the Everyday Problems Test for Cognitively Challenged Elderly.73 It is a 16-item performance-based test with 2 questions for each item. Possible scores range from 0 to 32. Higher scores indicate better IADLs performance. Validity and reliability have been supported in past studies.73

HRQL was measured using the Minnesota Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire, a 21-item questionnaire with 6-point response scales.74 The Questionnaire was designed to evaluate the impact of physical and emotional symptoms on the patient’s ability to live as wanted. Possible total scores range from 0 to 105, with higher scores indicating poorer HRQL. Validity and reliability have been supported.75

Depressive symptoms were measured using the Patient Health Questionnaire-8 (PHQ-8),76 an 8-item questionnaire with 4-point response scales (0 = “Not at all” to 3 = “Nearly every day”). Item scores are summed to obtain the total score that may range from 0 to 24. Higher scores indicate current more or more severe depressive symptoms. Cutoff scores have been validated for level of depression severity as follows: 0 – 4, minimal; 5 – 9, mild; 10 – 14, moderate; 15 – 19 moderately severe; 20 – 27, severe.76 In the planned analysis, the PHQ-8 scores were categorized into 2 groups: 0 – 9, minimal to mild; 10 – 24, moderate to severe. The PHQ-8 has documented validity and reliability among patients with HF.77

The BDNF and APOE candidate gene analyses were completed at laboratories at Indiana University and University of Maryland. The DNA was extracted from samples of whole blood obtained at the enrollment or baseline visit using an AutoGen DNA isolation kit (Qiagen, Venlo, Netherlands). The BDNF and APOE +/− 10 KB on either side were sequenced on the Twist Bioscience at Indiana University generating 150 bp paired-end reads. Quality of raw sequences was assessed by FastQC78 software. The reads were then aligned to the reference human genome (GRCh38) using Burrows-Wheeler Aligner software (version 0.7.12).79 Variants including single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and indels were identified using the algorithm HaplotypeCaller implemented in Genome Analysis Toolkit (GATK) (version 4.1.9).80 Integrative Genomic Viewer software (IGV) was used for variants visualization. The variant cell format file from GATK was annotated using SnpEff (version 5.0).81 The quality of the variants identified by GATK is expressed using Phred scale, whose value can range from 0 to infinity. The higher the value, the higher the accuracy and the lower is the error. Variants whose Phred was lower than 30, which corresponds to 99.9% accuracy versus a 0.1% chance of error. The 3 variants analyzed in this study (rs6265 Phred: 7878.64; rs429358 Phred: 4887.64; and rs7412 Phred: 9697.64) have a Phred value ranging from ~ 5,000 to ~ 10,000 corresponding to a very poor chance of error.

The BDNF variant of interest was the Val66Met polymorphism rs6265 which is a substitution of methionine at Codon 66.82 Patients were categorized into 2 groups: Met polymorphism absent (Val/Val) or present (Val/Met or Met/Met). Two SNPs, rs429358 and rs7412, were used to characterize the alleles of APOE.83 Patients were categorized into 2 groups: APOE-ε4 absent or present in at least 1 allele.

Demographic and clinical variables were obtained from patients’ interviews and medical records. Gender and age were obtained by patient self-report. Global cognitive function was measured by MoCA test at enrollment.62 HF severity was assessed using patients’ NYHA class documented by cardiologists at the clinic visits immediately prior to or on the day of study enrollment.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive statistics were computed to compare baseline equivalencies of demographic, clinical, and moderator variables among the 3 groups of patients. Mixed effects models84 were fit to test the moderation hypotheses (separately for each moderator) for the 5 outcome measures collected at baseline, 10 weeks, and 4 and 8 months. The main effects for group and the two-way interactions between group and the moderators were not included in the models to enforce the equal group mean assumption at baseline, given the RCT design.84 Stratifying variables (gender and MoCA) were controlled prior to testing the effects. Time, group-by-time interactions, moderator-by-time interactions, and the 3-way group-by-time-by-moderator interactions were included in the mixed models with an unstructured covariance matrix. The overall F-tests were computed for the 3-way interactions using F-tests for contrasts within the mixed effects models. In addition, comparisons were conducted among the 3 group means at each post-randomization time at each level of the moderators using F-tests for contrasts within the mixed effects models. Analyses were completed using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). The significance level was set at alpha < .05 for the overall F-tests. A Bonferroni-adjusted alpha of 0.05/6 = 0.008 was used when comparing groups within each post-randomization time and level of moderator (3 × 2 = 6 tests).

Post hoc analyses were conducted to examine 4 additional potential moderators and be hypothesis generating for future research. The 2 stratification variables of gender and global cognitive function at enrollment (MoCA scores normal 26–30 and low 19–25)62 were evaluated as moderators in separate analyses. Age was evaluated as a moderator using the baseline median age of 67.5 years as the cutpoint. HF severity was evaluated as a moderator using NYHA classes 1 and 2 compared with NYHA class 3.

The COVID-19 pandemic began in March 2020 (year 4, month 9 of MEMOIR-HF) while data collection was ongoing. All face-to-face research interviews were modified to telephone interviews to adhere to university requirements and prevent COVID-19 transmission. At the 8-month interviews, 24.5% of the data were missing for working memory CogState One Back Accuracy, IADLs Everyday Problems Test, and serum BDNF because of COVID-19 restrictions on face-to-face interviews; 18% were missing for delayed recall memory HVLT-R, and none were missing for the HRQL Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire.19 Multiple imputation models were not completed for this moderation analyses because the missing data did not affect the results in overall tests of the interventions.19

Results

A total of 276 patients consented to participate in the study, 256 patients were screened as eligible and randomized, and 233 finished the 8-month study. The 256 patients included 139 (54.3%) women and 117 (45.7%) men. The mean MoCA score was 25.3 (SD = 2.5) for the 256 patients. The mean age was 66.4 (SD = 12.3) years and the mean education was 13.9 (SD = 2.6) years. Self-reported race was: Asian, 1 (0.4%); Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, 1 (0.4%); Black/African American, 37 (14.5%); White, 216 (84.4%), and more than 1 race, 1 (0.4%). Ethnicity was: Hispanic or Latino, 4 (1.6%), Non-Hispanic or Latino, 251 (98.1%); unknown, 1 (0.4%). The mean LVEF of the 256 patients was 49.3% (SD = 14.4%) and NYHA class frequencies were 23 (9%) class I, 96 (37.5%) class II, 134 (52.3%) class III, 1 (0.4%) class IV, and 2 (0.8%) missing. Of the 256 patients, 243 had blood available for genotyping. Of the 243 patients, 71.6% were BDNF Met negative and 28.4% were BDNF Met positive and 67.9% were APOE-ε4 negative and 32.1% were APOE-ε4 positive.

Eighty-five patients were randomized to CCT using BrainHQ and 72 remained for final analysis. Six of the 85 patients did not receive the allocated CCT for the following reasons: too sick, n = 2; lack of interest, n = 2; too busy, n = 1; and died before intervention started, n = 1. Seven of the 85 patients were lost to followup for the following reasons: too busy, n = 3; too sick, n = 2; lack of interest, n = 1; moved out of state, n = 1. Eighty-six patients were randomized to the active control computerized puzzles intervention and 80 remained for final analysis. One of the 86 patients did not receive the allocated puzzles intervention because of entering hospice. Five of the 85 patients were lost to followup for the following reasons: too sick, n = 2; unable to contact, n = 2; died, n = 1.

Eighty-five patients were randomized to usual care and 81 remained for final analysis. Four of the 85 patients were lost to followup because they reported being too sick.

The descriptive statistics are presented for the demographic and clinical variables of the 3 groups in Table 1. There were no statistically significant differences in the pre-specified moderators of level of depressive symptoms, BDNF Val66Met, and APOE-ε4 or the post hoc moderators of gender, global cognitive function (MoCA) and age among the 3 groups (Table 1). Notably, there were significant differences in HF severity because more patients with increased HF severity (NYHA class 3) were randomized to CCT (57.7%) and computerized puzzles (59.3%) than to usual care (41.2%) (p = .049).

Table 1.

Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics by group (n = 256)

Characteristic BrainHQ (n = 85) Puzzles (n = 86) Usual Care (n = 85) P Value

Age, y, mean ± SD 67.1 ± 12.9 66.8 ± 12.2 65.2 ± 11.9 .544a
Age category, n (%) .119b
 < 67.5 y 37 (43.5) 41 (47.7) 50 (58.8)
 >= 67.5 y 48 (56.5) 45 (52.3) 35 (41.2)
Gender, n (%) .972b
 Men 39 (45.9) 40 (46.5) 38 (44.7)
 Women 46 (54.1) 46 (53.5) 47 (55.3)
Race, n (%) .560c
 Asian 0 (0) 1 (1.2) 0 (0)
 Native Hawaiian/Pacific 0 (0) 0 (0) 1 (1.2)
 Black/African American 11 (12.9) 11 (12.8) 15 (17.7)
 White 74 (87.1) 74 (86.1) 68 (80.0)
 More than one race 0 (0) 0 (0) 1 (1.2)
Ethnicity, n (%) .649c
 Hispanic or Latino 2 (2.4) 1 (1.2) 1 (1.2)
 Non-Hispanic or Latino 82 (96.5) 85 (98.8) 84 (98.8)
 Unknown 1 (1.2) 0 (0) 0 (0)
Marital status, n (%) .825b
 Married 42 (49.4) 45 (52.3) 46 (54.1)
 Not married 43 (50.6) 41 (47.7) 39 (45.9)
Education, y, mean ± SD 13.8 ± 2.9 13.9 ± 2.5 14.0 ± 2.4 .928a
LVEF, %, mean ± SD 48.9 ± 14.5 49.3 ± 14.1 49.5 ± 14.6 .965a
NYHA Class, n (%) .049c
 I/II 35 (41.2) 35 (40.7) 49 (57.7)
 III 49 (57.7) 51 (59.3) 35 (41.2)
 Missing 1 (1.2) 0 (0) 1 (1.2)
History comorbid condition, n (%)
 Atrial fibrillation 33 (38.8) 41 (47.7) 36 (42.4) .500b
 Hypertension 71 (83.5) 70 (81.4) 69 (81.2) .907b
 Coronary artery disease 36 (42.4) 39 (45.4) 37 (43.5) .924b
 Coronary artery bypass 17 (20.0) 21 (24.4) 13 (15.3) .328b
 Depression 19 (22.4) 13 (15.1) 18 (21.2) .440b
 Diabetes 42 (49.4) 39 (45.4) 36 (42.4) .651b
 Myocardial infarction 14 (16.5) 19 (22.1) 17 (20.0) .645b
 Sudden cardiac arrest 2 (2.4) 2 (2.3) 2 (2.4) 1.000c
 Stroke 6 (7.1) 9 (10.5) 9 (10.6) .669b
 Transient ischemic attack 5 (5.9) 4 (4.7) 2 (2.4) .555c
 Ventricular arrhythmias 13 (15.3) 13 (15.1) 14 (16.5) .966b
MoCA at enrollment, mean ± SD 25.4 ± 2.6 25.1 ± 2.6 25.4 ± 2.5 .683a
 Normal, mean ± SD 27.5 ± 1.3 27.4 ± 1.3 27.5 ± 1.1 .809a
 Low, mean ± SD 23.4 ± 1.6 23.1 ±1.6 23.5 ± 1.6 .518a
MoCA category, n (%) .974b
 Normal, ≥ 26 41 (48.2) 40 (46.5) 40 (47.1)
 Low, 19 – 25 44 (51.8) 46 (53.5) 45 (52.9)
PHQ-8, mean ± SD 5.9 ± 4.7 6.0 ± 5.8 5.7 ± 4.5 .913a
PHQ-8 category, n (%) .774b
 Minimal to mild 66 (77.6) 69 (80.2) 70 (82.4)
 Moderate to severe 19 (22.4) 17 (19.8) 15 (17.6)
BDNF Val66Met, n (%) .284c
 Met Absent 55 (64.7) 65 (75.6) 54 (63.5)
 Met Present 24 (28.2) 17 (19.8) 28 (33.0)
 Missing 6 (7.1) 4 (4.6) 3 (3.5)
APOE-ε4, n (%) .775c
 Absent 54 (63.5) 53 (61.6) 58 (68.2)
 Present 25 (29.4) 29 (33.7) 24 (28.2)
 Missing 6 (7.1) 4 (4.7) 3 (3.6)

P <.05

a

Analysis of variance

b

Chi-square test

c

Fisher’s exact test

Abbreviations: y, years; SD, standard deviation; LVEF, left ventricular ejection fraction; NYHA, New York Heart Association; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; PHQ-8, Patient Health Questionnaire-8; BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; APOE-ε4, apolipoprotein E ε4 allele.

In the CCT group, 42 (49%) of 85 patients met the 90% adherence rate (≥ 36 minutes) and mean total time spent on telephone calls was 35.2 (SD = 19.9) minutes by intervenors delivering the weekly nurse enhancement interventions. In the puzzles group, 57 (66%) of 86 patients met the 90% adherence rate and mean total time spent was 34.3 (SD = 20.3) minutes by intervenors delivering the weekly nurse enhancement interventions. In the usual care group, mean total time spent was 23.1 (SD = 9.6) minutes by intervenors delivering the weekly nurse enhancement interventions. Adherence was significantly higher among the patients in the puzzles group than the CCT group (p = .026). The time spent delivering the weekly telephone calls was significantly different among the groups (p < .0001).

The results are presented in Tables 2, 3, and 4 for the pre-specified mixed effects models with moderation analyses. Level of depressive symptoms, BDNF Val66Met, and APOE-ε4 were not statistically significant moderators. There were no cases in which group differences were significant for the moderators at specific time points with the Bonferroni corrections, and therefore, the hypotheses were not supported. In the post hoc mixed effects models with moderation analyses, gender, global cognitive function (MoCA), age, and HF severity (NYHA class) were not statistically significant moderators.

Table 2.

Moderation results for level of depressive symptoms

Predicted Means and 95% Confidence Intervals
Visit Depressive Level BrainHQ (n = 85) Puzzles (n = 86) Usual Care (n = 85) P Value1

Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised, Delayed Recall2 .298
10 Weeks Minimal to mild 9.1 (8.4 to 9.7) 8.9 (8.3 to 9.5) 9.1 (8.6 to 9.7) .834
Moderate to severe 8.1 (6.9 to 9.4) 8.8 (7.5 to 10.1) 8.8 (7.5 to 10.1) .674
4 Months Minimal to mild 9.3 (8.8 to 9.9) 9.5 (8.9 to 10.0) 9.9 (9.4 to 10.5) .249
Moderate to severe 9.8 (8.7 to 11.0) 9.1 (7.9 to 10.3) 8.7 (7.5 to 9.9) .366
8 Months Minimal to mild 9.3 (8.8 to 9.8) 9.6 (9.1 to 10.1) 10.0 (9.5 to 10.5) .161
Moderate to severe 9.2 (8.1 to 10.3) 9.1 (8.0 to 10.2) 9.2 (8.1 to 10.2) .995

Serum BDNF, ng/ml3 .709
10 Weeks Minimal to mild 16.4 (14.4 to 18.3) 16.9 (15.0 to 18.8) 16.6 (14.8 to 18.4) .901
Moderate to severe 17.2 (13.4 to 21.0) 17.3 (13.3 to 21.4) 15.6 (11.9 to 19.3) .720
4 Months Minimal to mild 16.1 (14.1 to 18.2) 17.9 (16.1 to 19.8) 16.6 (14.7 to 18.4) .297
Moderate to severe 19.6 (15.6 to 23.6) 17.8 (13.9 to 21.7) 15.9 (11.7 to 20.1) .385
8 Months Minimal to mild 15.8 (13.7 to 17.9) 16.0 (14.1 to 17.9) 16.9 (15.0 to 18.9) .654
Moderate to severe 19.6 (15.5 to 23.8) 15.9 (11.3 to 20.4) 17.3 (13.1 to 21.5) .429

CogState One Back Accuracy4 .777
10 Weeks Minimal to mild 1.30 (1.24 to 1.35) 1.33 (1.27 to 1.38) 1.33 (1.28 to 1.37) .670
Moderate to severe 1.26 (1.15 to 1.36) 1.33 (1.21 to 1.44) 1.23 (1.12 to 1.34) .446
4 Months Minimal to mild 1.32 (1.27 to 1.37) 1.34 (1.30 to 1.39) 1.33 (1.28 to 1.38) .825
Moderate to severe 1.27 (1.17 to 1.37) 1.32 (1.22 to 1.43) 1.35 (1.25 to 1.45) .548
8 Months Minimal to mild 1.30 (1.24 to 1.35) 1.35 (1.29 to 1.40) 1.31 (1.26 to 1.36) .380
Moderate to severe 1.29 (1.17 to 1.40) 1.31 (1.19 to 1.43) 1.33 (1.22 to 1.44) .833


Everyday Problems Test5 .827
10 Weeks Minimal to mild 26.8 (25.8 to 27.7) 27.4 (26.4 to 28.3) 27.9 (27.0 to 28.8) .132
Moderate to severe 26.5 (24.6 to 28.4) 27.7 (25.6 to 29.7) 27.5 (25.5 to 29.4) .607
4 Months Minimal to mild 27.0 (26.0 to 28.1) 26.9 (25.9 to 27.9) 27.8 (26.8 to 28.8) .359
Moderate to severe 26.5 (24.3 to 28.7) 27.0 (24.8 to 29.2) 25.9 (23.8 to 28.0) .744
8 Months Minimal to mild 26.1 (25.0 to 27.2) 27.1 (26.1 to 28.1) 27.9 (26.9 to 28.9) .044
Moderate to severe 27.5 (25.2 to 29.7) 28.1 (25.7 to 30.4) 27.2 (25.0 to 29.3) .893

Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire6 .633
10 Weeks Minimal to mild 23.7 (19.6 to 27.7) 24.3 (20.3 to 28.2) 23.4 (19.6 to 27.2) .937
Moderate to severe 51.2 (43.2 to 59.3) 50.3 (41.9 to 58.8) 48.8 (40.7 to 56.9) .893
4 Months Minimal to mild 25.2 (20.4 to 30.0) 23.5 (18.9 to 28.0) 25.9 (21.4 to 30.5) .702
Moderate to severe 54.1 (44.9 to 63.3) 41.9 (32.4 to 51.4) 48.1 (38.4 to 57.8) .148
8 Months Minimal to mild 27.0 (21.8 to 32.3) 24.5 (19.6 to 29.4) 26.2 (21.4 to 31.1) .752
Moderate to severe 51.6 (41.6 to 61.6) 47.1 (36.5 to 57.8) 52.8 (42.3 to 63.3) .712

P <.05

1

The first p-value for an outcome is the p-value for the overall test of 3-way interaction (compare to alpha = 0.05). The remaining six p-values test for group differences within each time and moderation value combination (compare to alpha=0.008).

Abbreviations: BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; ng/ml, nanograms per milliliter.

2

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (85, 69, 62, 57); Puzzles (86, 67, 71, 66); Usual Care (85, 81, 70, 66)

3

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (79, 64, 56, 49); Puzzles (82, 64, 70, 58); Usual Care (80, 75, 64, 58)

4

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (80, 68, 62, 54); Puzzles (85, 66, 70, 59); Usual Care (83, 81, 71, 61)

5

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (85, 69, 62, 54); Puzzles (85, 68, 71, 60); Usual Care (85, 81, 71, 62)

6

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (85, 74, 72, 72); Puzzles (86, 75, 80, 80); Usual Care (85, 85, 78, 81)

Table 3.

Moderation results for BDNF Val66Met

Predicted Means and 95% Confidence Intervals
Visit BDNF Met BrainHQ (n = 79) Puzzles (n = 82) Usual Care (n = 82) P Value1

Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised, Delayed Recall2 .253
10 Weeks Absent 9.4 (8.7 to 10.1) 8.8 (8.2 to 9.5) 9.4 (8.8 to 10.1) .308
Present 7.6 (6.5 to 8.6) 9.3 (8.1 to 10.5) 8.6 (7.7 to 9.5) .079
4 Months Absent 9.7 (9.1 to 10.3) 9.7 (9.2 to 10.3) 10.1 (9.5 to 10.7) .497
Present 8.4 (7.5 to 9.4) 8.6 (7.5 to 9.7) 9.1 (8.2 to 9.9) .530
8 Months Absent 9.4 (8.8 to 10.1) 9.6 (9.1 to 10.1) 10.0 (9.4 to 10.5) .363
Present 8.7 (7.9 to 9.6) 9.4 (8.4 to 10.4) 9.4 (8.6 to 10.3) .397

Serum BDNF, ng/ml3 .765
10 Weeks Absent 16.9 (14.8 to 18.9) 17.8 (15.9 to 19.8) 16.9 (14.9 to 18.9) .672
Present 15.6 (12.4 to 18.8) 14.2 (10.7 to 17.8) 15.2 (12.5 to 18.0) .811
4 Months Absent 17.4 (15.2 to 19.5) 18.1 (16.2 to 20.0) 17.2 (15.1 to 19.3) .737
Present 15.4 (12.3 to 18.6) 17.5 (13.9 to 21.1) 14.8 (11.9 to 17.7) .423
8 Months Absent 17.6 (15.3 to 20.0) 16.3 (14.4 to 18.3) 17.4 (15.3 to 19.5) .613
Present 14.4 (11.2 to 17.6) 15.2 (11.3 to 19.1) 16.0 (12.9 to 19.2) .720

CogState One Back Accuracy4 .480
10 Weeks Absent 1.29 (1.23 to 1.35) 1.33 (1.27 to 1.38) 1.33 (1.27 to 1.39) .501
Present 1.29 (1.20 to 1.38) 1.31 (1.20 to 1.42) 1.28 (1.20 to 1.36) .884
4 Months Absent 1.31 (1.25 to 1.36) 1.35 (1.30 to 1.40) 1.33 (1.27 to 1.38) .517
Present 1.31 (1.23 to 1.40) 1.32 (1.22 to 1.41) 1.34 (1.27 to 1.42) .832
8 Months Absent 1.32 (1.26 to 1.38) 1.34 (1.28 to 1.39) 1.29 (1.24 to 1.35) .515
Present 1.25 (1.16 to 1.33) 1.35 (1.24 to 1.45) 1.35 (1.27 to 1.44) .156

Everyday Problems Test5 .445
10 Weeks Absent 26.6 (25.6 to 27.7) 27.4 (26.4 to 28.4) 28.1 (27.1 to 29.1) .080
Present 27.2 (25.5 to 28.9) 27.7 (25.8 to 29.5) 27.3 (25.9 to 28.8) .907
4 Months Absent 27.5 (26.4 to 28.7) 27.0 (26.0 to 28.1) 27.9 (26.8 to 29.0) .480
Present 25.8 (24.1 to 27.6) 27.3 (25.3 to 29.2) 27.2 (25.6 to 28.7) .379
8 Months Absent 26.9 (25.6 to 28.1) 27.3 (26.2 to 28.3) 27.8 (26.7 to 29.0) .472
Present 25.6 (23.8 to 27.3) 27.5 (25.4 to 29.6) 27.8 (26.2 to 29.5) .111

Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire6 .958
10 Weeks Absent 30.5 (25.6 to 35.3) 29.4 (24.8 to 33.9) 29.9 (25.2 to 34.5) .926
Present 27.0 (19.5 to 34.5) 29.2 (21.0 to 37.4) 25.7 (18.9 to 32.5) .742
4 Months Absent 31.2 (25.6 to 36.9) 27.0 (22.0 to 32.1) 29.9 (24.5 to 35.3) .451
Present 29.4 (21.2 to 37.7) 26.4 (17.0 to 35.7) 31.3 (23.7 to 38.9) .675
8 Months Absent 32.2 (26.0 to 38.3) 28.3 (22.9 to 33.7) 30.9 (25.2 to 36.7) .569
Present 31.0 (22.1 to 39.9) 29.6 (19.5 to 39.8) 31.4 (23.1 to 39.6) .958
1

The first p-value for an outcome is the p-value for the overall test of 3-way interaction (compare to alpha = 0.05). The remaining six p-values test for group differences within each time and moderation value combination (compare to alpha = 0.008).

Abbreviations: BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; Met, methionine substitution for valine at codon 66; ng/ml, nanograms per milliliter.

2

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (79, 65, 58, 54); Puzzles (82, 66, 70, 64); Usual Care (82, 79, 69, 64)

3

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (78, 64, 56, 49); Puzzles (82, 64, 70, 58); Usual Care (80, 75, 64, 58)

4

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (75, 64, 58, 52); Puzzles (81, 65, 69, 59); Usual Care (80, 79, 70, 60)

5

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (79, 65, 58, 52); Puzzles (81, 67, 70, 60); Usual Care (82, 79, 70, 61)

6

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (79, 68, 66, 66); Puzzles (82, 71, 76, 76); Usual Care (82, 82, 76, 78)

Table 4.

Moderation results for APOE-ε4

Predicted Means and 95% Confidence Intervals
Visit APOE-ε4 BrainHQ (n = 79) Puzzles (n = 82) Usual Care (n = 82) P Value1

Hopkins Verbal Learning Test – Revised, Delayed Recall2 .409
10 Weeks Absent 9.2 (8.5 to 9.9) 9.2 (8.5 to 9.8) 9.3 (8.6 to 9.9) .964
Present 8.1 (7.1 to 9.1) 8.4 (7.4 to 9.3) 9.0 (8.0 to 9.9) .413
4 Months Absent 9.4 (8.8 to 10.1) 9.6 (9.0 to 10.3) 10.0 (9.5 to 10.6) .311
Present 9.0 (8.1 to 10.0) 9.1 (8.3 to 9.9) 9.1 (8.2 to 10.1) .984
8 Months Absent 9.3 (8.8 to 9.9) 9.6 (9.0 to 10.1) 10.1 (9.6 to 10.7) .111
Present 9.0 (8.1 to 9.9) 9.5 (8.7 to 10.3) 8.9 (8.0 to 9.8) .541

Serum BDNF, ng/ml3 .122
10 Weeks Absent 16.2 (14.2 to 18.2) 16.6 (14.5 to 18.6) 16.6 (14.7 to 18.6) .932
Present 17.1 (14.0 to 20.2) 17.7 (14.7 to 20.7) 15.8 (12.9 to 18.7) .589
4 Months Absent 17.3 (15.1 to 19.4) 16.5 (14.5 to 18.6) 16.7 (14.7 to 18.7) .863
Present 15.8 (12.6 to 18.9) 20.4 (17.6 to 23.2) 15.6 (12.5 to 18.7) .012
8 Months Absent 16.7 (14.5 to 19.0) 14.8 (12.7 to 16.9) 17.0 (15.0 to 19.1) .205
Present 16.0 (12.7 to 19.4) 18.6 (15.4 to 21.7) 16.6 (13.1 to 20.0) .463

CogState One Back Accuracy4 .056
10 Weeks Absent 1.32 (1.26 to 1.38) 1.29 (1.23 to 1.35) 1.32 (1.26 to 1.37) .761
Present 1.21 (1.12 to 1.30) 1.39 (1.30 to 1.48) 1.30 (1.22 to 1.39) .014
4 Months Absent 1.31 (1.25 to 1.37) 1.32 (1.27 to 1.38) 1.35 (1.30 to 1.40) .484
Present 1.30 (1.22 to 1.39) 1.38 (1.30 to 1.45) 1.28 (1.20 to 1.36) .167
8 Months Absent 1.29 (1.23 to 1.35) 1.32 (1.26 to 1.38) 1.30 (1.24 to 1.35) .778
Present 1.28 (1.19 to 1.37) 1.38 (1.29 to 1.46) 1.36 (1.27 to 1.46) .290

Everyday Problems Test5 .714
10 Weeks Absent 27.1 (26.0 to 28.2) 27.9 (26.8 to 28.9) 28.2 (27.2 to 29.2) .247
Present 26.0 (24.4 to 27.6) 26.5 (24.9 to 28.0) 27.2 (25.7 to 28.7) .452
4 Months Absent 27.7 (26.5 to 28.8) 27.1 (26.0 to 28.2) 27.8 (26.7 to 28.8) .586
Present 25.5 (23.7 to 27.2) 26.9 (25.4 to 28.4) 27.4 (25.7 to 29.0) .215
8 Months Absent 27.0 (25.8 to 28.3) 27.6 (26.4 to 28.7) 28.1 (27.0 to 29.2) .404
Present 25.1 (23.3 to 26.9) 26.7 (25.0 to 28.4) 27.2 (25.4 to 29.1) .200

Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire6 .582
10 Weeks Absent 29.1 (24.2 to 34.0) 30.8 (25.9 to 35.6) 30.1 (25.5 to 34.7) .855
Present 30.2 (22.8 to 37.5) 27.0 (19.9 to 34.1) 24.2 (17.2 to 31.2) .395
4 Months Absent 30.1 (24.6 to 35.6) 29.6 (24.3 to 35.0) 33.2 (28.1 to 38.4) .506
Present 32.0 (23.4 to 40.5) 21.9 (14.4 to 29.5) 23.2 (15.1 to 31.2) .126
8 Months Absent 32.8 (26.8 to 38.8) 31.0 (25.2 to 36.8) 34.4 (28.8 to 39.9) .658
Present 29.2 (20.0 to 38.5) 24.3 (16.2 to 32.4) 22.7 (14.1 to 31.4) .522

P <.05

1

The first p-value for an outcome is the p-value for the overall test of 3-way interaction (compare to alpha = 0.05). The remaining six p-values test for group differences within each time and moderation value combination (compare to alpha = 0.008).

Abbreviations: APOE-ε4, apolipoprotein E, ε4 allele; BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; ng/ml, nanograms per milliliter.

2

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (79, 65, 58, 54); Puzzles (82, 66, 70, 64); Usual Care (82, 79, 69, 64)

3

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (78, 64, 56, 49); Puzzles (82, 64, 70, 58); Usual Care (80, 75, 64, 58)

4

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (75, 64, 58, 52); Puzzles (81, 65, 69, 59); Usual Care (80, 79, 70, 60)

5

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (79, 65, 58, 52); Puzzles (81, 67, 70, 60); Usual Care (82, 79, 70, 61)

6

Sample sizes per group (Baseline, 10 weeks, 4 months, 8 months): BrainHQ (79, 68, 66, 66); Puzzles (82, 71, 76, 76); Usual Care (82, 82, 76, 78)

Discussion

The important results of this study were that depressive symptoms, BDNF Val66Met, and APOE-ε4 were not statistically significant moderators of intervention response to outcomes. These results suggest that the cognitive and HRQL improvements among patients in all 3 groups over 8 months19 were not significantly associated with these moderators.

The hypothesis was not supported that level of depressive symptoms moderated intervention response to outcomes. The percentage of patients with moderate to severe depressive symptoms was highest in the CCT group (22.4%) but lower than the percentage of patients with HF and depressive symptoms (29%) in past meta-analyses.24 The lower percentage of patients with moderate to severe depressive symptoms may reflect the medical management that the patients received to treat depression. In MEMOIR-HF, 85 (33.2%) of the 256 patients had been prescribed antidepressant medications prior to enrollment.19 The unequal numbers of patients with minimal to mild and moderate to severe depressive symptoms scores may have limited the ability to detect a moderating effect. A remaining question is whether CCT reduced or alleviated depressive symptoms among patients enrolled in MEMOIR-HF. In a 2-arm randomized controlled trial of CCT using a visual speed of processing intervention among older adults aged 55 to 102 years, CCT decreased depressive symptoms among adults living independently in the community but increased depressive symptoms among adults living in assisted living communities.85 Future studies may be considered to evaluate biological pathways of depression and CCT efficacy in HF.86

The hypothesis was not supported that BDNF Val66Met moderated intervention response. The prevalence was similar for this polymorphism in the sample compared with the U.S. population prevalence of 30%,44 although it did vary among the groups from 19.8% (puzzles group) to 33.0% (usual care group) which may have confounded the results. Multiomics approaches that examine biomarkers associated with this specific polymorphism (e.g., tropomyosin receptor kinase-B) may provide new insights into neuronal function and plasticity in response to CCT and other cognitive interventions.87

The hypothesis was not supported that APOE-ε4 moderated intervention response. In the U.S. the prevalence of people with at least one APOE-ε4 allele is 30%. In MEMOIR-HF, the prevalence was 28.2% to 33.7% across the groups. Other APOE alleles (e.g., APOE-ε2) may have neuroprotective effects and need to be evaluated in larger samples of patients.88

In post hoc analyses, no statistically significant moderation was found by gender, global cognitive function, age, and HF severity. There are at least 3 plausible explanations for these results. First, there may be no actual moderating effect of these variables. Past studies mainly evaluated associations rather than moderating effects.6,52,56 Second, variables that were not measured may have confounded the data and contributed to lack of significant results (e.g., pain, sleep-disordered breathing). Third, unidentified etiological pathways may be responsible for moderating effects. Studies of CCT may need to stratify or randomize patients by measures of HF severity, but it is not clear which measure would be most relevant (e.g., specific echocardiographic indicators; biomarkers like N-terminal prohormone of brain natriuretic peptide).89

A possible limitation of this study is missing data resulting from the COVID-19 pandemic. Multiple imputation models were computed to evaluate effects of missing data for the main hypotheses and no statistically significant differences were found.19 Therefore, it seems unlikely that the missing data accounted for the lack of statistically significant results in this moderation analysis. Another possible limitation is the low intervention adherence rates for some patients. However, the per protocol analyses did not support differences based on adherence rates of the CCT group in the main results.19

Conclusion

In conclusion, depressive symptoms, BDNF Val66Met, and APOE-ε4 were not significant moderators of intervention response to outcomes among the 256 patients randomized in the MEMOIR-HF study. Gender, global cognitive function, age, and HF severity were not significant moderators in post hoc analyses. Continued analyses are desirable for MEMOIR-HF data to evaluate variables that may have been associated with the improvement over time found among patients randomized to all 3 groups. These variables, measured in MEMOIR-HF, include the nurse enhancement intervention, change in clinical condition during the intervention phase, physical function, and excessive daytime sleepiness.19 Clinicians caring for patients with HF ought to continue to assess patients for cognitive dysfunction and how it may reduce their memory, performance of IADLs, and HRQL.6,10,90 Family caregivers need to be involved in care of patients with HF and cognitive dysfunction to ensure the best care is delivered and continued in home settings.90 Patients with HF are waiting for novel interventions to be developed and tested to prevent cognitive dysfunction and decline.

Highlights.

  • Depressive symptoms, BDNF Val66Met, and APOE-ε4 were not significant moderators.

  • Global cognition, gender, age, and HF severity were not significant moderators.

  • Studies are needed to elucidate biological mechanisms of cognitive dysfunction in HF and test novel interventions to improve memory in patients.

Acknowledgements:

We acknowledge Elham Algashgari, MSN, BSN; Samantha Endean, MA, BA; Mary Heskett, BSN; Judy Kline, BSN; Jia Liu, PhD, BSN; Ellen McAdams, MSW; and Suzanne Wang, BSN, for assistance with recruitment, intervention delivery, and data collection. We acknowledge William J. Gill, MD, Richard J. Kovacs, MD, and Nathan D. Lambert, MD, Indiana University School of Medicine, Krannert Institute of Cardiology, for assistance with recruitment and data collection. We acknowledge Heather Burney, MS, for assistance with statistical analysis. This study was supported by the National Institute of Nursing Research R01 NR016116 and Center for Enhancing Quality of Life in Chronic Illness, Indiana University School of Nursing. Serum BDNF analyte measures were completed by the Translation Core within the Center for Diabetes and Metabolic Diseases at Indiana University School of Medicine (P30DK097512). DNA analysis measures were completed by the Maryland Genomics at the Institute for Genomic Sciences.

Footnotes

Disclosures: The authors have no relevant disclosures to report.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Tsao CW, Aday AW, Almarzooq ZI, Alonso A, Beaton AZ, Bittencourt MS, et al. Heart disease and stroke statistics-2022 update: A report from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2022;145:e153–e639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Savarese G, Lund LH. Global public health burden of heart failure. Card Fail Rev. 2017;3:7–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Woo MA, Kumar R, Macy PM, Fonarow GC, Harper RM. Brain injury in autonomic, emotional, and cognitive regulatory areas in patients with heart failure. J Card Fail. 2009;15:214–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Siachos T, Vanbakel A, Feldman DS, Uber W, Simpson KN, Pereira NL. Silent strokes in patients with heart failure. J Card Fail. 2005;11:485–489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kumar R, Woo MA, Birrer BVX, Macey PM, Fonarow GC, Hamilton MA, et al. Mammillary bodies and fornix fibers are injured in heart failure. Neurobiol Dis. 2009;33:236–242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Pressler SJ, Subramanian U, Kareken D, Perkins SM, Gradus-Pizlo I, Sauve MJ, et al. Cognitive deficits in chronic heart failure. Nurs Res. 2010;59:127–139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ampadu J, Morley JE. Heart failure and cognitive dysfunction. Int J Cardiol. 2015;178:12–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Pressler SJ, Kim J, Riley P, Ronis DL, Gradus-Pizlo I. Memory dysfunction, psychomotor slowing, and decreased executive function predict mortality in patients with heart failure and low ejection fraction. J Card Fail. 2010;16:750–760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Alosco ML, Spitznagel MB, Cohen R, Sweet LH, Colbert LH, Josephson R, et al. Cognitive impairment is independently associated with reduced instrumental activities of daily living in persons with heart failure. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2012;27:44–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Sloan RS, Pressler SJ. Cognitive deficits in heart failure: Re-cognition of vulnerability as a strange new world. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2009;24:241–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lampit A, Hallock H, Valenzuela M. Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PLoS Med. 2014;11:e1001756. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hill NTM, Mowszowski L, Naismith SL, Chadwick VL, Valenzuela M, Lampit A. Computerized cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174:329–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ellis ML, Edwards JD, Peterson L, Roker R, Athilingam P. Effects of cognitive speed of processing training among older adults with heart failure. J Aging Health. 2014;26:600–615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Athilingham P, Edwards JD, Valdes EG, Ji M, Guglin M. Computerized auditory cognitive training to improve cognition and functional outcomes in patients with heart failure: Results of a pilot study. Heart Lung. 2015;44:120–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pressler SJ, Therrien B, Riley PL, Chou C-C, Ronis DL, Koelling TM, et al. Nurse-enhanced memory intervention in heart failure: The MEMOIR Study. J Card Fail. 2011;17:832–843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pressler SJ, Titler M, Koelling TM, Riley PL, Jung M, Hoyland-Domenico L, et al. Nurse-enhanced computerized cognitive training increases serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and improves working memory in heart failure. J Card Fail. 2015;21:630–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gary RA, Paul S, Corwin E, Butts B, Miller AH, Hepburn K, et al. Exercise and cognitive training as a strategy to improve neurocognitive outcomes in heart failure: A pilot study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;27:809–819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Pressler SJ, Giordani B, Titler M, Gradus-Pizlo I, Smith D, Dorsey SG, et al. Design and rationale of the Cognitive Intervention to Improve Memory in Heart Failure Patients (MEMOIR-HF) Study. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2018;33:344–355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Pressler SJ, Jung M, Gradus-Pizlo I, Titler MG, Smith DG, Gao S, et al. Randomized controlled trial of a cognitive intervention to improve memory in heart failure. J Card Fail. 2022;28:519–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51:1173–1182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Breitborde NJK, Srihari VH, Pollard JM, Addington DN, Woods SW. Mediators and moderators in early intervention research. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2010;4:143–152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hayes AF, Rockwood NJ. Regression-based statistical mediation and moderation analysis in clinical research: Observations, recommendations, and implementation. Behav Res Ther. 2017;98:39–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. 3rd ed. NY: Guilford. 2022 [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sokoreli I, deVries JJG, Pauws SC, Steyerberg EW. Depression and anxiety as predictors of mortality among heart failure patients: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Heart Fail Rev. 2016:21:49–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Motter JN, Pimontel MA, Rindskopf D, Devanand DP, Doraiswamy PM, Sneed JR. Computerized cognitive training and functional recovery in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2016;189:184–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chan JYC, Chan TK, Kwok TCY, Wong SYS, Lee ATC, Tsoi KKF. Cognitive training interventions and depression in mild cognitive impairment and dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Age Ageing. 2020;49:738–747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Legemaat AM, Semkovska M, Brouwer M, Geurtsen GJ, Burger H, Denys D, et al. Effectiveness of cognitive remediation in depression: A meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2021;1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Thérond A, Pezzoli P, Abbas M, Howard A, Bowie CR, Guimond S. The efficacy of cognitive remediation in depression: A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2021;284:238–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Woolf C, Lampit A, Shahnawaz Z, Sabates J, Norrie LM, Burke D, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive training in adults with major depressive disorder. Neuropsychol Rev. 2022;32:419–437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Egan MF, Kojima M, Callicott JH, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Bertolino BS, et al. The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal function. Cell. 2003;112:257–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, Small GW, et al. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261:921–923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Miranda M, Morici JF, Zanoni MB, Bekinschtein P. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor: A key molecule for memory in the healthy and the pathological brain. Front Cell Neurosci. 2019;13:Article 363. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, Chaddock L, Heo S, McLaren M, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is associated with age-related decline in hippocampal volume. J Neurosci. 2010;30:5368–5375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fisher M, Mellon SH, Wolkowitz O, Vinogradov S. Neuroscience-informed auditory training in schizophrenia: A final report of the effects on cognition and serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2016;3:1–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Vinogradov S, Fisher M, Holland C, Shelly W, Wolkowitz O, Mellon SH. Is serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor a biomarker for cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia? Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:549–553 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ng TKS, Ho CSH, Tam WWS, Kua EH, Ho RCM. Decreased serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) levels in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD): A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Mole Sci. 2019;20:257. 10.3390/ijms2002057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Pressler SJ, Harrison JM, Titler M, Koelling TM, Jung M, Dorsey SG, et al. APOE ε4 and memory among patients with heart failure. West J Nurs Res. 2017;39:455–472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Halloway S, Jung M, Yeh A, Liu J, McAdams E, Barley M, et al. An integrative review of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and serious cardiovascular conditions. Nurs Res. 2020;69:376–390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Mori Y, Tsuji M, Oguchi T, Kasuga K, Kimura A, Futamura A, et al. Serum BDNF as a potential biomarker of Alzheimer’s disease: Verification through assessment of serum, cerebrospinal fluid, and medial temporal lobe atrophy. Front Neurol. 2021;12:653267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dempster E, Toulopoulou T, McDonald C, Bramon E, Walshe M, Filbey, et al. Association between BDNF Val66Met genotype and episodic memory. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;134B:73–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hariri AR, Goldberg TE, Mattay VS, Kolachana BS, Callicott JH, Egan MF, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects human memory-related hippocampal activity and predicts memory performance. J Neurosci. 2003;23:6690–6694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lim YY, Villemagne VL, Laws SM, Ames D, Pietrzak RH, Ellis KA, et al. Effect of BDNF Val66Met on memory decline and hippocampal atrophy in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease: A preliminary study. PloS One. 2014;9:e86498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Zerbino DR, Achuthan P, Akanni W, Amode MR, Barrell D, Bhai J, et al. Nucleic Acids Res. 2018;46:D754–D761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Petryshen TL, Sabeti PC, Aldinger KA, Fry B, Fan JB, Schaffner SF, et al. Population genetic study of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:810–815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Raber J, Huang Y, Ashford JW. ApoE genotype accounts for the vast majority of AD risk and AD pathology. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:641–650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.National Institute on Aging. (2015, December). 2014–2015 Alzheimer’s disease progress report: Advancing research toward a cure. Retrieved from https://www.nia.nih.gov/alzheimers/publication/2014-2015-alzheimers-disease-progress-report/introduction
  • 47.Farrer LA, Cupples A, Haines JL, Hyman B, Kukull WA, Mayeux R, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium. JAMA. 1997;278:1349–1356. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bertram L, McQueen MD, Mullin K, Blacker D, Tanzi RE. Systematic meta-analysis of Alzheimer disease genetic association studies: The AlzGene database. Nat Genet. 2007;39:17–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Crean S, Ward A, Mercaldi CJ, Collins JM, Cook MD, Baker NL, et al. Apolipoprotein E e4 prevalence in Alzheimer’s disease patients across global populations. A systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2011;31:20–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Tang MX, Stern Y, Marder K, Bell K, Gurland B, Lantigua R, et al. The APOE- ε4 allele and risk of Alzheimer disease among African American, Whites, and Hispanics. JAMA. 1998;279:751–755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ward A, Crean S, Mercaldi CJ, Collins JM, Boyd D, Cook MN, et al. Prevalence of apolipoprotein E4 genotype and homozygotes (APOE e4/4) among patients diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroepidemiology. 2012;38:1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Vogels RLC, Oosterman JM, van Harten B, Scheltens P, van der Flier WM, Schroeder-Tanka JM, et al. Profile of cognitive impairment in heart failure. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55:1764–1770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Peterson RC, Thomas RG, Grundman M, Bennett D, Doody R, Ferris S, et al. Vitamin E and Donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med. 1995;352:2379–2388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lopez-Higes R, Rodriguez-Rojo IC, Prados JM, Montejo P, Del-Rio D, Delgado-Losada ML, et al. APOE ε4 modulation of training outcomes in several cognitive domains in a sample of cognitively intact older adults. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;58:1201–1215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Tsolaki AC, Tsolaki M, Pandria N, Lazarou E, Gkatzima O, Zilidou V, et al. Web-based intervention effects on mild cognitive impairment based on apolipoprotein E genotype: Quasi-experimental study. J Med Internet Res. 2020;22:e14617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Cannon JA, McMurray JJV, Quinn TJ. ‘Hearts and minds’: Association, causation and implication of cognitive impairment in heart failure. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2015;7;2–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gage FH. Brain, repair yourself. Sci Am. 2009;283:46–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Valenzuela M, Sachdev P. Can cognitive exercise prevent the onset of dementia? Systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal follow-up. Am Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;17:179–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Takeuchi H, Sekiguchi A, Taki Y, Yokoyama S, Yomogida Y, Komuro N, et al. Training of working memory impacts structural connectivity. J Neurosci. 2010;30:3297–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Howren MB, Jones MP, Dotson MM. A randomized controlled trial of cognitive training using a visual speed of processing intervention in middle-aged and older adults. PLoS One. 2013;8:e61624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Wolinsky FD, Vander Weg MW, Howren MB, Jones MP, Martin R, Luger TM, et al. Interim analyses from a randomized controlled trial to improve visual processing speed in older adults: The Iowa Healthy and Active Minds Study. BMJ Open. 2011;1:e000225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:695–699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Mahncke HW, Bronstone A, Merzenich MM. Brain plasticity and functional losses in the aged: Scientific bases for a novel intervention. Prog Brain Res. 2006;157:81–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Mahncke HW, Connor BB, Appelman J, Ahsanuddin ON, Hardy JL, Wood RA, et al. Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: A randomized, controlled study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006:103:12523–12528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Naylor MD, Brooten DA, Campbell RL, Maislin G, McCauley KM, Schwartz JS. Transitional care of older adults hospitalized with heart failure: A randomized, controlled trial. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:675–684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Koelling TM, Johnson ML, Cody RJ, Aaronson KD. Discharge education improves clinical outcomes in patients with chronic heart failure. Circulation. 2005;111:179–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Sochalski J, Jaarsma T, Krumholz HM, Laramee A, McMurray JJV, Naylor MD, et al. What works in chronic care management: The case of heart failure. Health Affairs. 2009;28:179–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Bennett SJ, Litzelman DK, Wright A, Perkins SM, Wu J, Meyer L, et al. The PUMP UP© tailored computerized program for heart failure care. Nurs Outlook. 2006;54:39–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Borelli B, Sepinwall D, Ernst D, Bellg AJ, Czajkowski S, Breger R, et al. A new tool to assess treatment fidelity and evaluation of treatment fidelity across 10 years of health behavior research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73:852–860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Benedict RHB, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Brandt J. Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-revised: Normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;12:43–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Naegelin Y, Dingsdale H, Säuberli K, Schädelin S, Kappos L, Barde Y-A. Measuring and validating the levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in human serum. eNeuro. 2018;5:e0419–17.2018,1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Maruff P, Thomas E, Cysique L, Brew B, Collie A, Snyder P, et al. Validity of the CogState brief battery: Relationship to standardized tests and sensitivity to cognitive impairment in mild traumatic brain injury, schizophrenia, and AIDS dementia complex. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24:165–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Willis SL, Marsiske M. Manual for the Everyday Problems Test. University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State University;1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Rector TS, Kubo SH, Cohn JN. Patients’ self-assessment of their congestive heart failure. Part 2: Content, reliability and validity of a new measure, the Minnesota Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire. Heart Fail. 1987;3:198–209. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Rector TS, Tschumperlin LK, Kubo SH, Bank AJ, Francis GS, McDonald KM, et al. Use of the Living with Heart Failure Questionnaire to ascertain patients’ perspectives on improvement in quality of life versus risk of drug-induced death. J Card Fail. 1995;1:201–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Kroenke K, Strine TW, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Berry JT, Mokdad AH. The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. J Affect Disord. 2009;114:163–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Pressler SJ, Subramanian U, Perkins SM, Gradus-Pizlo I, Kareken D, Kim J, et al. Measuring depressive symptoms in heart failure: Validity and reliability of the Patient Health Questionnaire-8. Am J Crit Care. 2011;20:146–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Andrews S FastQC: A quality control tool for high throughput sequence data [Online]. 2010. Available online at: http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/projects/fastqc
  • 79.Li H, Durbin R. Fast and accurate short read alignment with Burrows-Wheeler transform. Bioinformatics. 2009;25:1754–1760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Auwera GA Van der, O’Connor BD. Genomics in the Cloud: Using Docker, GATK, and WDL in Terra. 1st ed. Sebastopol, CA: O’Reilly; 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Cingolani P, Platts A, Wang LL, Coon M, Nguyen T, Wang L, et al. A program for annotating and predicting the effects of single nucleotide polymorphisms, SnpEff. SNPs in the genome of Drosophila melanogaster strain w1118; iso-2; iso-3. Fly (Austin). 2012;6:80–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Pezawas L, Verchinski BA, Mattay VS, Callicott JH, Kolachana BS, Straub RE, et al. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism and variation in human cortical morphology. J Neurosci. 2004;24:10099–10102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Belbin O, Dunn JL, Ling Y, Morgan L, Chappell S, Beaumont H, et al. Regulatory region single nucleotide polymorphisms of the apolipoprotein E gene and the rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Mol Genet. 2007;16:2199–2208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Fitzmaurice GM, Laird NM, Ware JH. Applied Longitudinal Analysis. 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: J. Wiley; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Wolinsky FD, Jones MP, Dotson MM. Does visual speed of processing training improve health-related quality of life in assisted and independent living communities?: A randomized controlled trial. Innov Aging. 2020;4:1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Sbolli M, Fiuzat M, Cani D, O’Connor CM. Depression and heart failure: The lonely comorbidity. Eur J Heart Fail. 2020;22:2007–2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Sonoyama T, Stadler LKJ, Zhu M, Keogh JM, Henning E, Hisama F, et al. Human BDNF/TrkB variants impair hippocampal synaptogenesis and associate with neurobehavioral abnormalities. Nature Sci Rep. 2020;10:9028. 10.1038/s41598-020-65531-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Reiman EM, Arboleda-Velasquez JF, Quiroz YT, Huentelman MJ, Beach TG, Caselli RJ, et al. Exceptionally low likelihood of Alzheimer’s dementia in APOE2 homozygotes from a 5,000-person neuropathological study. Nat Commun. 2020;11:667. 10.1038/s41467-019-14279-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Mueller K, Thiel F, Beutner F, Teren A, Frisch S, Ballarini T, et al. Brain damage with heart failure. Cardiac biomarker alterations and gray matter decline. Circ Res. 2020;126:750–764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Kitko L, McIlvennan CK, Bidwell JT, Dionne-Odom JN, Dunlay S, Lewis LM, et al. Family caregiving for individuals with heart failure: A scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2020;141:e864–e878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES